Gangguan Kordinasi Perkembangan
Gangguan Kordinasi Perkembangan
Gangguan Kordinasi Perkembangan
Developmental Coordination Disorder (DCD) is not a new condition. Over the years,
different terms have been used to describe children with a predominant picture of
motor difficulties. In 1925, Dupre referred to the debilite motrice (motorically
deficient). Clumsiness has been seen to be a more pejorative term, and was first
used by Orton (1937) to describe a group of children with motor difficulties.
Pioneering studies started to make their mark in the 1960s (Walton et al, 1962). In
1962, the first article on clumsy children appeared in the British Medical Journal (no
author cited) and referred to an earlier paper in the 1940s by Annell who had
described the clumsy child as being:
... awkward in movements, poor at games, hopeless in dancing and gymnastics, a
bad writer and defective in concentration. He is inattentive, cannot sit still, leaves his
shoelaces untied, does buttons wrongly, bumps into furniture, breaks glassware,
slips off his chair, kicks his legs against the desk, and perhaps reads badly.
Today, Dyspraxia is the term often used interchangeably in the U.K. with DCD,
causing some confusion and inconsistency in terminology. However, DCD is the
most recent, formal and widely internationally used term to describe these
individuals. It appears in both the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual for Mental
Disorders (American Psychological Association, APA, 1994; 2000) and the
International Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems (World Health
Organization, WHO, 1992; 1993).
DCD is defined on the basis of a failure of the acquisition of both fine and gross
motor skills, which is not explicable on the basis of impaired general learning and
similar exposure to opportunity to gain motor skills as their peers. DCD is often seen
as the Cinderella of developmental disorders and not always considered routinely
by clinicians (Kirby et al, 2007). However, there is extensive evidence that motor
difficulties have a pervasive effect on childrens lives. The difficulties affect the child
both in school and at home, and in contrast with similar aged children who acquire
skills with little effort such as dressing, playing ball games and handwriting, these
children take longer to learn and automate motor skills.
Increasing interest in these children, in academic research and in clinical and
educational practice, has focused on the need not only for early identification but
also to consider the presentation in adolescence and adulthood, as around 70% of
children continue to have difficulties when grown up (Kirby et al, 2008).
PREVALENCE
There have been wide discrepancies in the prevalence of this disorder and much of
this has been due to the use of different assessment methods. Two studies using
precise measures are contributing to the narrowing of prevalence range. Wright and
Sugden (1996) advocated a two-step approach to assessment using the Movement
Assessment Battery for Children (Movement ABC-2, Henderson & Sugden, 2007) as
the standardized measure for motor impairment and the Movement ABC Checklist as
a guide to examining the effects of motor difficulties on daily living. Using this
methodology, they found that the prevalence figure was 4-5% in mainstream primary
schools. More recently, the large U.K. based population study, the Avon Longitudinal
Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) has shown a prevalence of 1.7% with a
further 3.2% of children considered as having "probable Developmental Coordination
Disorder" in the case of broader cut-offs for coordination testing and activities of daily
living (Lingam et al, 2009).
Gender differences have been examined on numerous occasions, with the
consensus being that the condition is more prevalent in boys than girls, with
estimates ranging from a small gender difference to three or four to one. However
numbers identified may be related to a gender bias in the assessment tools used
(Lefebvre and Reid, 1998). Teacher perception of skills among boys and girls may
also influence identification (Rivard et al, 2007).
POSSIBLE CAUSES
Brain abnormalities
There has been a resurgence of interest in recent years regarding the underlying
aetiology of DCD. One of the first studies by Querne et al (2008) using functional
magnetic resonance imaging has shown increased activity in particular areas of the
left hemisphere of the brain, particularly in the middle frontal cortex (MFC) and
anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) to the inferior parietal cortex (IPC). Decreased
activity between the striatum and parietal cortex in the right hemisphere was also
evident. The authors suggest that DCD could be characterized by abnormal brain
hemispheric specialization during development.
Genetic influences
A twin study undertaken in Australia by Martin et al (2006) showed a strong additive
genetic component between subtypes of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder
(ADHD) inattentive subtype and DCD (fine motor). However, as in ADHD and other
developmental disorders, a single phenotype is highly improbable and any gene
identified will vary in its penetration.
Multiple influences
Morton's (2004) causal modelling approach considering biological, cognitive and
behavioural levels may be a good conceptual framework to use when undertaking
research and attempting to gain a greater understanding of the field. He presents
examples of various developmental disorders such as ADHD, Autistic Spectrum
Disorders (ASD) and DCD showing how a possible biological origin moves through
cognitive processes to produce the behaviours that are identified.
Diet
There has been some interest in the relationship between Omega 3 fatty acids and
DCD. However so far there has been no clear evidence of an association between
improved co-ordination with supplementation, nor of lower levels being associated
with poorer co-ordination (See Kirby et al, 2009 for review of current research).
Interestingly, antenatal maternal nutrition may influence motor outcomes in the child
(Hibbeln et al, 2007).
DIAGNOSIS AND ASSESSMENT
The American Psychiatric Association (APA) and the World Health Organization
(WHO) both have inclusive and exclusive criteria in their definitions. For APA the
inclusive criteria include: impairment in the development of motor coordination, which
can be manifested in delays in milestones such as standing and walking; poor
performance in sports activities; and untidy handwriting. This impairment leads to a
disturbance in academic performance and/or activities of daily living. Exclusive
criteria include the disturbance not being due to a general medical difficulty such as
cerebral palsy or a pervasive developmental disorder. In addition, if mental
retardation (learning difficulty) is present the motor difficulties are in excess of those
usually associated with it.
The WHO (1993) definition overlaps with the APA definition (2000) by noting that, on
a standardized test of motor impairment, a child would score two standard deviations
below the mean accompanied by interference with academic performance and/or
activities of daily living. It notes that there should be no diagnosable neurological
disorder and excludes those with an IQ below 70. The WHO recommendation that
individuals with an IQ of 70 and below are excluded from the formal definition is one
that would be agreed by most clinicians and researchers.
Despite the guidance provided by the APA and WHO, there are still a number of
issues surrounding the identification and definitions such as cut off points for
diagnosis, who undertakes the assessment, and what type of instruments are used.
Children with DCD are assessed in a variety of ways, but currently there is no
appropriate gold standard assessment instrument. In the U.K. the Movement ABC-2
(Henderson & Sugden, 2007) is the most widely used instrument, and contains a
standardized normative referenced test, plus a criterion referenced checklist.
However, other instruments such as Bruininks-Oseretsky-2 test of motor proficiency
(Bruininks & Bruininks, 2005) are also commonly used. There are also a range of
screening instruments available for use in clinical practice for example: the Early
Years Movement Checklist for 3-5 year olds (Chambers & Sugden, 2006); DCDQ-2
for school age children (Wilson et al, 2009) and Adult DCD Checklist (ADC) for the
16 plus age range (Kirby et al, in press).
An assessment of DCD must include a developmental history in order to identify any
developmental delay, especially in motor and language domains (Missiuna et al,
2002). The need for a neurological examination is also essential to exclude children
with other neurological conditions such as cerebral palsy, and muscular dystrophy.
Additionally, one needs to consider the presence of common genetic conditions
where co-ordination difficulties are commonly seen such as Fragile X and
Neurofibromatosis type 1. Other associated conditions such as BECTS syndrome
(Scabar et al, 2006) and Joint Hypermobility Syndrome (Kirby & Davies, 2007) have
been associated with DCD. Checking for common comorbid or overlapping
conditions is essential, including ADHD, speech and language impairment, Dyslexia
and ASD as these may impact on educational support and type of intervention
approaches undertaken.
Key areas of difficulties for children and adolescents with DCD are:
At home: difficulties with self care such as dressing, eating. Slower learning to ride a
bike.
At school: ball skills especially in team games, handwriting difficulties, changing for
games, copying from the board.
Common associated difficulties:
Poor executive functioning including organization of self and possessions, time
management, e.g. losing items, late with assignments, difficulty packing a bag.
Low self esteem, difficulty in making and maintaining friendships, potentially
associated with lack of opportunity and practice.
Weight gain.
COMORBIDITY
Green et al (2002) highlighted the widespread prevalence of motor impairment in
developmental disorders. The so-called pure DCD with only motor difficulties is the
exception rather than the rule (Peters & Henderson, 2008). When viewing DCD,
alongside other developmental disorders, it should be viewed as a dimensional
concept not a categorical concept as individuals appear to feature on a continuum of
disorder. Extensive evidence from a number of researchers has shown high levels of
overlap of DCD with other developmental disorders including ADHD, Dyslexia and
ASD (Pitcher et al, 2003; Rasta and Eliot, 1999; Kaplan et al, 1997). Additional
examples of this include: reading, attention and motor deficits (Kooistra et al, 2005);
social and emotional and behaviour, anxiety, and depression (Sigurdsson et al,
2002); speech and language impairment (Hill, 1998); and social and communication
impairment (Taylor et al, 2004).
PERSISTENCE AND IMPACT
There was a view until the early 1990s that children with DCD grew out of the
condition. Hall (1988) discussed in an article the need to look at the cause for
clumsiness as related to motivation and good teaching and referred to extrinsic
factors affecting the young person. He stated, motor difficulties seem to resolve in
the teen years, though they may re-emerge under stress of learning a new motor
skill.
USEFUL REFERENCES
American Psychiatric Association (1994) DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual
of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC.
American Psychiatric Association (2000) DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic and Statistical
Manual of Mental Disorders. Washington, DC.
Bruininks R. H., Bruininks B. D. (2005) Bruininks Oseretsky test of motor proficiency.
2nd Edition. Windsor, NFER-Nelson.
Cairney, J., Hay, J. A., Faught, B. E. & Hawes, R. (2005) Developmental
coordination disorder and overweight and obesity in children aged 914 years.
International Journal of Obesity, 29, 369-72
Chambers, M. E. & Sugden, D. A. (2006) Early years movement skills: description,
diagnosis and intervention. London, Whurr.
Dupr, E. (1925) Pathologie de L'Imagination et de L'Emotivite. Paris, Pavot.
ESRC Leeds University. UK-DCD Consensus Statement. Consensus Meeting Series
2004-2006. http:www.uk-dcd.org.uk.
Faught, B., Hay, J., Cairney, J. & Flouris, A. (2005) Increased risk for coronary
vascular disease in children with developmental coordination disorder.
Journal of Adolescent Health, 37, 376-80.
Green, D., Baird, G., Barnett, A. L., Henderson, L., Huber, J. & Henderson, S.E.
(2002) The severity and nature of motor impairment in Asperger's syndrome: a
comparison with specific developmental disorder of motor function. Journal of Child
Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 655-68.
Hall, D. (1988) Clumsy children. British Medical Journal, 296, 375-6.
Henderson, S. E. & Sugden, D. A. (2007) Movement Assessment Battery for
Children. Second Edition. London, The Psychological Corporation.
Hibbeln, J. R., Davis, J. M., Steer, C., Emmett, P., Rogers, I. & Williams, C. (2007)
Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in
childhood (ALSPAC study): An observational cohort study. Lancet, 369, 578-85.
Hill, E. L. (1998) A dyspraxic deficit in specific language impairment and
developmental coordination disorder? Evidence from hand and arm movements.
Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 40, 388-95.
Missiuna, C., Gaines, R. & Pollock, N. (2002) Recognizing and referring children at
risk for developmental coordination disorder: Role of the speech language
pathologist. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology & Audiology, 26,172-9.
Morton, J. (2004) Understanding Developmental Disorders: A Causal Modelling
Approach. London, Blackwell.
Orton, S. T. (1937) Reading, Writing and Speech Problems In Children. New York,
W. W. Norton.
Peters, J. M. & Henderson, S. E. (2008) Understanding developmental coordination
disorder (DCD) and its impact on families: the contribution of single case studies. In:
D. A. Sugden, A. Kirby & C. Dunford (Eds) Special Edition of the International
Journal of Disability, Development and Education, 55, 97-113.
Pitcher, T. M., Piek, J. P. & Hay, D. A. (2003) Fine and gross motor ability in males
with ADHD. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 45, 525-35.
Pless, M. & Carlsson, M. (2000) Effects of motor skill intervention on DCD: a metaanalysis. Adapted Physical Activity Quarterly, 17, 381-401.
Polatajko, H. J. & Cantin, N. (2005) Developmental Coordination Disorder
(Dyspraxia): An Overview of the State of the Art. Seminar Pediatric, 12, 250-258.
Querne, L., Berquin, P., Vernier-Hauvette, M., Fall, S., Deltour, L. & Meyer, M. (2008)
Dysfunction of the attentional brain network in children with Developmental
Coordination Disorder : A fMRI study. Brain Research, 1244, 89-102.
Rasta, S. P. & Eliot, J. (1999) Written expression in boys with attention deficit
hyperactivity disorder. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 41, 159-65.
Rivard, L. M., Missuina, C., Hanna, S. & Wishart, L. (2007) Understanding teachers
perceptions of the motor difficulties of children with developmental co-ordination
disorder( DCD). British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 633-648.
Scabar, A., Devescovi, R., Blason, L., Bravar, L. & Carrozi, M. (2006) Comorbidity of
DCD and SLI: Significance of epileptiform activity during sleep. Child Care Health
and Development, 32, 733-9.
Skinner, R. A. & Piek, J. P. (2001) Psychosocial implications of poor motor
coordination in children and adolescents. Human Movement Science, 20, 73-94.
Sigurdsson, E., van Os, J. & Fombonne, E. (2002) Are impaired childhood motor
skills a risk factor for adolescent anxiety? American Journal of Psychiatry, 159,1044 6.
USEFUL WEBSITES
http://www.dystalk.com/talks
Developmental Disorders experts talk about a variety of developmental Disorders,
including DCD.
http://dcd.canchild.ca/en/?_mid_=3276
This Canadian site has some excellent materials to support parents, educational
and health professionals
http://www.dyspraxiaireland.com/
The Dyspraxia Association, Ireland
www.dyspraxiafoundation.org.uk
The Dyspraxia Foundation, UK has a wealth of information for parents, professionals
and adults with Dyspraxia/DCD.
www.dcd-uk.org
This website contains information relating to the 2006 Leeds Consensus Statement.
http://www.hdcd.org.uk/
The website of the Highland Developmental Co-ordination Disorder group contains a
wealth of information for parents and has also produced a large selected of books,
educational materials and DVDs.
http://www.dyspraxicadults.org.uk/
This forum provides an opportunity for adults with Dyspraxia/DCD to post messages
around a variety of topics.
http://www.dyspraxicteens.org.uk/forum/index.php
This forum provides an opportunity for adolescents with Dyspraxia/DCD to share
stories.
USEFUL BOOKS
Ball, M. (2002) Developmental Co-ordination Disorder. Jessica Kingsley Publishers
Biggs, V. & Colley, M. (2006) Living with Dyspraxia. Jessica Kingsley Publishers
Boon, M. (2000) Helping Children with Dyspraxia. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Dixon, G. & Addy, L. (2004) Making Inclusion Work for Children with Dyspraxia:
Practical Strategies for Teachers. Routledge Falmer.
Drew, S. & Atter, L. (2008) Can't Play Won't Play: Simply Sizzling Ideas to Get the
Ball Rolling for Children with Dyspraxia. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.
Kirby, A. (2006) Dyspraxia: The Hidden Handicap. Souvenir Press.
Kirby, A. & Peters, L. (2007) 100 Ideas for Supporting Pupils with Dyspraxia and
DCD. Continuum International Publishing Group Ltd.
Kurtz, L. A. (2003) How To Help A Clumsy Child. Jessica Kingsley Publishers.