Insect Behavior 2ed PDF
Insect Behavior 2ed PDF
Insect Behavior 2ed PDF
Insect Behavior
Second Edition
123
Robert W. Matthews Janice R. Matthews
University of Georgia University of Georgia
Dept. Entomology Dept. Entomology
Athens GA 30602 Athens GA 30602
USA USA
[email protected] [email protected]
This book is for all who are interested in the biological sciences. Like the course
that originally inspired it, the text is designed for use at senior undergraduate level
for college and university students, so we trust that it will find readership among
those who have had some basic introduction to entomology and animal behavior.
However, we also hope it will prove useful to newcomers who may be approaching
behavioral study from other perspectives.
This is our second edition, born anew after thirty years. Much has changed during
that time, especially in the breadth and depth of a field that (like us) was fairly young
back in 1978. New technologies are allowing scientists to shape—and answer—
questions in ways that once could not even have been envisioned. Insect behavior
research now has wings, and is poised to take off. However, at this juncture, we all
must also take care not lose an awareness of our roots. Thus, as writers introducing
this exciting field of study to the next generation of scientists and insect enthusiasts,
we have tried to strike a balance between new ideas and old, and between modern
developments and historical insights.
Our objectives in writing this edition remain the same as they have always been.
The first of these has been to help readers understand how a number of major
behavioral systems function. Thus, this is not an encyclopedia, but an introduc-
tion to fundamental concepts and processes as seen from a comparative evolutionary
viewpoint. We have not documented numerous strings of examples merely for ‘com-
pleteness of coverage’ but instead have tried to give a flavor of the diversity of ways
in which insects approach similar life tasks. Because the Internet and excellent
search engines have made access to information sources easy and nearly instan-
taneous, we have not burdened readers with a cumbersome citations in the text;
searching on key terms, aided when necessary by references associated with fig-
ure credits at the book’s end, will provide entry into additional literature for those
interested in further pursuing subjects we can but introduce.
Our second objective has been to help readers gain insights into accessible
ways in which behavioral research can be conducted. Whenever possible, we have
included discussions of important experiments and investigations, rather than pre-
senting a rhetoric of conclusions. Selected principles are interwoven with case stud-
ies of specific situations, presenting actual examples in a manner compatible with
the dynamic, open-ended field and laboratory experiences in which they have arisen.
v
vi Preface to the Second Edition
vii
viii Contents
3 Spatial Adjustment . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93
3.2 Locomotion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94
3.2.1 Terrestrial and Aquatic Locomotion . . . . . . . . . . 96
3.2.2 Aerial Locomotion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98
3.3 Orientation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100
3.3.1 Locomotory Responses . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101
3.3.2 Posture and Position . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103
3.3.3 Orientation to Radiant Energy . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105
3.3.4 Magnetic Field Orientation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 108
3.3.5 Orientation to the Evidence of Others’ Presence . . . . 109
3.4 Thermoregulation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
3.4.1 Dormancy and Thermotolerance . . . . . . . . . . . . 110
3.4.2 Regulation of Heat Gain . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 113
3.4.3 Heat Production . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 114
3.5 Migration . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 116
3.5.1 Seasonal Migration . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 117
3.5.2 Migration Under Ephemeral Conditions . . . . . . . . 122
3.5.3 Dispersal and Navigation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 124
4 Foraging and Feeding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
4.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 131
4.1.1 Food Recognition and Acceptance . . . . . . . . . . . 134
4.1.2 Regulation of Feeding . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 137
4.2 Foraging Strategies . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 140
4.2.1 Herbivory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
4.2.2 Active Search . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 141
4.2.3 Trapping and Ambush . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 146
4.2.4 Parasites and Parasitoids . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 148
4.2.5 Theft and Kleptoparasitism . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 152
4.2.6 Insect Agriculture . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 154
4.2.7 Nest Symbionts: Becoming a House Pet . . . . . . . . 157
4.3 Coevolution and the Arms Race . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 164
4.3.1 Attack, Defense, and Counterattack . . . . . . . . . . . 165
4.3.2 Employing Mercenaries for Protection . . . . . . . . . 170
4.3.3 The Tommy Tucker Syndrome: Food in Return
for Services . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 173
4.4 Feeding as a Communal Activity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 177
4.4.1 Simple Groups and Feeding Aggregations . . . . . . . 177
4.4.2 Social Feeding Behaviors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 181
5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 185
5.1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 185
5.2 Defense Messages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 186
5.3 Passive Messages . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 187
5.3.1 Crypsis: ‘I’m Not Here!’ . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 187
Contents ix
xiii
Chapter 1
The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
1.1 Introduction
An overview of the insect world reveals two paradoxical characteristics: great diver-
sity and equally great constancy. On the one hand, there are over one million named
insect species, with estimates ranging up to three million. How can such a great
diversity be explained? Study of this basic question has become the domain of evo-
lutionary biology. On the other hand, each kind of organism tends to reoccur in
virtually the same form with the same basic features for generation after generation.
Why do they tend to show such constancy, such resistance to change? The study of
this question, in turn, is largely the domain of genetics. Together, these two great
branches of biology—evolution and genetics—form a powerful tool for the inves-
tigation of nearly every aspect of life. This introductory chapter deals briefly with
their application to the study of behavior and then turns to an overview of behavior
as a field of study to provide a perspective for the chapters that follow.
A flashing firefly flits through the evening shadows. In a tree, a caterpillar pauses
in its feeding, stiffens, and sways back and forth. Behind a stone, a cricket chirps,
while nearby ants scurry along in precise single file.
Behavior can be simply defined as what animals do. More precisely, it is the
ways in which an organism adjusts to and interacts with its total environment. As
such, insect behavior encompasses the relationships an insect has with members of
its own species, with members of other species, and with the physical environment.
A species must behave in the ‘right’ ways in order to survive, and its members must
survive (at least long enough to successfully reproduce) if it to be evolutionarily
successful.
Admittedly, the term ‘behavior’ covers a very wide range of activities, and it
can be helpful to recognize some subcategories. General locomotion, grooming,
and feeding, for example, are essentially individual matters. These maintenance
activities keep an insect in good shape but usually have little influence on others
of its kind. On the other hand, a broad range of communication activities are con-
cerned with conveying information to, and influencing the activities of others. Often
such actions are conspicuous and stereotyped, and not surprisingly they have been
a favorite study material for behaviorists. The firefly’s flash and the cricket’s chirp
may have the same function, to gain a mate. The caterpillar in essence sends the
predator world the message that it is not food. The ants share their message that
travel along this particular trail is apt to be rewarding. But while the firefly, cricket,
and ants are communicating with their own kind, the caterpillar obviously is not.
Thus, insect communication signals come in two broad, somewhat overlapping cat-
egories; even when the method of signaling is the same, the results of intraspecific
and interspecific communication are quite different.
This division is broadly reflected in the book’s organization. The first chapters
concern behavior of the individual insect—how it moves, orients, disperses, and
feeds, including the role of the nervous and endocrine systems in integrating behav-
ioral responses. The chapters on communication in a sense form the core of the book
and logically relate to defensive, reproductive, and social behaviors, all of which are
mediated by communicative codes.
The question ‘what is insect behavior?’ also can be answered another way. Insect
behavior is, of course, a discipline nested within the larger field of animal behav-
ior study. For many thousands of years, humans and their ancestors have keenly
observed animal habits and characteristics for entirely practical reasons, ranging
from the need to hunt game animals to the desirability of avoiding biting insects
and encouraging useful ones. Early Greek and Roman scholars such as Aristotle
and Pliny often wrote at some length about the natural world, including its insect
inhabitants. However, the rigorous scientific study of animal behavior only began
in the latter part of the nineteenth century, with the convergence of three major
developments—publication of the theory of evolution by natural selection, develop-
ment of a systematic comparative method, and studies in genetics and inheritance.
In the 1930s, Darwinian natural selection was combined with Mendelian inheritance
to form what is called ‘the modern evolutionary synthesis’, an explanation of ways
in which natural selection and genes interconnect (see Section 1.2.2).
Over time, from these beginnings, different major research areas arose within
the general field of animal behavior. One approach concentrated on the mechanisms
controlling behavior; this became the fields of comparative animal psychology and
physiology. A second approach concentrated on the functional significance and evo-
lution of behavioral traits, especially in natural settings; this became the field of
ethology. Somewhat later, a third area of study concentrated upon the biological
relationships between an organism and its environment, especially from an ecolog-
ical and evolutionary viewpoint; this became the field of behavioral ecology. Most
recently, significant technological genetic advances have been enabling and support-
ing the emergence of behavioral genetics and behavioral genomics as new research
concentrations.
In this way, four great disciplines have contributed to the study of behavior—
physiology (particularly neurophysiology), ecology, ethology, and psychology.
There are no distinct boundaries, yet each has had its own developmental history
1.1 Introduction 3
and tradition of established methods and brings its own viewpoint to the subject.
Somewhat facetiously, they once were distinguished from one another by behav-
ioral scientist Kenneth Roeder as follows: The ethologist, attempting to leave the
animal as unrestricted as possible in order to study its ‘normal’ behavior, tolerates
any necessary discomforts while enclosing himself in a blind. The psychologist,
attempting to reduce external variables, places the blind around the animal, thereby
making it uncomfortable. The physiologist, attempting to learn what makes the ani-
mal behave, removes it from the blind and probes directly into its nervous and motor
systems. The behavioral ecologist, we might add, spends his time studying how the
blind itself affects the animal’s behavior. Finally, the behavioral geneticist snips off
tiny samples of the animal and takes them behind the blind for a closer look.
As will become evident in the examples in this book, in recent years these
approaches have increasingly melded back toward a single discipline that contains
elements of all of these approaches. This does not mean that these fields have lost
their distinct character; in this chapter we’ll touch on what that means for behavioral
insights. However, it does mean that no matter what they may call themselves and
what direction they may approach a problem from, insect behaviorists today pursue
quite comparable goals and rely upon shared theoretical frameworks.
Insects make up a significant proportion of the world’s biota (Fig. 1.1). The approxi-
mately 920,000 species that have been described represent almost 85% of all known
animal species; many more have yet to be been given scientific names.
Insects belong to the phylum Arthropoda, a very large assemblage of animals
with jointed legs and a hard outer skeleton. One major group in this phylum, the
Chelicerata, have sickle-shaped jaws and lack antennae; they include the Arachnida
(spiders, mites, scorpions, etc.) and two smaller marine groups. The other major
group, the Mandibulata, possess antennae and have mandibles (mouthparts) that
work against each other. Besides the insects (including the entognathous hexapods),
other major groups in the Mandibulata are the crustaceans (a predominantly aquatic
group), the centipedes, the millipedes, and two smaller classes, Symphyla and
Paurapoda. Of all the land arthropods, insects are by far the most abundant, followed
by mites and spiders.
The class Insecta is divided into a number of orders. The exact lines along which
these divisions should be made remain a matter of dispute, but in general, ordi-
nal divisions reflect the present understanding of the evolutionary history of the
class (Fig. 1.2). More than one-third of the named species of insects are beetles, the
Coleoptera. The next largest orders, in descending numbers, are Lepidoptera (but-
terflies, moths, and skippers), Hymenoptera (wasps and bees, and Diptera (flies).
Together, these four orders include more than 80% of the named species of insects.
Broadly speaking, four important stages are distinguished. First was the appear-
ance of primitively wingless insects (including the three orders of entognathous
4 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Fig. 1.1 Tallying the numbers. Insects comprise a major proportion of the world’s biota, and
beetles make up a major proportion of the Insecta
hexapods) probably in the late Silurian Period. Bristletails and silverfish are living
representatives of these earliest insect forms. Second was the development of wings,
hypothesized to have occurred during the late Devonian or early Carboniferous.
These early winged insects had a wing-hinging mechanism that did not permit
the wings to fold, so they had to be held out from the body. The Ephemeroptera
(mayflies) and the Odonata (dragonflies and damselflies) are surviving remnants of
these ancient groups.
The third stage, the Neoptera, developed a different wing flexion mechanism, that
had evolved by the late Carboniferous. Now able to fold their wings down tightly
over their abdomens, insects could more easily run and hide from predators and
move into a wide variety of previously inaccessible niches. Among contemporary
insects, roughly 97% have flexing wings, and this mechanism is one reason for the
dominance of insects today.
The fourth important stage was the development of complete metamorphosis
(holometaboly), which also seems to have arisen by the late Carboniferous. The
earliest insects remained essentially similar in their wingless body form through-
out their entire lives. More advanced groups developed the simple metamorphosis
exhibited by insects such as grasshoppers today, where immature stages resemble
miniature adults but wings are lacking (although external wing buds are plainly
visible) until the last molt, when the insect becomes sexually mature. The most
highly advanced groups, however, evolved the complete metamorphosis illustrated
by the familiar life cycle of a butterfly. The immature stages, the larvae, bear no
resemblance to adults, and wing buds are developed internally, becoming visible
only when the larva transforms into the pupal stage, from which the winged adult
emerges.
1.1 Introduction 5
Fig. 1.2 Looking for connections. Probable evolutionary relationships among the living insect
orders
6 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Fig. 1.3 Depicting nature. An ancient pendant, considered one of the most outstanding examples
of early Minoan goldworking, shows two paper wasps, Polistes. Their legs embrace a granulated
disc thought to represent the paper nest; a droplet of food or wood pulp is held in their jaws. The
cage-like fixture atop the heads may be an attempt by the artist to depict the vigorous antennal beat-
ing characteristic of all encounters between individuals on a nest. The opposition of the abdominal
tips may have been intended to suggest mating, but is biologically inaccurate; more probably it
reflects the heraldic symmetry common in Minoan art. Most classics texts incorrectly refer to the
insects as bees, sometimes with elaborate explanations about the symbolism inherent in the ‘honey
droplet’ between their jaws
arise spontaneously from rotten meat. Maria Sibylla Merian (Fig. 1.5) took the bold
step of actually studying butterflies and moths throughout their entire life cycles,
taking detailed notes and making careful illustrations. However, when Merian pub-
lished her lavishly illustrated works, the scientific community largely ignored them
although both Linnaeus and Fabricius’s subsequent insect descriptions were appar-
ently influenced by her work. The invention of the microscope in the 1600s might
have resolved the matter of spontaneous generation but in fact it only served to
enhance the belief by revealing a whole new world of microorganisms that appeared
to arise spontaneously. The controversy around spontaneous generation continued
until it was ultimately settled by the experiments of Louis Pasteur in the nineteenth
century. Finally, well into the twentieth century, the work of Maria Merian was redis-
covered and recognized. Her portrait has been printed on German postage stamps
and was on the 500 DM note before Germany converted to the euro; in addition,
many schools (and a modern research vessel) have been named after her.
The first ‘major player’ to introduce elements of scientific discipline to insect
study was Rene Antoine Ferchault de Reaumur. Reaumur was a widely trained
scientist who made major contributions to areas as disparate as geometry, met-
allurgy, and meteorology, but many consider his greatest work to have been in
8 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Fig. 1.4 Explaining insect life. A sixteenth-century woodcut supposedly shows honey bees being
generated spontaneously from a dead animal. Some medieval texts contained detailed directions
for creating insects from substances as varied as dead animals, logs, and dirt
entomology. Published between 1734 and 1742, his six large volumes of Memoirs
pour Servir a l’Histoire des Insectes applied precise observation, detailed experi-
mentation, and accurate recording to phenomena as varied as social life, parasitic
habits, and leaf-mining.
The historical development of the scientific field of animal behavior (and insect
behavior as a sub-discipline within it) accelerated from about 1750 onward. At this
time, all of European society was becoming ‘scientific’, and each important expedi-
tion began to include at least one professional naturalist. As a result so many exotic
plant and animal specimens were being brought back to Europe from around the
world that chaos loomed for the naturalists who were trying to identify, classify, and
communicate what they gathered. Linnaeus’ method of classification was gaining
increasing exposure and acceptance as a way to organize all these data. When the
tenth edition (1758–1759) of his Systema Naturae was chosen as the starting point
for zoological nomenclature, it marked a major milestone in biology.
Thus, by the time the 1700s drew to a close, three kinds of entomological texts
were prevalent. First, there were beautifully colored illustrative works such as those
of Maria Merian. Second were descriptive classificatory works, such as Linneaus’
Systema Naturae with its system of binomial nomenclature. Third were detailed
works such as those of Reaumur’s Memoirs, detailing specific aspects of insects
such as their development, physiology, or internal anatomy.
1.1 Introduction 9
Fig. 1.5 Illustrating the truth. Rejecting spontaneous generation, Maria Merian accurately
unraveled the mysteries of metamorphosis in many Lepidoptera in the late 1600s, and published
her findings in beautifully illustrated detail. (left, a portrait by her son-in-law, Georg Gsell; right,
her drawing of the silkworm life cycle.)
However, for the next half-century it would still be a time when general observa-
tion predominated over specialization. Enthusiasm and subjectivity often affected
the accuracy of behavioral observations, and interpretations were often slanted
to embrace a particular philosophical creed. Most scientists still subscribed to
Aristotle’s Scala Naturae, a theory that all living beings could be classified on an
ideal pyramid. The simplest animals occupied the base, and complexity rose pro-
gressively to the top, which was occupied by human beings. Animal species were
generally thought to be eternal and immutable, created with a specific purpose (by
which most people meant ‘created by God to serve mankind’). It seemed like the
most logical, and perhaps only possible, explanation for the incredible variety of
living things and their surprising adaptations to their environment.
that upon seeing an animal, the first question of both layman and scientist alike is
always, ‘What is it?’ During the 1800s, a major driving force in science was the need
simply to identify, name and classify the diversity that was present. The problem
was particularly acute for places that were less well studied than the Old World that
most scientists called home. With a system of binomial nomenclature having been
established only relatively recently, and an entire world beckoning, the adventure of
collecting and classifying organisms called out to many natural historians.
One who answered that call was John Lawrence LeConte. Like many scien-
tists of his day, LeConte had the money and connections to indulge his passion.
A few months after he was born in New York City, his mother died; raised by his
father, a well-known naturalist, LeConte trained as a medical doctor. He never offi-
cially worked as an entomologist—the field was still too new for that. During the
American Civil War he worked as a surgeon with the California volunteers, reaching
the rank of lieutenant colonel. In 1878 he became the chief clerk (assistant direc-
tor) of the United States Mint in Philadelphia, and retained that position until his
death. Throughout his life, he traveled abroad extensively, explored the Colorado
River, and accompanied the teams that built the Honduras Interoceanic Railway and
Kansas Pacific Railroad.
Over the course of his life, LeConte almost single-handedly put American ento-
mology on the map. He published his first scientific work in 1844, while he was
still in medical school. Everywhere he went, he sent back insect specimens, some-
times by the tens of thousands. In all, he was responsible for naming and describing
approximately half of the insect taxa known in the United States during his lifetime,
including some 5,000 species of beetles. (Not surprisingly, he is often described
as ‘the father of American beetle study’.) Le Conte was probably the best known
entomologist of his century, but it was a time of rapid growth and expansion in
the sciences, and LeConte was part of an entire roster of insect afficionados whose
names are still familiar to working scientists today. His influence was considerable,
for LeConte was also very active in the scientific societies of his time; in addition
to serving as vice-president of the American Philosophical Society and president of
the American Association for the Advancement of Science, he was a founder of the
American Entomological Society, and a charter member of the National Academy
of Sciences.
Meanwhile, in France, the late nineteenth century had introduced the works of
Jean-Henri Fabré. Born a pauper, his education and opportunities were limited.
He was an avid naturalist, not a scientist; a moody and morose soul, his personal
life reads like a tragic drama. However, the publication of his romantically titled
Souvenirs Entomologiques, a set of ten volumes describing in detail the lives of
insects in his own backyard, popularized insect behavior in writings that achieved
the heights of literary excellence. (In both 1904 and 1911, he was nominated for
a Nobel prize in literature.) Modern scientific advances have rendered much of
his work obsolete, and for a time the scientific community had difficulty accept-
ing him, particularly because he vigorously rejected the theory of evolution and was
absolutely convinced of the ‘fixity of instinct’. However, Fabré was one of the great-
est popularizers of entomology the world has ever known, and his work served to
1.1 Introduction 11
Juan Finlay with identifying Aedes mosquitoes as the yellow fever vector and thus
determining how yellow fever could be controlled.) Not long afterward, Carlos
Chagas observed the peculiar infestation of rural houses in Brazil with Triatoma,
demonstrated that it was the vector of Trypanosoma cruzi, and proved experimen-
tally that it could be transmitted to marmoset monkeys that were bitten by the
infected bug. His description of the new disease was to become a classic in medicine
and brought him domestic and international distinction.
Fig. 1.6 Discovering a killer’s cause. Yellow fever was one of the most dreaded diseases in port
cities of the Americas. Poorly understood and feared as cancer is today, and known to have its ori-
gins in the Caribbean region, the disease was thought to spread directly and through contaminated
clothing and bedding. When yellow fever became a problem for the Army during the Spanish
American War, felling thousands of soldiers in Cuba, Major Walter Reed, M.D. (left) headed a
team that proved that yellow fever is transmitted only by Aedes mosquitoes (center), confirming
a theory first set forth in 1881 by Cuban doctor/scientist Carlos Finlay (right). The insight gave
impetus to the new fields of epidemiology and biomedicine, and allowed the United States to build
the Panama Canal, something that had confounded the French attempts only 30 years earlier
The ‘basic’ side of entomology also grew rapidly during this time. In 1901,
the first widely used textbook, A. D. Imms’ General Textbook of Entomology was
published; going through numerous editions, for close to a century it remained
one of the most widely used of all insect texts. At Columbia University, Thomas
Hunt Morgan became one the first to conduct genetic research with the fruit fly
Drosophila melanogaster, a development that would ultimately underpin the field
of behavioral genetics.
In a similar way, all of animal behavior study was growing and subdividing into
a multidisciplinary subject. The nineteenth century was a time when the nature of
scientific enquiry was changing in ways that would have major impact on the field of
insect behavior. In addition to general natural history studies, an increasing number
of specialized disciplines began to characterize the natural sciences. Psychologists,
anthropologists, ecologists, geneticists, and many others who contribute to the study
of animal behavior can trace early common roots back to this period. It was during
this time, born in the principles of evolution delineated by Charles Darwin and nur-
tured by this convergence of entomology and more objective scientific analysis, that
modern insect behavior arose. However, as a field of study it went on to show its
1.1 Introduction 13
greatest development after the turn of the century, with the emergence of the largely
European school of ethology.
The word ethology has a long pedigree. Based on the Greek ethos, which has a
variety of meanings, the term has been applied to everything from stage actors
who portray human characters to people who study ethics. However, as it is used
today, ethology means the study of the behavior of animals in their natural habitat.
The term was first popularized in English by the American myrmecologist William
Morton Wheeler in 1902, but it was hardly a new idea. Over 3,000 years ago,
King Solomon recommended the study of insects in their natural habitat with those
famous words in Proverbs 6.6: ‘Go to the ant, thou sluggard; consider her ways, and
be wise.’
In the early years of the twentieth century, two different major conceptual
approaches to animal behavior study split off from one another. For most of
the twentieth century, the discipline of comparative psychology developed most
strongly in North America, whereas ethology was stronger in Europe. This led to dif-
ferent emphases, different philosophical underpinnings, and different experimental
approaches. Comparative psychology came to pay particular attention to the psy-
chological nature of human beings in comparison with other animals. A practical
focus on laboratory studies made a logical base for experimental studies on human
and animal brain function, learning, and motivation. Well-known studies include
those of Ivan Pavlov on conditioning in dogs, Harry Harlow on the effects of social
deprivation in monkeys, and those of various researchers on language abilities in
apes.
In the 1920s and 1930s, when most behavioral research was laboratory based
emphasizing the role of learning and hormones in the modification of behavior,
two Europeans, Konrad Lorenz and Nikolaas (‘Niko’) Tinbergen, began publishing
intensive and extensive natural behavior studies that quickly caught the attention
of scientists and the general public. As their carefully detailed work unfolded, so
did the foundations of ethological theory. As in most cases, their scientific work
owed a debt to others. In a sense, Charles Darwin might be called the first modern
ethologist; his book, The Expression of the Emotions in Man and Animals, continues
to influence ethologists today. Some scholars would acknowledge Darwin’s protégé
George Romanes (see Section 1.2.3) as another ‘father’ to the field, despite his odd
methods. Other clear leaders were Oskar Heinroth and Julian Huxley, who both
concentrated on behaviors they called instinctive, or natural. Their focus was upon
building an objective cumulative base of data about behavior. Thus, their first step
in studying the behavior of any new species was to construct an ethogram, a kind
of glossary describing each type of natural behavior shown by the animal and the
frequency of that behavior’s occurrence.
Later chapters in this book will return to the ideas of Lorenz and Tinbergen;
for now it enough to realize that their research rested on a different practical and
14 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
development of sociality. But at the same time, even according to very liberal esti-
mates, fragmentary behavioral data (host records, food plants, etc.) are recorded for
no more than 5% of the described insect species; the number of species subjected to
intensive investigation is far smaller yet.
Few people have never heard of the theory of evolution by natural selection and
Charles Darwin’s book, The Origin of Species. Darwin formulated his idea of natural
selection in 1838 and was still developing his theory in 1858 when Alfred Russell
Wallace sent him a similar theory that he had independently developed from his own
data set. The works of both men were presented to the Linnean Society of London
in separate papers.
Though these publications set a major paradigm shift in motion throughout the
entire scientific community, their theory of evolution did not develop in a vacuum. It
drew its ideas from the contributions and suggestions of many other people, from the
selective breeding experiments of English farmers to the ideas of Thomas Malthus
about human competition for survival and the fossil observations by geologist Sir
Charles Lyell that suggested a process of continuous change in living material
through time.
Jean-Baptiste Lamark, not Charles Darwin, actually was the first prominent biol-
ogist to set forth a complex theory of evolution. His theory proposed two major new
ideas. One was that animal organs and behavior were not fixed; they could change
according to the way they were used, and these changes could be transmitted from
one generation to the next. The second idea was that every living organism, human
beings included, tends to reach a greater level of perfection over time. Darwin was
fully aware of these theories at the time of his journey on the H.M.S. Beagle, and
he was deeply influenced by them. As an interesting historical side-note, whereas
much debate ensued in England over Darwin’s publication, it was a non-event in
France. In the eyes of the French, Darwin was just attempting to reformulate theories
already put forward by two of their own scientists, Lamarck and Etienne Geoffroy
16 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Natural selection and genetic drift can only work on what is available, and for
this reason it is important to recognize that the traits and alleles that are present in
a population can vary over time. Various catastrophes, expansions into new habitats
(causing a founder effect), or events that divide a population can lead to a popula-
tion bottleneck, in which the number of breeding individuals in a population shrinks
temporarily and therefore the population loses genetic variation. On the other hand,
new traits can also arise, either from mutations in genes or from the transfer of
genes between populations and between species. Mutations are the ultimate source
of variation, but in species that reproduce sexually, new combinations of existing
alleles are also produced by genetic recombination (by exchange of chromosome
sections during meiosis). Gene flow is the exchange of genes between populations,
usually of the same species. For example, some grasshoppers migrate widely before
breeding; this migration brings previously isolated populations together and facili-
tates gene flow within the species, slowing the process of speciation that might have
occurred had the populations remained segregated. Migration not only provides an
opportunity for mixing; it also may add or remove genetic material from the gene
pool of a population.
Genetic drift is random change in the frequency of alleles, caused by the random
sampling of a generation’s genes during reproduction. It results from the role prob-
ability plays in whether a given trait will be passed on as individuals survive and
reproduce. Genetic drift is analogous to the way that flipping a coin over and over
may give a proportion of ‘heads’ and ‘tails’ that differs from the equal numbers one
would expect. Because the relative importance of natural selection and genetic drift
in a population varies depending on the strength of the selection and the effective
population size (the number of individuals capable of breeding), the size of a pop-
ulation can greatly affect its evolution. Natural selection usually predominates in
large populations, while genetic drift dominates in small populations. For example,
a population greatly reduced by a bottleneck will often show increased genetic drift.
The dominance of genetic drift in small populations can even lead to the fixation of
slightly deleterious mutations.
The changes produced in any one generation by selection, drift, mutation, and
gene flow are small, but these differences tend to accumulate with each subse-
quent generation and over time they can cause substantial changes in the organisms.
Consider a hypothetical case in which all of the beetles of one species living in the
trees on an isolated mountaintop represent a population. A single gene in this pop-
ulation exists as two alleles that account for variations in the physical appearance
(phenotype) of the organisms, in this case brown or green body color. A gene pool
is the complete set of alleles in a single population, so each allele occurs a cer-
tain number of times in a gene pool. The fraction of genes within the gene pool
that are a particular allele is called the allele frequency. Evolution occurs when
there are changes in the frequencies of alleles within a population of interbreeding
organisms. Suppose birds more commonly notice the brown beetles and eat them
before they can reproduce whereas the green beetles, successfully camouflaged by
the tree foliage, continue to breed. In each generation, the allele for green body
color in the population would become more common and the brown allele, less so.
1.2 Conceptual Frameworks 19
Over time, unless some other factor favored brown beetles enough to overcome this
difference, a population of green beetles would evolve.
Under what conditions would a population not evolve? One way to examine
whether a population is undergoing evolutionary change is through the use of the
Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium. The Hardy-Weinberg principle is an example of a null
model. Just as a null hypothesis is a hypothesis of no difference, this null model
tells us what to expect when no evolutionary forces are at work. Based on the fre-
quency of alleles, Hardy-Weinberg predicts the frequency of (diploid) genotypes in
the absence of evolution. If the genotype frequencies we observe in our study pop-
ulation are the same as those determined by the principle, our population is said to
be in Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium; it is not evolving. If we observe different fre-
quencies in our study population, we can conclude that some evolutionary force is
at work.
Like the fine print in an insurance policy, the Hardy-Weinberg equilibrium has
a great many restrictions. The equilibrium is relevant only for diploid organisms
with sexual reproduction. Mating must be random. The gene under consideration
has only two alleles, and allele frequencies are identical in males and females.
Population size is very large, so that genetic drift is essential imperceptible. Gene
flow and mutation are so small as to be negligible. Finally, natural selection is
not acting on the alleles under consideration. Of what possible use can such a
restrictive formula be? The answer lies in some of its permutations, which allow a
researcher to sample a population at one point in time and to determine whether it
is at equilibrium for a set of alleles without having to take repeated samples across
several generations.
A major condition under which evolution would slow or halt would be when there
is little or no genetic variability for natural selection to act upon. Clearly, for some
species, genetic variability is very important, as illustrated by those insect species
that seem to have enough persistent allelic diversity to be able to evolve resistance
to one insecticide after another. Genetic variability is also important in the ability
of both invaders and insects imported for biological control to establish themselves,
as well as in their potential to attack new and non-target hosts. On the other hand,
genetic diversity seems fairly unimportant for some species. For example, many par-
asitoid wasps such as Melittobia (see Fig. 9.13), typically undergo one generation
after another of brother-sister matings, and are assumed to be highly inbred. In the
case of some invasive insects, lack of variability may actually contribute to their suc-
cess. Research has shown that invasive populations of the Argentine ant Linepithea
humile have lost the genetic ability to distinguish one colony from another; as a
result, they escape from population control imposed by deadly conflicts between
colonies. So how important is genetic diversity itself to the persistence of insect
populations? Surprisingly, this question is still an open one, and difficult to answer
in a general way.
Ultimately, one of the goals of a behaviorist is to pair a particular behavioral com-
ponent with a particular gene, determining the actual site at which the gene wields
its influence and how it does so. In fruit flies (Drosophila), probably the single best
genetically studied insect genus, a host of intriguing behavioral abnormalities have
20 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Fig. 1.7 Testing for hygienic behavior. Honey bees of some strains will remove diseased and
dead larvae and pupae from brood combs infected with American foulbrood so rapidly and thor-
oughly that they effectively control the disease. (left) To test his or her breeding stock, a beekeeper
can pour 250–300 ml of liquid nitrogen through an open-ended soup can, freeze-killing the brood
in this spot, and then after 48 hours, count the number of dead brood the bees have removed. (cen-
ter, right) The response shown in the center frame indicates a lower level of hygienic behavior in
this colony than that of the colony at the right
6 hygienic
9 uncapped
(Conclusion: (Conclusion: Hygienic behavior due to (Conclusion: Two genes are involved,
Difference in the one or more recessive genes) one for uncapping and one for removal)
two lines)
Fig. 1.8 Determining hygienic behavior genetics. Experimental crosses and backcrosses done
by Walter Rothenbuhler in the 1960s suggested that two recessive genes were responsible for
hygienic behavior; later studies have indicated that additional genes are also involved
22 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Fig. 1.9 Choosing one’s (insect) parents. Three different modes of selection yield quite different
characteristic means and frequency distributions in the subsequent generations. Ordinate indicates
the frequency of individuals in the population. Abscissa indicates variation for the quantitative
character being considered, as expressed in some linear metric measure. Directional selection is
the most common mode
group are allowed to breed, and the mean score for their offspring (called the ‘filial
generation’) is determined.
These measurements are all we need to determine what proportion of our
observed trait variation can be attributed to genetic variance. As a result of our
truncated selection experiments, two measures can easily be determined empir-
ically (Fig. 1.10). One is the magnitude of the response (R)—how much the
mean values differ between the parent and offspring generations. The other is
the selection differential (S)—how much the chosen parents deviate from the
whole population in their mean value for the trait under selection. Dividing
R/S gives a very useful measure called heritability (h2 ). It is defined as the
degree of genetic determination of the variability that is present in the sam-
ple population, for the kind of behavior observed and for the precise method of
observation used.
population genetics, and animal behavior have found that many traits show low
(0.1–0.1) to moderate (0.1–0.4) heritability.
As a general rule, most arthropod behavior is highly stereotyped and will tend to
yield rather high heritability measures relative to those of vertebrates. For example,
selection experiments on milkweed bugs under laboratory conditions shifted the
age at first oviposition from 63.5 days to 16.7 days in seven generations. Average
heritability from all generations was 1.18, which indicates that heritability for this
trait must be very close to 1.0, since additive genetic variance cannot be greater than
total variance.
One caution should be noted before concluding this section. After several
generations in the laboratory, artificial selection may alter the cultured insects’
behavior in ways that adapt the insect for the more simplified environment.
Therefore, behavioral data based on laboratory-reared insects may not accurately
reflect the behavior of natural populations of the same species. In fact, different
behavioral results obtained by investigators working on the same insect in separate
laboratories may be explained as strain differences arising in cultures. Few behav-
iorists maintain careful checks on the ‘quality’ of the laboratory-reared insects that
serve as their research subjects, but all should consider it.
Biologists have long used the technique of comparing different species in order to
understand a trait better. If the first important development for the rigorous scien-
tific study of animal behavior was publication of the theory of evolution by natural
selection, and the second was the rise of genetics, the third almost certainly was
the development of a systematic comparative method, a development that is usually
credited to two Englishmen, George John Romanes and C. Lloyd Morgan.
The name of George John Romanes is much less well known than the names
Darwin, Wallace, and Mendel, probably because although he is generally credited
as being the first to formally use comparative methods to study animal behavior, his
motives and methods were a bit unorthodox. Romanes’ interest lay in studying ani-
mals to gain insights into the behavior of humans. Specifically, he wanted to show
that mental processes evolve in a continuous way from lower to higher forms, with
humans at the top of the list. Without much direct evidence, Romanes constructed
an elaborate scale for emotional states. Worms were lowest because he judged them
capable of feeling only surprise and fear. Insects were higher, being capable of var-
ious social feelings and curiosity; fish showed play, jealousy, and anger; reptiles
displayed affection; birds showed pride and terror; finally, various mammals were
credited with hate, cruelty, and shame.
A British psychologist, C. Lloyd Morgan was also interested in this field, which
he called ‘mental evolution’. Dismayed by the ways in which people like Romanes
relied on inferences and anecdotes rather than recorded facts or direct behavioral
observations, Morgan developed and promoted the use of the observational method:
1.2 Conceptual Frameworks 25
When discussing the conceptual frameworks that inform insect behavior, it is impor-
tant to acknowledge some other hazards that need to be watched for and avoided.
One set of traps arises from the nature of language itself.
All behavioral study rests on the bedrock of detailed description. However,
human language arises from human experience and thus we lack the words to
26 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
describe the behaviors of another species in a way that is fully divorced from our
own experiences. For one thing, almost all descriptive terms possess inherent human
connotations that often imply human purpose and/or motivation. Attributing human
characteristics to other species of animals is termed anthropomorphism (Fig. 1.11).
Even though this practice is generally unwarranted, anthropomorphic terms catch
people’s attention better than blander text does, so they are often used to describe
animal behaviors, particularly in items written for the general public. However,
when a scientist uses or encounters an anthropomorphic term, it should be with
full understanding that an interpretation is being made of the motivations of the
animal and that any such interpretation is highly suspect. Descriptions of a cricket
‘happily’ chirping on the hearth or a ‘fearful’ cockroach scurrying across the floor
may be commonplace in nature books for children but have no rightful place in the
scientific study of behavior. We simply don’t know what the sensation of happiness
is to a cricket or fear to a cockroach.
Fig. 1.11 Casting insects in human terms. Anthropomorphism has been common since the
dawn of human experience. A familiar example to most people is the story of the grasshopper and
the ants, which goes back at least to Aesop (620–560 B.C.), a slave and storyteller who lived in
Ancient Greece
A second difficulty with language use, more insidious and surprisingly pervasive
in the field of animal behavior, is the problem of teleology. A form of anthropomor-
phism, teleology is the doctrine that the processes of nature are directed toward some
discernible ‘goal’. In effect, teleology endows animals with motivations similar to
those that humans might show under similar circumstances. Much human behavior
is goal directed, or purposive; for example, when hungry, one goes to the table or
refrigerator in anticipation of food. However, most animal behavior is directive, that
is, the outcome of the response is not foreseen by the organism. Thus, there is a
confounding of ends and means in statements such as ‘bees visit flowers because
they want nectar.’ The bee is brought to the nectar by its response to the stimulus
provided by the flower, not because it consciously anticipates the outcome of its
actions.
1.2 Conceptual Frameworks 27
the best behavior in 10,000 years or even in 10 years. A previously beneficial trait
may become deleterious, or vice versa, under new conditions such as a changed or
different environment.
Fourth, not every trait can be optimized by natural selection, because traits are
linked together in many ways. In some situations a heterozygote has greater fitness
than either homozygote. In other situations, epistasis—where genes at different loci
interact to produce the phenotype—results in selection acting differently on genes
in different genetic backgrounds. Many genes affect more than one trait; one allele
may produce a positive effect on one trait but a negative effect on another. Thus, by
necessity, tradeoffs must occur. Furthermore, genes can also be physically so close
together on the same chromosome that they tend to be inherited together, making it
difficult for natural selection to optimize each trait independently.
Fifth, evolution does not have a predetermined goal for any species. There often
may be cases where an organism simply cannot evolve a particular trait that would
suit it well, because it does not have the relevant genes in its gene pool to do so.
Mutations are random; they do not arise to fulfill a need.
If these precautions can be kept in mind, adaptation and natural selection can
provide a powerful framework within which to generate sound, testable hypotheses.
Long before it was ever given its name, people have recognized the reality of
microevolution (genetic changes within populations or species) because of two
1.3 Phylogeny’s Role 29
based primarily on wing venation (thereby excluding the many wingless forms),
classified cockroaches into five families and 17 subfamilies. Two other cockroach
classification systems, Princis’ system and Bey-Bienko’s system were based on
diverse and apparently unrelated structural differences. Princis divided the group
into four suborders and 28 families, and Bey-Bienko recognized just three families
with a total of 15 subfamilies.
Behavioral characteristics and internal morphology were given little attention
in any of these early schemes, yet cockroach reproductive biology is exceedingly
diverse. Although it is unusual to find more than one mode of oviposition behav-
ior in a particular insect group, cockroaches display an almost complete spectrum.
Like most insects and birds, females of many cockroach species lay eggs in packets
(oothecae). Such reproduction, where fertilized eggs develop outside the female’s
body, is termed oviparity. Other cockroach species exhibit ovoviviparity—the eggs
are first extruded and then retracted into a brood sac where the embryos absorb
water from their mother until they mature; then the ootheca is again extruded and
the nymphs hatch and drop free from their mother. A third type of reproduction,
viviparity, is found in still other roach species. This oviposition method is super-
ficially similar to the previous type, but when first formed the eggs lack sufficient
yolk to allow complete development, so both nutrients and water must be absorbed
during embryonic development in the brood pouch.
When the species having these three types of reproduction are superimposed on
the three older classifications they intermingle throughout the suborders, families,
and subfamilies rather than sorting out into any consistent pattern. In 1964, this
puzzling fact prompted Frances McKittrick to undertake a new look at cockroach
classification. For comparative study, she chose four character systems: female gen-
italia, male genitalia, the proventriculus (a portion of the intestinal tract specialized
for grinding food, also referred to as the gizzard), and oviposition behavior. Upon
analyzing her results, McKittrick was able to recognize two phyletic lineages based
primarily on reproductive behavior. In one lineage all species remained oviparous,
undoubtedly the ancestral form of reproduction. This group (Blattidae) is exempli-
fied by the notorious American roach, Periplaneta americana. The other lineage
encompassed the ovoviviparous and viviparous species, including the equally noto-
rious German roach, Blattella germanica. In the years since her work was published,
McKittrick’s conclusions have gained widespread acceptance and support from a
variety of other studies (Fig. 1.12), and she is still recognized for being the first
researcher to stress reproductive behavior as an important trait in the evolution of
cockroaches.
The most recent systematic studies of the group, including both extensive mor-
phological and molecular analyses, still leave some phylogenetic details unresolved
and uncertainties remain. The one striking and fundamental difference between the
earlier and current phylogenies is that the increasing weight of available evidence
has revealed that the termites (Isoptera) properly fall within the cockroach lineage
as a sister group to the oviparous Cryptocercidae, a group of uncertain affinity in the
earlier classifications.
When comparative behavioral studies uncover a seeming progression in one
or more characters within a set of related species, it is tempting to conclude that
1.3 Phylogeny’s Role 31
Fig. 1.12 Classifying cockroaches. (left) Three older competing schemes (see text) based solely
on external features observed in museum specimens gave way to one in 1964 based primarily on
female reproductive behavior. (right) The currently accepted higher classification and phylogeny of
the Blattaria based on a much more extensive data set, including molecular data, also incorporates
the termites
evolution has actually proceeded through such a series. However, such a conclusion
should be tempered with caution. The fact that intermediate forms are possible and
do exist indicates only that evolution might have proceeded through a similar (but
probably not identical) series. In the absence of a fossil record or some compelling
logical argument, it does not even indicate the direction of the evolution of the trait
in question.
Prey transport, for example, varies considerably among predatory solitary wasps
(Fig. 1.13). Many species carry paralyzed prey back to the nest in their jaws; others
always hold the prey with their middle legs, and still others use their hind legs. Some
fly hunters even carry the prey impaled on their stinger, but perhaps the most remark-
able prey-carrying adaptation is found in a crabronid wasp, Clypeadon laticinctus,
that transports paralyzed ants on a specialized ‘ant clamp’ formed by the modi-
fied apical abdominal segments. While prey carriage seems to be constant among
all members of a particular morphologically defined genus, there appears to be no
correlation of prey carriage type with wasp phylogeny beyond this, nor with nest
type or phylogenetic position of the prey. This appears to be a case where similar
selective pressures have molded the evolution of prey carriage mechanisms.
What are these selective factors? In 1962, Howard E. Evans suggested some
answers. In the soil-nesting species, mandibular prey carriage tends both to obstruct
the major sense organs and to impede the wasps’ ability to dig into their nest
entrance. Furthermore, species that use their mouthparts in prey transport either tend
to leave the nest open between foraging trips or use such large prey that they must
drag them back to the nest; if they were to close the nest between prey trips, they
would be forced to lay the prey down temporarily while they reopened it, thereby
leaving the prey vulnerable to attack by predators and parasites. Shifting the prey
beneath their body and back from the head or even clamped or impaled on the stinger
32 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Fig. 1.13 Carrying prey. (Top) A female of the Australian eumenid wasp, Pseudabispa
clasps her caterpillar prey in her jaws, supported by her middle and hind legs as she flies to
her nest
would increase their own efficiency and better protect their offspring from intruders.
The relative rarity of abdominal clamp prey carriage mechanisms suggests that the
advantage of having all three pairs of legs free may be offset by the relatively greater
risk of exposure of the prey to parasites, especially to attacks by parasitic satellite
flies (Fig. 1.14).
Another important line of evidence is provided by detailed studies of adap-
tive radiation (the rapid evolution of new lineages). Isolated areas such as oceanic
islands are particularly good places to look for examples of adaptive radiation; only
a few species may manage to invade such remote areas, and in the relative absence
of interspecific competition, new forms may rapidly evolve. The famous case of the
adaptive radiation of Darwin’s finches on the Galapagos Islands is almost certainly
the world’s best-known example. The Bembix sand wasps of Australia provide a
behavioral example of adaptive radiation. Although flies are the prey of nearly all of
the 300-plus species of Bembix throughout the world, 11 of the 28 Australian species
for which biological data exist have switched to other prey (Fig. 1.15). This unique
radiation in prey preferences in Australia is presumably related to the absence of
related genera of sand wasps that occupy these food niches elsewhere in the world.
Other factors may also be involved, such as scarcity of usual prey in some habitats,
especially arid ones. Species such as B. variabilis may be in the earliest stages of
splitting into two distinct species because different populations seem to have spe-
cialized on quite different prey. Throughout most of Australia B. variabilis use flies
1.3 Phylogeny’s Role 33
Fig. 1.14 Looting its host. A satellite fly, Senotainia trilineata (Sarcophagidae), rests on a nest-
marker nail. Such flies typically follow just behind prey-laden wasps like a satellite, and attempt to
swiftly dart in and deposit their live larvae on the wasp’s prey as the wasp enters its nest. Smaller
prey slung under the wasp’s body may be less vulnerable to such parasites
as prey, but a population discovered in the far northwest of the continent seem to
exclusively use damselflies as prey.
Fig. 1.15 Undergoing adaptive radiation. Known prey preferences of 28 Australian Bembix
species. Most species hunt only flies (Diptera), the prey used exclusively by studied species of this
genus elsewhere in the world. However, seven species are specialists on three other orders while
three species (variabilis, kamulla, and moma) hunt both flies and species from a different order,
and one species (allunga) takes prey from three very different insect orders
level are equivalent. For example, although cats (Felidae) and orchids (Orchidaceae)
are both ranked as family level groups in Linnaean classification, the two groups are
not comparable. One has a longer history than the other and the extent of diversity
within each group differs dramatically.
In response to these concerns, scientists have developed other ways to show evo-
lutionary relationships in a tree-like or bush-like form. The best-known alternative
is based on an approach called cladistics. Cladistics began in the 1950s with the
work of the German entomologist, Willi Hennig, who referred to it as phylogenetic
systematics.
Cladistics differs from other taxonomic systems in its strong focus on evolution
rather than similarities between species, and in its heavy emphasis on objective,
quantitative analysis, particularly using DNA and RNA sequencing data. The cladis-
tic approach relies on identifying monophyletic groups, clusters of taxa that share a
more recent ancestor with each other than they do with other groups. Polyphyletic
1.3 Phylogeny’s Role 35
groups, by contrast, are ‘mixed bags’ that may seem to share similarities but actually
arose from two or more different ancestors. This approach has two main differences
from the Linnaean system. First, phylogenetic classification tells you something
important about the organism: its evolutionary history. Second, phylogenetic clas-
sification does not even attempt to rank organisms. In contrast to the traditional
Linnaean system of classification, phylogenetic classification names only clades, or
groups that include both a single ancestor and all its descendents.
The most important assumption in cladistics is that characteristics of organisms
change over time, because it is only when characteristics change that different lin-
eages or groups can be recognized. The original state of the characteristic before
it changed is called plesiomorphic; the new state after the change, apomorphic.
Though some people use the term ‘primitive’ instead of plesiomorphic and ‘derived’
instead of apomorphic, biologists generally avoid using these words because they
have inaccurate connotations. It is all too easy to think of primitive things as being
simpler and inferior, but in many cases the original plesiomorphic state of a charac-
ter is more complex than the changed, apomorphic state. For example, as they have
evolved, many cave insects have lost effective vision, and many island-dwelling
species have reduced wings.
Instead of a Linnean-based evolutionary tree, cladists construct (or in the words
of some, ‘reconstruct’) a phylogenetic tree. This is a diagram intended to repre-
sent the evolutionary history of modern taxa. The trunk, or earliest ancestor, gives
rise to limbs that give rise to branches that terminate in twigs. The tips of the
twigs represent species that are alive today. Sometimes branches fall off, represent-
ing extinction. (Some scientists prefer to think of a phylogenetic bush with many
branches, rather than a tree with a single trunk; both terms are used.)
How does one go about constructing such a phylogenetic tree? No diagram can
take every trait into consideration, so the first thing one must do is identify the par-
ticular characters (inherited traits) one will consider, and then describe the ways they
vary, i.e. their character states. Traits can be almost anything that has an assumed
genetic base and that can consistently be measured. Historically, trait measurements
were morphological or anatomical, and many were gathered from fossil evidence.
Sometimes traits corresponded to a single structure (such as hind leg lengths in
various grasshopper species). In other cases, traits were expressed as ratios that
described the measurements of a set of related structures (such as the tibia: femur
ratio for various grasshopper species). These ratios were helpful to account for envi-
ronmental influences on factors such as absolute body size. Behavioral (see Section
1.3.3 below) or physiological traits, such as reproductive modes (see Fig. 1.12), can
be useful as well. With the advent of molecular genetics, many traits are measured
as DNA sequences, and molecular genetic maps are used to help build phylogenies
by comparing and contrasting sequences across different species. Hopefully, one’s
wise choices will lead to trees that approximate reality. However, there is always
the possibility that choosing different characters might have led to different results.
Cladistics can produce profoundly complex analyses, so it is important to remem-
ber that however fancy an analysis may look or how many molecular data have gone
into it, the result is still a tree or bush that represents a hypothesis. New and better
36 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
data could change the outcome and support a different hypothesis about the way that
the organisms are evolutionarily related.
Next, one must judge which species are most closely related to each other and
thus are descended from a more recent common ancestor. This involves decisions
concerning homologies—characteristics assumed to be shared by species through
descent from a common ancestor rather than being a product of a similar environ-
ment. As theories go, homology is relatively simple, but since it involves judgments
about past events that can never be known with absolute certainty, in actual practice
it sometimes can be quite controversial. For whatever character set one has chosen,
the most similar pairs are considered the most closely related; more differences are
taken to mean a more ancient split in ancestry, based on the fact that it takes more
time for multiple mutations to occur. Unfortunately, all this is assumptive. History
is not something we can see. It happens only once, and only leaves clues behind for
those who attempt to reconstruct evolutionary history.
Whereas evidence for homology can help one make sound phylogenetic deci-
sions, a related possibility can sabotage it. Evolutionary convergence is the tendency
of different clearly unrelated species to develop similar characteristics in response
to a set of environmental conditions. For example, sociality appears in both ants
and termites, though they are only distantly related (see Chapter 10). Although the
selection pressures under which sociality evolved may have been similar, the trait
was not shared with a common ancestor.
After determining the characters and examining each taxon to establish its char-
acter states, some types of cladistic analysis require that one decide the polarity, or
direction of evolution, for each character; this can take some work. Then the taxa
must be grouped by synapomorphies—‘changed’ character states that they share. If
conflicts arise, they must resolved by some clearly stated method; the usual one is
parsimony (see Section 1.2.3).
Finally, when it is time to actually construct the phylogenetic tree, ideally it
should follow two rules. First, all taxa go on the endpoints of the tree, never ‘lower
down’ at nodes as Linnaean trees would. Instead, all nodes must have a list of
synapomorphies that are common to all taxa above the node (unless the charac-
ter is later modified). Second, all synapomorphies must appear on the tree only once
(unless the character state was derived separately by parallel evolution). In practice,
‘hybrid’ trees often appear.
Once constructed, a phylogenetic tree is a very helpful tool that can be employed
to make predictions about fossils or about poorly studied species. It also can help sci-
entists learn about the order of the evolution of a particular trait, a complex feature,
or observed diversity.
that behavioral patterns can be treated like morphological ones, and this included
showing homologies. Behaviors also could act as barriers that interrupt gene flow,
thus starting in motion the process of speciation, the division of a species into two
or more new biological species that are genetically unique.
Differences in mating behavior often constitute the strongest sort of species-
isolating devices. For example, consider the fireflies of eastern North America. This
is a confusing group in which the males show almost no differences in structure
or body coloring but much variation in flash pattern used to attract females dur-
ing courtship. This problem attracted the attention of Harry S. Barber; after much
study, in 1951 he published a description of 18 species of Photuris fireflies classified
mainly on the basis of male flash patterns. Ten of these were named as new since
they had not been previously recognized by morphology alone. Similarly, when
studying the common smaller fireflies of the United States, Jim Lloyd found several
such ‘hidden species’ in the related genus Photinus. First recognized by consis-
tent differences in flash signals (Fig. 1.16), these fireflies later were found to differ
Fig. 1.16 Sorting species by flash. Using behavior to inform decisions about speciation, Lloyd
compared flash patterns of Photinus fireflies. Differences among several North American species
in the male flash signals (left) and female response flashes (right; note different time scales) are
shown at typical field temperatures (signal timing is temperature dependent). Female responses
are timed from initiation of the last pulse in the male flash pattern (open bars indicate optional
responses)
38 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
In various versions of an old tale from India, a group of blind men (or men in the
dark) each touch one different part of an elephant to learn what it is like. When they
compare notes on what they felt, they find they are in complete disagreement. The
story is used to indicate that reality may be viewed differently depending upon one’s
perspective, suggesting that what seems an absolute truth may be relative due to the
deceptive nature of half-truths.
Over the years, behaviorists and non-scientists alike have asked questions about
almost every aspect of animal behavior, while trying to avoid being like these fabled
blind men. Even today, such attempts are sometimes successful, sometimes not.
Different ways of looking at a behavior clearly lead to different answers.
1.4 Questions and Perspectives 39
Fig. 1.17 Evaluating Trichoptera relationships. (top) The original hypothesis of Ross about
Trichoptera (caddisflies) phylogeny compared to subsequent worker’s hypotheses of subordinal
relationships based on differing character sets that show a lack of consensus. (below) A recent
composite phylogeny of family group taxa of Trichoptera proposed by K. M. Kjer and colleagues
incorporating data from molecular analyses strongly supports five monophyletic clades. With
respect to the relationships among the three suborders it closely resembles that proposed by Ross
on the basis of caddisfly behavior
40 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
Table 1.1 Tinbergen’s four questions: Basic approaches to the study of animal behavior
In a classic paper the pioneering ethologist Niko Tinbergen pointed out that
essentially all research questions address one of four major concerns about a behav-
ior: the nature of the immediate stimuli that evoke it, the way in which it develops,
the survival function it has, and how it has evolved (Table 1.1).
While Tinbergen’s Four Questions have become axiomatic in behavior study, others
viewing the list realized that they could be grouped into just two kinds of analyses:
proximate analysis and ultimate analysis. Proximate analysis focuses on the imme-
diate causes of a behavior—those that occur during a given organism’s life. Ultimate
analysis is defined in terms of the evolutionary forces that have shaped a trait over
time. For example, a group of scientists might ask why house flies are drawn to
sugar. Those favoring a proximate explanation might consider factors such as the
way sugar acts on the fly’s sensory system (Tinbergen’s ‘immediate stimuli’) or per-
haps the physiological events that occur in the fly’s brain (‘development’). Other
scientists in the group might approach the same question from a different analyti-
cal viewpoint, an ultimate analysis in terms of the ways that sugar attraction might
increase flies’ chances of surviving long enough to reproduce (‘survival function’)
or the selective forces in the evolutionary past that favored sugar-preferring individ-
uals (‘evolutionary history’) and whether relatives of the house fly display similar
responses to sugar.
It is sometimes said that proximate questions generally ask ‘how’ and ‘what’
whereas ultimate questions tend to ask ‘why’ but this simplistic division can be con-
fusing. It may also seem to imply that proximate analyses are less important than
ultimate analyses, but that is not the case. There are fundamental links between the
two analyses. Proximate analysis, by shedding light on neurobiology, endocrinol-
ogy, molecular genetics, and so on, illuminates the variation available for natural
selection to act on. At the same time, understanding something about the natural
selection pressures that have acted on a behavior can help in designing the best way
to do a proximate analysis. While each explanation of the fly’s attraction to sugar
is perfectly reasonable, none is inherently superior to the others. However, taken
1.4 Questions and Perspectives 41
together, the different explanations and approaches to studying the question provide
a fairly complete understanding of the behavior.
A premier example is the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Because of its importance to
agriculture and its advanced social system, it has become one of the most-studied
insects in the world. In Chapter 8, a case study considers its foraging from the aspect
of communication. Case Study 1.1 considers factors that start the bee out of the hive
door in the first place, and out into the world outside.
Fig. 1.18 Switching jobs. As they grow older, worker bees shift from hive work to foraging.
How might genes affect this behavior? Toma and colleagues measured mRNA. (left) Foraging
bees have substantially higher levels of per mRNA than younger, nonforaging hive bees. This
difference could be due to various factors such as age, behavior (forager vs. nonforager), or both.
(right) Tricking bees into searching for food much earlier than usual, Toma’s team produced 10-
day-old precocious foragers; when they were compared to normal 22-day-old foragers, there were
no statistical differences in per mRNA levels
44 1 The History and Scope of Insect Behavior
The ways in which genes affect the switch from hive work to foraging have
received a great deal of attention (Fig. 1.18). Rapid advances in molecular genetic
technology are leading to ever more detailed glimpses into honeybee foraging. In
a large-scale 2003 study of 5,500 genes, changes in mRNA levels were found to
be associated with about 2,145 genes involved in the hive work/foraging transition.
As such large-scale genomic approaches to behavior become more common, the
challenge will be to figure out how to handle the massive amounts of data they are
capable of gathering. One way to facilitate the molecular analysis of a complex
behavior such as honey bee foraging is to dissect it into simpler behavioral com-
ponents. This is currently a very active area of research. A foraging (for) gene has
been found to influence the phototactic elements of foraging, and a malvolio (mvl)
gene influences the responsiveness of honey bee workers to sucrose and through that
response, the age at onset of foraging and the tendency to forage for either pollen
or nectar. Presumably these are only two of many genes that will be found to play a
causal role in honey bee behavioral maturation.
Chapter 2
Programming and Integrating Behavior
2.1 Introduction
Lacewing males, Chrysoperla downesi, sit among the branches of an evergreen tree,
softly drumming their abdomens against terminal twigs and needles in a long, com-
plex pattern of volleys that attracts females. Moths of the black cutworm, Agrotis
ipsilon, fly by at recorded ground speeds of between 97 and113 km/h (60–70 mph).
Foraging Cataglyphis ants scurry around upon the floor in the Sahara Desert at sur-
face temperatures of up to 70◦ C (158◦ F), scavenging upon the corpses of insects and
other arthropods that have succumbed to heat stress in this extreme environment.
Insect behavior—hidden and obtrusive, incredible and yet somehow familiar,
often admirable and at other times repulsive—has always been a source of amaze-
ment and curiosity. Even with their extra legs and inexpressive faces, insects look
and act enough like little people to make one wonder how and what they think, or
whether they think at all. What regulatory and integrative mechanisms coordinate
the expression of their behavior?
Instead of answering the question, a bit of observation only leads to many more.
A headless male preying mantis can complete a sequence of mating behavior (see
Fig. 9.16). A decapitated cricket will sing several of its song patterns when its neck
connectives are stimulated. Headless cockroaches can learn to keep a leg flexed to
avoid an electrical shock. How necessary is an insect brain?
Having followed a tail-wagging dance for the first time, novice honey bees can
fly in the indicated direction and distance. The first time it strikes, a hand-raised
preying mantis will react to exactly the same stimuli as experienced mantises do.
How much of insect behavior is controlled by instinct?
Honey bees learn to avoid a food source from which they have received an elec-
tric shock, and can remember the previous location of a displaced hive even 12 days
later. Female sand wasps return to their temporarily closed nest and, on the basis of
a quick inspection, decide whether many or few caterpillars must be provided to the
young larva inside. How much reasoning can insects do?
Such questions have a long history and are still far from fully answered. Yet
behavior fairly begs for some degree of functional explanation, because ultimately
behavior means making physiological decisions—when to move and when to
remain still, what objects to approach and what to avoid, which muscles to contract
and which to relax. For any organism, such decisions require some sort of inter-
nal communication system that can organize, manage, and synchronize incoming
information and outgoing responses. Like most other complex organisms, insects
have two such internal communication systems: a nervous system and an endocrine
system. The nervous system is electrochemical, and acts through impulses traveling
over the surface of specialized cells called neurons. The specificity of the messages
in this system resides in the nature of the pathways over which they travel, not in
the differing nature of the messages, because all are due to the same type of physio-
chemical event. The endocrine system, on the other hand, is more purely chemical.
Messages are encoded in specific substances produced and released by specific body
cells. As these chemicals are transported through the body, many cells are exposed
to them, but only certain uniquely sensitive cells respond.
There are real advantages in this duality, two systems existing side by side.
Nerves send electrical messages rapidly, but a nervous system that reached every
cell would take an enormous amount of space. Hormones, on the other hand, can
bathe each cell; however, rapid responses such as escape would be poorly served
by the relatively slower speed of the hormonal bath. In effect, by evolving a dual
system, organisms have made a reasonable trade-off between response speed and
system complexity.
In another sense, however, one might also argue that the insect system is not dual
at all but a continuum broken only by imposition of the human need to classify.
Neurosecretory cells occur throughout the central nervous system and their secre-
tory products are important functional parts of it. Neurosecretory cells emit products
that serve as hormones, neuromodulators, neurotransmitters, regulators of hormonal
secretion, and a variety of other functions. Almost all are peptides or small proteins
that control physiological or biochemical processes in which sustained stimulation is
needed. Although over 100 have been identified in insects, clearly defined functions
have been demonstrated for very few.
‘What is the difference between a vertebrate and an invertebrate?’ ‘One goes squish-
crunch; the other goes crunch-squish.’ It’s an old riddle, but it sums up a vital
difference. The characteristic feature of arthropods, including the insects, is a hard,
jointed exoskeleton or cuticle made up of a series of hard plates, the sclerites.
For an insect, having its support frame on the outside is a mixed blessing,
because the same cuticle that provides a defense against assault and dehydration
also separates the insect from its environment. If the insect is to make behaviorally
appropriate responses, the nervous system inside this shell must be able to receive
information across the barrier and that requires a moist receptor surface, but for
such a small organism, water loss absolutely must be minimized. The compromise
for insects has been to expose receptor surface only through pores that, while open
2.2 Nerve-Based Coordination 47
continuously, are so extremely small that they minimize the potential for water loss.
The receptors also have been concentrated on a very few body areas, such as the
mouthparts, antennae, legs and cerci. Most of the body has remained waterproof
but also insensitive to external stimuli. Where sensory reception occurs, the basic
unit is the sensillum, which usually looks like a tiny hair in a socket (see Fig. 6.2).
The sensory neurons inside are bipolar—one end extends into the cuticular portion
to receive stimuli and the other end sends messages to the central nervous system.
Sensilla have many structures and functions, including not only those one might
expect such as taste perception, but also unexpected ones such as the infra-red irra-
diation detectors in species of buprestid beetles that breed only in trees recently
killed by fire.
All receptors code their information in units called action potentials, which are
self-regenerating standard signals that travel through other nerve cells along the
length of long cytoplasmic cell projections (axons) through small gaps (synapses),
the switchboards of the nervous system, where they are transmitted by the release of
chemicals having specific effects upon the neuron or muscle cell across the synapse.
Stimulation usually leads to the production of not one but many nerve impulses all
of the same amplitude. Information about the stimulus is coded in the number and
frequency with which they follow each other, within limits of the system.
The insect nervous system (Fig. 2.1) is made up of two highly structured, inter-
twined systems. The first is the visceral (also called stomatogastric or sympathetic)
system that controls alimentary canal movements and is closely concerned with the
process of neurosecretion. The second is the central nervous system (CNS), which
coordinates the peripheral sense organs and muscles. As concerns insect behavior,
the latter is of the most direct relevance. (A third term, peripheral nervous system, is
also sometimes used; it refers to all the nerves that radiate from the CNS to innervate
muscles, stretch receptors, sensory receptors, etc.)
Because insects evolved from the segmented system of annelid worms, their
organization still reflects that past. In the earliest insects, each body segment
probably contained a knot-like cluster of nerve cells, called a ganglion (plural,
ganglia) that was responsible for the activities of that segment. However, in all liv-
ing insects today, different parts of the system have fused together in various ways.
A ganglion typically contains a mass of neuron cell bodies at its outer edges and a
central region, the neuropil, where synapses occur. The largest and most complex
neuropil occurs in the brain, but all ganglia contain neuropil regions. The neuropil
appears to keep growing, even after rest of the nervous system stops; this suggests
the importance of new information processing and integration throughout an
insect’s life.
Nerve cells, like highways, have many shapes, and no single shape can be called
characteristic. Like superhighway systems connecting cities, nerves radiate from
48 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
Fig. 2.1 Visualizing the insect nervous system. (a) Side view of a representative grasshopper
head. A chain of ganglia trail from the brain, dipping below the gut to lie just inside the ven-
tral cuticle; nerves from the ganglia branch out to the sense organs and muscle systems, while
neurosecretory cells in the ganglia chemically link the nervous and endocrine systems. (b) Front
view shows the three main regions of the brain—the protocerebrum, the deutocerebrum, and the
small tritocerebrum—and the nerves that connect the tritocerebrum with the frontal ganglion and
labrum; circumesophageal connectives at the back of the tritocerebrum connect the brain to the
subesophageal ganglion, a complex structure supplying nerves to mouthparts, neck, and salivary
glands
2.2 Nerve-Based Coordination 49
the ganglia to organs and muscles, generally within the segment where the ganglion
resides. Also, like most superhighways, they generally (but not always), are two-way
streets, with sensory (or afferent) neurons delivering signals to the central nervous
system, and motor (or efferent) neurons carrying output back to muscles, glands,
and organs. The few that act like one-way streets (such as the purely sensory ocellar
nerve) do not directly synapse with one another; instead they connect through one
or many interneurons.
In most insects, the brain is a large group of neurons that lies above the
esophagus. For that reason it is sometimes called the supraesophageal ganglion.
(Surprisingly, while the brain is in the head of most insects, in dipterous maggots
it is several body segments back.) There is still some disagreement among insect
physiologists about the brain’s precise origins and number of primitive segments
that comprise it. However, three parts are generally recognized (Fig. 2.1b). The
most anterior section, called the protocerebrum, is the most complex part of the
insect brain. It directs neural traffic at the crossroads between sensory input and
motor output. At each side, optic lobes extend to the compound eyes; at its center, a
pair of large mushroom bodies (corpora pedunculata) process olfactory information
and control tasks that require visual coordination of locomotor activity and spatial
orientation (see also Plate 1). The mushroom bodies and associated cells provide a
structure for elaborate interconnections that allow learning and memory to occur.
The second brain section, called the deutocerebrum, connects to the antennae.
Its neurons are of two types—one type processes chemosensory information; the
other, mechanosensory. The former have received a great deal of research atten-
tion because of the importance of the chemosensory neuropil in mediating feeding,
sexual, and social behaviors.
The third and smallest part of the brain, the tritocerebrum, connects the central
nervous system to the ventral nerve cord through the circumesophageal connec-
tives. It also innervates the labrum (upper lip), pharynx (region between mouth and
digestive system), and the rest of the visceral nervous system.
Beyond the brain, ganglia line up along a nerve cord that runs along the under-
side of the insect’s thorax and abdomen. The first ganglion of this ventral nerve
cord (and the only one still in the head) is the subesophageal ganglion. Another
nerve supercenter, it controls the rest of the mouthparts, salivary glands, and neck
muscles; axons from its neurons travel both forward to the brain and posterior to the
thoracic ganglion. The subesophageal ganglion influences motor patterns involved
with walking, flying, and breathing, although those motor patterns actually begin in
other ganglia.
The rest of the ventral nerve cord consists of a further series of paired ganglia
joined by lateral connectives. Typically, the first three are in the thorax and control
the wings and the legs. The number of abdominal ganglia varies because they have
fused in various ways in different insect groups.
The brain and central nervous system almost certainly are not merely simple
relay stations; rather, they function as integrative machines. Part of the evidence for
this comes from ablation experiments, in which anatomically distinct parts of the
system are surgically removed and their role is deduced by comparing behaviors
50 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
before and after excision. The crudest of these is simply to cut off the insect’s head,
an ‘experiment’ that has been performed since far antiquity.
Ablation experiments have confirmed that for many stereotyped behaviors the
insect brain actually is unnecessary—an observation which led many early biol-
ogists to believe that insects had none. The confusion was understandable. A
decapitated insect may even show complex behavior for many days, because (in
contrast to the vertebrates) insects have not concentrated their life support systems
within the brain. Crucial controls of functions such as respiration are decentralized;
so are many postural, locomotory, sexual, and grooming mechanisms. At the same
time, however, certain crucial differences can be noted when an insect is deprived of
various parts of its brain. Most notable is that the relative incidence of various types
of actions changes markedly. Often, there is an uncontrolled release of competing
behavior modes.
In a well-studied example, a preying mantis deprived of its protocerebrum simul-
taneously and continuously performs two opposed behaviors—grasping, which
holds it back, and walking, which pulls it forward. This is behavioral nonsense,
resulting in a hopelessly entangled and exhausted mantis. Intact mantises, of course,
spend most of their time doing neither; rather, they wait motionless, often for hours,
to ambush unsuspecting prey.
When the subesophageal ganglion is removed along with the protocerebrum,
however, the mantis becomes permanently immobile. Does the neural program for
leg movement reside in the subesophageal ganglion? No, because electrical stimu-
lation of a disconnected thoracic ganglion produces vigorous and complete limb
movements. Rather, neural inhibition occurs—the capacity of a neuron to exert
a blocking action on cells connected to it. In an intact mantis, the protocerebral
lobes apparently send out inhibitory messages that differentially block parts of the
excitatory activity being generated by the subesophageal ganglion. The excitatory
messages that are allowed to pass are transmitted to the thoracic ganglia where
grasping or locomotion is initiated.
How, then, does one explain the copulatory behavior of the decapitated male
mantis? When his head is destroyed, as often occurs during courtship with the preda-
tory female (see Fig. 9.16), the mantis walks in a circle while vigorously performing
continuous copulatory movements. Here, the ventral ganglia themselves possess an
endogenous activity that is usually inhibited by the subesophageal ganglion. Such
inhibition is a fundamental property of nervous systems. If a nervous system were
unable to inhibit those circuits competing with the one responsible for a desired
behavior, the result would be behavioral chaos.
common form of response to a stimulus. It is also the least complex, because the
input and output have a one-to-one relationship. A knee jerk, for example, does not
repeat itself unless the knee is tapped repeatedly. Because of their relative simplic-
ity, reflexes provide some of the clearest examples of the ways in which behavioral
stereotypy depends on properties of nervous systems. Other examples include kine-
ses and taxes, two locomotory responses that will be covered in more detail in the
next chapter.
A related set of simple behaviors are those involved in various rhythmically
repeated motor patterns such as insect songs, flight, and walking. How are these
physiologically generated, maintained, and coordinated? Historically, one explana-
tion (called the cyclic-reflex hypothesis) stated that feedback from the act itself was
sufficient to cause the act to be repeated. Thus, such rhythmic actions would con-
tinue in a repetitive circular loop (like perpetual motion) until inhibited by other
reflex paths or by the brain. Since the 1960s, however, accumulating evidence
has supported the concept that many patterns are generated in the central nervous
system—the central pattern generator (CPG) hypothesis.
In the 1960s, Donald Wilson conducted a series of now-classic studies supporting
the CPG hypothesis. Technical advances in electrophysiology had made extracellu-
lar recording with cathode ray oscilloscopes into a powerful new tool for studying
the neural basis of behavior when Wilson began collaborating with Torkel Weis
Fogh, who had developed an experimental system in which a tethered locust could
be induced to fly normally in a wind tunnel.
Working together, the two men built a detailed picture of neural output to the
locust’s flight muscles, then compiled a catalog of sensory inputs to the thoracic
ganglia. Next, in a series of skillful experiments, Wilson successively eliminated
sensory input to the thoracic ganglia while recording motor output. First, he
removed the locust’s head and subesophageal ganglion; lacking its wind-sensitive
sensory hairs, the locust flew slowly but otherwise normally. Step by step, further
operations severed one source of sensory input after another, until Wilson was able
to show that even total removal of all sources of periodic input did not abolish pat-
terned motor output (Fig. 2.2). When the breezes in the wind tunnel were kept still,
the basic pattern of flight motor nerve discharge remained unaltered. When Wilson
severed the nerve fibers from the stretch receptors that register wing movement,
the pattern persisted. When he dispensed with timing cues from other moving body
parts, nothing changed. Clearly, the locust was not timing its wing beat through
sensory cues created by wing movement. The motor pattern must have originated
within the thoracic ganglia.
The concept of central coordination does not exclude peripheral influences,
however. Though the sensory and motor systems were not linked in the locust in
terms of timing, the average frequency of wing beat was correlated with the dis-
charge rate in the receptor nerves and it slowed when receptor nerves were cut.
Sensory feedback did not cue sequences, Wilson postulated, but instead modu-
lated genetically determined motor programs. Subsequent studies have added a
layer of complexity to Wilson’s conclusions, however, with the recognition of neu-
rohormonal modulations of these central circuits. Even though locomotion (and
52 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
Fig. 2.2 Demonstrating the central pattern generator. Experiments with tethered locusts,
which will perform normal flight movements in a wind tunnel, demonstrated that the insect cen-
tral nervous system, without sensory input from wing sense organs, generates a pattern of motor
neuron output that closely resembles the pattern produced in normal flight. Sensory discharges in
nerves from the wing and wing hinge were recorded by manipulating wires into a locust’s largely
eviscerated thoracic cavity. Down-stroke muscle potentials repeat at the wing beat frequency, while
stretch receptors fire two to three times per wing beat
Fig. 2.3 Signaling cockroach escape. (above) Apparatus used by Kenneth Roeder to study startle
response in the American cockroach, Periplaneta americana, one of the commonest cockroaches
throughout the world. Glued to a stick, the cockroach is allowed to grasp an unattached cork ball
that turns as the roach runs. Near the cockroach’s cerci are an air nozzle and a small paper flag. As a
puff of air is delivered to the cerci the flag is deflected and an impulse registers on the oscilloscope
screen. As the cockroach reacts a second deflection is registered and the difference between the
two is the startle response time. (Below) Air puffs stimulate receptor hairs on the cerci that relay
impulses to giant interneurons running up the central nerve cord to the metathoracic ganglia, from
which signals activate motor neurons in the legs. Though the sensory hairs hang below the cerci,
they can detect wind from all directions
several seconds after all input from the giant fibers had ceased. This illustrated
an important adaptive aspect of evasive behavior—movement continues for
some time after contact with the stimulus, thereby taking the animal out of
harm’s way.
Since Roeder’s studies, others have confirmed that for most invertebrates,
the receptor system associated with detecting danger is associated with a
rapidly conducting nervous system that can bring about action through the
shortest/fastest neuronal route. In most cases, this is via rapidly conduct-
ing giant fibers in the ventral nerve cord. For a number of species, a single
interneuron is enough to trigger normal escape. In many cases, all the neural
circuitry needed for the basic oriented response occurs in the ventral nerve
cord and thoracic ganglia.
While the nerve circuits guiding escape reactions superficially seem
quite simple, detailed studies over the past forty years have shown that
cockroach escape behavior is actually quite complex, both behaviorally and
physiologically. Jeffrey Camhi and colleagues have been major contributors
to this new understanding. They began with a few simple questions. Could
cockroaches actually determine a threat’s direction from a puff of air, or
were they simply scurrying off in a random direction after being startled?
Plotting many trials showed that the insects usually did turn away from the
source of the wind before running away, so they must be able to determine
wind direction. How? In a series of trials, various areas on the cerci were
covered with wax. When the cerci’s upper side was covered, the cockroach’s
directional response was unaltered. When the underside of either appendage
was covered, most of the turns were away from the uncovered one, regardless
of the wind direction. When the underside of both cerci was covered, the
cockroaches no longer responded to an air puff at all.
Close examination of the bottom side of the cerci shows that the many tiny
pivoting hairs run in columns from the front to the rear. In the late 1960s,
R. Nicklaus, a German researcher, had shown that in any given column, all
the hairs deflect most easily in the same two opposite directions, and that the
directions of maximum pliancy differ from one column to the next. By record-
ing sensory output from a number of individual hairs, Nicklaus showed that
deflection of a hair in one direction maximized the number of action poten-
tials fired by its sensory cell. Deflecting it in the other direction inhibited even
spontaneously occurring action potentials.
Researchers in Camhi’s laboratory set out to determine the best excitatory
direction for each of the nine columns of cercal hairs of P. americana, and
to show the range of wind directions to which each column responded. Their
results showed that each sensory axon responds maximally to a fairly narrow
range of wind directions. However, as a group, the hairs provide 360-degree
56 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
coverage. (Thus, even if you approach a cockroach head-on, the cerci at its
tail end sense you coming!)
What happens to this directional information as it transfers up the neural
chain of command? Camhi and his colleagues made intracellular recordings
from individual giant interneurons, then marked each with various colors of
injected dyes. They learned that each giant interneuron responds characteristi-
cally to air puffs from different directions, and each also differs in its breadth
of response. Moreover, each giant interneuron has a characteristic firing rate
for a particular wind velocity. Comparing the firing rates of two or more giant
interneurons reveals differences uniquely related to wind direction. But how
do the interneurons ‘decide’ where the wind is coming from and which direc-
tion to turn in response? Various mechanistic theories have been proposed;
in recent years, Camhi and colleagues have been addressing these by stimu-
lating cells of identified giant neurons with artificial spikes added during the
sensory-induced behavior, then analyzing the resulting directional leg move-
ments. Though the jury is still out, it appears that directional determination is
based on collaborative calculations of direction by the giant interneurons as a
group, an approach called distributed neural processing.
Male crickets reared in solitary confinement still sing species-typical aggressive and
rivalry songs when confronted with another male.
In many ways, the behavior of insects gives the impression of acting from a
prewritten script. Faced with a given situation, even the novice behaves appropri-
ately. Perhaps for this reason, the concept of instinct, so emotionally controversial
in vertebrate behavioral studies, has been more easily accepted when applied to
insect behavior. As numerous studies have shown, even among insects that have
never been in contact with conspecifics, all individuals of the species still exhibit
many species-specific motor patterns in exactly the same form.
While their counterparts in physiology and neurology were examining insect
behavior from a mechanistic viewpoint, the early ethologists (see Chapter 1) were
more interested in a functional view of behavior. Where the physiologists and
neurologists saw pre-wired endogenous motor programs, the ethologists saw fixed
action patterns (FAPs). First described and elucidated by the pioneering ethologists
Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen in the 1930s, fixed action patterns generally
have several common characteristics. First, once initiated, fixed action patterns
require few or no external stimuli or additional sensory cues for their maintenance
or completion. For example, even after certain body parts are removed, many insects
will proceed to clean the missing appendages as though they were still there; FAPs
have been likened to a piece of memorized music played by a pianist without
hearing, seeing, or feeling tactile sensation from his hands.
Second, fixed action patterns can be evoked by a variety of stimuli, some-
times from different sensory modes, and they may show variability in orientation.
However, once evoked, their basic structure is stereotyped. Third, they are found
in all individuals of the species that displays them. Like morphological characters,
FAPs are subject to selective pressures and have a genetic basis. Fourth, once they
are initiated, fixed action patterns occur in the absence of positive feedback. Nothing
additional is necessary to keep them going to completion.
Finally, fixed action patterns generally involve the coordination of several dif-
ferent muscle groups, and can be quite complex. In crickets, 48 separate FAPs
are involved with molting; the system, coordinated by a hormonal cascade, is
multifaceted and intricate. Why such complexity? The general explanation is that
selection has been intense. With behaviors as fundamental as eclosion, there is no
room for error; mistakes are deadly.
What triggers the release of a fixed action pattern? Tinbergen, Lorenz, and other
ethologists studied dozens of instincts seeking an inclusive theory. They found that
first, an organism’s internal physiological state has to be such that it is in a condition
of readiness to respond, and second, the sensory-neural mechanism involved has to
be exposed to very specific forms of stimulation to set a given FAP into motion.
They called these cues releasers or sign stimuli. In some situations, the releaser
appeared to be the total stimulus configuration, or Gestalt. However, in most cases,
only certain simple, specific aspects of the stimulus were needed. For example, the
attraction of a male mosquito to conspecific females may be evoked by a particular
sound frequency, which we recognize as the females’ distinctive hum; artificially
produced sounds will produce exactly the same behaviors in the complete absence
of females.
58 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
A releaser need not in itself have any inherent relevance to survival. Rather, it
may be some property of a particular situation that has biological importance and a
property that is shared by all other similar situations. For example, skatol and ammo-
nia release feeding behavior in both dung beetles and blowflies; these substances are
produced by decaying matter, but neither are by themselves of any nutritive value
nor do they owe their existence to dung beetles or blowflies. This type of releaser
has been termed a token stimulus (see also Chapter 4).
In passing, it should be noted that the naturally occurring releaser for a behavior
is not necessarily the optimally effective one. Male Argynnis butterflies are more
strongly attracted to solid orange than to the orange-black pattern of the female.
Such a supernormal stimulus—a signal that is even more effective in eliciting a
given behavior than the natural signal is—usually represents an evolutionary com-
promise. For example, in the sexual approach of Heliconius butterfly males, use of
experimental models has shown that pure red wings upon the female would be the
optimal attractant. However, since the coloration also has mimicry functions, natural
selection has dictated limiting the red signal to a stripe.
A releaser that is simple can maximize an animal’s recognition of biologically
important stimuli while minimizing the amount of neural circuitry required, but it
also increases the risk of exploitation by other species that use the FAP for their own
purposes, a tactic known as code breaking. A well known example of this is the way
in which many tropical orchid flowers provide the visual and olfactory releasers that
trick male wasps into attempted copulation (see Fig. 9.8).
These classic ethological observations interface with modern insect physiology
through the useful concept that for each FAP, there exists a sensory-neural release-
controlling mechanism called the innate releasing mechanism (IRM) that evaluates
incoming signals from one or several sense organs and triggers (or fails to trig-
ger) the signals that go out to initiate a reaction. Complex behaviors resolve into
a sequence of stereotyped motor patterns; each step depends on the presence of
appropriate external stimuli and internal physiological state before the IRM will
act. If any link in the chain is broken, the preprogrammed sequence of behaviors
(reaction chain) does not continue and hence cannot run to completion.
By itself, the IRM is a ‘black box’ sort of concept, because it considers only
input–output without considering the physiological components behind the control
of fixed action patterns. At each step, the IRM activation threshold may be highly
variable and may respond to both internal and external influences. In common expe-
rience, we say an organism shows varied motivation to perform the act. A highly
motivated individual may be so ready to act that it will perform a fixed action pat-
tern when confronted by a stimulus bearing but slight resemblance to the typical
releaser. For example, if the sand wasp Ammophila is thwarted too long from pulling
a paralyzed caterpillar into her nest, she will begin to retrieve substitutes. Most moti-
vation initially generates quite unspecific behavior, sometimes simply locomotion.
The behaviors that follow become increasingly specific until they ultimately culmi-
nate. For example, in the chain-like reciprocal signal exchange that occurs between
the two sexes during courtship (see Fig. 9.4), the culminating act of copulation can
occur only at the end of a hierarchical sequence of several releasers and FAPs. The
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 59
prey-catching behavior of the bee wolf (see Chapter 4) is another example of such
hierarchial sequences.
What happens when the environment contains sign stimuli for quite different or
even contradictory forms of behavior? Or when motivations conflict? Under normal
circumstances, behavior outcome depends on the relative strengths and effective-
ness of the different motivational factors and sign stimuli involved, and the behavior
that is activated partially or wholly suppresses other non-compatible behaviors. In
most insects, escape behavior is inhibited during copulation. In aphids, flight and
settling behaviors inhibit each other. If honey bees are exposed to releasers for
activation and inhibition of their communicative dances simultaneously, trembling
dances may take place. When stimuli for completely contradictory behaviors com-
pete for attention, apparently nonfunctional stereotyped actions called displacement
activities often occur. Most recognized cases have involved cleaning or preening
behaviors. Faced with the opposing behavioral motivations to stay at a food source
and to leave it, bees will begin grooming, independent of actual grime. Dusting the
bee with pollen or flour does not change the frequency of this behavior; rather, it
merely directs the preening movements toward the dustier body parts.
Most behaviors are continuously influenced through feedback from both their
internal environment and the external one that surrounds them. A classic exam-
ple of internal feedback is provided by the feeding behavior of the blowfly (see
Fig. 4.3). When stimuli that release feeding are continuously present, more or less
continuous food uptake ends only when inhibitory stimuli arise; these come from
stretch receptors in the gut wall that serve as internal monitors, firing as the gut fills.
Action potentials carried to the brain via the recurrent nerve trigger the eventual
motor response, namely, retraction of the fly’s proboscis from the food. Cutting the
recurrent nerve eliminates the negative feedback from the stretch receptors, with the
result that the fly literally explodes from overeating!
takes the form of one cell that both receives the environmental stimuli and relays it.
All sensory receptors of insects are primary sense cells. The same cell produces both
a receptor potential and an action potential, and there is no need for a second neu-
ron. Vertebrates have secondary sense cells—cells of non-neural origin that receive
a stimulus and link to a neuron to send the message along. For example, touch recep-
tors in the vertebrate skin are modified epidermal cells. More complex sense organs
may contain thousands of tiny sensory devices comprised of specialized nerve cells.
Uniquely sensitive to particular kinds of environmental energy, they simultaneously
screen out all stimuli outside a selected signal range.
Honey bees learn landmarks around a nectar feeder, but ignore these same land-
marks when presented at a novel location. Studies with mutant Drosophila suggest
that mushroom bodies (see Fig. 2.1) are important for odor learning, but not for
visual or tactile learning. Several species of butterflies land preferentially on leaves
of particular shapes, with further discrimination occurring only after landing.
Ever since ethologists first recognized releasers and sign stimuli, it has been
clear that certain types of receptors are tuned to very specific aspects of a gen-
eral stimulus (see Fig. 6.4). One could envision neural information processing as a
hierarchical system through which the various stimulus properties are filtered until a
particular behavioral response is ultimately released. Ethologists suggested the con-
cept of two sensory filters, one peripheral and the other central. Peripheral filters
functioned at the level of the sensory receptors. Central filters occurred within the
nervous system, sorting out incoming information, selecting relevant stimuli for fur-
ther action, and eventually producing a particular response. Central filters thus were
components of what they called innate releasing mechanisms, or IRMs (see Section
2.2.3).
Chemosensory systems provide many examples. Partially because insects are
known to live in a chemical sensory world and partially for the practical rea-
sons of control of agricultural pests, chemoreception has received a great deal of
research attention in the context of stimulus filtering. There are an inestimable
number of odors in the world. How can the sensory and nervous system of any
given insect species decipher them all? The answer, as numerous studies have
shown, is that among both taste receptors and olfactory receptors, some are rela-
tive specialists. For example, each feathery antenna of a male polyphemus moth
has about 150,000 sensory neurons; 60–70% of these are specialized for detect-
ing female-produced sex odors. Other receptors are generalists that respond to
several or many kinds of chemicals but varying in their pattern of sensitivity. By
summing the responses of the different chemoreceptors, the CNS could obtain
characteristic total response profiles, each uniquely representative of a particular
chemical compound. An additional dimension of information would be acquired if
instead of being silent in the non-stimulated (resting) state, chemoreceptor cells
maintained some constant spontaneous baseline level of firing activity. Then a
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 61
a few receptors. It is likely that the relative sensitivity of different receptors also
regularly fluctuates, since it is known, for example, that many insects are rhythmic
in their degree of susceptibility to insecticides.
A supreme example of sensory tuning occurs in predatory bats and the moths
on which they prey. As a simple experiment, suddenly jingle a bunch of keys
near a group of moths flying around a streetlight or window screen. The response
of the moths will at first seem chaotic. Some nearly fall to the ground, while
the flight of others becomes quickened and more erratic. Some that were flut-
tering may become motionless, while others, previously motionless, may take
flight.
What pattern can there possibly be to all of this? To answer this question, one
must appreciate the importance of a principal predator of moths, namely, bats. Fifty
years ago, precise and ingenious experimentation by Donald Griffin revealed a bat
echolocation system (Fig. 2.5) capable of indicating size, distance, location, and
considerable detail about its surroundings, down to items smaller than midges.
However, taking the story to the next level took the work of several researchers,
including the same Kenneth Roeder that unlocked the basis of the cockroach escape
reaction. Case Study 2.2 presents some of the story.
Fig. 2.5 Listening for bats. The relative acoustic sensitivity of a noctuid moth at various frequen-
cies (open circles), contrasted with summed intensities of all natural environmental noise recorded
in the moth’s environment at night (solid line). Sounds below 15 kHz were mostly of insect origin.
Those between 23 and 50 kHz came from passing bats. It is apparent that moth ears are maximally
sensitive to sounds in the latter range and relatively insensitive to other sounds
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 63
bat flying a time intercept course while the moth undertakes selective evasive
tactics on the basis of the bat’s range and speed of approach.
To reduce this complexity, Roeder replaced the bat with an artificial one:
a stationary multidirectional electronic transmitter of ultrasonic pulses, linked
to a camera system and perched 16 ft in the air. When exposed to these elec-
tronic bat cries, moths showed the same bewildering variety of reactions as
they did to real bats. After many hours of observation and over a thousand
photographically recorded moth tracks, Roeder determined that the nature of
the evasive tactics of the naturally flying moths was related to the distance
between the moth and loudspeaker. Moths that flew close to the loudspeaker,
encountering ultrasound that pulsed at 10–30 times a second, responded with
a variety of maneuvers that usually ended in a dive. Those cruising at greater
distances characteristically turned and flew directly away from the sound
source.
The adaptive value of this response difference seems obvious. There would
be little survival advantage to the moth in attempting to flee a bat at close range
or in making erratic turns and twists while the predator was still distant, but
every advantage in erratic behavior when the bat was close. Not surprisingly,
straight directed fleeing occurred only in response to low-intensity sounds;
when the loudspeaker signals were made progressively weaker, the distance
at which directed fleeing was released was correspondingly reduced.
Interestingly, moths cruising at about the same height as the loudspeaker
turned and flew away in the horizontal plane; those above it were observed to
fly directly upward, or redirected their flight by making a sharp turn before
flying straight away. How can a moth equipped with only four sense cells ori-
ent and steer itself with respect to a sound that comes from various angles? As
background for an answer, one must remember that free-flying moths typically
flap their wings many times per second. Unlike an airplane, they rarely fly for
long in the same direction on an even keel. Because the moth ear is located
below its wing, its wing movement has profound effects on the acoustic sensi-
tivity and directionality; on the downstroke, most sound will be screened from
reaching the ear, while at full upstroke the ear is accessible to sound from all
directions.
To investigate the effects of wing position more fully, Roeder and his
associates devised an elaborate apparatus that would measure the acoustic
sensitivity of one ear to sound coming from all points in an imaginary sphere
surrounding a moth fixed in different flight positions. Obtaining these record-
ings was a long and arduous task, and Roeder was fully aware that the surgical
insult of implanting electrodes, plus the restriction of wing movements that
was necessary, might affect the results. However, recordings of the A fiber
responses revealed two types of acoustic asymmetry. One was a left–right
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 65
asymmetry. The right ear detected sounds most effectively when they origi-
nated on the right half of the sphere, and best of all when the sound source
was roughly at right angles to the body axis with the wings at the top of
their upstroke; at the same time, hearing in the left ear was at its minimum.
Alternating with this was a dorsal–ventral asymmetry. The flapping of the
wings had the effect of damping the sound when the wings were in the down
position. Thus, the left–right differences in sound intensity would alternate
with the temporary disappearance and reoccurrence of sound occurring some
10–40 times per second in synchrony with the rate of wing flapping.
How do these asymmetries relate to a moth’s ability to escape a hungry bat?
A left–right difference in A-fiber discharge when the wings are up probably
provides the moth with a rough horizontal bearing on the position of a bat
with respect to its own line of flight. The absence of a left–right differential
discharge and the presence of similar levels of on and off from both ears might
inform the moth that the bat was above it. If neither variation occurred at the
regular wing beat frequency, it would mean that the bat was below or behind
the moth (Fig. 2.6).
It would seem that a single sense cell in each ear could transmit enough
information to inform the moth of a cruising bat’s bearings. Why does each
ear have two acoustic sense cells? Roeder suggested that the answer might
lie in their differential sensitivities. His comparative measurements showed
that A1 responded first, firing over a range of low to moderate intensities.
Cell A2 began to fire only at moderate intensities and fired even faster at
high sound intensity, a range within which A1 was saturated and therefore
incapable of further increase. Thus, by operating in piggyback fashion, A1
and A2 might provide a combined signal from which intensity differences
could be discriminated over a range wider than either alone could accomplish.
In effect, their combined signal would inform the moth how far away the bat
was, which ultimately would decide the form of the moth’s evasive behavior.
He suggested testing this theory by observing the anti-bat behavior of another
group of moths (Notodontidae), because their ears each contain only the A1
cell.
A decade after Roeder’s research raised the puzzle about the A2 receptors, a
report appeared that European notodontids do, in fact, show a two-part response
to approaching bats. However, years passed before the A2 question could begin to
be addressed directly rather than inferentially. The problem lay in the difficulty of
neurally recording moth auditory responses not just to one set of constant acoustic
parameters, but to the complex sounds of real attacking bats. As a bat approaches its
prey, its echolocation calls shift in duration, intensity, rate and frequency. The ideal
situation would be to record the moth’s responses during an actual bat attack, but
the recording equipment itself made it impossible to set up this scenario.
66 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
Fig. 2.6 Assessing bat direction. Roeder showed that the activity in the A1 receptors of a moth’s
ears differs upon detecting bat cries from different points in space. (A) When the bat is to one side
of the moth, the receptor on the closer side fires more rapidly than the shielded receptor. (B) When
the bat is directly behind the moth, both A1 fibers fire at the same time and rate. (C) When the bat
is above the moth, activity in the A1 receptors pulses in synchrony with its wing beat. The role of
the A2 receptors, which are missing in some moths, is less clear
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 67
In 1994, James Fullard and colleagues found a way around the problem by train-
ing a bat in the laboratory to attack a microphone that it expected would be an
edible target. These recordings (and a noise-reduced, computer-generated digital
replicate) were then broadcast to five species of noctuid-like moths to observe their
ears’ responses. The results appear to confirm that the A1 cell encodes the approach
calls of an attacking bat up to about 100–200 ms before the bat would capture the
moth. Then, at this last moment of the moth’s life, the A1 firing drops off to a level
similar to that detecting a faraway bat. Surprisingly, the A2 cell responses were
fundamentally the same, but Fullard found variations among the moth species he
tested. In some, the A2 cell response was vigorous; in others it dropped off early,
was sporadic, or did not appear at all. Fullard and colleagues have suggested that this
may mean that the A2 cell is vestigial, and no longer used in the flight responses of
moths.
A sound also has several other features that could convey information to the
moth’s nervous system, including its intensity, pulse duration, the interval between
pulses, and so on. Furthermore, the receptor is but one point along the chain of neu-
rons leading to the brain where selective decoding of stimuli might occur. Thus, in
the case of the ultrasonic bat cry, moth ear receptor cells might selectively respond
to a particular range of frequencies at one point in the filtering, but after the recep-
tor cells transmit an impulse, subsequent interneurons along the ventral nerve cord
would no longer respond to frequency; rather, other sound properties such as pulse
intensity, pulse duration, or interval between successive pulses would now be mon-
itored. For example, Kenneth Roeder identified a ‘pulse marker’ interneuron in the
moth that responds to three or four sensory impulses separated by short intervals by
firing just once, but the interneuron made no distinction as to duration of a pulse; to
it, 0.5 and 500 μs were the same.
The dramatic conflict between moths and bats has caught the attention and imagi-
nation of scientists in many disciplines. A number of other insects quite distant from
moths have been found to respond evasively to bat ultrasonic cries, from mantises
that loop and plummet erratically downward to lacewings that take a power dive.
The moth-bat story also has come to include some interesting side plots. One is the
production of anti-bat sounds—some arctiid and ctenuchid moths produce clicking
sounds at frequencies close to those used by bats, and bats respond by veering away.
The click may disorient the bat, jam its echolocation system, and/or act as a warning,
signaling the bat that the moth is distasteful (see Chapter 8 and Plate 28).
Another side plot involves mites, common ectoparasitoids that often attack insect
tracheal systems. The mite Dicrocheles phalaenodectes infects only one ear of its
noctuid moth host, destroying the typanum, but leaves the other ear intact; a related
species infests both ears, but leaves the tympanum intact. Presumably, both strate-
gies leave the moth host with some chance of still avoiding bats and thus living on
to support its parasites.
Since many moths have evolved ways to hear bats, why don’t bats evolve a coun-
termeasure such as calls that are too soft for a moth to detect or outside the frequency
range of moth hearing? Mostly likely such drastic changes would severely restrict
the range and acuity of the bat’s sonar system and would only result in related
68 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
adaptive changes in the moths. Such an evolutionary arms race may in fact be under-
way now. Studies in Canada indicate that where bat species-diversity is high with a
wide range of sonar frequencies, moth sensitivity covers this frequency range, but in
areas where the range of bat species is small and the sonar range is more restricted,
moths have a similarly reduced frequency sensitivity range. Similar relationships
have been shown in other areas, including between moths of Panama and those of
Hawaii.
Dragonfly larvae of the genus Anax raised in aquaria soon come to associate the
sight of their caretaker with food; when he or she appears, they may even snap in
anticipation before food actually appears. A mantis, although it originally would
attack, will learn not to strike at an object after it has received an electric shock
or bad taste from this object. Upon emerging from their pupal cases, new adults of
many ant species notice and retain the scent of their colony.
In the past, it was repeatedly suggested that insects show little or no learning,
for ‘obvious’ reasons such as their small brain and short life span. However, even
little Drosophila larvae apparently can easily learn to avoid odors linked to an elec-
tric shock, and parasitoid wasps rapidly learn cues associated with host availability.
In fact, current knowledge indicates that many insects learn extensively during
all major life activities, and furthermore that individuals within a species show
genetically based variation in learning abilities.
Learning may be defined as any relatively permanent, usually adaptive change
in behavior that occurs as a result of experience or practice. This change usually
progresses gradually with continued experience to some asymptote. It often can be
modified by novel experiences, and the effects of experience eventually wane if not
reinforced. Memory—the capacity to store information—is a prerequisite, resulting
in a linkage between stimulus and response that would not have occurred without
the previous experience with that stimulus. In physiological terms, one could define
learning and memory as the acquisition and retention of neuronal representations of
new information, respectively.
One of the stranger things about studying learning is that while learning undoubt-
edly involves neuronal modifications, it still can be assessed only indirectly through
its potential effect on behavior; there is no way to directly quantify it. To infer its
presence or absence takes a series of very unusually well controlled experiments
to rule out both observer bias and the presence of feasible alternatives. Likewise,
claims for a lack of learning ability in a certain species could be due to low moti-
vation or be an artifact of the experimental set-up being used, rather than a genuine
inability to learn. Thus, one must be cautious, both in conducting experiments and
in interpreting the published literature.
Not surprisingly, something as widespread and far-reaching as learning eludes
any easy, satisfying categorization, but it can be useful to think of it as coming in
two forms, non-associative and associative. Non-associative learning includes both
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 69
Fig. 2.7 Bloodhound wasps. Small parasitic wasps such as Microplitus croceipes (above, inspect-
ing host frass) track their prey from the larva’s scent and/or odors in its frass, a mixture of feces and
undigested plant materials. This is a learned response; odors differ when the caterpillar feeds on
different host plants. As a result, by classical conditioning techniques, the wasps also can be taught
to respond to tiny amounts of novel substances that are not biologically relevant. This suggests
practical applications such as the detection of explosives, narcotics, or contraband. After even one
conditioning trial, these wasps learn to associate a novel odor with successful host-finding, feeding,
and oviposition. When exposed to the odor again, they actively antennae and energetically search
the vicinity (bottom left, a wasp responding to odor wafting through a hole). When the odor is not
present or is different, they remain relatively passive or recoil (bottom, right)
sounds from the light switches were being detected. To evaluate their unconditioned
arousal responses, the cockroaches were treated to an odor cue (a puff of air passed
over peanut butter), a light cue (a flash from the green LED), a mechanosensory cue
(a high-current air puff), or auditory cues at a frequency of 1.8 kHz; during each
trial, antennal movements were videorecorded so that antennal angles could later be
measured from the digitized images. Then cockroaches were trained to project their
right antenna toward the green light as the peanut butter air puff was presented.
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 71
The cockroaches were surprisingly bright (Fig. 2.8), and quickly learned to asso-
ciate the visual cue through classical conditioning with no other reward than an
attractive odor, which could be explained as intrinsically rewarding in itself. After
just five training trials, 90% showed significant learning, and it persisted unex-
pectedly long. Thirty minutes after training, 60% still showed antennal projection
responses to the CS, and almost half responded even after 72 hours. Optimal learn-
ing performance was achieved when the odor and light cue were simultaneous or
nearly so, which one might expect because in nature, foraging animals detect salient
cues just before or concurrent with the reward.
Fig. 2.9 Learning landmarks. After the nest entrance of Philanthus triangulum is ringed with fir
cones, the wasp learns to associate her nest with this distinctive landmark through an orientation
flight (a). While the bee wolf is away hunting, the ring of cones is displaced. Upon return the
prey-laden female flies to the center of the fir cones (b)
74 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
Fig. 2.10 Memorizing a trapline. The tropical bee Euplusia surinamensis constructs her nest (1)
130 ft above the ground under loose tree bark. Each day she flies this foraging route through the
Costa Rican forest, visiting ground plants (2, 11, 13), shrubs (6, 7, 9), understory trees (10, 12),
vines (3, 5, 8), and an epiphytic shrub (4) to collect nectar (n) and pollen (p) from newly open
flowers. It is a trapline worth remembering because although each plant produces only one to a few
new flowers each day, flowering may continue for up to six months. Marked females returned to
the plants daily even when flowers were artificially removed prior to their visit, showing that they
actually memorize the trapline
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 75
in France showed that these crickets adapt their predator-avoidance behavior after
having observed the behavior of knowledgeable others, and they maintain these
behavioral changes in a lasting way after the demonstrators are gone. Moreover,
evidence suggests that Nemobius are actually more likely to acquire social infor-
mation about the presence of predatory wolf spiders through encounters with other
crickets than to acquire personal information through direct encounters with the
spiders.
Many vertebrates have been shown to learn various tasks and concepts most
easily during a critical period in development; if learning does not occur during
that window of opportunity, it is difficult to nearly impossible. Does a similar phe-
nomenon occur with insects? A few studies suggest it is possible. For example,
Cotesia congregata are braconid wasps that hunt for hosts and mates among plant
foliage. If a young adult female wasp is exposed to wild cherry trees, an inherently
unattractive plant, during the first four hours after she ecloses, she will continue
to search among wild cherry trees rather than cabbage, a normally attractive plant.
Such a general increase in responsiveness is referred to as ‘priming.’
Certainly, for many or perhaps most insects, searching behavior relies upon spe-
cific host-derived stimuli that the adults recognize innately, often during a critical
phase such as the time around adult emergence. However, sometimes cues appear
to be learned by the immature insect, a process called ‘preimaginal conditioning’
and these are subsequently manifested in their responses as adults. Many cases
of preimaginal conditioning have been documented. Early in the twentieth cen-
tury, researchers observed that phytophagous beetles selected oviposition sites on
plant species similar to the ones on which they had been reared. This phenomenon,
sometimes called the Hopkins’ host-selection principle, has been used to explain
host preference or selection in various groups. For example, in one of the earliest
detailed studies on parasitoid learning, when the ichneumonid parasitoid Venturia
(Nemeritis) canescens was reared on wax moths, a species they do not attack under
natural conditions, as adults they preferred the odor of wax moth larvae over that of
their natural meal moth host. Likewise in a much later study, when tobacco horn-
worm caterpillars were trained to avoid the odor of ethyl acetate by pairing it with a
mild electric shock, they retained this response as adult moths.
Does actual learning persist through complete metamorphosis? This is still a
debated topic, and although some research suggests it might be so, the idea has
its skeptics. Metamorphosis is an extreme event; not too long ago, it was gener-
ally thought that a larva’s body essentially turned back into a sort of soup during
the pupal stage, only to be completely restructured into an adult insect. Yet despite
the drastic nature of the metamorphosis process, neurophysiological evidence from
Drosophila and various Lepidoptera that indicates that parts of the brain involved in
learning do remain intact. An interesting one involves a parasitoid that attacks the
codling moth, the insect responsible for the ‘worm’ that is (hopefully not) in your
apple (Fig. 2.11).
The study of Hyssopus pallidus outlined in Case Study 2.3 was one of the first to
study preimaginal learning with such careful attention to detail and control of vari-
ables. For example, a major criticism applied to other preimaginal learning studies
76 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
Fig. 2.11 Learning as a youngster. The parasitoid wasp, Hyssopus pallidus, finds the concealed
caterpillar of its codling moth (Cydia pomenella) host by chemical cues in the host’s frass. (above)
Exposure to either apple fruit extract (a) or apple fruit (b) during the parasitoid’s larval stage
resulted in significantly higher response to moth larval frass as an adult female compared to con-
trols (c). (center) Addition of apple fruit extract either to the host moth larval diet (a) or to the diet
of the developing parasitoid (b) also resulted in significantly higher adult female responses to moth
larval frass compared to controls (c) lacking apple fruit extracts. These findings suggest the adult
parasitoid ‘remembers’ its larval experiences
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 77
was the difficulty of excluding a ‘chemical legacy’ of contamination from the larval
to the adult environment, especially when learned cues are directly present in the
insect food or in the host cocoon. For this reason, these researchers did not offer
their chemical cue as a liquid or solid substrate, but as a volatile substance that was
removed long before pupation and adult emergence. To counter the possibility that
test organisms might have become contaminated by the relevant chemical cues as
they eclosed into adulthood, in each case the researchers placed their treated filter
papers 4 cm away from the host caterpillar. Because the caterpillar is paralyzed and
cannot move during parasitoid development, and the parasitoids do not leave the
host, the wasps were never in direct contact with the cue-laden filter papers during
the exposure period.
caterpillars, waited until they reached the fifth instar, then cut apart worm-
laden apples to extract them. Rearing the larvae individually in plastic boxes,
the researchers fed some with the wheat-germ based diet alone. These would
serve as their control group. For a second group of larvae, the artificial diet was
treated with a solvent extract of grated apple skin, pulp, and seeds; after the
solvent evaporated, the mixture simulated the apple components of frass. In
each of a third set of boxes, a piece of apple was suspended; in each of a fourth,
a piece of filter paper treated with their apple extract. Then they introduced a
mated female parasitoid in with each host so that it could lay eggs.
When the new generation of parasitoids emerged, they were transferred to
new cages without ever being exposed to apple cues. As a bioassay, when
the mated parasitoid females were four days old, the researchers prepared
filter paper discs by treating them with apple extract or host frass extract,
transferred these second-generation wasps into individual dishes, and placed
the open side of the dishes on top of the filter paper. For ten minutes, they
watched each wasp and recorded the amount of time it spent in ‘searching’,
which they defined as intensive antennal examination of the filter paper.
Comparing the groups, it was clear that rearing Hyssopus larvae on hosts
that were fed either apples or an artificial diet treated with apple extract had a
similar effect—the parasitoids showed a significantly greater adult response to
host frass than parasitoids reared without apple cues (see Fig. 2.11). Did a high
response to host frass require exposure to both the taste and smell of apple?
To find out, the researchers compared the responses of parasitoids reared on
caterpillars fed with apple-supplemented diet to those reared on caterpillars
exposed only to the odor of apple fruit extract. Both of these treatments sig-
nificantly enhanced female responsiveness to host frass cues, indicating that
olfaction alone was as effective as taste and smell together. This was surprising
because studies with other insects had suggested taste to be more important
than smell in the chemical learning process.
Was there a sensitive window in the parasitoid’s life history for learning the
relevant stimuli? Gandolfi, Mattiaci, and Dorne reared another set of wasps on
caterpillars fed on plain artificial diet, but this time they exposed the wasps to
apple fruit extracts at different developmental stages. For comparison, they
used both parasitoids exposed to apple extract during their entire develop-
ment and parasitoids that were never exposed at all. As an extra precaution,
a parallel series of experiments was conducted in which female wasps were
introduced into untreated vials and into vials from which extract-treated fil-
ter papers had just been removed after having been in the vial for 12 hours.
Then the searching activity of the wasps in these two groups was bioassayed.
The lack of any statistically significant difference between them satisfied the
researchers that no contamination had been left on the inner surfaces of the
vials after the treated filter papers were removed.
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 79
Fig. 2.12 Remembering through metamorphosis. Codling moth parasitoids, Hyssopus pal-
lidus, exposed to odors from apple fruit extract at different developmental stages differ in their
response as adult females to frass extracts of their apple-fed moth larvae hosts. Those exposed at
their earliest stages of development (egg to late larva-pupa) gave significantly stronger responses
as adult females to larval frass than those exposed only after pupation or as adults; the latter
responding no differently than controls with no exposure to apple fruit extracts at any time in
their lives
80 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
stage, during emergence, or at the adult stage one day before the bioassay
was ineffective, even though in these cases the interval between removal of
the bioactive cues and the bioassay was much shorter.
Some other studies had found cases in which an insect showed preimaginal
learning of some cues and not others; the presumption was that learning had
to be biologically relevant if it was to occur. Gandolfi, Mattiacci, and Dorne
wondered if this would be true for Hyssopus. Was preimaginal learning con-
fined to fruit cues, or was it a more general phenomenon? They repeated their
studies, this time exposing parasitoids from egg to larval stages to menthol, a
compound not detectable in apple extracts or codling moth caterpillars. As
a control, they used only the solvent. Parasitoids that were never exposed
to menthol during development were repelled by it as adults, and actually
responded more to the control than to the menthol. However, when parasitoids
had been exposed to its odor from egg to larval stages, as adults they showed a
significantly higher tolerance to menthol than did parasitoids without menthol
exposure, and responded to it in essentially the same way they responded to
the filter papers with the solvent control.
Thus, it appears that the parasitic wasp Hyssopus pallidus learns to locate
its concealed host by zeroing in on its frass, and this recognition is a two-
stage process. A high response requires both host-derived chemicals in the
frass that are recognized innately, and apple-derived chemicals that must be
learned through exposure early in the parasitoid’s life cycle. The preimaginal
learning is a general phenomenon in which the wasps are capable of learn-
ing a variety of cues. While chemical cues are indispensable, for this wasp
species at least, physical stimuli such as visual or tactile cues seem to be
of only minor importance. The parasitoids did not need to contact the fruit
directly; learning occurred just as well when they were reared on hosts fed
an apple-supplemented artificial diet. In contrast to general expectations, the
sensitive period for learning these olfactory cues was not during eclosion or
early adulthood, but in the earliest stages when the wasp was developing from
egg to young larva.
The codling moth parasitoids exposed to odors from apple fruit extract at differ-
ent developmental stages seem to show that some sort of learning persists through
metamorphosis. Or is this not ‘really’ learning? The phenomenon is undoubtedly
real; the definitions used to describe the effects of experience on insect responses
may be the cause of confusion. One could consider it to be a case of priming, in
which certain experiences make the parasitoids more responsive to foraging cues.
Alternatively, one might call it preference (and avoidance) learning, a case of asso-
ciative learning where the increase in responsiveness is specific for the cues that
the insects encounter during the experience. Then again, one might say it is at least
analogous to sensitization to chemical stimuli, a mechanism well known among
2.3 Life in a Stimulus-Rich World 81
adult insects in which the response to a stimulus gradually increases with repeated
exposure to that stimulus.
Such studies offer tantalizing glimpses into the complexity of neuroethology,
where questions outnumber answers. Studies that couple behavioral research with
detailed neurophysiology and genetics are still embarrassingly meager, and have
almost entirely concentrated upon a mere handful of insects from four of the 28
or so insect orders. These animals—mostly cockroaches, grain beetles, fruit flies,
blowflies, and a few of the many ants, wasps, and bees—have been chosen not
because of their representative nature but primarily for convenience, availability,
ease of rearing, and sometimes, perceived importance to society.
Consider Ammophila pubescens, a European wasp that raises its young in bur-
rows that it digs in the soil. Unlike most other digger wasps, it attends to up to 15
nests at a time, remembering the locations of each. Because the nests are begun
at different times, the young inside may range from a newly hatched larva capable
of consuming but a single caterpillar a day to an older offspring requiring three to
seven caterpillars, or a full grown larva does not need more food but should have
its burrow sealed so it can safely mature underground. The ability to do so many
complex things at once would seem to indicate a fairly high degree of intelligence,
but rationality is not this wasp’s strong suit.
Watching Ammophila closely in the early 1940s entomologist G. P. Baerends
noted that the female begins each day with inspection visits, flying from nest to
nest and inspecting the contents of each before she begins to bring prey to any
of them. Might this behavior provide a clue to her ability to multitask? Baerends
located a series of nests being attended by a single female and carefully replaced
the nests with plaster of Paris casts he could open and close. In the following days
he spent many hours substituting larvae of various sizes for each other and adding
and removing caterpillars the female had brought. Very quickly it became apparent
that the single morning inspection visit set the wasp’s behavior for the rest of the
day. Substituting a larger larva after the inspection visit made absolutely no differ-
ence, nor did it matter whether he added prey or removed some that she had already
brought. If her inspection visit indicated permanent closure, this she would do, even
if the larva had subsequently been removed so she now was sealing a completely
empty nest! Thus, provisioning behavior in Ammophila is paced to meet the needs
of each growing larva in a way that is both sophisticated and restricted. Each step
in the unfolding behavior patterns of the day is guided by the sign stimuli present in
the single brief examination of nests, and after this each appropriate motor pattern is
performed in a genetically determined sequence. In nature, in the absence of med-
dling entomologists, the information obtained through a single daily visit would be,
and clearly is, sufficient.
Insects also appear almost entirely incapable of insight, that is, reorganizing their
memories to construct a new response in the face of a novel problem. The French
naturalist Fabré (see Chapter 1) demonstrated this long ago with another hunting
wasp that briefly drops her cricket near the hole to enter her burrow before reap-
pearing to drag it inside. While she was inside the burrow, Fabré moved the cricket
a short distance away. With insight, a wasp might be expected to recognize that
her prey had merely been moved and that her burrow, having been inspected, was
ready for stocking. Instead, when the wasp reappeared, she returned the cricket to its
proper place at the edge of the hole once more, then descended again, alone. Fabré
moved the cricket; again, the wasp reappeared to reposition it and reenter her hole
alone. Fabré re-elicited this response 40 times before he lost patience!
For a long time during the twentieth century, the ‘nature–nurture’ controversy
was vigorously debated; instinct and learning were pictured as diametric oppo-
sites in command of the behavior of different kinds of animals. However, the
controversy really is a spurious one, and few behaviorists nowadays seek to deter-
mine whether a particular behavior response is learned or instinctive. Rather, it is
2.4 Hormone-Based Coordination 83
generally agreed that behavior is rarely determined either solely by the type of out-
side events impinging on the individual or by inborn heredity alone, but rather by
interaction of the two. A high reliance upon learning ability may be adaptive when
an animal is relatively long-lived and/or faced with a good deal of uncertainty or
variability regarding aspects of its environment that are biologically significant.
In contrast, the advantage of a high reliance upon innate behavior patterns lies in
their reliability. When a particular environmental cue can be linked dependably to
a biologically appropriate response, innate fixed action patterns are certain to be
successful responses. Selection also will favor innate behavior when the cost of an
initial mistake is high, such as would be true in the case of the cockroach and moth
escape reactions. Furthermore, innate behavior presumably permits economy in the
nervous system.
Reproductively active adults of the lacewing Chrysopa carnea are green, but they
turn brown when they enter diapause in the autumn; in the spring, when they become
active, they again turn green. A grasshopper typically goes through a series of five
to seven molts over several months between hatching from its egg and becoming a
sexually mature adult; if its prothoracic gland is removed when it is still an early
nymph, its development halts, and its behavior never matures. A female cockroach
produces chemical sex attractants; if the corpora allata in her brain are surgically
removed, she is incapable of attracting males and will not mate.
It is well known that insects rely heavily upon circulating chemistry. They pos-
sess a traditional endocrine system that produces blood-borne chemicals that act
on receptor-bearing target tissues elsewhere in the body. They have nerve cells that
rely upon chemical neurotransmitters to propagate their messages (and thus could
be considered a hormone that is acting locally within the synapse). And they have
functional hybrids of neurons and endocrine glands called neurosecretory cells that
cluster in their brains and throughout their central nervous systems.
Hormones, by classical definition, are substances secreted by endocrine glands
and transported by the circulatory system to other body parts, where even tiny
quantities evoke physiological responses in target tissues. A broader definition
recognizes that in addition to endocrine glands, single cells can produce hor-
mones. Various body organs, especially those associated with reproduction (such
as ovaries, testes, spermatheca, etc.), also are known to have a secondary endocrine
function.
Hormones connected with growth, development, sexual maturation and repro-
duction (see Chapter 9) have received the most study, but internal hormone
secretions have been implicated in nearly every aspect of insect life history, includ-
ing important controls on migration, orientation, and periodic behaviors as well as
activation of adult behavior.
Behaviorally, hormones often act as primers or modifiers that start the internal
motivation of an insect to later perform a particular behavioral act. Strong evidence
84 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
hormone was an inhibitor, but as soon as he realized its true effect he named it
juvenile hormone (JH) to reflect that fact. Juvenile hormone is now recognized to
be not just a single hormone, but a group of sesquiterpenes having at least six major
members. These mediate a wide variety of functions in addition to metamorphosis.
Another, ecdysone, was the first insect hormone to be structurally identified.
As new techniques have allowed others of its type to be isolated, ecdysteroid has
become the preferred name for the group. Ecdysteroids’ primary claim to fame is
that they directly affect gene expression and thus trigger the separation of the epider-
mis from the overlying cuticle, which begins the molt. Like JH, they now have been
shown to have wide-ranging effects at every developmental stage but particularly in
embryogenesis and other reproductive and developmental tasks. The primary site of
ecdysteroid synthesis is the prothoracic gland, but despite its nervous connections,
the primary mode of gland activation is hormonal. The classic understanding is that
when a winged (pterygote) insect becomes a full adult, it no longer molts, so this
gland is no longer needed. Exposure to ecdysone in the absence of JH then leads to
apoptosis (programmed cell death) that causes the gland to degenerate. Apterygote
insects, on the other hand, continue to molt as adults, and they retain their active
prothoracic glands. Recent discoveries add complexity to this picture but do not
change its general outlines.
A third important hormone goes by the acronym PTTH (short for a rather
unwieldy term, prothoracicotropic hormone). It actually was the first insect hor-
mone to be discovered, but the last major hormone to be structurally identified. In
1917 Stefan Kopec reported that the brain of gypsy moth caterpillars was neces-
sary for successful pupation, based on his well designed experiments in which he
surgically removed brains from some larvae while controls had sham surgery (an
incision made, but the brain left in place). He also tied silk string tightly around var-
ious parts of gypsy moth larvae, then watched their subsequent development. When
a caterpillar was tied around its midsection early during its last larval instar, only
the head half later pupated; when tied late during that instar, both halves pupated.
Based on this, Kopec proposed the concept of a critical period, a time period when
the brain was necessary for its hormonal influence to be exerted. Removing the brain
before the critical period prevented development but afterwards, it had no effect. He
postulated a brain hormone was responsible.
Unfortunately, Kopec was ahead of his time, and his work was poorly accepted.
The prevailing wisdom was that insects had no hormones. Furthermore, everyone
‘knew’ the nervous system and the endocrine system were functionally distinct;
brains and nervous tissue certainly did not produce hormones. Neurons secreting
chemicals at their synapses had not even been envisioned. When later work by others
showed that the brain produced not just one but many different hormones, even his
terminology fell out of favor.
Today, however, Kopec would be vindicated. His ‘brain hormone’ has been
demonstrated to be PTTH, and it shows a satisfying complexity. The PTTHs from
only a few insects have been identified, but they appear to fall into two size groups.
The ‘small’ PTTHs were renamed bombyxin; their exact role is unknown, but they
appear insulin-like and may be involved in ovarian development and the utilization
86 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
of carbohydrate during egg maturation. The ‘big’ PTTHs show the activity for
which they were named, acting on the prothoracic gland to regulate the synthesis
of ecdysteroids. When an insect receives the proper stimuli from the environment
(such as photoperiod or temperature) or from its nerves (such as stretch receptors
firing after an assassin bug’s blood meal), it releases PTTH. This in turn activates
the prothoracic glands to produce ecdysteroid, triggering the molt. Interestingly, for
many insects that undergo pupal diapause, PTTH release is regulated by photoperiod
during a circadian window.
Ecdysteroids, juvenile hormones, and PTTH are still recognized as the three
major hormone groups regulating insect development and reproduction, but other
important hormones that play more specialized roles are continually being dis-
covered. Though their functions have yet to be fully elucidated, many hormones
previously isolated from vertebrates have been found. Insects produce insulin and
melatonin. They have counterparts to vertebrate adrenaline and noradrenaline. Their
reproductive hormones are close enough that female rabbit fleas depend on the hor-
mones circulating in the blood of the pregnant rabbit host in order to reproduce.
Considering that a hundred years ago, insects were thought to lack both hormones
and brains, it is somewhat humbling to learn that they have counterparts to so many
vertebrate systems—as it is to recognize that, because they are the more ancient
group, it would be more correct to salute the insect-type life processes that are found
in us.
code for a protein (PER) and an enzyme that degrades PER. Production of PER
varies over a 24-hour schedule, gradually building up inside and outside the cell
nucleus. When PER is in peak abundance in the cell, tau turns on the enzyme’s pro-
duction, slowing PER’s rate of accumulation. At the same time, extra PER bonds
with a protein coded by a third gene, tim. In this bonded form, PER is carried back to
the nucleus, where it blocks the activity of per, the gene that produced it. However,
this blockage is only temporary, and soon a new cycle begins. Surprisingly, these key
genes of vertebrates have also been found in both Drosophila and honey bees, where
they play an equally critical role in enabling circadian rhythms. Thus it appears
likely that they arose early in evolutionary development, perhaps from an ancient
animal that lived about 550 million years ago.
Reiterative rhythms occur with a regular repeated periodicity in the life of a sin-
gle individual. Some reiterative cycles cover a relatively long period, such as the
lunar periodicity of nesting behavior in the nocturnal sweat bee, Sphecodogastra tex-
ana (Fig. 2.13). In many temperate insects, seasonal rhythms are based on changes
in photoperiod, involving an assessment of the duration and accumulation of daily
changes in day or night length over a period of time. At the other extreme, reiterative
cycles of feeding and locomotion may be very short. Typically, however, reiterative
Fig. 2.13 Responding to moonlight. The sweat bee Sphecodogastra texana shows lunar peri-
odicity. In good weather adult female activity always begins about sunset. Concentrated pollen
collection and nest cell construction occur only during that part of the lunar cycle where moonlight
is continuous with the twilight, thus permitting extended nocturnal foraging. When the moon rises
after the close of twilight (ca. 9:30 pm), the bees close their nests at the end of twilight and make
very few pollen collections from their host plant (the evening primrose, Oenothera rhombipetala)
even when pollen is abundant. Brood development in excavated nests correlates with the observed
foraging activity
88 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
behavioral rhythms have a periodicity of about 24 hours and thus are called cir-
cadian rhythms, derived from the Latin circa, about, and diem, day. For example,
anyone who has cohabited with cockroaches has noticed their characteristic pattern
of activity that begins shortly after dusk. The timing of moths’ sex odor release is
also on a circadian cycle.
Circadian rhythms all share some important characteristics. First, although these
cycles average about 24 hours, they are not exactly so, and will drift out of phase or
free run for some time unless they are entrained (kept in line, reset) by certain exter-
nal environmental cues. Crickets are a well-studied example. Each evening under
natural conditions, males begin singing at about the same time relative to nightfall.
In a room with constant bright light, although they still sing, each day their call-
ing starts about 25–26 hours later than it did on the previous day. The very fact
that the length of a free-running cycle is not exactly matched to the earth’s many
24-hour environmental cycles provides strong evidence that internal components
drive a given rhythm. However, it does not prove it, because one cannot always
exclude other environmental cues with certainty.
The cyclical environmental cue that entrains a circadian rhythm is called a
Zeitgeber. Most circadian rhythms studied to date have relied upon light/dark
transitions as their Zeitgeber, but other cues may also prove important. Some insects
held in continuous darkness, for example, distinguish between long and short days
in a temperature cycle. However, in general, circadian rhythms are temperature
compensated. Unlike most other physiological processes, within normal biologi-
cal limits the periodicity remains stable under changing temperature conditions.
Consider the midge, Clunio marinus, that spends its larval and pupal life among
red algae between the tidemarks of certain European beaches. Adults emerge only
at low tide; they live for just 2 hours and must mate and oviposit before the tide
advances back over their breeding ground. A number of genetically distinct geo-
graphical races exist, each with its own emergence rhythm precisely adapted to its
local tidal conditions. In southern races the daily periodicity is circadian and free
runs in constant light. In the Arctic, however, emergence is strictly tidally con-
trolled and stops in constant light or dark; it is as though an internal alarm clock
were measuring 10 hours from the time of first exposure to the previous ebb tide.
Subsequent research has shown that the receptors for circadian rhythms and for
seasonal activity are located in the brain of some insects, in the compound eyes
of others, and in other cases, in both. Cryptochrome, a light-sensitive pigment that
contains the vitamin riboflavin and a protein, is a major player in sensing circadian
light information. Interestingly, in Drosophila, cryptochrome in the brain regulates
the morning activity rhythm and cryptochrome in the compound eyes controls the
evening activity peak.
Many behavioral events, such as the periodical cicada’s emergence (see Plate 44) or
the mass emergence of mayflies from a pond, look like a rhythm when one views the
whole synchronous population, but occur only once in any individual’s life. These
2.4 Hormone-Based Coordination 89
one-time events are called gated rhythms, because at the appropriate stage of the
life cycle it is almost as though some mechanism were opening a behavioral ‘gate’
at the appointed time. All those individuals streaming through are allowed to begin
a given behavior, but an individual that misses a given gate opening time must wait
for the next one.
Ideally, one attempting to investigate gated rhythms should seek an organism
with a relatively simple nervous system, choose neurons important to a clear-cut
behavioral alteration, and then follow the changes occurring in those neurons during
the course of hormone action. In the 1970s, two researchers found their model sys-
tem in giant silk moths (family Saturniidae), not to be confused with the silkworm
moth, Bombyx mori.
An adult silk moth acts very different than the pupa it previously was. How do
hormones and the nervous system interact to produce this change?
In moths, as in most insects, emergence of an adult from its pupal skin
occurs only during a specific period of the day; individuals that do not emerge
during that particular time cannot eclose until the proper time on the following
day. However, simple escape from the pupal skin does not herald the arrival
of a fully functional adult. A newly eclosed individual displays little in the
way of adult behavior and will make only immature rotary movements of
its abdomen. The problem is not lack of appropriate neural machinery; it
possesses a fully developed adult nervous system. Nor is the lack of adult
behavior merely due to the insect’s restraint—peeling the pupal cuticle away
from the mature moth within still does not mature the behavioral repertoire
before its normal emergence gate. Complex motor patterns such as flight or
walking, on the rare occasions when they can be elicited at all, are uncoordi-
nated and abortive. Even simple reflexes, such as the ‘righting reflex’ when
overturned, are missing.
At the arrival of the eclosion gate, however, the behavior of a ‘prematurely
peeled’ moth changes strikingly. In perfect pantomime, the moth sheds its
phantom pupal skin and escapes from its nonexistent cocoon. At the end of
the performance, it inflates its wings and assumes the full repertoire of adult
behavior. This behavior suggested that adult actions must be controlled in a
central manner that was closely linked to the timing of eclosion.
Hoping to locate the site of the eclosion clock, James Truman and Lynn
Riddiford performed a series of surgical ablation studies on various parts of
the pupal nervous system. When the brain itself was removed shortly after the
onset of adult development, development proceeded normally and the result-
ing moths went on to shed their pupal skins. However, their emergence was
quite abnormal; some usual behaviors were entirely omitted and others were
out of sequence. Even more striking, the eclosion was no longer gated. Moths
90 2 Programming and Integrating Behavior
emerged at all sorts of odd hours randomly distributed throughout the day and
night.
Was all this behavioral confusion simply due to removal of important neu-
ral centers? The researchers tackled this suggestion by implanting loose brains
into the abdomens of debrained pupae. In these ‘loose brain’ moths, neural
connections between the brain and nervous system were never established. Yet
the resultant moths emerged at the proper gating time and displayed proper
emergence behaviors! Clearly, the appropriate messages were being carried
through chemical channels rather than along nerve fibers. An eclosion hor-
mone appeared likely, and the case was strengthened when injecting brain
homogenates into moths prior to their normal eclosion time resulted in moths
emerging early.
But what was the actual role of the brain? For example, did the brain
include the photoreceptor, the clock measuring the time after lights-on
or lights-off, or both? Knowing that different species of giant silk moths
have quite different eclosion gating times, the researchers performed brain
transplants between species. The results (Fig. 2.14) clearly confirmed their
suspicions. The moth brain contained the gating clock. By interchanging the
brain, one could interchange the time of emergence.
Fig. 2.14 Interchanging brains. Hyalophora cecropia silk moths eclose in the morning, whereas
Antheraea pernyi ecloses just before dark. When their brains are removed, eclosion rhythmicity
is abolished in both species. When their brains are reimplanted in their abdomens, rhythmicity
returns even though there are no nervous connections between brain and CNS. If the brains are
removed and implanted into individuals of the opposite species, the restored rhythmicity is that of
the donated brain rather than the recipient body. The hormonal action is not species-specific, only
the timing of its release
2.4 Hormone-Based Coordination 91
The research outlined in Case Study 2.4 concentrated on one critical point of
development. However, molts between larval instars, from larva to pupa, and from
pupa to adult differ in the degree of internal morphological change that occurs. The
system usually runs smoothly, but errors do occur. As noted for the assassin bug
Rhodnius given supplemental juvenile hormone (Section 2.4), insects sometimes
molt more times than usual, becoming ‘supernumerary larvae’ rather than changing
into adults as expected.
How is a proper degree of change regulated? The brain is in charge, as Truman
and Riddiford demonstrated. Upon receiving proper stimuli, it secretes PTTH that in
turn stimulates the prothoracic glands to synthesize and secrete ecdysteroids. These
combine with a receptor protein in the cell nucleus, bind with DNA, and induce tran-
scription of a few master genes. These transcripts, in turn, start a flurrying cascade
of gene activity that ultimately results in everything from changes in morphology
and physiology of internal organs to secretion of a new cuticle. However, it is
juvenile hormone—through its timing and quantity at target cells—that modulates
ecdysteroid-induced gene switching so that the proper amount of change occurs.
More exactly how it does so at the gene level is a matter to be elucidated by current
and future research.
Chapter 3
Spatial Adjustment
3.1 Introduction
The burrows of most wood-boring cerambycid beetle larvae are very irregularly ori-
ented; why? At temperatures so low that most other insects are inactive, bumblebees
still fly about; how? Migrating locusts appear to swarm single-mindedly toward a
fixed goal. Do they?
Though the scale of movement varies widely, the ability to change position
within the environment is essential to the survival of nearly every animal, including
most insects. Escaping predators, gathering food, locating a mate, adjusting to envi-
ronmental variables such as temperature and humidity—these and other important
behaviors all depend upon an insect’s ability to adjust its spatial relations.
One of the most generally known facts about insects is that they possess three
pairs of legs. This is, in fact, the fundamental ground plan of insects, and one that is
amply represented in the fossil record. It is derived from an ancestral arrangement
in which serially uniform legs occurred on the majority of body segments. Over
time, some legs became modified into various appendages such as mouthparts, tho-
racic legs, genitalia and cerci, while others on the abdominal segments typically
were lost. With the passage of further evolutionary time, insect thoracic legs have
developed an enormous diversity of structure and function (Fig. 3.1). In addition
to differences between taxa, variation can be found within an individual, between
larvae and adults, and between males and females.
The acquisition of wings was a second major development, the importance of
which can hardly be overstated. Insects were not only the first organisms to develop
the capacity for powered flight, they remain the only group of invertebrates to pos-
sess this ability. This development opened the third dimension to insects, setting the
stage for improvements in such diverse but crucial behaviors as dispersal, escape,
thermoregulation, feeding, and mate location. It probably also led to an expansion
in neural capabilities. It has been noted that some of the most ‘intelligent’ insects
(i.e. those that are most capable of learning), as well as those with the most acute
vision and olfaction, are found among actively flying predators and pollinators.
Together, diversification in the structure of legs and wings undoubtedly has been
a key factor in the overwhelming success of insects worldwide. Through variations
Fig. 3.1 Showing a bit of leg. Diversity of insect legs and some of the purposes of their modifica-
tions. (A) Drosophila cracens foreleg—courtship, (B) thrips foreleg—crawling, (C) grasshopper
hind leg—jumping, (D) Corixa water boatman hind leg—swimming, (E) Cicindela tiger bee-
tle hind leg—running, (F) cicada nymph fore leg—digging, (G) Rhagovelia obesa water strider
middle tarsus—walking on water, (H) Ranatra fusca water scorpion fore leg—prey capture, (I)
caterpillar thoracic leg—crawling, (J) honey bee hind leg—pollen transport
on their basic body plan, insects have surmounted the barriers to inhabit nearly every
terrain but deep saltwater. Moreover they have done this so elegantly that they have
attracted the attention of engineers who study insect locomotion as inspiration for
moving robotic devices (Fig. 3.2).
3.2 Locomotion
A tiny flea’s jump may be 13 inches long. A blood-sucking bug, Rhodnius, may
move about with a meal 10–12 times its own body weight, corresponding to a human
drinking 200 gallons and subsequently weighing nearly a ton. Click beetles can
suddenly flip into the air to a height of four times their body length. Cockroaches
have been clocked at speeds of nearly 3 miles per hour—a remarkably high speed
in relation to their body size.
3.2 Locomotion 95
Fig. 3.2 Creating a useful mimic. Hexapodal robots have potential for performing many tasks
such as exploring terrain of distant planets. Around the world, robots such as this one are being
developed that mimic the analogous working morphology of common insects. The wireless camera
mounted atop the robot’s rear monitors the environment and gives the robot the appearance of
having a stinger like a scorpion
would cover 800 ft. However, we must realize that a flea the size of a man would
have relatively much more mass per unit cross-sectional area of muscle than does a
normal-sized flea.
In groups such as Orthoptera and fleas, jumping has become a pronounced spe-
cialization. Efficient long-distance jumping presents a special challenge—in most
cases it requires a powerful and rapidly accelerating movement of the jumping legs,
and this in turn requires morphological modifications. Even the large femur extensor
muscles of grasshoppers’ jumping legs cannot generate the quick extension needed
for an efficient jump without some mechanical modification within the leg struc-
ture for storing energy. Locusts meet this challenge with a tendon of the tibial flexor
muscle that moves over a stop; the stop allows the extensor to contract without mov-
ing the leg when the muscles are coactivated. This stores energy in the mechanical
distortion of the femur, tibia, and extensor tendon in a manner analogous to the bow
and arrow of an archer.
Among fleas, a rubbery protein called resilin in the cuticle stores and subse-
quently releases energy for the jump (see Plate 2). The material displays a 97%
recovery after stress is applied, exceeding that of elastin, the human elastic protein.
In 2005, an Australian research team produced resilin protein in purified form by
cloning a portion of the ‘resilin gene’ in Drosophila, with the hopes developing
ways to use the material for human spinal disc implants.
As far as is known, all biological pre-launch amplifiers depend on the same
mechanism—energy storage in deformed elastic materials. One of the most spec-
tacular jumping strategies does not even involve legs. Click beetles shoot upward
as much as four times their body length by rapidly accelerating the joint between
two thoracic segments; here again a mechanical stop prevents movement until large
isometric force has been achieved.
Aquatic insects have evolved two general sorts of locomotory adaptations—
those enabling them to propel themselves upon or up to the top of the water and
those by which they ‘swim’ beneath the water surface. Diving beetles in the family
Dytiscidae are Olympic-quality insect swimmers; their body shape is so similar to
a small-span wing profile that it is believed to create dynamic lift during fast swim-
ming. Many bottom-dwelling insects such as larval Odonata and Trichoptera walk
over the substrate just as terrestrial insects do (even though the larval case of some
caddisflies can be quite a hindrance to movement).
Insects that live in lakes and other slow-moving waters generally swim well. The
trunks of their bodies are streamlined and well adapted to flow. They generate thrust
by synchronous power strokes, and often have adaptations such as flattened rear
legs. Most free-swimming insects paddle with their hind legs, sometimes together
with the middle legs. Efficiency is often increased by devices such as hairs or cutic-
ular blades and/or modification in the morphology and relative size of the legs. In
contrast to the general rule in terrestrial locomotion, in swimming the two legs of a
segment sometimes work together like oars.
Surface dwellers take great advantage of the relationship between their body size
and the physical properties of water at temperatures and pressures characteristic of
their environment. Specifically, under these conditions water tends to have a rela-
tively high surface tension, so that the water-repellent surface of the insect cuticle is
sufficient to support many small surface dwellers as though upon a thin elastic mem-
brane. Many insects also secrete additional waxy material upon their tarsi, allowing
98 3 Spatial Adjustment
them to walk or row across the water film without breaking its surface. Some, such
as water striders in the family Gerridae, have hydrophobic tarsal hairs and special-
ized claws on their long legs that allow them to skate on the water’s surface. One
of the more spectacular surface dweller adaptations occurs in Stenus, a genus of
staphylinid beetles that live on grasses along mountain streams. If they accidentally
tumble into the water, as they often do, Stenus can walk upon the water’s surface,
but only slowly. In response to apparent danger, however, they release an anal gland
secretion that lowers the surface tension of the water behind them. Drawn forward
by the higher surface tension in front, the beetles propel themselves along at speeds
of 45–70 cm/s, moving their abdomens from side to side to direct their movements.
Insects with gills or other aquatic respiratory adaptations can live permanently
submerged. Locomotion methods among insects that live beneath the water sur-
face vary greatly. Most live predominantly at or in the surface layer of the stream
bottom, but an unusual behavior occurs among a few species in which normally
terrestrial adults dive to reach submerged areas to oviposit. Female black flies dive
through shallow moving water to reach the surface of rocks where they affix their
eggs. Similarly, some female caddisflies dive vertically and swim to oviposition sites
below inclined submerged stones. Although adult stream insects rarely swim, larvae
of many insect groups can swim by body undulations. Some mayfly larvae escape
from predators or aggressive conspecifics with such strong, active swimming that
they can travel against a current. Dragonfly larvae force jets of water rapidly out of
the rectal chamber so that the body is driven forward. Still other species use claws,
silk, suckers and other devices to help themselves maneuver.
The drift of insects downstream with the current, a behavior that typically occurs
at night, is perhaps one of the most frequently studied topics in stream ecology,
but because of the diversity of stream insects and the diversity of running water
conditions, researchers have found it difficult to identify clear patterns, much less
to develop predictive models. It is clear, however, that drift is more than a passive
activity. When a habitat patch is overcrowded and resources are low, as much as
10–30% of the insect population of a stream may drift in a single night, traveling
between 2 and 20 m during one drift movement. Black fly larvae drift by first fixing
a silk thread to a rock on the stream bottom, then prolonging the thread by spin-
ning and in essence rappelling themselves several centimeters downstream before
resettling at the bottom.
Insect wings appear to have arisen upon rather large active insects sometime prior
to the Late Carboniferous Epoch. They were not modified limbs, but two or three
pairs of sideways expansions of the upper part of the thorax, and presumably, at
first these expansions only allowed an insect to glide. Flapping and steering would
come later.
On one point, scientists agree: Despite a stunning amount of structural diversity
in insect wings today, they evolved only once. Throughout the fossil story, wing
venation has remained relatively consistent, and the changes that have occurred can
be homologized across insect orders, as can many other important morphological
aspects. Beyond this, the simple question, ‘What is the origin of insect wings?’ has
been the subject of competing theories for over one hundred years. For one thing,
the question is actually two queries. One centers on homology, asking what mor-
phological elements gave rise to wings. The other concerns behavior and evolution,
asking what purposes early wing-like structures served and what conditions favored
their origin.
Currently, the best accepted theories are that insect wings may have arisen either
from pronotal lobes on the thorax or from modified gills. As a result, unlike the
wings of birds or bats, the wings of insects contain no intrinsic muscles. Instead,
they attach to the thorax by a complicated hinge structure that amplifies the tiny
strains of the flight musculature into the large sweeping motions of the wing. To
transmit force to the wings, the flight muscles are attached to the thorax by two
different systems. In one system, direct flight muscles connect directly to the wing
sclerites. In the other, the flight muscles insert within the thorax at some distance
from the wing base, and deform the overall shape of the entire thorax so that parts
push on the wing base and move it up and down (Fig. 3.3).
Odonata still possess only direct flight muscles, but most other insects possess
some combination of direct and indirect muscles so that while the muscles altering
wing inclination remain attached to the wings themselves, the muscles responsi-
ble for wing flapping are attached to the thoracic walls. Most present-day insects
also have developed a musculature that allows the wings to fold backward over
the abdomen. Thus while the wings are flapping, wing inclination is synchronously
changing, so that the overall wing flight pattern becomes much like that of a pair of
small propellers directing an air stream downward and backward. In typical forward
flight, each wing traces a pattern that resembles the numeral 8 relative to the body
at its base, and many insects can hover or loop by changing the inclinations of this
‘figure 8’ relative to their body.
By necessity, as insects became smaller their wing movement rate increased.
While a house fly may have a rate of about 200 beats per second, mosquitoes
have a rate of up to 600 beats. Tiny ceratopogonid midges have been clocked at
a wing vibration speed of over 1,000 beats per second. How can this be possible?
No known animal nerves are physically capable of transmitting stimuli fast enough
to cause contraction and relaxation at these high speeds. The elastic nature of the
insect thorax and the action of resonating flight muscles hold the key to this para-
dox. In many insects, especially certain Diptera and Coleoptera, the wings have
two stable positions—completely elevated and completely depressed. As the wings
move downward, normal thoracic elasticity resists this motion until a certain point
100 3 Spatial Adjustment
Fig. 3.3 Flapping their wings. Rather than flapping up and down, most insects wings move in
a back and forth motion. This is a result of the arrangement of two groups of large antagonistic
flight muscles that attach to the inner thorax walls rather than directly to the wings (thus they
are called indirect flight muscles). One group (DLM = dorsal-longitudinal muscles) is inserted
at right angles to the other group (DVM = dorsal-ventral muscles). Alternating self-sustaining
contractions of these two muscle groups changes the shape of the thorax driving the wings forward
and backward. Tiny steering muscles attached to wing base apodemes alter the wing attack angle,
with the result that with each wing stroke the wings move propeller-like around their base before
flipping over and sweeping back in the opposite direction, following a figure-8 pattern
is reached. At this ‘click point,’ three things happen simultaneously. First, the resis-
tance vanishes and the wings click into a new position below the thorax. Second,
their inclination automatically changes in readiness for the upstroke. Finally, the
muscles that have been contracting are suddenly released. As they relax, the oppos-
ing muscles are suddenly stretched, which causes them to contract instantly. In this
remarkable oscillating process, these insects have developed a system that does not
require the synchronous nervous control for every contraction that is characteristic
of dragonflies, locusts, and butterflies. Once initiated, this ‘improved model’ can be
operated at almost any speed, depending on thoracic elasticity, and can be modified
by secondary controls as circumstance dictates.
3.3 Orientation
Having briefly viewed how insects move, let us turn our attention to why and where
they move, first as individuals and then as populations. The subject of spatial adjust-
ment is a critical one touching many facets of the life of an organism. A major part
of an insect’s behavior is in fact orientation to factors such as food, mate, prey, host,
3.3 Orientation 101
etc. Thus, it is unsurprising to find that the study of orientation and navigation is a
dynamic part of modern biology, with a rapidly growing literature. We can only be
concerned here with some of its more general tenets.
Orientation is the self-controlled maintenance or change of an organism’s body
position in relation to external cues. It occurs when certain stimuli in the envi-
ronment elicit a responsive sequence of behaviors that results in a non-random
pattern of locomotion, direction of body axis, or both. The fact that orientation is
self-controlled in this way distinguishes it from passive transport. That it includes
position maintenance means that orientation also may be taken to include postural
adjustments such as response to gravity.
When too far from its host plant to receive directional cues, the red cotton bug
Dysdercus congregates in humid areas; it does not directionally follow a humid-
ity gradient, but simply moves about randomly more actively when in drier areas
and more slowly in more humid areas. To escape predators, a male grayling but-
terfly will fly upward toward the sun; if blinded in one eye, he will ‘escape’ in
circles. Caterpillars move down the stems of their food plant when they are about to
pupate in the ground. Sexually mature female crickets turn to face and approach the
recorded song of a male cricket.
In the early years of the twentieth century, Jacques Loeb theorized that orienta-
tion occurred because of asymmetrical stimulation of an animal’s sensory organs.
For any bilaterally symmetrical animal, Loeb argued, a stimulus that registered
unequally on the animal’s two sides would simply cause the animal to turn until the
stimulus was equalized. Loeb called such a directed movement a tropism. However,
since then, the term tropism has come to refer primarily to movement in plants, and
the term taxis (plural, taxes) is used when referring to this behavior in animals. Some
also broaden the definition of taxis to describe any oriented heading of an animal,
whether moving or stationary.
While Loeb’s explanation was useful in stimulating research on orientation,
investigators soon found many examples like those above that did not fit Loeb’s
simple scheme. In 1940, Gottfried Fraenkel and Donald L. Gunn proposed a more
comprehensive classification of orientation behavior according to the reaction mech-
anisms involved. A response like that of the red cotton bug would be called a kinesis
(plural, kineses). This is perhaps the simplest type of locomotory response that an
animal can make to a stimulus—moving in a way that is related only to the intensity
of that stimulus while disregarding any spatial properties that the stimulus might
possess.
A response like that of the grayling butterfly continued to be called a taxis, now
defined as a directed reaction in which the organism’s long body axis is aligned with
the stimulus and movement is more or less directed toward or away from the stim-
ulus. Movements such as these would seem to be among the most straightforward
types of orientation to study because the insect’s track appears obviously related in
102 3 Spatial Adjustment
Fig. 3.4 Learning to adjust. When a honey bee’s left eye is blackened, initially it circles toward
the intact eye, but after 20+ repeated trials the bee learns to compensate and again moves directly
toward the light. Only a sample of trials is shown; the light was directly above the starting point in
each case
3.3 Orientation 103
At first, Loeb’s theory had such appeal that taxes were considered to be forced
movements over which the animal had little or no control. But gradually the accu-
mulating data began to give biologists a new appreciation of the complexities of
animal orientation, and Loeb’s simple theory was discredited on several counts.
For one, taxes are obviously as variable as the rest of behavior. A given taxis may
depend on environment, context, experience, and/or the organism’s internal state
(nutritional, sexual, developmental, etc.). For example, although when preparing to
pupate some caterpillars move downward, their first responses as emerging moths
may be to climb upward as high as possible. In addition, a tactic response may
change in type or sign at short notice. Thus, the blinded circling grayling butterfly
will immediately follow in a straight line should a female grayling pass by.
Tactic responses are often far less simple than they appear, and tactic interactions
are common. On the one side, taxes grade into kineses, from which they differ in
being directed responses of the insect relative to the stimulus source. In another
direction, they overlap with such longer-range phenomena as migration, discussed
later in this chapter; at times the two are difficult to distinguish. Taxes also grade into
still another quite sophisticated set of responses—the whole subject of positional
orientations in general and transverse orientations in particular—that may or may
not be called a subcategory of them.
Not surprisingly, other systems for classifying orientation behaviors have arisen.
One considers that orientation basically involves the positioning of an organism
in response to various stimulus fields such as heat, magnetism, light, gravity,
pressure, and chemicals. Thus, one may consider chemical orientation, gravity
orientation, astronomical orientation, orientation to polarized light, or any of a host
of other orientation subdivisions. (For discussion of a variety of sense organs used
in perception of chemical, visual, and mechanical stimuli, refer to Chapters 6, 7,
and 8, respectively.) Another system is based on the observation that orientation
occupies an interface between behavior and ecology. For any organism, the envi-
ronment contains both positive and negative factors—not only resources needed
for sustaining life or their absence but stress sources such as intense sunlight,
which can be rapidly debilitating in the absence of compensating behaviors. A
maximally fit organism is expected to behave in a manner that consistently works
to minimize its body distance from resources (food, shelter, etc.) and maximize
its distance from sources of stress. Viewing orientation from the perspective of its
adaptive significance has led to a more complete classification of orientation than
past attempts. However, for most scientists, taxes and kineses remain a major con-
ceptual scheme for understanding and investigating the actions of insects and other
organisms.
Locusts in flight maintain an even body keel partly through visual reactions to incli-
nation of the horizon. Flies mechanically sense angular acceleration and angular
motion by rapid oscillations of their gyroscope-like halteres (modified knoblike
104 3 Spatial Adjustment
Fig. 3.5 Heeding unnatural cues. The larva of the diving beetle Acilius normally swims to the
surface for air, cued by its dorsal light reaction; placed in an aquarium lit from below, it will
descend with its back down, attempting to get air at the bottom as though it were the surface.
Arrows indicate direction of swimming movement
3.3 Orientation 105
in ants, gravity receptors are located at points of body articulation (neck, antennal
joints, thorax and petiole, petiole and gaster, and joints between thorax and coxae);
stimulation of any one point alone is enough for gravity orientation, although the
different joints are not equally reliable.
Visual input sometimes serves as an effective substitute for gravitational forces.
In fact, phototaxis and geotaxis, two of the best-studied taxes, have several aspects
in common. For most insects the sun is upward, and positive phototaxes and neg-
ative geotaxes are the norm. One of the most remarkable features of honey bee
communication is the two-way transfer from the angle between sun and food source
to the angle between vertical and direction of the straight part of the waggle dance,
then back again (see Chapter 8). For the dancing honey bee, positive phototaxis is
coupled with negative geotaxis; if the food is in the same direction as the sun, the
straight run of the waggle dance is directed upward, opposite the direction of grav-
ity. When information about gravity is experimentally altered, the dance performed
by the bee reflects the changed input (Fig. 3.6).
Probably the single most striking aspect of postural control among insects is
the manner in which it depends upon input from a great number of sources acting
in concert to the point of redundancy. For example, an ant can correctly orient to
gravity using any one of its five proprioceptive joint systems alone if the others are
fixed in position with wax.
When an ant on its way back to the nest is placed in a dark box for a period of
time, how will it orient when released? Lasius niger proceeds in the same course
as before, relative to the sun, but because the sun has shifted position in the sky
during the ant’s incarceration, the released ant’s orientation is incorrect in terms
of its nest.
An orientation like this one—locomotion at a fixed angle relative to light rays—
is termed a light-compass reaction. It has been demonstrated in a wide variety
of insects, including caterpillars, bees, and certain beetles and bugs. Light reac-
tions also have been well studied in the context of navigation during migration
(see Section 3.5.3). The polarization of light rays often serves as an orienting
cue. In one of the more unusual examples, certain African dung beetles apparently
use polarized moonlight to whisk balls of dung in a straight line away from the
dung pile.
Have you ever noticed that various crawling and flying insects nearly always
travel in a straight line across roads at right angles to their direction? This appears to
occur irrespective of compass directions or other external stimuli. One hypothesized
explanation is that the insects orient by balancing their reception of a symmetrical
source of shortwave (infrared) radiant energy. Although roads are relative newcom-
ers to the environment of insects, the adaptive significance of such behavior may
be a survival advantage conferred in crossing large bare spots of earth or bodies of
water in the shortest possible time with minimal energy expenditure and exposure
to the elements or predators.
106 3 Spatial Adjustment
Fig. 3.6 Turning gravity on its head. Proper orientation during the honey bee waggle dance
depends on gravity perception. Because of the way it is connected at the neck, the lower half of
a honey bee’s head weighs almost twice as much as the upper half. (A) When an unaltered bee is
dancing on the vertical surface of a comb inside the hive, gravity causes this lower portion to swing
downward, tilting against the sensitive proprioceptive hairs on the neck, and stimulating nerves at
their base. Severing these nerves causes total disorientation. (B) A tiny weight glued to the top of
the bee’s head alters its balance. (C) The proprioceptive information received by the bee’s brain is
reversed, resulting in a dance that is the opposite of normal
3.3 Orientation 107
Insect reactions to infrared radiation are rarely reported, but they have been
demonstrated across diverse taxonomic groups, and with further study they may
prove to be more common than expected. Certain wasps that parasitize beetle larvae
found in dead timber can detect their hosts by means of infrared receptors on their
antennae. The beetle Melanophila acuminata, which lays its eggs in freshly killed
conifer trees, uses a pair of specialized infrared sensory pits to sense forest fires,
even over long distances (Fig. 3.7). Thermoreceptors located in the wings and anten-
nae of species of darkly pigmented butterflies have been shown protect them from
heat damage while sun basking. Research has shown that some blood-sucking bugs
are able to perceive the radiant heat emitted by their warm-blooded prey, even at a
distance, and others have the ability to locate blood vessels under their host’s skin by
sensing temperature gradients. Likewise, Atta leaf-cutter ants can learn the location
Fig. 3.7 Finding fires. The jewel beetle, Melanophila acuminata, possesses a pair of infrared
detector organs in pits next to the junction of its middle legs and body. Each organ contains a cluster
of about 70 individual dome-shaped sensilla (below, highly magnified), which are exposed during
flight. Electrophysiological recordings reveal that these receptors respond to infrared radiation,
characteristic of a forest fire, enabling the beetle to find smoldering wood in which to lay its eggs.
Biomimetic engineers have developed an infrared sensor based on this organ. The beetle is about
10 mm long
108 3 Spatial Adjustment
Fig. 3.8 Orienting magnetically. Mounds of the magnetic termite, Amitermes meridionalis, in a
seasonally flooded field in northern Australia. The long axes of all these nests run north–south
3.3 Orientation 109
to that of the combs in the original parent colony. Since all directional landmarks
had been removed in the round cylinders, the researchers hypothesized that the bees
were somehow able to orient to the earth’s magnetic field. To test this hypothesis,
they placed a powerful magnet outside the experimental nest cylinder so that the
natural magnetic field was deflected. Invariably, the same bees that had previously
reconstructed faithful new combs in experimental cylinders now built combs that
differed from those in the previous nest by 40◦ —the exact angle of the artificial
magnetic deflection.
The magnetic sense of insects and its adaptive importance have been most thor-
oughly investigated in social insects, especially ants, bees, and termites. There is
now growing experimental evidence for magnetic field sensitivity in some Diptera,
Coleoptera, and Lepidoptera as well. Contexts for its use have included home
range orientation, homing, long-distance migration, and nest and body alignment.
In experiments with artificially induced magnetic fields, the insects have always
responded to changes in the magnetic field’s declination, which implies sensing of
magnetic polarity.
Although the sensory system that mediates magnetic signals has not yet been
definitively identified, evidence favors the use of intracellular, submicroscopic mag-
netite particles that have been found in honey bees, monarch butterflies, and some
ants. An alternative hypothesis draws on a system that has been demonstrated in
some birds, in which certain photochemical reactions are magnetically sensitive; if
such reactions are linked to light reception in the eye, then changing the wavelength
of ambient visible light could alter the directional orientation to the geomagnetic
field. Such effects have been obtained in male Drosophila melanogaster.
3.4 Thermoregulation
The sphinx moth Celerio lineata stabilizes its thoracic temperature during flight
over a range of ambient temperatures. A wide variety of insects, from butterflies and
beetles to cicadas and arctic flies, bask in sunny spots. Male tettigoniid grasshoppers
elevate their thoracic temperature prior to singing.
Most people think of insects as being purely ‘cold-blooded’ (ectothermic) crea-
tures that passively reflect the temperatures that surround them. However, this view
is overly simplistic. In most habitats, seasonal and diurnal temperature oscillations
are considerable.
Heterothermic is probably a better term to describe the insect’s life with fluctu-
ating body temperatures. Because of their very high ratio of surface area to mass,
small organisms readily lose and gain heat; insect body temperatures can change
abruptly by 10◦ C (50◦ F) or more when direct sunlight is obscured by the shade of a
passing cloud. This can be a serious matter—for insects, as for other animals, body
temperature is closely attuned to activity and energy supplies, and thus affects all
aspects of life from the rate at which food can be located and harvested to the ease
with which predators can be avoided. Thus, one should perhaps not be surprised to
find that insects have ways to control their body temperatures within a far narrower
range than that of their surroundings.
Adaptations to temperature fluctuations can (and do) take many forms. Some
adaptations are primarily behavioral, whereas others rely more heavily on physi-
ology, but any attempt to strictly divide them soon shows the two systems to be
intertwined.
Most insects face periods of adverse conditions, in which temperatures are outside
their physiological limits and/or food is wanting. However, being heterothermic,
they have an advantage that is denied to their homeothermic brethren. As long
as the temperature does not reach lethal extremes, insects are capable of ceasing
development and reproduction temporarily, then resuming these activities when con-
ditions again become favorable. This ability is undoubtedly one more reason behind
the enormous success of Class Insecta, because it allows them to exploit seasonal
resources while successfully bridging the harsh periods between.
The external conditions that temperate-zone insects must tolerate differ markedly
in summer and winter, so despite some similarities the insects’ responses have come
to be known under different names. We speak of winter hibernation (although it
is not directly comparable to hibernation in mammals) and summer aestivation.
Both actually jointly serve two different purposes. One is to promote survival
by depressing metabolism and energy utilization during adverse times. The other
is to synchronize periods of feeding, growth and reproductive activities with
those times of the year when food is available and environmental conditions are
suitable.
3.4 Thermoregulation 111
Short- and long-term shut-downs follow different strategies. To cope with rela-
tively short periods of non-lethal but unfavorable circumstances, insects commonly
go into a state of easily reversible, directly temperature-dependent developmen-
tal arrest called quiescence. Depending upon the temperature, the insect’s growth
slows, gonads mature more slowly, and feeding and other behaviors lose speed.
Quiescence is a rapid response that can occur at any life history stage.
Severely hostile conditions that last several months, such as a temperate-zone
winter or a hot, dry desert summer, require a different approach because an insect in
the active state simply could not survive. A programmed developmental arrest called
diapause occurs. Unlike quiescence, diapause happens only during a specific devel-
opmental stage, usually (but not always) before adulthood, and its timing depends
on the insect species. For example, the European corn borer diapauses only as a fifth
instar larva; the cecropia moth, only as a pupa; the Colorado potato beetle, only as
an adult.
Diapause can be facultative, occurring in response to environmental cues, or it
may be obligatory, occurring during each generation. A number of economically
important species (such as the gypsy moth) have an obligatory diapause, but fac-
ultative diapause is far more common. For an insect with facultative diapause, a
relevant environmental cue must be received during a sensitive period in develop-
ment for diapause to occur; otherwise, development simply continues to proceed.
This system can allow exquisite fine-tuning in the insect-plant arms race, as many
home gardeners can attest. Throughout the warm months, these insects can pro-
duce multiple non-diapausing generations, building up high populations. Then, in
late summer or autumn they produce a generation that goes into diapause and thus
escapes the winter months when their plant hosts are unavailable.
While the diapause itself is a physiological event, behavior is intimately and
intricately tied to it. Usually, the insect moves to a site (or in the case of
cocoons, constructs one!) that offers additional protection from the full onslaught
of inclement conditions. Specific behavioral changes are often associated with
movement to overwintering sites called hibernacula. This may involve short- or
long-range migration, a subject covered later in this chapter. It also includes local
movements such as are displayed by certain caterpillars that have been feeding up
in trees but now climb down and begin burrowing into the soil.
Feeding usually stops during diapause, so it no surprise to find that many species
‘fatten up’ in preparation. Of course, their exoskeleton does not allow this in a lit-
eral sense, but insects that are about to enter diapause often sequester twice as much
lipid reserves as their counterparts that are not so programmed. Because metabolism
slows with dropping temperatures, energy reserves may be less a problem, however,
than water loss. With no access to free water and possession of a large surface-to-
volume ratio, diapausing insects are particularly vulnerable to loss of water across
their cuticle. Insects that diapause as adults (such as many beetles, bugs, and butter-
flies) often gather into large aggregations (see Plate 5). For those that are distasteful,
this may increase their protection during this inactive life phase. Equally impor-
tant but perhaps less immediately obvious, an aggregation provides a more stable
microenvironment for its members and increases local humidity.
112 3 Spatial Adjustment
range from forming thicker cocoons to voiding the gut to remove food particles that
would be powerful ice nucleators.
A third way to increase thermotolerance is through a response called rapid heat
hardening, in which brief exposure to an intermediately high temperature provides
protection from injury at a more severe temperature. For example, Sarcophaga flesh
flies live only a brief time at 45◦ C (113◦ F) when moved there directly from a 25◦ C
(77◦ F) chamber. However, if they are first exposed to 40◦ C (104◦ F) for two hours,
they survive much longer at 45◦ C (113◦ F). This protection develops within minutes,
reaches a maximum within a few hours, and then decays rather slowly over several
days. Does diapause represent a shutdown in gene expression or does it represent
the expression of a unique set of genes? Perhaps both. Studying Sarcophaga flesh
flies, David Denlinger has shown that far fewer proteins are synthesized in the flies’
brains during diapause. However, in addition, their brains synthesize a set of pro-
teins that are not observed in brains of non-diapausing flies, and various classes of
diapauseupregulated genes can be distinguished based on their expression patterns.
Other research suggests that insects respond to heat stress by suppressing the nor-
mal pattern of protein synthesis and concurrently synthesizing several new proteins
called heat-shock proteins. Several types of heat-shock proteins have been found
in organisms ranging from bacteria to plants to insects, and behavioral geneticists
suspect their upregulation may be common to many different types of dormancies.
The most highly expressed heat-shock proteins are highly conserved; the gene that
encodes one group called Hsp70 is over 50% identical in bacteria and Drosophila
melanogaster.
Certain Pheidole ants forage above ground all day during cool, cloudy periods but
when the weather is hot and sunny they restrict their foraging to the period from
late evening to early morning. The adult form of a flightless midge called Diamesa
walks on glacier ice even when its body temperature is chilled to –16◦ C (3.2◦ F). It
is so sensitive to heat that if you were to pick one up in your hand, the warmth of
your skin would kill it.
Cryptic insects that rest by day on a matching background, predators that use
a lie-in-wait strategy, and many other arthropods that are exposed to sunshine all
have at least a potential problem with heat gain. Not surprisingly, various behavioral
adaptations have evolved that involve postural adjustments to minimize the body
surface area exposed to the heat source or that lead to avoidance of solar radiation
totally during certain periods. Structural features also help. For example, the long
legs of many ants and beetles living on sand in direct sunlight lift their bodies above
the substrate, while light body pigmentation reduces heat input from above.
Especially when the weather is warm, flying insects face an additional problem—
dissipating their own body heat. Small insects have much lower body temperatures
in flight than large insects do. However, this is not because they produce less heat—
instead, they actually produce more. Rather, it is because smaller insects have
114 3 Spatial Adjustment
more surface area relative to their mass, so that internally generated heat is lost
more rapidly by convection. Insect flight is actually one of the most energetically
demanding activities known. Most insects produce more heat per unit muscle mass
when they fly than almost any organism on earth. Almost 94% of the energy
expended by their contracting flight muscles is degraded to heat, while only about
6% appears as mechanical force on the wings.
Most insects apparently do not cool themselves as we do with active evapora-
tive mechanisms, but some of the exceptions are striking. The tsetse fly, Glossina
morsitans, is able to feed while standing on the hot hide of a mammal’s back under
the equatorial sun; to lower its body temperature, it opens its spiracles, allowing the
water drawn from its blood meal to evaporate. Diceroprocta cicadas of the Sonoran
desert overheat when they sing in hot weather; to cool down, they actually sweat
through large pores on their back, using liquid they suck from deep-rooted shrubs.
Social insects exhibit a variety of architectural and behavioral devices that main-
tain nest temperature and humidity within carefully controlled tolerance ranges, irre-
spective of season or outside temperature (see Chapter 10). While such insects are
individually heterothermic, socially they are nearly as homeothermic as birds and
mammals. Honey bee workers maintain the hive interior at temperatures between
34.5 and 35.5◦ C (94–96◦ F) by fanning with their wings to promote air circulation
and cooling by water evaporation. (In winter, the bees cluster loosely at warmer
temperatures and very tightly during extreme cold, keeping that same range.)
High muscle temperature is, however, not just a consequence of muscle activity.
In many situations, especially flight, it is also a prerequisite. Until the temperature
of the muscles is sufficiently high, there is little overlap in the contractions of the
antagonistic muscles, the wing-beat frequency is very low, and the insect remains
grounded. Different relative wing sizes and power requirements determine the mus-
cle temperature and wing-beat frequency necessary for a given insect to become
airborne; this varies with body size, between different muscles used for different
activities, and between different species.
Large wings are one way around the problem—they allow an insect to fly with
a low wing-beat frequency. As a result, some butterflies are able to initiate flight
without prior endothermic warm-up and to continue flight by gliding; this also
reduces the energy expenditure of locomotion. Another response is to evolve a dense
pile coat; this can cut the rate of convective heat loss from the insect’s body by
half. Bumblebees (Bombus) provide an elegant example; their combination of high
metabolic rate, relatively large body size, and good insulation helps them not only to
elevate thoracic temperatures passively during free flight but also to maintain a suf-
ficiently high thoracic temperature to fly at very low ambient temperatures. Due to
their rapid metabolism, their body temperature while flying usually exceeds ambient
temperatures by 5–10◦ C (9–18◦ F) and sometimes b as much as 20–30◦ C (36–54◦ F).
However, thermoregulation is still a costly behavior, especially when an insect is not
physically active. When temperatures are very low, it takes nearly as much energy
for a stationary bumblebee to maintain its body heat as it does to fly. Thus, it is all
the more impressive that queen bumblebees use thermoregulation behaviors to raise
the temperature of their brood.
The requirement for a high thoracic temperature to start flight poses a real behav-
ioral problem. When an insect comes to rest in the shade, its body temperature
rapidly becomes practically the same as the ambient temperature. Small flies such
as midges and fruit flies have rapid heat loss and little buildup of body heat during
flight; their wing-beat frequency and flight speed varies nearly directly with ambi-
ent thermal conditions. Some of the larger insects, such as bumblebees and some
moths, however, must warm their flight muscles to about 40◦ C (104◦ F) before they
can attain sufficient wing-beat frequency and lift to support themselves in free flight.
Without some means of increasing muscle temperature, the insect could remain
permanently grounded.
Shivering is a widespread mechanism for increasing thoracic temperatures before
flight. It involves many patterns of flight muscle activation and can work flight mus-
cles harder than flight itself does, but it is well suited for variable rates of heat
production because it can occur at a wide range of activation frequencies. Among
many Lepidoptera, the rates at which wings vibrate during such shivering have been
shown to be directly correlated with muscle temperature. Interestingly, it took scien-
tists a long time to recognize that insects shiver, because it is almost never externally
visible even if one looks very closely.
The evolution of shivering is clearly related to the evolution of flight, but it is
unrelated to an insect’s place on the phylogenetic tree. A physiological warm-up
is found in all large, active flyers among the dragonflies, moths and butterflies,
116 3 Spatial Adjustment
katydids, cicadas, flies, beetles, and wasps and bees. It is missing in small (and
therefore non-endothermic) members of the same groups.
In addition, an insect may regulate its body temperature through discontinuous
activities such as intermittent flight, intermittent shivering, or some combination of
the two. However, some insects are behaviorally better suited to make use of this
option than others. For example, a hovering sphinx moth or dragonfly in continuous
flight is in a less advantageous position in this regard than is a bee that lands on
flowers at frequent intervals while foraging.
3.5 Migration
Foraging army ants, Eciton hamatum (see Chapter 10) commonly move out of the
nest in columns along branching trails to seize and carry back to the nest all small
prey in their way. However, as new brood matures within the colony, instead of
simply returning to the nest, workers reverse to lead a mass exodus that carries
the whole colony away along one of the day’s trails. During their march the ants
neither react to prey nor branch off. Meanwhile, in California large numbers of
convergent lady beetles, Hippodamia convergens, spend the winter at high altitudes
in the Sierra Nevada mountains, then move down to agricultural areas in the Central
Valley in March to lay their eggs; beginning in June, their offspring fly back to
intermediate altitudes, gather into aggregations, then move back to higher altitudes
to overwinter. On a much smaller scale, two species of pierid butterflies regularly
maintain a directional flight across Gatun Lake in the Panama Canal.
What triggers the mass exodus of army ants? How are the movements of the
lady bugs coordinated and maintained? Why do the butterflies go back and forth?
Up to this point, we have been viewing insect spatial adjustment primarily as a
phenomenon involving a single individual within its immediate environment. Now
it is time to step back and view insect movement on a larger scale, considering these
behaviors in terms of the population or species.
A fundamental concern for most insects is finding the optimal habitat in which to
live and reproduce. For dispersing individuals, the problems are particularly com-
plex and acute. At what point should an individual stop expending energy on the
search and settle for whatever situation is available? When should selection favor the
evolution of ways for individuals to return to specific sites after displacement from
them? How should areas of fluctuating or disparate resources be best exploited?
When times are hard, does it make more sense to move, or to just hunker down and
ride it out? Such theoretical questions have been receiving much attention in recent
years, and some interesting answers are beginning to appear.
In general, insect movements are of two main kinds. The first kind, concerned
with resources and home ranges, has the ecological outcome of keeping an insect
in the habitat where it can obtain the resources it needs for given stages of its life
cycle. The second kind is quite different. Migration, one of the most important forms
of insect dispersal, has been succinctly defined as ‘adaptive traveling.’ It involves
3.5 Migration 117
leaving the current home range and habitat. (As a fine point of terminology, although
migration acts as an active mass movement functioning to displace populations,
those who study it usually distinguish between migration as an individual behavior
and dispersal and aggregation as population processes; see Section 3.5.3. However,
in popular usage, the distinctions are drawn less finely.)
Behaviorally, migration is characterized by persistent, enhanced locomotion in
a straightened-out manner, and migrating individuals do not typically respond to
stimuli for ‘vegetative functions’ such as feeding, reproduction, etc. For example,
the flight of certain scolytid bark beetles cannot be arrested by their host plant’s odor
until after they have been flying for many minutes. After long-distance flight, the
thresholds for vegetative activities are lowered, and further migration is inhibited.
Insects move around quite a bit, so it is important to distinguish migration from
those local movements of varying length and orientation that are concerned with
food and mate finding, escape from potential enemies, location of suitable oviposi-
tion sites, territorial defense, and other such activities. Such ‘appetitive’ behaviors
may lead to some dispersal, but often no effective displacement occurs at all, despite
a good deal of activity. Migration, in contrast, involves a phase in adult life during
which directed movement (usually flight activity) dominates over all other forms of
behavior. In many insects, such activity is restricted to a short period, after which
only appetitive movements occur; in some species, the flight muscles may break
down after migration so that no further flight is even possible.
Why do insects migrate? Such a question is more properly two questions. First,
the proximate one: what triggers the migratory ‘urge’ and how is it maintained?
Second, the ultimate one: what ecological and/or evolutionary conditions might
favor development of a migratory mode of life?
Migration clearly involves more than simply responding to the onset of adverse
conditions by escaping to a new habitat. Endocrine changes occur in correlation
with particular environmental effects (crowding, food deficiency, short days, etc.),
and in turn cause physiological and developmental changes. The insect’s behavior,
ecology, and genetics all come into play.
Many examples of insect migration have been described around the world. One
set involves the many insects that leave the breeding site, oviposit elsewhere, and
die, all in a single season. A second group includes short-lived adults that emigrate
and return. For example, many dragonflies depart from ponds to terrestrial feeding
sites some distance from water; after the eggs mature, females return to the vicinity
of their original breeding site and oviposit. Another category includes longer-lived
adults that hibernate or aestivate away from the original breeding site, then return to
it the following season.
How can one tell if a flight is truly migratory? The definition of migration should
offer an objective and experimental test: during the flight, the insect should not
be responsive to stimuli triggering vegetative behavior. The test has seldom been
applied, but where it has, there does appear to be a reciprocal interaction between
migratory flight and vegetative activities. Stimuli that evoke flight inhibit settling,
and stimuli that evoke settling inhibit flight. One example where such migratory–
vegetative interaction has been approached experimentally is that of the milkweed
bug, Oncopeltus (Case Study 3.1).
feeding rate fell markedly after day six to persist at a very low rate for the
remainder of life.
Temperature appeared to have a very direct effect upon migration. Raising
the temperature from 23◦ to 27◦ C, a warmth about optimal for population
growth, Dingle found that a lower proportion of the population now exhib-
ited tethered flights of 30 minutes or longer (his operational criterion for
migration). This suggested that once the bugs reached a thermally favorable
environment, they would tend to settle there.
Laboratory studies clearly indicated that peak flight preceded reproduc-
tion. Would prolonging the pre-reproductive period also prolong migration?
By altering day length in the laboratory, Dingle knew that he could delay
oviposition from 15 to 45 days after the adult molt. Raising bugs under both
regimes, he tested comparable groups of females for duration of tethered
flight 25 days after adult molt. The early-ovipositing females generally flew
for only a few minutes or less. The delayed females, which showed no signs
yet of reproductive development, performed like typical migrants. Under
field conditions, Dingle reasoned, this phenomenon would have important
consequences. One would be that in the autumn females would be capable of
migrating for much longer periods, thus improving their chances for escaping
the oncoming winter.
In what other ways might a short photoperiod affect migratory behavior?
In tethered flights, Dingle compared sets of short-day and long-day bugs of
both sexes repeatedly between 8 and 30 days after adult molt. Invariably,
a greater proportion of the short-day bugs flew for long periods (at least
30 minutes, usually 2–3 hours). Significantly, the results held true for both
sexes. Evidently, in addition to its indirect effect via ovarian development,
photoperiod was having a direct effect upon migration as well.
Environmental factors obviously had great importance as determinants of
migratory behavior. What about hereditary influence? Under strong selection,
Dingle found that he could increase the proportion of migrants of a population
of milkweed bugs from 25% to over 60% in one generation. Clearly, migratory
capability in Oncopeltus could be altered rapidly.
Not just in milkweed bugs, but in a great many insects, migration is associated
with an additional photoperiodic response—diapause, discussed earlier in this chap-
ter. Short days in autumn cue an inactivation of the reproductive system and thus
permit long flight using energy that otherwise would be channeled to reproduction.
Perhaps the best known of these insects is another milkweed-feeder, the monarch
butterfly, Danaus plexippus (see Plate 3); these long-distance fliers seek out and con-
gregate in sheltered cool locations. Through this behavioral thermoregulation, they
conserve the energy reserves they need to tide themselves over the winter diapause
portion of their migratory cycles.
120 3 Spatial Adjustment
Earlier biologists thought migration was probably just a way to escape from one
habitat to another more suitable one. On first examination such a simple answer
seemed sufficient. Faced with a temporarily unfavorable period or untenable habi-
tat, an insect species could adopt one of two evolutionary strategies. It could migrate
first or it could go into immediate diapause where it was already living. If the
change in habitat were reversible, as with seasonal changes, diapause in place
would be favored. When habitat changes were irregular, migration would have a
clear advantage over diapause. A recrystallization of ideas drawing from the fields
of physiology, behavior, and population ecology now suggests that migration is
not only a means of escape from unfavorable environments, but a positive act of
dispersal over all available habitats. Under such a view, insect migrants are more
accurately to be viewed as colonizers than as refugees, and migration as an evolved
adaptation, not a reaction to current adversity.
Entomologists and government agencies have paid particular attention to some
major agricultural pests—particularly several noctuid moth species and various
leafhoppers and plant-hoppers—that make round-trip seasonal migrations spanning
several generations. Many of these seasonal migrants may have originally found
migration adaptive for locating ephemeral habitats induced by rainfall in more
tropical parts of their range, but with extensive acreage now under agricultural
cultivation, they build up enormous populations that spread far beyond the insects’
ancestral range over the temperate zone spring and summer. Late-season migrants
are trapped by cold weather and die, because none of these species can diapause,
either as adults or juveniles. How can this be adaptive? At first, some researchers
speculated that the scenario was a ‘Pied Piper’ phenomenon, but others argued that
such massive losses would result in severe selection against migration. Current
evidence suggests the phenomenon of late-season deaths may be overstated.
Though many of these migrants do die, weather systems safely bear many other
individuals southward to winter breeding areas.
Early studies on insect migration understandably concentrated on the long-
distance flight of spectacular insects such as milkweed bugs and butterflies.
However, it has become increasingly obvious that migration is a far more
widespread phenomenon than previously suspected, including many small species
whose movements, relatively speaking, are neither far nor spectacular. In all
instances, migration is a distinct behavioral and physiological syndrome closely
intertwined with reproductive timing and strategy.
First, migratory flights are limited to a specific stage in the life history, after
the cuticle has hardened but before reproduction begins. Second, whether or not the
males are in accompaniment, migration always involves the female sex, and migrant
females are generally sexually immature and thus have a high reproductive value or
expected contribution to population growth (a high r, see Chapter 9). Most migration
correlates strongly with age (Fig. 3.9). That is, it occurs prior to egg development,
and while the development of the flight system is maximized, that of the reproduc-
tive system is minimized, a phenomenon that results in migration occurring chiefly
in young female adults. In the 1960s, C. G. Johnson, a leader in the development of
migration theory, termed this the oogenesis-flight syndrome. Third, migratory flight
3.5 Migration 121
Fig. 3.9 Making trade-offs. Duration of tethered flight as a function of age in three insects: (top)
the frit fly, Oscinella frit, (middle) the fruit fly, Drosophila funebris, and (bottom) the milkweed
bug, Oncopeltus fasciatus. The decline in flight occurs concurrently with an increase in reproduc-
tion (the oogenesis-flight syndrome). Similar effects have been demonstrated in a wide range of
insects, including bugs, flies, mosquitoes, aphids, moths, grasshoppers, and beetles
model that related locust outbreaks and weather patterns (particularly those around
the Intertropical Convergence Zone) for the desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria.
During the locust plague years of 1954–1955, Rainey’s theories were put to the test
in a large international effort that monitored locust populations on the ground and
followed swarms by aircraft over much of the northern two-thirds of the African
continent. Results appeared to confirm the theory’s predictions, and for decades this
theory was established dogma.
Later reexamination of the data, combined with studies of other species, con-
siderably complicated the picture and cast some doubt whether the Inter-Tropical
Convergence Zone was involved at all. Rainey’s theory relied heavily on obser-
vations of gregarious swarms carried passively along during the day by winds at
different heights. A major discovery, confirmed by radar studies, was that soli-
tary locusts fly about at night, often for longer distances than gregarious locusts
do. Radar also showed that even when the wind was blowing strongly enough to
carry them along, these night-fliers exhibited considerable mutual alignment and
collective orientation. In other words, they were navigating (see below).
These discoveries changed the entire scientific view of locust migration. It now
appears that most large-scale migration takes place by single individuals migrating
at night, not by swarms moving across the countryside by day. In all locusts and
grasshoppers that have been studied, swarms have been found to function more as
extended foraging groups than as migratory groups.. The rolling swarm so visible
to an outside observer actually occurs from the movement of feeding individuals.
At the leading edge of the swarm, locusts settle and feed. This reduces the vegeta-
tion available to those locusts that are behind them, so these latter individuals pass
over this area, settle into a new leading edge, and feed. Most recently, it also has
been found that some locust species help this movement along by nipping at the
abdomens of the locusts in front of them, and escaping from others approaching
their own rear end.
Although the details of the swarming-foraging and individually migratory
aspects of locust life cycles will continue to be debated, most scientists agree that the
system provides a very effective way to exploit ephemeral habitats in arid to semi-
arid regions. Not surprisingly, a number of other insects take the same approach
to the problem, in the process making themselves major agricultural pests in drier
regions around the world. This includes several moths in the family Noctuidae,
particularly in the genera Heliothis, Helicoverpa, and Spodoptera. Interestingly,
large-scale mark-recapture, radar tracking, and detailed laboratory studies have con-
firmed that Spodoptera armyworms do disperse in a pattern that is closely associated
with rainfall occurring with the passage of the Inter-Tropical Convergence Zone.
Scelionid wasps ride upon the backs of female grasshoppers; ultimately, those tiny
parasites will oviposit upon the grasshopper’s eggs. Tropical leafcutter ants are the
3.5 Migration 125
original suburban commuters; day after day they travel hundreds of meters back and
forth along trails that extend both horizontally along the forest floor and vertically
into the canopy. Every spring, vast numbers of insects are transported by winds up
through the Mississippi Valley into upper midwestern agricultural regions of the
United States and Canada.
Dispersal—movement away from one’s place of birth or from centers of popula-
tion density—is part of the life cycle of almost every living thing, and unsurprisingly
it occurs by a wide variety of methods. Dispersal may be random or directed, pas-
sive or active. Young individuals quite commonly leave the area in which they were
born. Usually such dispersal occurs either passively or under the juvenile’s own
volition, but in some cases dispersal may be encouraged by the indifferent or even
hostile behavior of their parents or nest mates. As a result, through time all pop-
ulations have a tendency to spread out spatially. Many different mechanisms may
be involved, from relatively simple responses to gradients of certain environmental
factors, to various active or passive dispersal mechanisms associated with the search
for a mate or food.
The distinction between passive and active dispersal is made mainly for
convenience; in reality a continuum often exists between them. For example, a
Pemphigus aphid, which lives on the roots of the sea aster growing in salt marshes,
is photonegative for most of its life. However, first instar nymphs are photopositive,
climbing up the sea asters until they set themselves adrift on the rising tide. Sea
breezes send them scudding across open water to be deposited at low tide on another
mud bank where they seek out and colonize new plants. Upon arrival, as a result
of their waterborne dispersal experiences they reverse their reaction to light and
become photonegative. In another example, female gypsy moths are unable to fly,
so natural dispersal of this well-known pest occurs primarily through young larvae
being blown on their silken threads by wind. (A similar mechanism, ballooning,
is employed by many newly hatched spiderlings.) However, gypsy moth larvae
vary in their dispersal propensity, and the behavior of first instar larvae depends on
several variables such larval density, food availability, and their mother’s nutritional
status.
Many flightless insects are transported by others, a phenomenon known as
phoresy. Human bot flies will attach their eggs to the legs and body of mosquitoes,
in this way transporting them to a human host for hatching and larval development.
Some Trichoptera larvae undergo their development within gelatinous capsules
upon the bodies of chironomid midges. Tiny wingless Mallophaga attach themselves
to the bodies of the hippoboscid flies which parasitize their bird hosts, in this man-
ner being carried from one host to another (Fig. 3.10). A great variety of mites ride
upon beetles, ants, and other insects; the insects are probably not injured unless the
numbers of mites become excessive. In several groups of wasps, a unique type of
phoretic dispersal has independently evolved on more than one occasion. All cases
involve parasitic females that have become so highly modified for tunneling into
the soil or food that they have permanently lost their wings. During copulation the
genitalia in these species lock together, so that the males carry the smaller females
about, suspended in this way, for considerable periods of time. This phenomenon,
126 3 Spatial Adjustment
Fig. 3.10 Hitching a ride. Two examples of insect phoresy. (left) Feather lice firmly attach
their mandibles to the abdomen of hippoboscid flies, obtaining transport to new bird hosts; it is
not unusual to find 20 lice (arrow) upon a single fly. (right) Phoretic copulation in a parasitic
wasp, Dimorphothynnus haemorrhoidalis (Tiphiidae). The male both inseminates and disperses the
smaller, short-legged wingless female (arrow), often carrying her to sources of nectar or honeydew
and in some cases actually feeding her by regurgitation
termed phoretic copulation, allows adaptation for a burrowing life combined with
effective dispersal of inseminated females into areas where new populations of hosts
may be discovered.
When wind, currents, or other organisms can move an insect to a suitable habi-
tat, little active orientation is necessary. However, many migrants cover very long
distances and/or must find very precise locations for breeding, feeding, or diapause
(Fig. 3.11). For these species, accurate navigation, the act of moving through a
place or along a route, becomes a major life task. As with so many other major life
tasks, navigation generally uses multiple cues and these cues serve as backups to
each other. It is postulated that each species recognizes some sort of cue hierarchy.
(However, even for such well-studied organisms as birds, there is no consensus for
any organism as to exactly what the hierarchy might be.)
Complex navigation is often divided into three general categories. The first is
piloting, which is considered to be the ability to use fixed known reference points
(‘landmarks’) to orient or navigate. The second is compass orientation, directional
ability without reference to a particular origin or destination; in essence, the insect
does not know where it is, but only what direction it is heading. The third is true
navigation, the complex ability to move toward a particular goal in completely unfa-
miliar territory without sensory contact with that goal. It is important to recognize,
however, that these categories are for human convenience; they do not necessarily
reflect increasing levels of complexity or the order in which the capabilities evolved.
Even though a number of common themes seem to apply across migration systems
and taxa, there are simply too few data to make such sweeping generalizations.
Navigation, particularly across long distances and/or lengthy time frames, often
appears so impressive that humans have been reluctant to ascribe it to simple pro-
cesses. However, a few detailed studies have shown that quite simple mechanisms
such as wind transport can result in surprisingly precise results.
To many researchers, true navigation implies that an organism possesses an inter-
nal ‘cognitive map’ that represents the geometric relations among points in the
3.5 Migration 127
Fig. 3.11 Migrating monarchs. (above) The fall, spring and summer migratory routes of North
American populations of the monarch, Danaus plexippus. Fall migrations take the butterflies to
congregation sites in coastal California and central Mexico; marked individuals have been docu-
mented to fly over 2000 miles during the fall migration to Mexico. (below) The exact destination
in Mexico was unknown prior to 1976; since then, at least 22 overwintering aggregation sites have
been reported in stands of mature oyamel (Abies religiosa) trees in Mexico’s trans-volcanic belt
at altitudes between 2700 and 3600 m. Since 1986 several of these forest areas (shaded) are now
legally protected in the Monarch Butterfly Biosphere Reserve
environment. To humans, ‘map’ implies visual cues. However, given the importance
of chemistry in other aspects of insects’ lives (see Chapter 6), such a map for many
migrating insects might well be chemically based instead, operating analogously to
the ways that homing salmon use the odor structure of water masses to move from
the ocean into river mouths.
128 3 Spatial Adjustment
Some migrants travel by day, others by night, and the sky provides potential guid-
ance at both times. For insects, day travel is by far better studied. At its simplest use,
a day-flying insect could just orient to the sun’s brightness, and move toward it with-
out correcting for the sun’s movement during the day. Even the elegant navigation
of the monarch butterfly has a component of this type; because the butterflies do
not start flying until they have warmed up, following the sun’s path takes them in a
southwestward orientation during their fall migration. However, additional precision
may rely upon other factors (Fig. 3.12).
In 1911 while studying desert ants in Tunisia, Felix Santschi demonstrated insect
sun-compass orientation for the first time. Using mirrors, he altered the sun’s appar-
ent direction as viewed by ants and showed that they would change direction at the
same angle as the displacement of the sun. Others have since shown that insects can
compensate for the movement of the sun across the sky. Time-compensation adds
more accuracy to sun-compass orientation; this requires that the insect have an inter-
nal timing method or biological clock. Such a system also has been demonstrated
for monarchs.
Finally, in addition to information provided by its position and movement, the
sun provides information via the plane of polarization of light or the e-vector. Some
insects clearly use both the position of the sun and patterns of polarized light, as has
been shown in elegant detail for the honey bee. Mechanisms for detecting e-vector
Fig. 3.12 Shifting direction. The rotational orientation hypothesis holds that monarch butterfly
orientation shifts clockwise at a rate of 1 degree per day throughout the year for all generations
of the annual migratory cycle. The spring equinox at overwintering sites apparently triggers north-
ward migration from Mexico. As time goes on, the orientation of subsequent generations shifts
as shown. Once the last generation reaches the overwintering sites, migration is repressed, but the
internal migratory clock keeps running
3.5 Migration 129
Fig. 3.13 Polarizing cues. Two representations of the e-vector sky or plane of polarization pattern
◦ ◦
of light with the sun in different positions above the horizon (24 left and 60 right) represented by
the black dot on the axis. Orientation of each black bar gives the e-vector direction for that bar, and
bar width indicates degree of polarization. Below each is the pattern of an optic interneuron firing
response obtained from a cricket exposed to changes in the polarization of incoming light. Arrows
indicate onset and end of the light stimulus
information have been well established in ants, bees, crickets and other insects.
Scanning the sky, the insect matches a small patch of polarization detectors in a
specialized portion of the retina to the distribution of the e-vector pattern in the sky
(Fig. 3.13); the firing of receptor interneurons speeds up or slows down, depending
on the degree of polarization.
Although they operate at different scales, both navigational cues and positional
cues are intertwined elements essential to spatial adjustment. Every information
source that is available—from infrared and magnetic waves and planes of light
polarization to visible landmarks of the earth, sea, and sky—is used somewhere.
We’ll be returning to these in following chapters in different contexts.
Chapter 4
Foraging and Feeding
4.1 Introduction
Gypsy moth larvae defoliate vast areas of forest across Europe, northern Africa,
and North America, feeding on over 50 tree species and reaching densities of tens
of thousands of individuals per hectare. South American phorid flies lay their eggs
on fire ants, and the grub that hatches invades the ant’s head, consuming the brain;
ultimately the ant’s head falls off and a new fly emerges. Bed bugs live only with
humans, and do poorly when fed other kinds of blood.
Much of the financial and social support enjoyed by entomologists derives from
the negative impact that some insects cause by feeding upon human food plants,
fiber sources, stored foods, waste products, farm animals, and even upon humans
themselves. In turn, the propensity of other insects to feed upon man’s pests has
made them noticeable allies. For the insects themselves, finding and consuming
food while simultaneously avoiding becoming food for other organisms are two of
life’s most essential behaviors. The former is the focus of this chapter; the latter will
be the subject of Chapter 5.
Let’s begin with an overview of foraging and feeding to put things in context.
First, a note on vocabulary: Purists would say that foraging is the preferred collective
term for all the behaviors involved in obtaining food, whereas feeding should be used
more narrowly for the actual act of consuming that food. As such, feeding research
generally is directed toward proximate factors, the ‘how’ questions that examine
ways in which behaviors are directly produced and regulated. Foraging research
tends to concentrate more on questions of ultimate causation. Simplistically, one
could call these ‘why’ questions. In practice, the terms feeding and foraging are
often used interchangeably, and in the end of course a complete picture involves
understanding both proximate and ultimate factors whenever possible.
One popular way to sort feeding habits is by principal nutrition source, e.g.,
plant, animal, carrion, dung, etc. It provides a convenient way to describe indi-
viduals or species, but the larger the taxonomic group one attempts to apply it
to, the less useful such a simple scheme becomes. Important exceptions occur in
most taxa. Furthermore, many insects that appear to feed on one type of food upon
closer scrutiny may be found to be entirely dependent upon another. For example
Fig. 4.1 Defying easy classification. Mosquito feeding habits differ with life stage and sex. Here,
female Aedes mosquitoes feed on an adult mantis; most of their feeding punctures are through
intersegmental membranes. The extent of such invertebrate blood feeding by mosquitoes in nature
remains to be documented, but these laboratory females were able to develop fertile eggs in the
same manner as vertebrate blood-fed individuals
although most people associate Drosophila fruit flies with decaying organic matter
such as overripe bananas, their larvae are actually eating, not the fruit itself, but
microorganisms associated with the decay.
Feeding behaviors also can be partitioned in various ways throughout a species’
life; many species feed at different trophic levels at different stages in their life
cycles. For example, mosquito larvae feed upon plankton and suspended organic
matter. Additional partitioning arises from sexual differences; adult females suck
vertebrate and invertebrate blood (Fig. 4.1) but males feed only on nectar. For a
surprisingly large proportion of insects, partitioning can be so extreme that entire
life stages do not feed at all—e.g., the pupa stage of holometabolous species. Insects
such as the silk moths, bot flies, and mayflies do not feed at all as adults. In such
cases, the larva acts as a rapidly growing ‘feeding machine,’ storing huge quantities
of reserves for adult life.
Another broad way to classify insect feeding habits is by selectivity. How choosy
are insects about their diet? Some insects accept a wide variety of foods (polyphagy),
a fact that often makes them significant pests. Migratory locusts, carpet beetles,
and cockroaches are familiar examples. Generalist feeders are especially likely to
adapt rapidly to new crop varieties, including transgenic crops, and to insecticides.
It is said that over 500 insect species targeted by crop protection strategies are now
resistant to a variety of insecticides.
4.1 Introduction 133
However, most insect species accept only a limited range of foods and usually
prefer one or two (oligophagy). Often this is reflected in the common name they
are given. Tobacco hornworm moth larvae, for example, feed on various solana-
ceous plants but prefer tobacco or tomato. Just as most insects are herbivores, so
also most herbivorous insects are food specialists, feeding on closely related plant
species (or sometimes just a single species). It is generally assumed that chemical
or physical differences among plants select for insect host-specificity. The suppo-
sition is that physiological costs impose trade-offs among adaptations to different
plant characteristics, but only a few genetic and physiological studies provide strong
explicit support for this hypothesis. Host specificity also might be favored by other
advantages such as use of specific plants as mating/aggregation sites, more efficient
host-finding, or better defense through such means as sequestering host-produced
toxic compounds.
Some insects take their choosiness to the extreme and exhibit strict specificity
to one food, often a single host (monophagy). Monophagous specialists occur in
almost every insect group. For example, certain leaf-mining caterpillars and gall
insects can develop successfully on only one species of host plant. Parasitic species
also tend to show a high degree of host specificity.
Like other organisms, insects need the appropriate balance of proteins and car-
bohydrates. Can insects ‘choose’ to balance their diet? Under laboratory conditions,
both locusts and caterpillars have been able to select foods that provide the appropri-
ate balance, but the composition of these foods was artificially extreme. It remains
to be shown whether insects can and do fine-tune their choices when feeding on
natural food with much smaller deficiencies.
Regardless of an insect’s food choice and the way it is classified, the ability to for-
age successfully resolves into a remarkably constant chain of behaviors, each link of
which facilitates the next: (1) food habitat location; (2) food finding; (3) food recog-
nition; (4) food acceptance; and (5) food suitability. (For some parasitic species, a
sixth link—host regulation—may operate as the parasite accelerates, retards, or oth-
erwise modifies the host’s physiological development.) This chain can involve any
or all of the senses, but the chemical sensory systems predominate. Consider the
bee wolf, Philanthus triangulum, which stalks honey bees that it captures, stings,
paralyzes, and carries back to its nest. As the pioneering ethologist Niko Tinbergen
described it:
A hunting female of this species flies from flower to flower in search of a bee. In this phase
she is entirely indifferent to the scent of bees; a concealed bee, or even a score of them put
out of sight into an open tube so that the odor escaping from this is clearly discernible even
for the human nose, fails to attract her attention. Any visual stimulus supplied by a moving
object of approximately the right size, whether it be a small fly, a large bumblebee, or a
honey bee, releases the first reaction. The wasp at once turns her head to the quarry and
takes a position at about 10–15 cm to leeward of it, hovering in the air like a syrphid fly.
Experiments with dummies show that from now on the wasp is very susceptible to bee scent.
Dummies that do not have bee-odor are at once abandoned, but those dummies that have
the right scent release the second reaction of the chain. This second reaction is a flash-like
leap to seize the bee. The third reaction, the actual delivery of the sting, cannot be released
by these simple dummies and is probably dependent on new stimuli, probably of a tactile
nature.
134 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.2 Determining insect food preferences. A simple smorgasbord of potential foods (clock-
wise from 12 o’clock are fresh pieces of Brassica rapa leaf, apple, lettuce leaf, white cabbage,
purple cabbage, and carrot) was offered in a petri dish with two Pieris rapae caterpillars released
at the center. Two hours later relative feeding damage, readily apparent visually, may be quantified
by comparing the food weights before and after exposure to the caterpillars. With modifica-
tion a similar technique can also be used to assay the relative importance of various plant
products by incorporating chemicals to be tested in standard agar discs made with powdered
leaves
Many insects that are deprived of food will sample items they would otherwise
reject. In general, grasshoppers were once considered to be unselective in their
choice of foods. In newer observations, several grasshopper species have shown
an increasingly wider acceptance of a range of host plants with increasing length of
food deprivation. Sometimes this may signal a simple need for water rather than for
nutrients. A well-hydrated locust will move away from a piece of wet filter paper,
but a dehydrated one will attempt to eat it.
Despite voluminous records of individual insect–plant and parasite–host rela-
tionships, a deeper understanding of the evolution of insect diet selection has
been difficult to gain. For example, why are some insects monophagous and
others polyphagous? A feasible explanation is found in the congruency hypothe-
sis advanced by Vincent Dethier. Both feeder and food are continually evolving
against a background of multiple pressures—the plant, prey or host by synthe-
sizing different chemicals, the insect by developing different sensory and central
decision-making capabilities. Specific changes occur in both organisms as a result
of random mutations. Congruency, or match between food source on one hand and
insect as feeder on the other, occurs whenever these two independently mutating
systems interact in such a way that formerly non-attracting chemicals now stimu-
late feeding. Sometimes, the change may be in the insect, resulting in the addition,
subtraction, or substitution of capabilities. For example, insect neural changes may
cause formerly neutral chemicals to become attractive or repellents to be no longer
detected. At other times, various chemicals may arise by mutation in the plant, prey,
or host.
For phytophagous insects, little information exists upon which to test the con-
gruency hypothesis, although a number of documented cases of sudden irreversible
shifts in insect feeding habits suggest that it is not unreasonable. It provides explana-
tions for a number of previously puzzling phenomena, such as why some plants such
as ferns have feeding deterrents even though they evolved long before phytophagous
insects did. The theory also explains why shifts in diet may occur in any direction,
including from polyphagy to monophagy, and why some apparently suitable plants
are not eaten.
At the same time, like any model, the congruency hypothesis presents an over-
simplified view. The model assumes reproductive isolation of the new mutant and
that mutations represent quantum jumps rather than intermediate states. It ignores
ecological factors. For example, the host plant or animal is not only a source of
food; often it equally may be a place to live. Thus, for a new feeding habit to be
established, the host need not necessarily be better nutritionally. For many insects,
plant selection is selection of a whole community—a microclimate, a shelter, a set
of predators and diseases. The picture at any given moment must be one of dynamic
equilibrium, and feeding must be viewed as a compromise between nutritional and
ecological optima.
Feeding habits also reflect only a subset of the choices that could potentially be
made, because for a plant and insect to interact they must be in contact with one
other. This has been graphically illustrated by introduced pest species throughout
the world, enthusiastically munching upon certain plants not formerly available to
4.1 Introduction 137
them. Nor does accessibility require that it be the pest that is introduced; a striking
example is the Colorado potato beetle. This native American insect fed upon weedy
scrub until humans introduced the potato into its world; with this new opportunity,
the beetle population evolved to prefer potatoes over native hosts.
Preying mantises will consume a relatively constant number of house flies each day
when flies are continually available. Blowflies will maintain a relatively constant
daily food intake at a constant sugar concentration; if the concentration is decreased,
daily intake increases, and vice versa. In fact, most insects will feed to a point and
then stop. What causes a feeding insect to finally stop eating? What determines the
timing—how long and how often—of feeding?
One of the most thoroughly studied cases of feeding regulation involves Phormia
regina, a blowfly investigated by Vincent Dethier and his associates. The adult
blowfly needs only water, carbohydrates, and oxygen for maintenance, receiving
all other necessary materials during its larval stage; adult feeding occurs only to
provide locomotive energy. Thus, the blowfly offers a simple system for studying
two essential aspects of feeding behavior: the nature of the ‘on/off’ mechanism and
the nature of quality control.
The ‘on/off’ part is relatively straightforward. Blowfly feeding starts when a
fly steps on potential food, stimulating taste receptors on its tarsi. These lead to
the extension of its tubular mouthparts (proboscis), which thus contact the food
solution. At the proboscis tip, a lobe (labellum) has mechano- and chemorecep-
tive hairs that experience the consistency and taste of the food. The food quality,
along with peripheral and central adaptive processes, determines the sucking reac-
tion. Cessation of feeding behavior involves a homeostatic mechanism (Fig. 4.3) so
that, in the presence of excess food, a constant amount is ingested per day. As the
gut is filled, stretch receptors in the foregut are activated, and their firing inhibits
brain input from the external chemoreceptors that elicited feeding. At this stage in
feeding regulation, nonnutritive or metabolically useless foods are not distinguished
from nutritious ones. A blowfly will, for example, take up the useless sugar fructose
and regulate the amount until it dies.
Timing of insect feeding is more complex. In general terms, the length of the
period between meals is usually related to the quantity and nature of the previous
meal and to the amount of energy expended in the interim. For most insects, feeding
occurs at relatively short intervals of minutes or hours. However, some insects, such
as certain filter feeders, eat almost continuously. Others, such as some parasites,
may feed only at wide intervals of many hours, days, or weeks.
Even long-term changes in feeding behavior such as seasonality may rely on the
same basic physiological mechanisms as short-term feeding regulation, however.
A good example is provided by the face fly, Musca autumnalis, studied by John
Stoffolano. During the summer, face flies, which feed upon cattle blood and lay their
138 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.3 Stopping when full. Metabolic homeostasis in the blowfly, Phormia regina: a model
system illustrating the interactions of external and internal stimuli and resultant feedback which
regulate the physiological aspects of feeding. The actual cessation of food intake is probably
mediated through negative feedback from internal receptors. The internal sensing upon which this
depends may be based on any of quite a number of variables, such as food bulk and/or gut capacity,
level of sensory stimulation, and length of time spent at continuous feeding
now asserts itself. In the fall, under the influence of short days and low temperature,
the ovaries of the last generation of flies of the season fail to develop. Now face
flies of both sexes ignore cattle and feed only on nectar. On this diet and under
changed hormonal influences, a face fly’s fat body swells and its abdomen distends
until abdominal stretch receptors apparently respond with impulses to the central
nervous system, nullifying the sensory input from tarsal receptors. Feeding stops.
But gradually through the winter, fat is utilized; the fat bodies shrink and the crop
gradually empties. Spring marks the end of this developmental arrest. Flies then
feed on nectar until cattle are put to pasture.
Among plant-eating insects, feeding periodicity is also common. One important
evolutionary reason undoubtedly is that a plant is not a homogeneous chemical
entity. Rather, it is a heterogeneous, ever-changing microchemical environment.
Important plant constituents such as carbohydrates, fats, proteins, minerals, alka-
loids and essential oils vary with a number of factors, such as time of day and
season, plant growth stage and tissue, climatic and soil conditions, etc. For example,
consider the seasonal changes that occur in oak leaves. New leaves contain rela-
tively more protein, water, and sucrose and less tannin than older leaves. As tannin
increases, protein availability is reduced until its decline becomes a limiting factor
for most herbivores. Not surprisingly, most lepidopteran larvae attacking oaks feed
early in the season on new foliage; by so doing, they make the best use of available
protein and avoid most of the toxic tannins that slow growth rates and reduce fecun-
dity. Plants also show shorter-term cycles, the most familiar example of which is
the diurnal rhythm of nectar production and flower opening in many plants. Honey
bees, apparently able to remember not only the location where food is available but
the time of day at which this occurs, continue to gather only during the previously
learned hour.
Environmental variables are also important in the establishment and maintenance
of feeding periodicity, and it would be a mistake to underestimate their effect. For
example, most ants are more active at some times of day (or temperature and light
ranges) than at others. Many ant species in temperate climates forage throughout the
day, but in hotter lands some regularly stop feeding for a midday break. Many others
forage only at night; 54 out of 58 species of ants living in the Sahara are nocturnal.
The effect of such foraging rhythms, of course, is to confine the outside activity of
the ants to times when temperature and humidity are least harmful and/or to when
their food is most easily obtained. It is sometimes difficult to determine the degree
to which an insect’s foraging rhythm depends upon internal biological clocks, how-
ever. For example, leaf-cutting ants, Atta cephalotes, normally collect leaves all day,
beginning early in the morning. Shading their nest entrance between 5:30 a.m. and
6:00 a.m. will delay the time of appearance of the first workers, but no amount of
light before 5:30 a.m. will make foraging start. Some internal clock is apparently
responsible for bringing the workers to the nest entrance to inspect for light. At the
same time, many studies have shown that when ants are fed regularly at the same
time for three to five days, they learn to search for food at that time on succeeding
days. This implies that ants in the wild would learn to forage when food was most
plentiful. Thus generalizations must be made quite cautiously. Probably the daily
140 4 Foraging and Feeding
a generally favorable cost-benefit ratio has to include not only the need for food but
also the need to escape from predators and the need to reproduce effectively. And
what about situations where insects compete with other insects for the same lim-
ited resource? In the real world, a decision-making animal often must consider the
behavior of others. In 1974, John Maynard Smith elaborated upon optimality theory
to predict optimal outcomes when individuals interact with one another. Borrowing
from economic theory, he applied game theory to explain how animals develop a
strategy that considers competitors’ behaviors. Equivalent to coaching strategies
in a competitive sport, game theory predicts that an individual will choose differ-
ent strategies in proportion to their relative effectiveness against choices made by
opponents.
4.2.1 Herbivory
Plant eaters comprise almost half of the world’s insect species today; they occur in
about one-third of the major insect groups, including some of the most numerically
successful ones such as the Lepidoptera. Fossil evidence indicates that terrestrial
arthropods have been consuming plants for at least 400 million years, and studies
of a range of ecosystems demonstrate that insects consume more plant mass than
all vertebrate herbivores combined. Even the vast ungulate herds of the African
savannas are estimated to be responsible for only 15–35% of the herbivory there,
with insects causing 10–75%.
Herbivory (also called phytophagy or simply plant-feeding) most commonly
involves feeding upon foliage, but one can also find sap feeders, root feeders, seed
feeders, and, in the case of woody plants, bark feeders. (In fact, with the excep-
tion of coal there is hardly a source of organic carbon not used to some extent by
some insects.) To these directly phytophagous species may be added the complex of
insects that feed on the rotting debris below the plants and even the opportunists that
partake of the sugary honeydew excreted by the sap feeders. Each of these species
in turn serves as a potential host for one or more parasitic insects that may in turn be
attacked by their own specific enemies, called hyperparasites. Thus, even a single
plant species can form the base of a complex food web (Fig. 4.4).
Herbivory is such an important part of insect behavior that it keeps cropping up in
different contexts. Thus, for example, in Section 4.2.6, we’ll consider those special
cases in which insects can be said to be farmers, rather than hunter-gatherers. In
Section 4.3, we’ll be returning to the subject of herbivory from the perspective of
insect-plant coevolution.
Fig. 4.4 Life is seldom simple! Food web associated with cabbage plants in Minnesota. Herbivore species include 11 leaf feeders, 10 sap feeders, and
4 root feeders. Other trophic levels were less thoroughly studied, but included at least 21 detritivores, 79 honeydew feeders, and 85 predatory, parasitic, or
4 Foraging and Feeding
hyperparasitic carnivores
4.2 Foraging Strategies 143
‘If you want dinner, go find it!’ For by far the majority of insects, food location
involves some manner of active search. Plant-feeding insects (if not hatched on their
host) may find their hosts by seeking appropriate habitats, by increased activity that
maximizes their chances of encountering a plant, by completely random activity
halted by strong arrestant properties of the plant, or by attraction to a plant from a
distance by smell or vision or both. Predatory insects, on the other hand, are usu-
ally tuned to movement, which is probably the most widely shared characteristic of
potential prey; parasites often cue in to by-products of their hosts’ feeding activity
such as frass (see Chapter 2).
Because active search for food requires energy expenditure and time involve-
ment, one would expect selection to favor behaviors that increase its efficiency.
Optimality theory predicts that when food is mobile, it is advantageous to catch
more food in less time, to catch larger food items in the same time required to take
smaller ones, and to avoid chasing food or hosts likely to escape or prove unsatis-
factory for some other reason. The variety of means by which such ends have been
approached is nearly limitless.
What of those species like gall midges, in which the mother oviposits directly
on or in the plant, or like parasitic wasps that oviposit on an animal host? Locating
food would appear to be no crisis for these species, because the young are literally
surrounded by it, but in actuality the burden of food location has simply been shifted
to another life stage.
When food is unevenly distributed, organisms must develop ways to locate and
identify the richest clumps, or patches, of that food. Studies first done with birds
but probably applicable to most searching foragers show that optimal foraging has
two phases. When they first locate a food source, many animals spent the first few
minutes in a ‘sampling phase’ that looks like random wandering though several
patches. Then, in what is called the ‘exploitation phase’ they spent most of their
time foraging in the richest patch.
Under natural conditions, however, food density and condition are often in a state
of flux. Consequently, a useful strategy is to sample food availability and condition
periodically to keep up with current conditions. In the case of nectar feeders, the
amount of food that can be obtained from any one flower is rather limited, and
for maximal efficiency one might expect bees, for example, to be able to exploit
new food resources as they become available and to differentiate between more
and less rewarding flowers in bloom at any one time. Studying bumblebees in the
1970s, Berndt Heinrich performed some of the first direct analyses of insect forag-
ing behaviors in terms of caloric costs and benefits. He confirmed that bumblebees
generally collect nectar from several different plants during a foraging trip, spending
most of their time on the richest flowers while simultaneously hedging their bets by
investing some energy in less productive species. By experimentally enriching some
of these ‘minor’ flowers with sugar syrup, Heinrich demonstrated that an individual
forager immediately will switch from its previously most preferred flower and adopt
the fortified flower for as long as the sugar syrup is added. (Nectar is far more than
just sugar water, however. It contains considerable quantities of various amino acids
that are nutritionally essential to adult insects unable to synthesize them.)
144 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.5 Foraging optimally. The marginal value theorem allows one to predict how long an
animal should continue to feed at a particular location (food patch) based on cumulative food
intake. For example, it might be applied to bees visiting patches of flowers for nectar. A simple
graphical solution is depicted here. First, generate a curve that represents the cumulative food
obtained over time from an average food patch. Initially when food is abundant the curve rises
rapidly; later as food is depleted it levels off. Next, quantify the travel time between food patches
and determine an average travel time (t). Starting at point t, draw a line to the right that is tangent
to the cumulative food gain curve. From the point of tangency, drop a line to the x-axis. The
intersection of that line with the x-axis gives the predicted optimal time (T) for the forager to
remain in the patch, optimizing benefit/cost ratio
4.2 Foraging Strategies 145
generally poor quality when the forager enters one, it actually will stay longer than
if it were foraging in an environment full of more profitable patches. This is because
in a poor patch, it takes longer to accumulate the amount of energy needed to make
up for the travel costs associated with a move.
In some cases, researchers have been able to gain insights into the nature of the
selective forces acting upon a selected insect species by building models for various
alternative foraging decisions and comparing the predictions of each model with
its observed behavior. For both practical and theoretical reasons, a major focus of
insect research on marginal value theorem concepts has been with parasitoid wasps,
which lay their eggs on insect hosts that are distributed in discrete patches in the
environment. Many of these parasitoid-host systems lend themselves easily to labo-
ratory manipulations of pertinent factors such as host quality and host and parasite
density. Applied ecologists have hoped to gain information that could inform bio-
logical control measures; population ecologists have seen these insects as a way to
explore the dynamics of victim-enemy relationships.
Models such as those provided by optimality theory and the marginal value the-
orem should be seen for what they are: not some sort of inviolate truths, but instead,
valuable research tools. Rarely has there been a perfect quantitative fit between
model predictions and empirical test results. Still, their use has yielded valuable
insights and sometimes has led in unexpected directions. For example, optimal
host selection models predict that when an insect has its choice of two foods or
hosts of different potential values, the choices it makes between them depend on its
encounter rate with the more profitable species. If the insect’s encounter rate with
the more profitable species is above some threshold value, the less profitable species
should always be ignored; if encounters with the more profitable species are below
this value, the less profitable one should always be attacked.
Studies with a braconid parasitoid Asobara tabida that attacks Drosophila show
both the ways such studies work, and the potential pitfalls involved with laboratory
experiments that purport to represent natural field behavior. At the high population
densities typical of most laboratory experiments, Asobara is selective when offered
the choice between two host species that differ in survival probability for its off-
spring; it also avoids superparasitism. However, as is probably true for most insects,
encounter rates under natural conditions are much lower. When Asobara encoun-
ters only one host or less per hour, the wasp always accepts either host and also
superparasitizes.
Prey-catching wasps show a relatively constant host stinging sequence that
appears to be genetically programmed rather than learned; in all known cases, the
first sting is directed at ganglia involved in locomotion and defense, in particular
paralyzing the prey’s legs, which might otherwise be used as powerful defensive
weapons. Then the prey is transported back to the nest in various ways, often with
considerable effort. However, the wasp Ampulex compressa has evolved an effi-
cient alternative, zombification; its cockroach prey (usually Periplaneta americana)
follows it home like a submissive dog on a leash.
To accomplish this feat, Ampulex relies upon two injections of venom. The first,
delivered into the thorax, transiently paralyzes the roach’s prothoracic legs. The
146 4 Foraging and Feeding
second, given very precisely into the neck, targets the cerebral ganglia. In response,
rather than fleeing, the roach stays put, frantically grooming itself, while the wasp
cuts the roach’s antennae with its mandibles and feeds on its hemolymph. Then
Ampulex grabs one of the roach’s antennal stumps and, walking backward, leads the
unprotesting roach back to a pre-selected burrow for oviposition. The stung roach
follows the wasp inside and lethargically waits while the wasp glues an egg to its
cuticle, then exits and closes the burrow entrance with small pebbles. For days, while
the egg hatches and the larva feeds, the roach remains alive but passive; it only dies
after the pupa finally becomes ensconced in its abdomen.
Researchers have become quite interested in the proximate mechanisms involved
in zombification. To resolve whether the venom is injected directly into the central
nervous system or simply diffuses in from the hemolymph, Frederic Libersat and
colleagues injected Ampulex with a mixture of carbon-14-radiolabeled amino acids;
they found that the radioactive signal was positioned in the central nervous system
so precisely that they compared it to the most advanced stereotactic administration
of drugs.
How can a small change in one particular brain center, protein, or gene lead
to such a dramatic effect? In most cases the mechanisms that underly such a dra-
matic behavioral change are unknown. In a first step toward an answer, D. G. Biron
and colleagues have been examining the observed behavioral changes in the insect
host using proteomics. A rather young discipline, proteomics can be defined as the
qualitative and quantitative comparison of proteomes (genome operating systems)
under different conditions to unravel biological processes. Because it allows one to
study the host-parasite interaction at the genetic level precisely in the midst of the
manipulative process, the technique appears very promising as a tool for exploring
the proximate mechanisms responsible for host manipulation.
Fig. 4.6 Trapping supper. Ambush by the worm-lion larva, Vermileo comstocki. In frames (1)
and (2), the larva makes a pit; then it hides at the base of the pit (3). An approaching ant (4) slides
into the pit, where it is seized and paralyzed (5). Hauled under the surface of the sand, the ant’s
body fluids are sucked out by the worm lion (6)
weaving but also quite a wide range of other ambush methods. In a similar vein,
many predatory insects such as tiger beetles, ant lions, and worm lions construct
devices such as pits or snares that increase the probability of locating and captur-
ing prey (Fig. 4.6). Considerable time and energy are often invested in the building
and maintenance of such devices. However, they can result in a good return in food
which otherwise would be so widely dispersed as to be uneconomical to wait for
and perhaps difficult to catch as well.
Lures can improve the odds that dinner will appear. Certain assassin bugs have
forelegs covered with hairs that exude small droplets of a highly viscous substance
that looks like dew; when the assassin bug spots small, fast-moving prey such as fruit
flies, it raises its forelegs and holds them parallel to the ground. Flies attracted to
the ‘dew’ become entrapped and quickly are consumed. The Javan bug, Ptilocerus,
has a tuft of bright-red hairs on its body, marking the spot where a gland opens
beneath the abdomen; secretions from this gland are very attractive to ants. However,
after partaking of these secretions, an ant collapses, apparently from narcotic action,
whereupon the bug pierces the ant through the neck and promptly sucks it dry.
Attracting prey often involves some sort of deceit or false sensory message.
Certain Malaysian preying mantids greatly resemble flowers; unsuspecting prey
attracted visually to these ‘blossoms’ become a quick meal. Similarly, female
Photuris fireflies flash false coded messages to sexually lure unrelated males,
148 4 Foraging and Feeding
which are then eaten. The predatory larvae of many green lacewings have the
habit of covering themselves with camouflaging debris of one type or another; in
the case of Chrysopa slossonae, this disguise allows access to the woolly aphids
that are its sole diet without alerting the aphids’ ant guards. These behaviors are
described as aggressive mimicry; despite the superficial similarity in terminology,
however, they are quite different from the classical types of mimicry treated in
Chapter 5.
Obviously, a lie-in-wait strategy requires that the food itself be mobile. Usually
this implies a diet upon animal prey. However, when wind or water currents exist,
small plants and microorganisms may become an important diet item for lie-in-wait
food gatherers. Trichoptera larvae of the family Hydropsychidae use nets spun with
their silk glands to capture drifting food particles in streams. These feeding nets
sometimes have an exceedingly fine mesh, allowing the caddisfly larvae to graze
upon fine particulate organic matter, phytoplankton, and bacteria strained out of
the moving water. The evolution of hydropsychid larvae reveals a tendency toward
more complicated larval feeding structures and smaller capture-net mesh size. In
some species, plant detritus may comprise more than half of the diet of younger
instar individuals.
Little wasps in the genus Copidosoma, which lay their eggs in the eggs and young
larvae of noctuid moths, regularly produce brood of over 2,000 young. Tsetse flies
have been dubbed the world’s least specific bloodsucker; they will apparently feed
indiscriminately upon any vertebrate they encounter. The wasp Ichneumon eumerus
attacks the caterpillars of Maculinea rebeli that live inside the brood chambers of
ant nests; to reach them, the wasp releases chemicals that cause the ants to lock into
combat, leaving the caterpillars defenseless and available for oviposition.
Organisms that feed upon a host that they do not kill are termed parasites.
Examples include mosquitoes, lice, bed bugs, fleas, and the many other insects
that feed on humans and other animals that often have significant health and med-
ical impacts. Haematophagy or the utilization of blood as food occurs in six insect
orders. The order Diptera has the largest number of haematophagous species and
includes both ectoparasitic forms that feed from the animal’s body surface and
endoparasitic forms that feed from within. Distinctions can be drawn even more
finely, as for example between ectoparasites that stay permanently on their hosts
and those that are off their hosts except when feeding, or between ectoparasites that
are the most to the least host-specific with regard to both their host location and host
preference patterns.
Parasitism is a major life style, occurring in so many of the world’s organisms that
it deserves, and has, coverage in textbooks of its own. Here, we will restrict mention
to some of the relatively unusual insects that are external and internal parasites of
other insects. Their parasitism can sometimes have enormous effects on the behavior
of their hosts.
4.2 Foraging Strategies 149
First, however, a distinction must be made between the true parasites and another
much larger group of insects that live at the expense of another insect (host) that
is always consumed and dies as a result. Members of this group (which includes
the so-called ‘parasitic’ Hymenoptera and some Diptera and Coleoptera) are termed
parasitoids, and like true parasites they may feed either externally (ectoparasitoids)
(Fig. 4.7) or internally (endoparasitoids). Predators also consume their prey, of
course, but parasitoids require only a single host individual to complete their
development; predators generally require more than one prey. Most parasitoids are
parasitic only in the larval stage, being free-living as an adult, but in the real world
exceptions to strict categorization are not unusual.
Parasitoids may be solitary or gregarious in their development. Some paralyze
their host and develop rapidly, a situation that differs only slightly from predation.
Others manipulate host physiology in ways that permit the host to continue to grow
and develop after the parasitoid lays its eggs; meanwhile parasitoid development
may be concurrently delayed until the host becomes sufficiently large enough to
sustain it.
Studies of parasitoid-host interactions are fundamental for implementing biolog-
ical control of pest insects. A large body of published literature addresses aspects
of parasitoid biology for a wide range of species. Some of these are discussed
elsewhere in this book, in contexts as varied as enemies of solitary wasps, host
finding and learning host odors, foraging strategies, host marking, courtship, and as
a selective force in parental care.
In considering parasites and parasitoids, one once again is faced with a choice of
viewpoints. Whereas many researchers study the proximate mechanisms involved
in such behaviors as predation and parasitism (such as the zombification already
Strepsiptera attack many kinds of insects in at least seven different orders, with
hymenopterans being one of their more preferred hosts. It should be noted in pass-
ing that communal and social insect colonies must present a particularly appealing
target for parasites, given their high density of potential hosts of a similar genotype.
However, successfully breaking through these social defenses is generally so diffi-
cult that colonies have been called ‘factory fortresses’. Studies of social wasps have
suggested that nest aspects such as multiple comb construction and nest envelopes
may have evolved in part to provide some defense against parasitoids and preda-
tors. Behavior is also important; for example, in Chapter 1 we discussed honey bee
defense against brood diseases.
Every aspect of Strepsiptera that has been studied has proven to be highly
unusual in some aspect, from their bizarre and complex life cycles to their genome,
which is one of the smallest known for any insect, although the size of the
entire 18S ribosomal DNA sequence is among the biggest, containing a number
of unique expansion segments. No aspect, however, is more unusual than the many
ways in which these parasites alter the morphology, physiology, and behavior of
their hosts.
Striking changes in host morphology are often evident even before adults become
visible. The parasites’ presence may change the color and shape of the host’s
abdomen. In many host species, both male and female become sterile, having
been effectively castrated by their parasites. Females of solitary bees often have
their pollen collecting baskets greatly reduced, and the sting is also often reduced
in size. In males, the copulatory apparatus is often greatly reduced. Male and
female hosts often appear to have acquired some appearances of the opposite
sex.
Behavioral changes also occur. In ants, unhealthy workers become diurnal, aban-
don their nests, and climb high on grasses and bushes. When a Stylops female is
releasing larvae, her normally slow-flying Andrena bee host will suddenly race
from flower to flower, dragging her abdomen through the stamens and spreading
triungulins that other foraging bees will take into their crops, becoming vectors for
infection.
It was with this background in mind that Laura Beani and William Hamilton
watched an aggregation of Polistes behaving strangely in the surroundings of her
house in Italy. It looked like an overwintering aggregation (see Plate 5) but it was too
early in the season. At Hamilton’s urging, Beani began to investigate their unusual
behavior. From her own inspection and previous observations by others, she was
able to piece together a remarkable story. The wasps in the aggregation were para-
sitized by the strepsipteran Xenos vesparum, and their strange behavior was part of
an even bigger picture of aberrant acts that spanned the seasons.
After overwintering, affected Polistes females did not form a stable association
with healthy nest foundresses; they showed no inclination to nest or to act as helpers.
However, some made unusual short-range ‘migrations’ from one nest to another.
Resting on the nest combs, they were occasionally attacked by the nest’s owners,
but usually they were just ignored.
152 4 Foraging and Feeding
In the summer, wasps hung around listlessly in unusual aggregations like those
Hamilton and Beani had observed; these were always away from the nest, often
at lekking or pre-hibernant sites. The groups were initially and mostly formed by
workers, not sexuals, and though aggregations might persist for days, individual
turnover rates were high.
When Beani artificially infected wasps with strepsipterans and moved the wasps
into large cages in the laboratory, these infected workers deserted the colony early.
Though they were of no help at the nest, they were not evicted by healthy wasps,
but left to their own volition. When she caged naturally parasitized and unparasitized
wasps together, the stylopized wasps hung out sluggishly in the corners, ignored by
the healthy ones, and they lived a long time, unusual since workers normally die and
do not overwinter.
What does all this mean? It appears that the Xenos/Polistes interaction represents
a case of adaptive manipulation. How might such diverse alterations enhance par-
asite success? By castrating its hosts, Xenos shifts the host’s resource allocations
from reproductive organs to energy reserves that can directly benefit the parasite.
By causing infected female wasps to cluster in groups while healthy gynes scatter
to occupy good nesting sites in early spring, triungulin-laden overwintered females
essentially wait within an aposematically defended herd until healthy wasp larvae
are available to be parasitized. Likewise, the high turnover rate in the strange sum-
mer aggregations can be explained by the fact that these are mate-encounter sites
for the parasite, not for the castrated hosts.
If only for curiosity, one must, however, return to a proximate-mechanism ques-
tion. How do the parasites do this? In Polistes, normal worker behavior is known
to depend on juvenile hormone. The stylopization syndrome resembles the pre-
hibernation physiological state of overwintering wasps, when they are in a diapause
ovarian state that can be broken by applying juvenile hormone. Studies have shown
that the strepsipterans alter juvenile hormone production, not the wasps’ sensitivity
to the hormone, by their infection. Strepsipterans are the only parasitoids that cause
a host’s life span to be lengthened to allow for the parasitoid’s life cycle completion.
What about looking at the wasps’ behavior from a colony perspective? Some
earlier observers, noting how infected workers leave their nest to aggregate, sug-
gested that this nest desertion could be interpreted as an ‘altruistic act’ (see Chapter
10) because it reduces the infection of kin. However, only wasps carrying Xenos
females with triungulins are vectors for the next generation; these have never been
found in these aggregations.
are used as a food source or placed in brood chambers; these dung balls must be
rapidly moved away from their source, because other dung beetles will abscond with
them.
Individuals of the same species usually feed on the same type of food in much
the same way, and even the diets of other closely related species may not differ by
much. This may not cause much problem in situations where food is abundant and
evenly distributed. (Why put out the energy to fight when there is nothing additional
to be gained by winning?) However, animals that live on patchily distributed foods
can experience considerable competition and even overt aggression. When compe-
tition is intraspecific, it sometimes leads to the establishment of social hierarchies
and/or territoriality, systems that reduce the expression of aggression between group
members.
When competition is between members of different species, extreme conflicts are
expected. These conflicts exemplify the well-known ecological competitive exclu-
sion principle: no two species can coexist in the same locale and utilize the same
resources at the same time. The less successful competitor will either be driven to
extinction or be forced to modify its feeding pattern. Modification is probably the
more common outcome. An insect species may switch to a slightly different type of
food, feed at a different time of day or night, or forage in a slightly different area.
As a result, such competition promotes evolutionary divergence in food choice and
feeding patterns.
Cuckoo bees and cuckoo wasps deposit their eggs in the nests of other bees and
wasps. These insects are normally referred to as kleptoparasites, sometimes spelled
cleptoparasites. (Their counterparts the cuckoo birds are a special case of kleptopar-
asitism known as ‘brood parasites’ because the immature parasite is fed directly by
the adult of the host and raised as the host’s offspring; this situation often occurs in
birds but is almost unknown among insects.)
In the broadest sense, both parasites and kleptoparasites can be thought of as
predatory thieves. Parasitism is a type of thievery in which one organism attacks
another, stealing its energy stores and sometimes its life. In contrast, a kleptoparasite
attacks the host’s young not primarily as food but rather to free up the host’s food
store for its own offspring. In many cases kleptoparasitism probably began with
simple prey stealing, such as the brigandage reported often among solitary wasps.
Many kleptoparasites are closely related to their hosts and appear to have shared
a recent common ancestry. Several species of a spider wasp, Ceropales, enter the
nest burrow of close relatives after the rightful owner has prepared and stocked the
hole with paralyzed spiders and laid an egg; inside the burrow, they lay a cleverly
concealed egg of their own upon the stored prey (Fig. 4.8). Some Ceropales species
substitute their own egg after eating the host egg themselves; others leave the host
egg for their own young to eat.
Theft can be a great strategy when it works. Small flies that look superficially
like house flies (see Fig. 1.14) are among the most abundant parasites of solitary
wasps. Each of the several genera involved has its particular method of attack, but
all of them produce eggs that hatch just before they are laid. These young maggots
attack and destroy the wasp’s egg or larva, but the maggots develop primarily upon
154 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.8 Hiding the baby. The kleptoparasitic Australian spider wasp Ceropales ligea (left) hides
its eggs in the book lungs of the spider prey used by its host, the wasp Elaphrosyron socius (right).
Oviposition occurs very rapidly when the wasp briefly drops the spider in order to clear the entrance
to her nest burrow
the prey in the cell. Being ‘food thieves,’ the flies are not at all host-specific and
will attack species in several solitary wasp families indiscriminately if they are in
the proper ecological location.
There is no free ride, however, and anticipated victims often fight against such
theft. Many kleptoparasites have become armored in various ways, apparently as an
adaptation for deflecting the bites and stings of their intended hosts. For example,
the exoskeleton of cuckoo wasps (Chrysididae) is very hard and coarsely punctured
(see Plate 7); when disturbed or threatened, these wasps curl into a small tight ball
that is nearly impenetrable and difficult to grasp.
Leaf-cutting attine ants of the New World have entered into partnership with various
species of fungi that do not occur outside of their nests. A nest-founding queen has
a blind pouch off her pharynx in which to carry a small fungal mass to inoculate the
garden in her newly established nest. The fungus will serve as the colony’s primary
and probably sole food source; these ants are unable to feed directly upon cellulose.
Under the ants’ care the fungus flourishes in the underground nest chambers, but if
ants are denied access to this garden, it rapidly deteriorates. The leaf cutters file out
along well-defined trails to cut leaves, flowers, and stems to be transported to the
nest, where each will be cleaned and scraped, then chewed into a small pulpy mash
with salivary secretions. As the ants insert it into the fungus garden, they cover the
4.2 Foraging Strategies 155
mash with a transplant of several tufts of fungal mycelium and one or more drops of
fecal ‘fertilizer.’ This fecal material contains significant quantities of all 21 natural
amino acids, a nutrient supplement of considerable importance, as well as providing
the fungus with a proteolytic enzyme that breaks polypeptides in the leafy mass
into forms that are usable by the fungus. The ants have even evolved a means to
protect their food source from destruction, transporting and cultivating a crust of
Streptomyces bacteria that produce antibiotics against serious bacterial parasites of
the fungus.
This example is classic, but not alone—mutualistic feeding arrangements such
as this are relatively common. Termites, ants, and beetles appear to have become
fungiculturists by different evolutionary routes (Fig. 4.9). In termites it is believed
that fungi were an important food source before cultivation, and fungiculture arose
when the termites developed an ability to manipulate fungal growth in their nests.
In contrast, ancestral beetles may have begun as vectors for fungal spores, as
many non-fungus-feeding relatives of ambrosia beetles still are now. It is not
clear which of these evolutionary pathways might have given rise to fungiculture
in ants.
The parallels with farming have intrigued observers for centuries. Other exam-
ples involve plants and ‘livestock’ such as aphids. Cornfield ants (Lasius spp.) gather
eggs of the corn root aphid, Anuraphis, in the fall and take them home for the winter.
All winter the ants solicitously move the eggs about the nest, keeping them at appro-
priate temperature and humidity. When the aphid eggs hatch in the spring, the ants
place the young aphids near weed roots to feed. Later in the season, these solicitous
caretakers move their aphid herd to corn roots, dispersing them around the cornfield
where their feeding can cause considerable damage.
Many insects have a symbiotic relationship with microorganisms. Sometimes,
as in this leaf-cutting ant example, the microsymbionts live outside the insect’s
body, though they may be temporarily stored in special organs of ectodermal origin
for purposes of dissemination. A special type of ectosymbiotic relationship occurs
among some of the wood-inhabiting insects—the ambrosia beetles, wood wasps
of the families Siricidae and Xiphydriidae, and some bark-feeding bark beetles.
Sometimes, the fungus is eaten together with wood particles; in the most impressive
cases the insects feed upon their fungus alone.
Ambrosia beetles generally have an extremely wide host range but tend to occur
as secondary insects on diseased trees or felled logs. Each species is symbiotically
associated with one or more specific fungi indispensable for the development of
its brood. Identification of fungal symbionts is a difficult matter because they do
not produce fruiting bodies; however, it appears that the true ambrosia fungi are
highly specialized forms that cannot grow in the host plant in the absence of the
symbiotic insect. In return, the fungus provides a rich and available food containing
important vitamins for the insect and its larvae. How is fungus transmitted from
an old tree or log to a new one? Early workers thought spores were simply carried
in the beetle’s gut or upon its integument. It now appears, however, that certain
specialized organs of variable location and structure but usually confined to one sex
are involved. They serve to protect the fungi from desiccation, provide secretions
156 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.9 Farming fungus. Comparison of the patterns of evolutionary diversification in three
major groups of fungus-farming insects—termites, ants, and ambrosia beetles—and their cultivated
fungi food. In the left cladograms, farmer lineages are shown in black and non-farmer lineages in
gray; in the right cladograms, fungal cultivar lineages are shown in black and non-cultivated feral
fungi in gray. Photos depict typical examples of each group: (d) Macrotermes bellicosus fungal
garden grown on walls of termite fecal pellets; (e) Atta cephalotes queen resting on her incipient
fungal garden; (f) Trypodendron lineatum gallery with brood niches showing the black ambrosia
fungus lining the walls
necessary for germination and arthrospore formation, and ensure the mechanical
dissemination of the fungus on the tunnel walls.
Wood wasps (Siricidae) also attack weakened trees or freshly cut logs. However,
their symbiosis with wood-inhabiting fungi is entirely different from ambrosia cul-
tivation. Only the larvae bore in the wood, but larval development may last two or
three years. During their adult stage, wood wasps are on the wing. Female wasps
possess intersegmental pouches at the base of the ventral parts of their stingers;
during oviposition, symbionts are pushed out with the egg.
4.2 Foraging Strategies 157
Whereas some insects live mutualistically with ectosymbionts, more often the
mutualism takes the form of an internal symbiosis, with the microorganism living
within one of several organs inside the insect’s body. Most of these endosymbionts
of insects are microscopic. Their discovery dates back to the nineteenth century, but
initially their existence as independent living beings was difficult to comprehend,
and they were variously described as sporozoa, yeasts, metabolic products, and yolk
spheres.
Partnering with endosymbionts has enabled insects to feed on a wide range of
nutritionally incomplete and/or indigestible matter. The first, and the most numer-
ous, category of endosymbiotic hosts includes insects that suck sap, which is rich
in carbohydrates but poor in protein—scale insects, leaf lice, and aleurodid and
psyllid ‘flies.’ Among the bugs (Hemiptera), predaceous forms possess no sym-
bionts, but those such as stink bugs (Pentatomidae) that have switched over to
plant juice for nourishment have symbionts. Blood-suckers are a second important
host category. All insects that suck vertebrate blood for the whole of their lives
have symbionts; this includes the bed bugs and their relatives. Vertebrate blood is
deficient in certain vitamins that the symbionts can synthesize. Mosquitoes are a
familiar exception; they need no symbionts because they suck blood only as adults
and have a bacteria-rich nourishment at their disposal during earlier developmen-
tal stages. A third group of insects with symbionts includes species that feed on
cellulose-rich substrates: most famously termites. Symbionts also occur among that
small group of insects whose whole life is spent on keratin-containing food, the
feather lice.
Endosymbionts pose an interesting problem for their hosts—they must be rigidly
retained, but they also must be passed on to offspring. Varied arrangements have
arisen for housing them; these are species specific in both origin and development.
Depending upon the location of these guests within their host, these include oral
uptake of symbionts by young brood, smearing of eggs with symbionts, or infection
of eggs or embryos before laying.
The nests of social insects provide living quarters for a diversity of creatures besides
those building them. Like our own homes despite our efforts to keep them clean,
insect nests are besieged by everything from cockroaches to flies. In addition to
these uninvited guests, some social insects purposely keep guests of other species in
their nests, much as we keep various pets.
Depending on their hosts’ identity, such symbionts or symphiles are referred to
as termitophiles, mellittophiles, etc. A particularly impressive range of vertebrate
and invertebrate species can be associated with long-lived termite and ant nests,
in particular, including organisms as diverse as microorganisms, plants, insects,
amphibians, reptiles, and mammals. In the case of species that live in the nests of
others of their own kind, the term ‘inquiline’ is used (see Chapter 10).
158 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.10 Begging like an ant. Larva of Atemeles beetle being fed by regurgitation from a worker
Formica ant. When the beetle larva secretes a chemical from a row of paired glands along the sides
of its abdomen, ants respond by intensively grooming the larva. In response, the beetle larva rears
up and attempts to make mouth-to-mouth contact with the ant
Nest guests are particularly frequent in the ants, where all manner of organ-
isms have specialized as myrmecophiles, literally, ‘ant lovers.’ Many bristletails
(Thysanura), for example, live with ants. Feasting on debris, food scraps, and even
ant corpses within their common dwelling, these bristletails reward the ants with a
‘tidy house’ in turn for being inadvertently fed by their ant hosts. When army ants
emigrate, bristletails may be seen running in the column or riding along upon ant
larvae and booty.
Beetles are also common in the nests of social insects. Sometimes, acting as
scavengers they appear to be ignored by the nest owners. In other cases, the ants not
only tolerate the interlopers but feed, groom, and rear their guests, as happens with
Atemeles rove beetles (Fig. 4.10; see Case Study 4.1).
In many cases, even when their intruders eat the host ants’ young, they are treated
cordially. For example, Formica sanguinea ants avidly seek out beetle larvae of
Lomechusa strumosa, even though the latter usually destroy the whole population
of the ant hill. William Morton Wheeler expressed the puzzle well when he stated:
Were we to behave in an analogous manner we should live in a truly Alice-in-Wonderland
society. We should delight in keeping porcupines, alligators, lobsters, etc., in our homes,
insist on their sitting down to the table with us and feed them so solicitously with spoon
victuals that our children would either perish of neglect or grow up as hopeless rhachitics.
4.2 Foraging Strategies 159
How do nest symbionts gain such acceptance? Clues are apparent in the case
of one well-studied example.
Among the beetle family Staphylinidae a number of myrmecophilous
relationships have evolved. Some of these beetles only live along ant food-
gathering trails or at ant garbage dumps outside the nest. Others live within the
nest’s outer chambers, but a select number have penetrated all the way inside
the brood chambers. Of this last group, one well-known example is Atemeles
pubicollis, a European rove beetle that spends its larval stage within the nest of
the mound-making wood ants, Formica polyctena. In examining their behav-
ior in some detail, Bert Hölldobler unraveled much of the communicative
behavior between host and symbiont.
Watching the behavior of ants encountering Atemeles larvae, Hölldobler
noticed that brood-tending ants respond to Atemeles by grooming them
intensely. Might the ants be imbibing something from the larval cuticle?
Hölldobler ran experiments with radioactive tracers, and confirmed that some
sort of chemical signal was passing between the ants and the larvae. To deter-
mine its action, he variably coated large numbers of beetle larvae with shellac,
then placed them at the nest entrance. As long as at least one body segment
was left unpainted, the ants carried larvae into the nest and adopted them.
When the entire larva was covered, however, the ants either ignored it or
deposited it in the garbage dump. Hölldobler washed some beetle larvae with
acetone; ants dumped most of these ‘deodorized’ larvae as well. Then he made
filter paper dummies soaked with the acetone ‘bath water’ and left them at the
nest entrance; like real larvae, many were carried into the nest. Paper dum-
mies soaked with fresh acetone, however, were either ignored or carried to the
dump.
Hölldobler turned his attention to a second puzzling behavior. Within the
brood chambers, adult ants scurry about feeding their young. When an adult
ant’s antennae or mouthparts touch an Atemeles larva, the latter rears up and
attempts to contact the ant’s head. When the attempt is successful, the ant
regurgitates a droplet of food (see Fig. 4.10). It appeared that the beetle larvae
were imitating the behavior of ant larvae. Wondering how the beetle’s success
rate compared with that of the ant larvae, Hölldobler gave the ants food labeled
with radioactive sodium phosphate. In a mixed brood of beetle and ant larvae,
the beetles obtained significantly more than their proportionate share of food.
Apparently, they were more intense as beggars than were the ant larvae. As
if this were not enough, the predacious beetle larvae actually ate small ant
larvae, as well. How could the ant colony survive such intense competition
and predation? Observation provided the answer—the beetle larvae were not
only predacious but cannibalistic. Unable to distinguish their fellow larvae
from ant larvae, they cut down their own population ruthlessly, whereas ant
160 4 Foraging and Feeding
larvae did not. Thus, the brood chambers soon contained clusters of ant larvae
but only a few scattered beetle larvae.
Further studies revealed even more behavioral complexity. Atemeles bee-
tles, it turns out, have not one but two ant homes: a summer woodland
domicile with Formica and a winter grassland one with Myrmica. After the
beetle larvae have pupated and eclosed in their original home, they beg for
one final, ample food supply. Drumming rapidly with their antennae upon
an ant to attract its attention, they touch the ant’s mouthparts with their own
maxillae and forelegs; since this mechanical signal is quite similar to that used
by ants among themselves, the ant responds by regurgitating food. Now fully
fed, the adult staphylinids begin to migrate out of the nest. Guided primarily
by light and odor, the beetles move into open grasslands and find Myrmica
nests. When a staphylinid encounters one, it wanders around until it encoun-
ters an ant worker. Going through a brief ritual, the beetle moves in, carried
by its host right into the brood chamber. Although the beetles are now adult,
they are still sexually immature. Within these latter nests, they continue to be
fed until sexual maturity the following spring, at which time they return to
Formica nests to mate and lay eggs.
To penetrate Myrmica nests, Atemeles must adopt a new set of skills and
a second language. Hölldobler again suspected chemical cues, because as the
guest and host first encounter one another, the beetle antennates the ant lightly,
then raises its abdomen toward its host. In response, the ant licks the tip of the
beetle’s abdomen, seeming to grow calmer in the process, then moves on to
the side of the abdomen. Finally, the beetle lowers its abdomen. The ant then
grasps bristles around the beetle’s sides and carries its tightly curled guest
inside. True to prediction, Hölldobler found two types of glands and secretions
(see Fig. 4.11). At the tip of the abdomen, ‘appeasement glands’ produce a
partially proteinaceous secretion that apparently suppresses aggressive behav-
ior in the ant. Along the sides, a series of ‘adoption glands’ produces a
chemical necessary if the ants are to welcome the beetle in. Apparently, this
odor mimics the odor of members of the ant species.
Chemistry plays a large role in the acceptance of adult Atemeles (see Case
Study 4.1), but this is not unusual. For example, ‘appeasement glands’ (Fig. 4.11)
turn out to be fairly widespread among the better-integrated myrmecophiles includ-
ing many of the ant nest beetles and lycaenid butterfly caterpillars. They are of a
number of novel forms (including glandular hairs) and exist in different locations.
Many types of glands aid in mediating myrmecophily, and appeasement devices are
just one of a syndrome of changes in morphology and behavior that social symbionts
belonging to various insect groups have independently undergone. Three are worth
special mention.
4.2 Foraging Strategies 161
Fig. 4.11 Getting adopted. Adult Atemeles beetles’ entry into nests of Myrmica ants is largely
chemically mediated. Encountering a potential host and worker, the beetle presents the tip of its
abdomen and taps the ant lightly with its antennae (1). The ant responds by licking the abdomen,
ingesting an ‘appeasement’ chemical from the ‘appeasement gland’ that apparently serves to sup-
press the ant’s normally aggressive behavior toward intruders (2). Next, the ant licks the sides of
the beetle’s abdomen where it obtains an ‘adoption’ secretion (3, 4) that releases its brood-carrying
behavior. It picks up the tightly curled beetle and carries it into the nest brood chamber (5) where
the beetle now has access to the ant’s brood
First, many symphiles look very much like their hosts. Commensal staphylinids,
in particular, strikingly resemble ants, with a slender body form, antlike ‘petiole,’
and even body sculpturing and color (Fig. 4.12); this antlike appearance is found
almost nowhere else in this large family of beetles. Early investigators working from
162 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.12 The habits of ant nest beetles (Carabidae: Paussinae) range from free-living and preda-
tory to obligatory myrmecophily, and these differences are reflected in their morphology. All these
beetles are displayed here with the right legs and antennae at their broadest view and the left
extremities at their narrowest view. Scale bar, 1 mm
museum specimens believed that the antlike body form of the symphiles was tactile
mimicry to deceive their host ants and that color was visual mimicry to deceive birds
and other predators that attack marching ant columns. However, for ants chemical
identification is paramount; if an ant’s surface odor is disturbed even slightly, other
workers will immediately attack it, even though its morphology has not changed.
4.2 Foraging Strategies 163
Therefore, it seems unlikely that such mimicry is tactile and directed toward ants.
Current thought is that predators watching ant columns for edible morsels have been
the selection agents for both the color and morphological mimicry.
A second adaptation characteristic of many unrelated symphiles is physogastry,
a condition in which the abdomen is greatly enlarged, particularly in its membra-
nous parts. Physogastry appears to be one of the major ways in which termitophiles,
particularly among Coleoptera and Diptera, mimic their hosts. To human eyes,
such swelling often makes them superficially resemble termites, but the question
of whether physogastry is a primarily visual or primarily chemical adaptation is dif-
ficult to answer because direct study of termites and termitophiles poses problems.
Termitophiles are extremely delicate, and it is usually necessary to crack open the
termite nest to reach them and then make one’s observations under quite artificial
laboratory conditions. In at least some cases, there appears to be a mutual exchange
of exudates between termites and termitophiles, and histological studies of some
termitophiles demonstrate the existence of various abdominal glands and pores.
Whether termitophiles and their exudates are beneficial to termites is unknown, but
it is clear that termitophiles depend on their hosts. Most physogastric species have
such rudimentary mouthparts that they could no longer feed themselves and such a
heavy dependence on the controlled temperature and humidity of the termite nest
that they do not live long if they are removed from it.
A third morphological adaptation of many termitophiles and myrmecophiles is
a limuloid (teardrop-shaped) body. Generally, it is assumed that this body form
has arisen for defense because it would be difficult for an ant or termite to grasp
the smoothly streamlined dorsum. However, actual field observations of limuloid
symphiles are even scarcer than those of physogastric species.
Termites, ants and other social Hymenoptera all care for their larvae progres-
sively and exhibit fundamentally similar ways of communicating; no really great
differences exist in colony size or in length or stability of colony life. However,
specialized symphiles are almost unknown in nests of the social bees and wasps.
The few mites, beetles, and flies that live as scavengers and brood commensals are
quite generalized in form and behavior by comparison with symbionts of ants and
termites. Most are probably either attacked by their hosts or treated indifferently.
Why this difference? Probably the most plausible explanation centers around nest
structure, location, and feeding habits. Ants and termites live in relatively open
systems rich in refuse. Many chambers and galleries go unguarded from time to
time, and the nest interiors are generally made of material not too different from
the immediate environment—soil and rotting vegetable matter. Furthermore, young
are reared clustered in groups. Social wasps and bees, in contrast, ‘run a tight ship’
and construct compact (and often tightly sealed) nests, typically in arboreal loca-
tions. Thick envelopes of carton or wax pose formidable obstacles; nest entrances,
often narrow and tightly guarded, may also be lined with sticky substances and/or
repellants. Young are reared individually in specially constructed cells that make it
more difficult for a symbiont to conceal itself. Furthermore, rubbish inside is usu-
ally sparse; even when detritus occurs, workers simply heave most of it out the nest
entrance.
164 4 Foraging and Feeding
Potato plants are members of the Solanaceae, a family of plants whose species
are often rich in alkaloids with effects that range from mildly irritating to fatal in
small quantities. Yet consider the common potato beetle, Leptinotarsa decemlin-
eata. Although the mother beetle actively chooses a site for egg laying, the larvae do
not usually begin feeding near the place of hatching. Instead, they begin searching,
guided mainly by vision, until they encounter a potato plant; at this point, they begin
randomly biting the substrate over which they are walking. Close-range olfactory
stimuli from the plant are attractive to the larvae. If the proper smells are present,
they clip off any hairs on the plant surface and pierce the epidermis. Tasting the
potato plant, they finally decide whether to reject it or begin feeding.
Despite a common misconception, coevolution does not necessarily enhance
mutual harmony or promote stable coexistence of species. Obtaining, maintaining
and protecting an energy pool is the major preoccupation of every individual in
any species. Sharing this energy with organisms of other species is evolutionarily
maladaptive unless one gains, or expects to gain, at least indirect benefit in return.
Therefore, an insect should protect its own energy pool against other would-be con-
sumers and at the same time try to break through the defenses of other species,
particularly those at a lower trophic level, to take advantage of a lucrative poten-
tial energy source. For each species, this results in a more or less continual battle
of attack and defense or counterattack, waged over the ultimate trophy of enough
energy for successful reproduction.
Plants likewise seek to protect themselves, often by chemical means, but many
insects turn this around and use it to their own advantage. Odor normally mediates
166 4 Foraging and Feeding
the first steps in herbivory, determining whether the insect detects the plant as a
potential host for sampling and whether it takes the first bite. Removing or block-
ing olfactory organs in a variety of insects leads to acceptance of non-host plants
otherwise treated as inedible.
Considerable experimental and observational evidence confirms that a particu-
lar subset of plant chemicals are utilized by insects as distinctive cues to identify
and discriminate among their host plants for feeding and breeding. They include
alkaloids, terpenoids, essential oils, and quinones. Terpenoids are particularly
widely represented. The lower molecular weight representatives include highly
volatile compounds that function as floral scents that attract pollinators. Those with
higher molecular weights include plant resins, cardiac glycosides, and saponines
that are typically distasteful or toxic to herbivores.
Because these chemicals, which are of almost universal occurrence in plants,
included a diverse array that were not known to have any function in plant growth
or metabolism, early researchers called them ‘secondary substances’. One view
held that they were either metabolic by-products (waste products) or metabolic pre-
cursors needed for some as yet unknown physiological functions. Another school
contended that such metabolically expensive ‘curiosities,’ which are often produced
in large quantities, have a primary significance to the plant as defensive substances,
and to label them as ‘secondary’ was in a sense an ironic injustice.
From the 1950s on, evidence has rapidly accumulated in favor of the latter inter-
pretation. Some authorities prefer the term ‘plant natural products’ for this reason.
However, the term ‘natural product’ also has come to mean a chemical compound
or substance produced in nature that has a pharmacological or biological activity of
potential use in pharmaceutical drug discovery and drug design. Many of the sec-
ondary substances do in fact fit this definition, but others do not (at least yet). At
present, secondary plant metabolites (SPMs) seems to be the preferred name for
these chemicals. The term ‘secondary’ distinguishes them from ‘primary’ metabo-
lites, the removal of which results in immediate death to the organism. Sometimes,
elimination of a secondary plant metabolite may have no apparent effect at all, but
more often closer study reveals that removal can impair long-term survivability or
fecundity.
Whatever one chooses to call them, to an insect these chemicals function widely
as signals of edibility, either by their presence or by their absence. Even though they
may differ widely in other respects, the food plants of a particular insect species,
genus, or even family often share similar secondary plant metabolites. The con-
centration of these chemicals often differs in various parts of the plant in ways
that seem to correlate with the need to protect them from attack. For example,
the natural insecticide pyrethrin is concentrated in flower heads, which seems log-
ical because protecting future seeds would seem more important to a plant than
protecting individual leaves.
Some of the best SPM examples occur in the cabbage family Cruciferae, which
includes many garden vegetables from turnips and radishes to broccoli and kale.
The pungent smell and tastes of cruciferous plants are due to mustard oil glucosides,
particularly sinigrin, that cause most insects to consider them inedible. However, for
4.3 Coevolution and the Arms Race 167
some species, the reverse is true; experimental studies show that sinigrin acts as a
feeding stimulant for the turnip aphid and the cabbage aphid, and these species con-
sider plants inedible that lack the chemical. (Interestingly, inedible does not always
equate to non-nutritious. When leaves of 10 non-host plant species were treated
with sinigrin, aphids readily fed upon them all; five of the plant species turned out
to be nutritionally adequate to support the growth and development of at least one
generation of aphids.)
Producing strong chemicals is a knife that cuts two ways, however. Plant defen-
sive compounds can substantially reduce herbivore feeding, but these compounds
may be energetically expensive to produce, and their production competes with the
production of other necessary compounds and tissues. Moreover, insects also iden-
tify plants by their chemical fingerprints, and many have come to use plant volatiles
for their own purposes. One way in which plants can balance the tradeoff is to pro-
duce two groups of chemical defenses. One is a chemical fingerprint made up of
constitutive defenses that are always present; it tends to consist primarily of rel-
atively unspecific but generally effective compounds. If and when actual damage
occurs, however, these general compounds are joined by inducible defenses that
are more specific to particular types of injury. Bark beetles on pine trees provide a
classic example. During the initial beetle assault, the trees produce pitch, a gummy
terpenoid-containing mix that prevents bark penetration and seals wounds. However,
if the beetles successfully break through the bark, the tree produces more complex
phenols that can kill the beetle and cause lesions to form in the tissues surrounding
the wound.
Other herbivores steal the plant’s chemical defenses for protection against their
own enemies. The relatively inert exoskeleton provides an ideal site for passive stor-
age of toxic compounds. A famous example is the monarch butterfly whose larvae
sequester vertebrate heart poisons from their milkweed host plant; later, stored in
the butterflies’ wings, the chemicals confer protection from many birds. Some go
well beyond passive defense. Some sawfly larvae that feed upon foliage of conif-
erous trees store the resinous defensive chemicals in diverticular pouches in their
foreguts and regurgitate the fluid to repel predators. Likewise, some coccinellid
beetles store plant toxins derived from their prey in their hemolymph, and can
bleed the toxic fluid reflexively from their leg joints. We’ll return to this subject in
Chapters 5 and 6.
Still other insects use secondary plant metabolites for sexual communication.
Some male nymphalid butterflies congregate to feed at the dead shoots of plants
containing dehydropyrrolizine alkaloids and then biochemically modify the ingested
alkaloids to produce chemicals used during courtship. Many moths similarly have
an intricate relationship with plant chemistry. Likewise, virgin female polyphemus
moths will not begin their sexual call unless stimulated by trans-2-hexenal which
emanates from leaves of their host plant, oak. (In other tree species, the activity of
this chemical is apparently masked by other odors.)
Insect responses to plant chemicals take many forms (Table 4.1), but generally
can be divided into two categories: immediate and delayed reactions. So far, we
have concentrated on immediate responses, which are basically behavioral. Now,
168 4 Foraging and Feeding
Table 4.1 Some ways that plant-produced chemicals may affect plant-eating insects
let’s briefly consider delayed reactions, which are largely physiological and include
developmental anomalies, toxicity effects, and hormonal changes.
Insect development and reproduction are governed primarily by two hormones—
molting hormone (ecdysone) and juvenile hormone. A large number of plants
produce their own version of these hormones. Some of these analogues are
extremely potent; plant-produced ecdysones have been identified that are as much as
20 times more active than the ecdysones produced by the insects themselves. These
hormone mimics are highly disruptive to insect development, typically preventing
maturation or producing imperfect and sterile adults. They can function as potent
deterrents and anti-feeding agents, sometimes even at dosages strikingly lower than
expected. (As might be expected, however, some insects have evolved enzymes
capable of detoxifying such compounds.)
Research is finding that many ferns and gymnosperms produce ecdysone ana-
logues. There is a common myth that ginkgo trees and ferns are free from insect
attack; as survivors of relatively ancient plant groups, they are said to have outlived
all their enemies. As with all myths, there is a kernel of truth. Chemical analysis of
ginkgo leaves has confirmed that the leaves are highly acidic and inhibitory to insect
feeding. The ‘bullet-proof’ reputation of ferns has not held up as well. A number of
common and widespread ferns are almost insect-free because they produce high
phytoecdysone concentrations, but other fern species quite commonly suffer attacks
from insects that range from bark beetles to Lepidoptera larvae (Fig. 4.13).
Certain plants also have mimicked that other major insect hormone class, the
juvenile hormones. A well-known example is the so-called ‘paper factor’ story.
The setting for this episode was the Harvard University laboratory of Carol M.
Williams in 1964, when Karl Slàma came from Czechoslovakia to spend a year
and brought along his favorite laboratory insect, the native European fire bug,
Pyrrhocoris apterus. Very soon it became clear that cultures of Pyrrhocoris were
not faring well in William’s laboratory. Instead of metamorphosing into adults at the
end of the fifth instar, the fire bugs molted to an extra larval stage to form giant sixth
4.3 Coevolution and the Arms Race 169
Fig. 4.13 Feeding on ferns. Christmas fern frond, knotted (corner insert) at the tip as a result of
feeding by a Herpetogramma aeglealis (Pyralidae) moth larva (arrow). The larva notches the stem
so that it coils, but leaves the vascular bundles of the rhachis intact. No one yet knows whether this
is an adaptation to avoid plant-produced ecdysones
instar nymphs, which ultimately died without becoming sexually mature. Every pre-
caution had been taken to eliminate all sources of hormone contamination from
the culture containers, yet the problem seemed to be clearly of a hormonal nature.
Baffled, Slàma and Williams undertook a systematic analysis of every item in the
bug’s culture environment, searching for evidence of suspected juvenile hormone.
By a process of elimination the culprit was at last found—the paper toweling used to
line the cage floors. When the researchers substituted the filter paper that Sláma had
always used in Prague, the bugs again developed normally. Further detective work
traced the origin of the hormone to paper pulp derived from the Balsam fir (Abies
balsamea), a principal pulp tree indigenous to North America. This bit of serendip-
ity was the first indication of the existence of juvenile hormonal materials occurring
naturally in plants. As with ecdysone, juvenile hormone analogs have since turned
up in a diverse array of plants.
Whereas ferns and gymnosperms reply primarily on insect hormone analogues
and terpenoids for defense, angiosperms more commonly produce alkaloids, phe-
nols, and many other compounds. In fact, just as it is often said that ‘no two
snowflakes are alike,’ many scientists postulate that each plant species has a unique
‘chemical fingerprint’ conferred by its own mix of secondary plant metabolites.
Plants also produce analogues of essential amino acids; over 300 of these are
known, primarily from legume seeds. These are potent defenses against insect feed-
ing, because proteins constructed with these analogues are toxic, interfering with
many physiological processes. A particularly potent group of these analogues are
the alkaloids, which have been identified from about 20 percent of higher plant
families. Used with extreme medical care, many of these (such as atropine, bel-
ladonna, digitalis, strychnine) are familiar pharmaceuticals, but even at relatively
low concentrations they are highly toxic and teratogenic.
170 4 Foraging and Feeding
Not all plant defenses are chemical, of course. An arsenal of spines, thorns,
pubescences, and tough cuticles characterizes many plants from cactus to acacias.
Such structural features of plant surfaces have long been assumed to confer a cer-
tain measure of resistance to herbivore attack, and their prevalence seems to bear
testimony to the intensity of herbivore pressure. (However, some may have arisen
in response to other selective pressures; for example, the waxy texture of succulent
leaves undoubtedly helps to prevent desiccation.)
Physical barriers can be very effective. Female cereal leaf beetles lay signifi-
cantly more eggs on smooth-leaved strains of wheat than on strains with leaves
covered in dense, fine hairs; furthermore, larval survival is much lower on densely
pubescent plants. Various morphological traits of the bean family (Leguminosae)—
including such devices as flaky-surfaced pods that scale off eggs, gummy sap, and
pods that explode when penetrated—lower the success of bruchid weevils attempt-
ing to oviposit in or on the seed pods. The foliage of many species of Neotropical
passion flower vines is eaten by larvae of flashy heliconiine butterflies. However,
when vines of one species, Passiflora adenopoda, were exposed to Heliconius
attack in the laboratory, no feeding damage occurred. In fact, the larvae that had
been placed on the plants were found dead and desiccated by the next day, even
though other Passiflora species in the laboratory were heavily eaten. The cause of
the immunity of P. adenopoda was traced to the cloak of hooked trichomes (hairs)
that cover the plant’s surface. Scanning electron micrographs verified their defensive
function—the trichomes made numerous puncture wounds in the larval integument,
immobilizing the larvae and causing them to starve to death.
When they occur together, mechanical deterrents and chemical ones provide
considerable plant defense against feeding, but as always, some insects have
turned the tables. Camphorweed (Heterotheca psammophila), a common road-
side plant of the American southwest, has leaves, stems and buds covered with
tiny glandular hairs which produce droplets of aromatic resin that effectively
deter most insect feeding. However, the female of a reduviid bug Apiomerus
spends hours harvesting these droplets, which she then uses to fasten her eggs
into a resin-covered cluster. The resin, a complex mixture of terpenes, deters ants
and other egg-eating insects and may protect the eggs from microbial pathogens
as well.
One of the stranger types of defense employed by plants against herbivores uses
other organisms as a front guard or standing army. These mercenaries are paid, in
turn, through benefits they receive from the plant. Mutualistic relationships between
organisms are widespread. Some of the best illustrations may be found among the
ants, many of which have become specifically adapted to live upon certain plants.
One of the best known is that between ants and a diverse assortment of sap-sucking
plant bugs that excrete sugary drips of semi-processed phloem sap called ‘honey-
dew’ as they feed. Various ants exploit the concentrated food bonanza in such a
4.3 Coevolution and the Arms Race 171
systematic fashion that some early naturalists likened it to cattle farming. In return
for the aphids’ honeydew—produced not only passively during feeding but also
on demand in response to stroking by the ant—the aphids receive protection from
their predators and other services (see Plate 10). These ant-aphid protection systems
are generally very well integrated. For example, when ants are exposed to artifi-
cial sources of the green peach aphid’s alarm odor, they respond aggressively. Ants
guarding Prociphilus will even readily attack a human finger that approaches their
wooly aphid flock.
In a very real sense, plant-ants may be regarded as analogous to the secondary
compounds found in the foliage of most plants, the primary defense of their plant
against herbivores, as was illustrated by Daniel Janzen’s studies of acacia bushes
and the ants that guard them (see Plate 8 and Case Study 4.2). Many other ant–
plant relationships have been noted both before and since Janzen’s work. Each will
require the same sort of detailed investigation in order to understand the dynamics
and evolutionary adaptations on the part of the participants before they can be truly
termed mutualisms. One of these is the interaction between Cecropia plants and
certain species of Azteca ants throughout Central America; like the acacia, Cecropia
produces glycogen-rich food bodies (trichilia or Müllerian bodies) that are harvested
by its ants. Another is a ‘parasite’, Pseudomyrmex nigropilosa, that Janzen later
discovered; this ant harvests the resources of the swollen-thorn acacias but it does
not protect the acacia, attack foreign objects, or clean debris from the foliage.
The mutualism between plant ants and ant plants is highly coevolved and costs
each contributor a substantial energy outlay, but each participant also derives
considerable benefits.
In disturbed areas in the lowlands of Mexico and Central America, a com-
mon shrubby tree is the bull’s horn acacia, Acacia cornigera, so named for the
pair of swollen, hornlike thorns that occur at the base of most of its leaves. A
rapidly growing woody plant that cannot tolerate shading, it quickly springs
forth as sucker growth from old rootstocks in pastures, along roadsides and
in natural disturbance sites such as riverbanks and arroyos. Close examina-
tion of the plants reveals that they crawl with small Pseudomyrmex ferruginea
ants, which pour forth from holes in the thorns whenever the acacia is touched
(see Plate 8). In the 1800s the naturalist Thomas Belt had discovered a similar
relationship between an ant and acacia in Nicaragua and noted that the ends
of each leaf segment were modified into peculiar little oval structures. These
‘Beltian bodies’ seldom survived long, because the ants cut them off and either
ate them or fed them to their young. Subsequent studies have shown that they
172 4 Foraging and Feeding
have an unusually high food value for foliar tissue, on the order of yeast in
quantity and quality of nutrients.
Following Belt’s discovery there was a great deal of armchair speculation
on the exact nature of the relationship between ant acacias and acacia ants. The
ants clearly depended upon the acacias because most acacia ant species had
been recorded only from living ant acacias. But did the acacias benefit from
the presence of the ants? Two viewpoints persisted: one stated that the ants
were merely exploiting the acacia, while the other regarded the relationship as
a true symbiosis. This question became the focus of intensive eastern Mexican
field studies by Daniel Janzen.
One of Janzen’s early observations was that these acacias were commonly
left untouched in pastures with cows and other domestic animals. From a local
farmer, he borrowed a pet native deer that was thought not to have had any
previous experience with A. cornigera. When he offered it foliage that had
been cleaned of all ants, the deer ate the foliage readily, including some of
the thorns. After several days of feeding the deer unoccupied foliage, Janzen
offered a branch complete with some very agitated ant workers. As the deer
began to eat the foliage, ants ran onto its face and stung; immediately the deer
stopped feeding and withdrew to clean them off. Similar simple experiments
with cows and burros—animals that may have had previous experience with
A. cornigera—gave results that did not differ materially but suggested addi-
tionally that these herbivores may have learned to recognize the alarm odor of
the ants and learned to avoid contact with the plant.
The survival of the bull’s horn acacia is dependent upon rapid growth so
as to remain unshaded. Thus, Janzen began to examine the relative growth
and development of occupied versus unoccupied shoots of A. cornigera by
treating selected shoots with an insecticide. A number of striking differences
emerged. First, the frequency and extent of phytophagous insect damage was
greatly increased in shoots from which ants were removed; this resulted in
a great lowering of the growth rate of the shoots. In contrast, ant-occupied
shoots remained virtually free of phytophagous insects because the latter were
quickly attacked and removed from the shoot by worker ants. At least 40
species of insects fed on unoccupied shoots of A. cornigera, but only eight
species attacked occupied acacia shoots, and even the cumulative sum of their
feeding was not serious. During the rainy season, phytophagous insect activity
increased greatly, and the differential growth rates of occupied versus unoccu-
pied shoots became even more striking. Unoccupied shoots showed almost no
growth, but occupied shoots grew vigorously. Because of their slow growth
rate, surrounding vegetation quickly shaded the unoccupied acacias, and this
further slowed their growth. Many unoccupied shoots simply died.
Data from experimental plots also confirmed another observation that
Janzen had made, namely that occupied shoots were almost always free of
4.3 Coevolution and the Arms Race 173
living vines even though the acacia plant appeared ideal for the support of
vine growth. Whenever foreign vegetation contacted the acacia plant, the
ants would maul it and chew off the growing tips. In this way they pre-
vented vines and lateral branches of neighboring plants from growing into
the canopy of occupied A. cornigera. Unoccupied acacias, in contrast, accu-
mulated heavy masses of vines during the rainy season. Vegetation-mauling
activity of worker ants also resulted in a bare circle on the ground around
the base of the shoot. Janzen felt that this behavior was of significance in
protecting the shoot from fires as well as in lowering the incidence of phy-
tophagous insects reaching the shoot. Even when the shoot was killed by fire,
suckers from protected rootstock in the bare basal circle quickly sprouted and
were immediately colonized by the mature ant colony, giving the new shoot
an immediate competitive advantage in the post-fire succession.
Based on these data and observation of occasional naturally unoccupied
acacias, Janzen concluded that a shoot of A. cornigera must be occupied by
a colony of P. ferruginea for a substantial part of its life to produce seeds
and become a part of the reproductive population. Because of the interdepen-
dence of the ant and acacia for normal population development, the interaction
between them could properly be called obligatory mutualism.
‘Little Tommy Tucker sang for his supper . . .’ Mother Goose may have been
on to something. Many insects transport pollen and/or seeds in return for food
or other rewards. Violets, for example, reward ants for collecting their seeds
by incorporating lipid-rich compounds (elaiosomes) into them that are highly
attractive. In the previous section we discussed coevolution in the context of plant
defense. However, this is only one part of a much broader coevolutionary picture
that has been particularly well studied in the context of pollination.
Pollination in its most basic sense is simply the transfer of pollen from the male
sex organ (anther) to the receptive portion of the female sex organ (stigma) in flow-
ering plants. If the transfer is successful, it leads to fertilization and seed production,
and the plant reproduces. This process often involves some external vector, usually
wind, water, or animals. Authorities are in general agreement that the first insects to
become anthophilous, or pollen-loving, were the Coleoptera; even today, most sim-
ply rummage around in flowers, destroying some flowers by feeding on them but in
the process managing to pollinate others. Pollination apparently appeared next in the
Diptera, but fly pollination is understudied and probably also underrated. With the
development of pollination habits in Lepidoptera, highly mutualistic associations
began to become quite common, reaching their culmination in the Hymenoptera,
174 4 Foraging and Feeding
especially among the bees. Today, while many insect taxa visit flowers and thus can-
not be dismissed as pollination agents, members of these four largest insect orders
still are pollination biology’s major players.
Many flowering plants provide primary rewards—including nectar, pollen, lipid
secretions, food bodies, scents, resins, and nest building materials—that attract pol-
linating insects and keep them returning. In addition, most insect-pollinated flowers
also produce sensory cues—including odors, colors, shapes, textures, and tastes—
that distinguish their own flowers from other species and make it easier for an insect
to find more of their own flower type. For most flowering plants, this step is crucial
for pollination success because they are self-incompatible.
Have plants tailored these cues to attract certain pollinators and exclude oth-
ers? The concept remains controversial. Patterns certainly exist. A typical flower
pollinated by a night-flying hawk moth (see Plate 42) is white and has a strong
sweet fragrance; a typical flower pollinated by day-flying butterflies is red and
has very little odor. Particularly among tropical flowering plants, these signals
are often combined into patterns that some scientists have recognized as polli-
nation syndromes related to the type of pollen vector. However, it is also true
that rather than a single pollinator type, many flowers are visited by ‘guilds’ that
include diverse insect taxa, and the composition of these guilds may change over
time. Furthermore, just because an insect is an apparent guild member and visits a
Fig. 4.14 Pollinating figs. (left) Caprification in an orchard in Greece involves hanging bunches
of ’goat figs’ from tree branches. Pollination by the wasps that develop from these inedible fruits is
necessary to produce a mature edible fig crop. (right) Not all figs require fig wasps for pollination,
but among those that do, the fruits come in two types; compare the internal flower structure of the
‘goat figs’ at the left and the edible fig on the right. Fig wasps develop in the ovule of the short-
necked female flowers that pack the inside of goat figs. Before dispersing, newly emerged winged
females (middle bottom) mate with wingless males (middle top) and collect pollen from the male
flowers that cluster around the upper end of the goat fig. When the females find the new crop of
fruit, they enter the figs and thus pollinate them, but inside these edible figs the flower necks are too
long for their ovipositors to reach the flower ovaries. Thus, they cannot lay eggs or rear offspring
in the figs’ edible generation
4.3 Coevolution and the Arms Race 175
flower does not always mean it is actually an effective pollinator of that plant. For
these reasons, some pollination biologists see little conceptual value in pollination
syndromes.
Because pollination is so closely tied to reproductive behavior, not just of the
plants but of the insects as well, this subject will come up again in Chapter 9. For
now, let us consider an example of the extreme forms that pollination mutualism
can take—the relationship between fig wasps (Agaonidae) and their fig plant hosts
(Fig. 4.14).
Although the Smyrna fig story has received the most attention (see Case Study
4.3), figs actually include about 850 species of woody trees, shrubs, vines, and
epiphytes, most of which are native throughout the tropics. The fruits of many
species are edible and serve as important food resources for wildlife. Most tropi-
cal Ficus species bear several crops of syconia through the year, with short- and
long-necked female flowers and male flowers in the same syconium. Because the
female flowers are receptive several months before the male flowers release pollen,
they require wasp pollination between synchronized syconia of the same or different
species.
The most delicious figs require tiny symbiotic wasps to pollinate their minute
flowers. The relationship is so perfectly timed and intricate that if one partner
should die out, the other would inevitably follow.
Although European honey bees were valued co-travelers to the New World
for their honey production, one of the first insects introduced into North
America specifically to pollinate a crop came to southern California in 1899
to help with a problem—whereas common figs had been grown successfully
in the American Southeast and Mexico for two to three hundred years, the
choice cultivar called the Smyrna fig consistently failed to produce a crop.
The trees grew well, but immature figs simply dropped off rather than ripen-
ing. Why? Farmers were puzzled until they realized that the trees were missing
an essential pollinator that was not native to California. However, they could
not merely import the wasp, because the mutualism was very complex.
What we call a fig is actually a syconium, a specially adapted involuted
group of multiple flowers, rather like an almost-closed jug with flowers lin-
ing the inside. The flowers cannot be seen unless the fig is cut open; doing so
reveals the flowers as fleshy ‘threads’ that each bear a single seed inside. There
are many kinds of figs, but only one, Ficus carica, is cultivated on a commer-
cial scale. Its ancestors arose long ago in Asia Minor; some archeologists say
fig trees were cultivated before cereal grains were domesticated. Wild pop-
ulations of this tree include both male trees and female trees. Early authors
176 4 Foraging and Feeding
thought they were different species; a key to understanding this complex sys-
tem came with the realization that they are actually two natural sexual forms
of the same species, and that the two flower sexes develop asynchronously. A
second breakthrough was the realization that these Old World fig trees house
a tiny wasp, Blastophaga psenes, that is vital to their reproduction.
Male Old World fig trees actually bear both staminate and pistillate flowers,
but they produce fruits that contain only wasps and pollen. Generally not eaten
by humans, the fruits were named ‘caprifigs’ because they were commonly
fed to goats. Male trees produce three crops of inedible seedless figs per year
that maintain the pollinating wasps—profichi that produce abundant pollen
and ripen in early summer; mammoni that have short-style flowers and ripen
in the fall; and mamme with long-style flowers that overwinter on the tree
and ripen in spring. Within the profichi, female flowers develop first; their
shape is modified in a way that makes egg-laying easy for dispersing female
Blastophaga wasps that push their way through a tiny pore into them. Soon
afterwards, the pore closes, and the syconium becomes virtually impervious
to insect entry. In response to oviposition, the flowers swell like tiny galls,
each nourishing a developing wasp larva that feeds safely inside.
As the profichi reach maturity, so do the young male and female wasps
within. Wingless males chew holes in the sides of their nursery chambers,
escape, find females still imprisoned in adjoining chambers, chew into these,
and quickly mate. Pollen-bearing male flowers in the profichi are now also
mature; to leave the fig where they were born, females wasps must pass
through a ring of these male flowers surrounding the fig’s entrance, becoming
well dusted with pollen.
Female trees, by contrast, produce only one or two crops: a parthenocarpic
(seedless) breba crop that ripens in early summer and a main crop that ripens
from late summer to fall. Though the figs on female trees also have flow-
ers of both sexes, the male flower parts fail to develop. Breba figs are never
pollinated because they develop before male profichi ripen on nearby male
trees. Main crop figs may become pollinated or not, depending whether they
receive pollen via pollen-loaded B. psenes wasps from nearby mature profichi.
(Commercially cultivated fig tree types differ in their production of breba figs
and their need for pollination.)
As the female wasps emerge from caprifigs and begin to search for
new immature syconia in which to oviposit, they encounter an evolutionary
surprise—the fig trees are now producing not profichi, but edible figs that have
only large numbers of female flowers with long-necked styles. Ovipositing in
these is all but impossible, but in their repeated vain attempts to do so, the
female fig wasps successfully pollinate what will become edible figs. Some
fig wasps find still another type of small, inedible figs on the tree’s uppermost
branches. These ‘mamme’ or mother figs contain only short-styled female
4.4 Feeding as a Communal Activity 177
flowers in which the female wasps can oviposit successfully. Young male and
female wasp larvae hibernate within, to begin the profichi–fichi–mamme cycle
anew the following spring. The delicious Smyrna and Calimyrna figs are a
dead-end for the pollinating fig wasps, however.
As a result of this understanding of the fig/fig wasp mutualism (see
Fig. 4.14), Calimyrna figs are now grown successfully throughout California’s
hot San Joaquin Valley. Every June, visitors see a most unusual sight—
thousands of acres of fig orchards ‘decorated’ with small brown or white
paper bags stapled to the lower limbs. Inside each bag is a caprifig branch
with pollen-bearing profichi, picked prior to emergence of its associated
fig wasps. Although wasp-bearing caprifig trees have become naturalized in
moist riverbeds and creeks in California, they are kept at some distance from
orchards. Controlling the pollination process using only the bagged caprifigs
is important, for if too many wasps pollinate an edible fig, it splits open and
has no commercial value.
Typically, only one wasp species is capable of fertilizing the flowers of each
species of fig. Therefore, plantings of fig species outside their native range results
in effectively sterile individuals. It should be noted, however, that the common fig
cultivars grown in home gardens in southern and western North America require
no pollinator. A single dominant mutant gene is responsible for this change; rather
than prematurely dropping unpollinated fruits as the Smryna figs do, the common fig
trees expressing this gene retain unpollinated developing figs to maturity. The ability
to produce sweet fig fruits without the need for male trees that carry symbiotic fig
wasps within their syconia is very advantageous to farmers in regions where wild
caprifigs and natural pollinator wasps do not occur. However, these fig trees must be
propagated by cuttings because they do not produce seeds.
Fig. 4.15 Gregarious feeding. ‘Escalator style’ feeding by larvae of the Brazilian sawfly, Themos
olfersii, whose host plant possesses extremely tough leaves. Aligning themselves in two convergent
rows, the young larvae use their enormous heads and jaws to gain access to the thick edges of the
leaves (a). As the larvae feed, the leading individuals are pushed forward until they collide (b).
Upon collision, the leading larvae retreat to the ends of the rows (c) where they cannot readily
feed, while others are moved forward for a turn at eating. This process continues for many hours
4.4 Feeding as a Communal Activity 179
the nutritional and defensive ecology of exposed leaf feeding that selects for this
syndrome.
Gregariousness, the tendency to gather in groups, begins with laying eggs in
batches. Often, even the eggs of gregarious species are chemically defended; the
tip-off is their bright coloration. It might seem puzzling for vulnerable eggs to be
advertised, but advertising is the point. Such eggs are, in fact, packed with unpalat-
able or even toxic compounds, often in such high doses that a single egg is enough
to kill an ant.
All gregarious leaf-feeding species that have been studied are chemically
defended as well. There have been relatively few empirical tests of the efficacy of
these chemicals against an insect species’ known natural enemies, but some suggest
that predators are more likely to be deterred than permanently stopped. This may be
enough, however, to give prey the statistical advantage necessary to select for the
strategy.
Because of these chemical defenses and bright coloration, it is generally assumed
that almost all simple feeding groups and feeding aggregations function to improve
defensive capabilities. However, increased feeding efficacy may be equally or even
more common. One well-studied example involves the imported willow leaf beetle
(see Case Study 4.4).
Among some group-feeding larvae, the Dr. Jekyll of better defense and feeding
efficiency has a flip side, the Mr. Hyde of cannibalism. What balance of costs
and benefits maintains such patently antisocial behavior?
Across eastern North America each spring, females of the willow leaf bee-
tle, Plagiodera versicolora, (see Plate 12) emerge from overwintering sites
under loose bark and begin laying the first of their two or three clutches of
15–20 eggs. The larvae of this little metallic blue-green chrysomelid complete
their development in about two weeks and soon become reproductively active
themselves, so that even on a single tree, broods of different generations and
ages can be found as the summer progresses. The larvae stay in tight aggrega-
tions throughout the first two instars, then gradually become less gregarious as
they continue to grow. Like many larvae with a social childhood, Plagiodera
produce copious chemicals, in this case through paired dorsal glands. They
also often assume a ‘cycloalexis’ formation with their heads facing out, like
pioneers circling their wagon train.
Since 1985, Michael Wade and Felix Breden have been thoroughly
researching Plagiodera group behavior. Their early field studies yielded puz-
zling results; the first year, larval survivorship showed a significant positive
relationship to initial group size. The next year, they could only say that the
group size-survivorship relationship varied considerably in space and time.
180 4 Foraging and Feeding
What was going on? Breden and Wade designed a clever way to address
the question. First they located natural P. versicolora clusters in the field, and
randomly assigned them to group size and duration classes. Then they ‘down-
sized’ the aggregations to create replicate groups of various sizes that were
followed and collected at various intervals over a week’s time. When they
analyzed their data, once again survivorship was unaffected by initial group
size, but an interesting new relationship surfaced. Larvae gained significantly
more weight in larger groups. Thus, Breden and Wade concluded that for this
species, the benefit of group living related more to increased feeding effi-
ciency than to increased defense. Careful observation provided the reason. In
1994, Wade described feeding facilitation in this species. Each larva attempts
to break through the cuticle of a willow leaf by rocking from side to side as
it bears down with its mandibles. When one larva succeeds, others stop their
own rocking and move to the break to feed. Larger groups presumably allow
more and earlier breaches of the plant’s defense.
Gregariousness begins with laying eggs in batches. Imagine the surprise
of these observers upon finding that newly emerging Plagiodera larvae are
intensely cannibalistic for about 24 hours before they switch to become strict
vegetarians. At first glance such behavior looked very antisocial and selfish;
if it happened frequently enough, it seemed as though it could completely
destroy the group. There must be some serious trade-offs in costs and benefits
for this cannibalism to persist, they reasoned.
Feeding efficiency might be one benefit. At the end of 24 hours, cannibal-
istic individuals weighed a very noticeable 14% more than non-cannibalistic
larvae in the cluster. Presumably these larger larvae would have an increased
chance of early successful leaf penetration, and through their success, other
group members’ feeding would be facilitated, as well. However, the positive
effect of group size on larval survivorship was weakened in groups with can-
nibals simply because cannibalistic feeding has the obvious effect of reducing
group size.
At present, perhaps all that can be conclusively said is that for a non-
cannibalistic P. versicolora larva, being a group member is clearly advan-
tageous in terms of survival and growth, but that the presence of cannibals
in the group can completely undermine these advantages. Interestingly, for a
cannibal, the work of Wade and Breden indicates that fitness is not dependent
on group size or the percent of cannibals in the group. Rather, it depends on
the average larval fitness of everyone in the group, cannibal and non-cannibal
alike, plus a personal fitness gain that the cannibal experiences from dining
on its siblings.
Like water from a bottomless well, complex and fascinating new questions
about P. versicolor keep arising. For example, if average survivorship declines
in the presence of cannibals but the cannibals themselves do well, is this
4.4 Feeding as a Communal Activity 181
When a social wasp larva is fed by an adult, the larva almost always secretes a
droplet of salivary fluid, which the adult imbibes. In many social insects, chemi-
cal communication signals are spread through the colony by an exchange of liquids
between nest mates. The reciprocal exchange of liquid foods between colony mem-
bers, a behavior called trophallaxis, occurs both between adults and between adults
and larvae.
Trophallaxis is highly developed in social insects. It appears to occur generally
throughout the eusocial wasps. Among bees and ants, on the other hand, its occur-
rence is highly variable, apparently determined both by phylogenetic position and
ecological constraints. In termites, trophallaxis has multiple functions. Lower ter-
mites share both ‘stomodeal food’ from the salivary glands and crop and ‘proctodeal
food’ from the hindgut. The former is the principal nutrient source for the royal pair
and nymphs; the latter, a milky material quite different from feces, contains sym-
biotic flagellates. These break down the cellulose that the termites are otherwise
unable to use as a source of nutrition. With each molting these hindgut symbionts are
lost, and the nymphs must acquire new ones. Among the higher termites, the habit
of proctodeal trophallaxis has been lost along with dependence on symbiotic flagel-
lates for cellulose digestion. However, the nymphs have become entirely dependent
on stomodeal exchanges and no longer even possess functional mandibles.
Since 1918 when entomologist William Morton Wheeler first coined the term
for this unilateral or bilateral liquid food exchange, there has been strong dispute
over the signal value of these liquids. Some entomologists have even suggested
182 4 Foraging and Feeding
Fig. 4.16 Sharing a ‘communal stomach.’ Formica fusca is an ant species that engages in rapid
oral trophallactic exchanges. Within a day after a single worker was fed small amount of honey
mixed with radioactive iodide, evidence of the radioactive food was present in varying amounts in
every colony member, including the 2 queens (stippled part). Over time, the frequency distribution
of individual shares of the radioactive food became progressively more normalized
4.4 Feeding as a Communal Activity 183
both ants and bees appear to favor larger individuals, with the result that queens,
males, and larger workers tend to receive more than they give.
In social wasps, the release of trophallactic behavior is more structured and
complex, because dominance relations place severe constraints on food exchange,
slowing the rate of food distribution and increasing the variance in crop contents
among workers. For example, an inert severed head does not release trophallaxis in
Vespula; rather, the pair must engage in a very definite pattern of continuous recip-
rocal antennal signaling. Dominant workers receive more food than they give. The
mother queen always receives and seldom ever gives, and virgin queens dominate
over their worker sisters. Males, possessing a quite different antennal form, are quite
inept at begging and must rely primarily upon surreptitious sips of the regurgitated
liquids being passed between others or upon stimulating larvae to give forth salivary
secretions.
In the previous section, we considered cannibalism in the context of simple
feeding groups; now let’s revisit the subject in the context of eusocial insects. All
termites studied to date eat their own dead and injured on at least some occasions,
a degree of cannibalistic behavior that is far more intense than in any other social
insect group. Some Reticulitermes termite workers even eat apparently healthy nest-
mates when grooming is carried too far. If the cuticle of a leg is broken, for example,
the leg is eaten, and then the whole termite is consumed. Winged Coptotermes repro-
ductives that are unable to leave on a normal nuptial flight are finally killed and
eaten by workers. Alien conspecific workers that enter a termite nest are generally
disabled, then consumed. Because termite diets under natural conditions are usually
low in protein, it is generally believed that this cannibalism functions as a protein-
conserving device. Colonies of Zooptermopsis angusticollis, for example, become
intensely cannibalistic when reared on a laboratory diet of pure cellulose, but adding
sufficient casein to the diet reduces the cannibalism to almost zero.
Among the social Hymenoptera, cannibalism of adults is rare in some groups,
unknown in most. However, a related phenomenon is common—eating immature
stages. In ant colonies, for example, injured eggs, larvae, and pupae are quickly
consumed. When colonies are starved, workers begin attacking healthy brood as
well. Hunger and the degree of such brood cannibalism are so precisely related as
to suggest that the colony’s store of immature stages functions normally as a last
ditch emergency food supply to keep the queen and workers alive.
A related phenomenon, widespread in the social Hymenoptera, is egg canni-
balism, oophagy. In the more primitive social groups, the dominant queen eats
the eggs laid by subordinates, thus ensuring that her own progeny will predomi-
nate. This exploitive character has been transformed into something quite different
among the higher social Hymenoptera, however. In these groups it has become
an important form of food exchange between cooperating members of the same
colony. Sometimes, oophagy functions like brood cannibalism; once colonies are
well under way, if workers are starved the eggs are the first brood stage to be
eaten.
In other cases, workers lay eggs that seem designed only to be eaten. Most
workers of Atta leafcutter ants have rudimentary ovaries that never produce eggs.
184 4 Foraging and Feeding
However, a few small and medium workers form a retinue around the queen and lay
large, flaccid eggs that lack yolk. The eggs are not viable; they serve only to feed
the queen. Such trophic eggs have been reported in a wide diversity of ants and are
apparently laid in enormously varying frequencies among the various ant species.
As a rule, however, the more frequent the exchange of trophic eggs, the less fre-
quent the liquid exchange through trophallaxis. Other cases of trophic egg-laying,
primarily in which mothers provide non-developing eggs for offspring to eat, have
been found not only among social and subsocial insects, but throughout the animal
kingdom from amphibians to fish and marine gastropods.
Chapter 5
Defense: A Survival Catalogue
5.1 Introduction
A great variety of moths spend their days resting safely upon tree trunks, so perfectly
matching the mottled bark that their very invisibility forms their defense. To repel
honey thieves, some stingless social bees erect walls of sticky resin in front of or
around the nest entrance, while other species smear a repugnant liquid there. Taking
a more active and direct approach, pentatomid bugs earn their common name of
‘stink bug’ from the odorous, distasteful chemical they discharge when disturbed.
Because herbivory is such a major insect lifestyle, Chapter 4 concentrated upon
the evolutionary arms race between herbivorous insects and plants. This chapter will
consider the analogous evolutionary battle of wits between insect predators and par-
asites and their insect prey. Almost all insects face a nearly constant threat of death.
A familiar estimate states that a single pair of houseflies is capable of producing
125 billion great-great grandchildren were all their offspring to survive. Obviously
this does not occur. When averaged over a span of many years, the hundreds or
thousands of eggs a female insect may lay usually result in only a few adults that
reproduce again. This high percentage of losses is due to a variety of factors, but a
major one is predatory attack.
Defined broadly, defense includes all the ways that organisms respond to per-
ceived threats by potential predators, parasites, and microorganisms. Whereas the
insect immune system generally provides the ultimate defense against bacteria,
viruses, protozoans, and fungi (and backup defenses after parasite attack), behav-
ior and life history strategies provide front-line defenses against assault from
macroscopic predators and parasites.
Most past attention focused upon insects as prey of vertebrates, with defense
against invertebrates tending to be overlooked or its importance discounted.
However, insects are often their own worst enemies. Aphids are preyed upon by
a wide variety of insect enemies from adult pemphredonine wasps to coccinellid
beetle larvae, syrphid flies, and lacewings, yet these small, soft-bodied creatures are
far from helpless. A formicine ant stumbling into a foreign ant colony is quickly
hit with venom spray; the victim responds with much grooming and rubbing of its
mouthparts on the ground. The nests of certain tropical social wasps are suspended
on a single stalk that is continually treated with secretions from the wasps’ abdom-
inal glands; ants are repelled from walking upon treated stalks but otherwise enter
and destroy developing brood within the nest. Likewise, relatively little research has
accounted for the fact that insects are also continuously under siege by less conspic-
uous but nonetheless significant microorganisms, such as bacteria, fungi, viruses,
nematodes, and parasites. In these contexts, such chemicals may have long-term
antagonistic or even antibiotic effects.
The potential predator as an evolutionary force has important implications in
every form of defense. For example, in considering color crypsis, the optic system
of the predator must be considered. Insects and vertebrates inhabit what amount
to two very different spectral and focal worlds, a subject that will receive more
attention in Chapter 7. Moreover our own sensory world often differs from that of
an insect’s vertebrate predator, a fact that can lead human investigators astray. For
example, Chapter 8 raises the possibility that many insect ‘protest sounds’ might
actually be vibrational signals intended for conspecifics.
the air. Finally, when the attack persists, the caterpillar forcibly ejects from its pro-
thoracic gland a burning, colorless fluid containing 40% formic acid. The adult puss
moth has none of these defense options open to it.
At the same time, certain defense strategies have widespread utility, so evolution-
ary convergence commonly occurs in various structural and behavioral adaptations.
Thus, defensive behaviors have been classified in various ways. One way is to exam-
ine each sensory modality in turn; for example, Table 5.1 presents basic types of
visually perceived anti-predator adaptations. Another approach to classifying insect
defenses is to consider the prey-to-predator message that is implicit in a particular
behavior or structural adaptation. Approximately half a dozen such messages will
handle the bulk of the variety of insect defenses. Lets examine these and see how,
when, and why these defenses have survival benefit.
I. Crypsis Countershading; disruptive Prolonged immobility; night Genetic plasticity and/or Fails to discriminate prey
(a) Imitate background coloration; homochromy; feeding; correctly orient; polymorphism for color from substrate
dorsoventral flattening; select proper background; forms; low population
body flanges; often special poses; dash and density; dispersed
secondary startle freeze or move slowly distribution
mechanisms
(b) Resemble inedible Correct shape, color; finely As above; rocks, teeters, Dispersed distribution; low Confuses prey with inedible
objects detailed patterns favored; mimetic poses population density objects
often have startle
mechanisms
II. Bizarre forms Unusual appearance, style, Unexpected, unusual and/or Dispersed distribution; low Fails to see prey as food;
Gestalt; may possess startle startling behaviors population density startled, lets prey escape
mechanisms
III. Simple aposematism Weapons (stings, bites, Usually diurnal; may Clumped distribution; high Learns to recognize prey as
poisons); bold vivid aggregate; conspicuous population density distasteful and/or
pattern, often red, yellow, behavior; warning displays dangerous
orange, black; conspicuous
structure
IV. Mimicry Same as for III; superficially Conspicuous behavior similar Clumped distribution; high Learns Gestalt is distasteful
(a) Müllerian similar to others in complex to others in complex; may population density and/or dangerous
aggregate
(b) Batesian Conspicuous coloration Similar to model(s) Mimetic polymorphisms; low Confuses prey with one it has
and/or structure similar to population density; learned to avoid
model(s) dispersed
(c) Wasmannian Similar to predatory host, at Similar to predatory host that Low population density; Allows prey to approach; is
least in releasers; chemical, serves as both model and linked to social insects with exploited, may become
auditory, tactile mimicry selective agent large colonies at maturity prey itself
favored
5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
5.3 Passive Messages 189
Fig. 5.1 Disrupting the outline. (left) A threadlike reduviid bug (Emesiinae) from Panama is so
thin that it ‘disappears’ except when viewed from the side. Its banded legs are an example of disrup-
tive coloration that enhances its ability to merge into the background. (right) The banding pattern
on the antennae and hind legs of this resting Neotropical long-horned beetle (Cerambycidae) create
the additional illusion that the beetle is going in the opposite direction
striking accuracy. Katydids may imitate leaves to such an extent as to include copies
of blemishes, fungal spots, or bird droppings in addition to reproducing the proper
leaf tint and venation.
By far the commonest method by which insects evade potential predation is
through camouflage or crypsis, that is, by imitating certain environmental back-
ground features; a wide variety of diverse unrelated insect groups often have evolved
to simulate the same inedible (to a carnivore) object. Crypsis involves at least shape,
color, and color pattern (see Plate 14). In some instances it probably also involves
scent and sound matching although human sensory apparatus may not be equal to
the task of discerning it. To be maximally camouflaged, a cryptic individual also
must solve the major problem of body contour. Probably the most widely used solu-
tion is disruptive coloration, a visual breaking of the insect’s outline so that parts of it
appear to fade separately into the background (Fig. 5.1). A second way of minimiz-
ing contour cues involves actually or apparently reducing any telltale shadows. This
may be accomplished through a dorsoventral flattening, as occurs in many aradid
bugs, often in combination with lateral flaps or various irregular body protuberances
that bridge the gap between body and substrate.
Of course, even the most exact reproduction will be of little value in concealment
without appropriate behavior, such as resting on the proper background in the right
190 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
orientation and attitude, moving seldom and/or slowly and in such a manner as to
attract as little attention as possible. Thus, properly considered, crypsis is not solely
a matter of coloration or morphology but a type of behavior as well. For exam-
ple, many moths have wing stripings that resemble grooves in bark; those moths
with vertical stripes rest on tree trunks with their heads pointed up or down, but
horizontally striped moths orient themselves at right angles to the trunk.
Another widely encountered method for eliminating shadows is countershad-
ing, that is, a compensatory deepening or lightening of body color to counteract
Fig. 5.2 Countershading. Nearly every green caterpillar that rests in the open has color shad-
ing that compensates for the shadow on its lower parts, producing an apparent flattening. (above)
A hornworm caterpillar (Manduca) in normal resting position. (below) Inverting the caterpillar
destroys the illusion, and the addition of countershading to natural shadow makes the caterpillar
highly conspicuous
5.3 Passive Messages 191
for apparent color changes due to light intensity. Insects that have countershading
invariably rest with the darker surface directed toward the light, a behavior critical to
the success of countershading (Fig. 5.2). Shadow also may be minimized by proper
body alignment. (It has been suggested that many cryptic butterflies perch relative
to the sun so that their wings throw the least shadow, but the observation is compli-
cated by the fact that butterflies also control body heat by soaking up or avoiding
sunlight.)
Some insect species have the chameleon-like ability to change their color to
match temporary backgrounds. By varying the quantities of orange, yellow, and
black pigments they form, many locusts can adapt their color from dirty white to
yellow, brown, or black; kept for a few days on burnt ground, even adult locusts can
darken. Only when the hoppers are kept amid green vegetation and fed with abun-
dant moist food in a very humid atmosphere do bright-green locust hoppers appear.
Likewise, certain caterpillars are able to change color and markings to match the
background of their varied diets. When they feed upon birch, the larvae of peppered
moths are smooth and purplish brown with darker ‘lenticels’ resembling a birch
twig; on oak, however, they are brownish green with markings suitable to these
trees. On oak covered with lichen, they become mottled in such a way as to mimic
a lichen-covered twig!
Genetic variations in cryptic coloration also occur. One of the most thoroughly
studied cases of rapid directional selection in progress involves crypsis in this com-
mon English insect, the peppered moth (also sometimes called the salt-and-pepper
moth), the subject of Case Study 5.1.
What happened to change the color of a little cryptic moth? The research that
answered this question has become a classic story in biology textbooks.
Peppered moths, like many others, fly at night and rest quietly by day on
the sides of tree trunks. Naturalists of the nineteenth century knew them well
and noted that they were usually found on lichen-covered trees and rocks,
against which their pale mottled coloring made them practically invisible.
Prior to 1845, all museum records of Biston betularia were light-colored or
‘typical’ specimens, but in that year, near the growing industrial center of
Manchester, England, one black or ‘melanic’ moth of that species was cap-
tured, so distinctive that it was given the name B. betularia f. carbonaria (see
Fig. 5.3). As the years passed, black individuals turned up with increasing
frequency, mainly from the vicinities of industrial towns. By the mid-20th
century, the peppered moth population around Manchester consisted of nearly
99% carbonaria.
192 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
Fig. 5.3 Attracting attention—or not. The peppered moth, Biston betularia, and its black form
carbonaria at rest on a lichen-covered tree trunk in a non-industrial area of England. The typical
light form (upper center) illustrates the principle of disruptive coloration, but the dark form (lower
center) shows conspicuous contrast against the same background (see also Plate 13)
first half hour, leaving only two ‘inconspicuous’ survivors. Subsequent repli-
cations followed the same pattern, suggesting that the birds required a period
of contact with the prey before recognition but that after recognition was
established, all the moths were under risk of predation. Satisfyingly, the data
seemed to indicate that the most conspicuous were preyed upon first.
Encouraged, Kettlewell turned to field experiments in two study sites: a
polluted area and an uncontaminated woodland like that which must have
prevailed 200 years before. Setting up a camp that included sheds to house
some 3,000 B. betularia pupae to raise for release, Kettlewell began round-
the-clock studies. During the day, he released melanic and typical moths in
known numbers, carefully marking each with a paint spot beneath the wings
where it could not be seen by a predator. Then he watched wild birds’ behavior
toward the moths. During the night he operated light traps, capturing the pep-
pered moths of both forms that were attracted to the ultraviolet bulbs. (The
use of such a method was essential because of the moths’ crypticity. When
Kettlewell attempted to visually discover and count his released moths again
immediately after their release in an area where his continual presence pre-
cluded predation, over a third of the cryptic forms were already ‘missing,’
that is, so well hidden that they eluded even Kettlewell’s experienced eye.)
With help from the prominent ethologist Niko Tinbergen, Kettlewell
watched bird predation on the moths, both with binoculars and with a movie
camera from behind camouflaged blinds. In this way he was able to document
at least five bird species actually selecting and eating the moths, which they
did with such alacrity that it is surprising they were not previously observed.
More important to his hypothesis, on the majority of occasions all the bird
species at both locations took all of the more conspicuous moths before any
of the inconspicuous phenotypes. Further proof of the selective advantage of
having coloration appropriate to one’s background was obtained by recap-
ture of marked moths at night in the light traps. In unpolluted woodland,
the survival rate of light moths was about twice that of dark ones, while in
soot-darkened woodland the ratio was reversed.
Do moths discriminate among a choice of possible resting substrates? As
a pilot study, Kettlewell outfitted a barrel with a lining of alternate black
and white stripes of identical texture to eliminate tactile cues. Each night he
released three moths of each form into the barrel; in the morning, he recorded
their resting positions. Of 110 moths so tested, 65% chose the ‘correct’ back-
ground. However, the artificiality of such tests disturbed Kettlewell, so he
selected a number of lichen-covered tree trunks and then carefully removed
the bryophytes from one side. Over this denuded half he painted a soot sus-
pension and then placed the trunk upright and covered it with a muslin tent.
At the midline of the two sides, he released equal numbers of females of each
form and recorded their final resting position. Every one of 31 carbonaria
194 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
individuals chose the black background; of the light forms, 75% selected the
‘correct’ background. These highly significant results provide quite conclu-
sive evidence that female Biston betularia are able to selectively choose their
background with respect to their own coloration, probably through visual cues
(see Plate 13).
Several important additional implications arose from Kettlewell’s studies.
One major revelation was the speed with which evolution can occur at the level
of the single gene. Geneticists had previously considered a selective advantage
on the order of 0.1% as being ‘normal’—Kettlewell illustrated an advantage
on the order of 50%! This selection, intense enough to cause a significant
change in gene frequency within a single generation, when extended over tens
of generations could result in a nearly complete reversal in the favored gene.
In the years since Bernard Kettlewell’s study, others have continued to research
what is now called ‘industrial melanism’ in peppered moths, and there has been
some evidence that other differences in fitness among the phenotypes that are not
directly related to the visual differences in color pattern may also be involved.
However, studies continue to support the fundamental finding of a relatively rapid
switch in survival of the pale and dark phenotypes in response to corresponding
changes in crypsis arising from an altered environment (Fig. 5.3). Interestingly,
as Great Britain and other countries in northern Europe have reduced levels of
air pollution from soot and gases such as sulfur dioxide, the tree trunks are once
more showing their pale bark’s true color; in response, the relative fitnesses of the
two moth phenotypes are changing again. The fully black form is again becoming
more conspicuous and vulnerable to birds, and the now-better-camouflaged light
phenotype of the peppered moth is on the increase.
Fig. 5.4 Stinging caterpillars. (top) Feeding group of pine processionary caterpillars
(Thaumetopoea pityocampa). The short hairs along the dorsal surface contain skin-irritating chem-
icals. The processionary behavior of this species was subject of a study by J. H. Fabré over a
hundred years ago. Fabré attributed the circling to blind instinct, and it is one of his best known
insect stories, perhaps because it came to be viewed as a metaphor for mindless living or blindly
following a leader. (below) The buck moth, Hemileuca mala, is another stinging caterpillar. The
impressive crowns of urticating spines that adorn each segment (inset closeup) can cause severe
itching,
Often such questions cannot be answered, if for no other reason than incom-
plete knowledge of the particular system. Lethal dosages almost certainly depend
on the predator involved, a fact often unknown. In still other cases, even if predatory
organisms neither die nor discriminate against the causative agent, systemic chemi-
cals may impair vigor or fecundity; any substance that does this to the predators of
an area is advantageous to the species that produces the poison.
5.3 Passive Messages 197
prey and the learning predator benefit from a correct interpretation of the sig-
nal, rare or new variants within a prey population are at a disadvantage; they
will not be recognized as distasteful and thus will suffer higher predation. This
selection against rare forms translates into positive frequency-dependent selection.
Thus, not surprisingly, most distasteful Müllerian mimics are monomorphic in local
populations, with polymorphic forms that bridge narrow hybrid zones between
color-pattern races.
Third, as more Müllerian mimics use the same warning signal, the protection
given by the signal becomes stronger. Predators generalize more, and the selection
for close resemblance appears to be somewhat relaxed. Ultimately it would seem
as though there should come to be just one giant mimetic cluster in the entire habi-
tat, but nature seems to behave otherwise. Particularly in the tropics, aposematic
insects of similar size and shape usually group into not just one, but several distinct
complexes. A ‘mosaic mimetic environment’ is usually invoked as an explanation—
if different mimicry rings occupy different microhabitats and predators do not move
between them, then each ‘subpopulation’ might have different fitness peaks and
also be small enough to be particularly sensitive to genetic drift into different color
patterns in different patches.
Still, despite these theoretical differences, Batesian and Müllerian mimicry both
depend on stimulus generalization by their vertebrate predators, and both are prob-
ably labels for extreme cases in what is essentially a mimicry continuum. Within a
mimicry complex, for example, some members may be more palatable than others.
Should these less protected individuals be considered Müllerian mimics for which
selection for bad taste has been relaxed? Or are they Batesian mimics that have sub-
sequently evolved (or are in the process of evolving) a bad taste? Such questions
are nearly impossible to resolve. In another example, certain mimetic cerambycid
beetles often feed upon the distasteful lycid beetles that serve as their models. Such
mimics might be alternatively Batesian or Müllerian, depending upon how recently
they had fed upon a lycid.
Although Batesian and Müllerian mimicry receive most of the attention, other
types of mimicry are also documented and classified by a number of different names.
One of the better known is aggressive mimicry. James E. Lloyd s investigation of
female fireflies of the genus Photuris revealed they emit the same light signals that
females of the genus Photinus use as a mating signal; male fireflies from several dif-
ferent genera are attracted to these ‘femmes fatales’, and are subsequently captured
and eaten (see Chapters 1 and 7 for more on fireflies).
Whereas in this example the fireflies are the predators, in other cases the reverse
is true and an insect may mimic its major predator and thereby reduce the risk of
being eaten by it (Fig. 5.5).
A slightly different matter is automimicry, or mimicry that occurs within a single
species when a population includes both mimics and models because of a palatabil-
ity dimorphism. For example, monarch butterflies have been mentioned as a classic
Batesian model, but not all monarchs are equally disagreeable. Their caterpillars eat
various milkweed plants in different parts of the monarch’s range and these vary in
the amount of cardenolides they contain, some having little or none. The butterflies’
5.3 Passive Messages 201
Fig. 5.5 Sending false signals. The tethritid fly Zonosemata vittigera has a leg-like pattern on its
wings (left) and waves these patterns such that these ‘legs’ mimic the agonistic territorial displays
of its primary predators, jumping spiders. In response, the spiders do not attack, but either return
the display or retreat. Both the pattern and the behavior are necessary for effective mimicry. In
transplant experiments (right), spiders attacked tephritids with house fly wings and house flies
with tephritid wings. Each treatment used 20 flies, and responses were measured as the highest
level of aggression attained during 5 minutes of interaction in a test arena
palatability depends on the amount of cardenolide eaten as larvae; this means that
individuals in a population vary in their palatability to predators. Those lacking
chemical protection are effectively mimics of their fellow species mates that are
distasteful.
Many insects change shape, size, and behavior during growth and metamorpho-
sis; when different instars imitate entirely different models it is called transfor-
mational mimicry (Fig. 5.6). Beetles and other guests of ants sometimes greatly
resemble their hosts (Chapter 4); mimetic resemblance that facilitates cohabitation
with a mimic’s host, its model, has been termed Wasmannian mimicry.
The mimicry catalog could go on and on. However, as in other areas of insect
behavior, studies of mimicry have steadily been stepping back from a natural history
of individual examples to a broader synthetic view—one that includes modeling,
predator behavior (including the psychological processes of learning and forget-
ting), and evolutionary dynamics. A major driving force has been the inadequacy
of older theories to explain the phenomenon of imperfect mimicry. As more and
more mimicry systems have been studied, it has become clear that most Batesian
mimicry, in particular, is of rather poor quality, at least to human eyes looking at
its visual aspects. This poses a theoretical puzzle. Conventional evolutionary theory
202 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
Fig. 5.6 Changing tactics. Transformational mimicry in larvae of the eastern tiger swallowtail
butterfly, Papilio glaucus. (left) a first-instar larva resembles amorphous bird droppings, and rests
in full view at the center of a leaf. Notice the nibbled spot at the leaf tip. These larvae feed only at
night, when their diurnally active predators have stopped hunting. (right) Older larvae have grown
too large to effectively resemble droppings. Colored bright green like the leaves they feed upon,
they also possess eyespots and perform a striking snake-like display (see Fig. 5.12) when disturbed
would predict that because individuals with closer resemblance to the model gain
more protection, Batesian mimics should be under constant selection to improve
their mimetic resemblance; hence eventually only very good mimics should exist. A
variety of (often not mutually exclusive) new explanations for this seeming puzzle
have arisen; testing, proving, and disproving them should keep mimicry researchers
busy for some time. One key to the puzzle may lie in the fact that many if not
most vertebrates tend to avoid stimuli similar to, but not identical with, an origi-
nal conditioning stimulus, a learning phenomenon called stimulus generalization.
Experimental studies have revealed that under certain conditions, stimulus general-
ization provides advantage to a wide gamut of prey even in cases where they may
appear only vaguely alike to our perception.
The Carolina locust, Dissoteira carolina, combines vivid yellow and black coloring
with a loud crackling sound. Brightly colored bees and wasps sound a warning buzz
sufficiently effective to be mimicked by a wide variety of non-relatives. Caterpillars
that possess stinging hairs often undulate conspicuously as they crawl about. Other
5.3 Passive Messages 203
Fig. 5.7 Bleeding reflexively. When gripped with forceps, an ithomiine day-flying moth from
the Neotropics foams released hemolymph with air from its spiracles
206 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
highly developed in the Mexican bean beetle, which may even rotate its leg around
to bring the oozing knee in closest contact to the point of stimulation.
When handled or disturbed, a wide variety of arthropods respond by regur-
gitating or defecating. Often very effective as predator repellents, such enteric
discharges might almost be considered a form of short-range attack. The familiar
frothy ‘tobacco juice’ regurgitate of some grasshoppers, for example, is quite toxic
to mammals. It is a topical irritant to eyes, may induce vomiting when swallowed,
and may cause severe symptoms and even death when injected. When tethered
grasshoppers placed beside ant colonies are induced to regurgitate, the fluid causes
their assailants instantaneously to disperse and begin intense cleaning movements.
When pieces of cut-up grasshopper, some of which have been treated with regur-
gitate, are placed along ant trails, foragers carry away only the untreated pieces.
Such protection can be extremely important, particularly in defense against massed
predators such as ants. Various morphological and behavioral adaptations prolong
the effectiveness of such residual secretions (Fig. 5.8).
Spraying is a common way to apply non-injected secretions, and it is often
accomplished with a high degree of accuracy and sometimes over a considerable
distance. In general, these sprayed chemicals are much stronger than most enteric
discharges. The predaceous reduviid bug Platymeris rhadamantus sprays its saliva
directionally several feet in response to predator attack; the toxic fluid, ordinarily
used to kill prey, is said to be similar to cobra venom. The large two-striped walk-
ing stick Anisomorpha buprestoides of the southeastern United States is exceptional
in that it will spray approaching birds from a distance, whereas most non-stinging
Fig. 5.8 Prolonging repellency. (left) The pupal stage of a Chrysomela leaf beetle hangs fully
exposed to predators on the undersides of the host plant. Persistent odor of the larval defensive
secretion impregnates the shed skin, which remains attached (arrow) and acts as a potent repel-
lent to foraging ants. A second larva preparing to pupate is also present. (right) A larva of the
chrysomelid beetle Cassida rubiginosa forms a shield of cast skins and feces that is held on a fork
projecting from its hind end. When branched spines on its body detect a predator’s probing, it inter-
poses the shield between itself and the enemy, often in such a way that fecal material is smeared
upon the offender. After even a brief contact with the pasty material, ants immediately flee and
clean themselves
5.4 Active Messages 207
insects require direct contact stimulation before discharging their chemical defenses.
Some of the commonest ant species use formic acid as a poison spray and thus have
been used as a natural source of this acid from Roman times until quite recently.
Formic acid is a potent irritant and effectively repels a variety of potential ant
predators. It also serves as an alarm odor (see Chapter 6) that alerts nestmates to the
source of the disturbance. The ants employ stereotyped postures to control spraying
direction (Fig. 5.9a).
Probably the best known spraying insects are the 500+ species of carabid ground
beetles known as bombardiers (Fig. 5.9b). They occur through much of the world,
and vary in size but all have essentially the same defensive mechanism. The
chemicals discharged are 1,4-benzoquinones, irritant chemicals that many other
arthropods also have independently evolved the ability to produce. What is remark-
able about the bombardiers is that they do not store their benzoquinones as such.
Instead, they produce them by explosive synthesis at the moment of ejection, mix-
ing the contents of two internal chambers and catalyzing the reaction with two kinds
of enzymes. In the process, enough heat is generated to bring the resultant discharge
to the temperature of boiling water. Excellent marksmen, bombardiers can spray in
virtually any direction to accurately target any part of their body that is subjected to
assault. The combination of a burning and stinging spray and the audible popping
sound that accompanies the emissions is understandably effective against a wide
range of potential predators, including humans.
The active principles of most defensive secretions are highly volatile substances
of low molecular weight, usually strongly odorous and irritating and in some cases
even painful to inhale. Often, they are present in very high concentrations. How do
these arthropods withstand their own discharges? In many cases, external immunity
appears to be gained through possession of an especially impervious integument.
The cuticle of Hemiptera with defensive glands, for example, is generally imperme-
able to hemipteran secretion unless abrasion takes place. Internally, secretions do
Fig. 5.9 Taking aim. (left) A carpenter ant, Formica integer, responds to an alien in the nest by
bending its abdomen under its body and directionally spraying formic acid that both repels the
intruder and alerts nestmates. (right) A bombardier beetle aims its abdominal tip so accurately that
it can spray its hot quinone secretion in any direction
208 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
not come into contact with living tissue because the storage glands are membranous
saclike invaginations of the body wall, and thus are lined with cuticle as well.
But how can the glands produce the poisons without poisoning themselves?
Some arthropod gland cells possess certain more or less elaborate cuticular
chambers and ducts; toxicant synthesis occurs within the lumen of these cuticular
organelles, not in the cytoplasm of the living gland cells associated with them. Such
a situation has been shown in certain tenebrionid beetles. Other arthropods (like the
bombardiers mentioned above) have ‘reactor glands’ constructed in such a way that
chemical precursors of the secretion are mixed only at the moment of discharge.
Not all defensive/offensive secretions are purely chemical in effect. Many arthro-
pods manufacture sticky or slimy materials that mechanically hinder predators.
From their pointed cephalic nozzles Nasutitermes soldiers (Fig. 5.10) eject a sticky,
resinous terpene-containing secretion that dries quickly in the air. Mechanically
incapacitating or even killing insect predators, it also acts as an alarm substance
inducing other termites to converge upon the site. The nymphs of some cockroach
species have rear ends coated with a sticky secretion; when attacked by ants, they
sling slimy droplets over some distance. Although analytical studies have failed to
reveal any chemically active substances in this material, ants targeted by the fluid
are instantly incapacitated. As a final example of secretion taken to the extreme, the
workers of a tropical formicine ant protect their colony by literally blowing them-
selves apart. The large mandibular glands of these ants take up not only the head,
but much of the abdomen. When an ant is under assault, it compresses its abdomen
so rapidly that it bursts, casting the sticky contents of its mandibular glands in all
directions, effectively trapping attackers in the glue.
Fig. 5.10 Squirting string. Soldiers of Nasutitermes orienting to a predator. These termites pos-
sess a hollow nozzle at the front of their heads, from which they shoot strands of a sticky secretion
produced in their frontal glands. Attacking arthropods find the tacky filaments almost impossible
to remove; the material is also a potent irritant
5.4 Active Messages 209
Fig. 5.11 Mimicking a predator? (center) Lanternaria, a large fulgorid bug from Panama, has
a hollow sham ‘head’ and lizard-like markings that cause it to resemble a small carnivore such as
a gecko (top). If this first line of defense fails, it flashes open its wings to reveal vivid eye spots
(bottom)
5.4 Active Messages 211
Fig. 5.12 Startling display. The mature larva of the tiger swallowtail, Papilio glaucus, responds
to danger by abruptly everting its forked defensive gland, the osmeterium, from beneath its neck
integument. The side nearest the danger tends to be extruded furthest; in some cases the caterpillar
may even arch its body and wipe the horns directly upon the offender. The osmeterium, which
contains butyric acid, is commonly colored bright yellow or red. Thus, the defense is probably a
combination of a visual threat and a repellent odor
Fig. 5.13 Imagining a resemblance? Apparent mimicry of arboreal carnivores by a large but-
terfly, Caligo, that rests by day on tree trunks. (a) The mimetic pattern on the butterfly’s wings is
said to include not only the eyes but the head profile, pectoral region, and amphibian typanum. (b)
The supposed model is the tree frog Hyla crepitans, a widespread predator that also spends the day
resting on tree trunks
212 5 Defense: A Survival Catalogue
at close range as larger rivals of their own species, the lizards avoid them. At the
same time, the larger Caligo resemble full-grown tree frogs that are quite capable of
eating lizards, and thus the lizards avoid them as well.
Another hypothesis, not mutually exclusive, is that eye spots may distract or
redirect a predator’s attention from more vulnerable body parts. Many of the small
lycaenid butterflies commonly known as ‘blues’ have eye spots on the underside
of their hind wings and, close to them, long wing ‘tails’ that somewhat resemble
antennae. It has been suggested that these ‘tails’, which are kept in motion by the
resting butterfly, draw attack away from the true head where a wound would be fatal.
Attacking the eye spot instead, a predator gains only a piece of wing for its efforts.
It is certainly true that for most vertebrates, ourselves included, an eye spot has an
inherent conspicuousness that is difficult to ignore (Fig. 5.14).
Many cryptic insects use flash coloration in reverse, making normal camouflage
more effective through sudden contrasts. Thus, for example, many grasshoppers that
are colored to match the stony or sandy soil when they settle have hind wings of bril-
liant red, blue, or yellow. The grasshoppers are highly conspicuous in flight, but they
conceal their hind wings so abruptly upon landing that they appear to vanish. A wide
variety of other startling display behaviors have also evolved in otherwise cryptic
insects. Some orthopterans perform stereotyped defensive displays when discovered
(Fig. 5.15), raising their long spiny legs in the air over their heads. Various moths
react like whirling dervishes, dramatically flapping their wings about and rocking
from side to side, thus warming up their flight muscles while temporarily startling
their predator. In this way they can often escape before the predator is able to react.
Fig. 5.14 No ignoring it! This diagram illustrates the inherent conspicuousness of an eye spot,
which attracts attention to itself in preference to a variety of other, and even larger, objects in the
visual field
5.4 Active Messages 213
Fig. 5.15 Flailing in defense. Display in two normally cryptic orthopterans. (a) The weta
Deinacrida heteracantha from New Zealand. (b) The katydid Neobarettia spinosa from Mexico
The strategy of startling a potential predator includes another very dramatic and
widespread behavior. In the same way that a fleeing escapee might slip free from
a jacket grasped by his assailant, many insects elude potential predators by leav-
ing behind various dispensable body coverings, a phenomenon known as autotomy.
Perhaps the most familiar of these are the easily shed scales which cover the wings
of butterflies and moths; the loose hairs of adult caddisflies and the scales of sil-
verfish and bristletails may be similar protections against entrapment. Legs of crane
flies represent a more extreme example of dispensable body parts. The larvae of cer-
tain carpet beetles use interlocking hairs for defense; on their abdomens, they bear
prominent tufts of barbed setae that easily detach and hopelessly entangle small
predators such as ants.
Detachable coverings need not be strictly morphological. Gathering on their back
a variety of debris from feces to old cuticle to the sucked-out remains of prey, larvae
of groups as diverse as assassin bugs and lacewings construct shields for protection
and camouflage. Other species are able to maneuver their shield with considerable
agility against predators, in actions that sometimes cross over from defense to attack
(see Fig. 5.8).
a number of closely spaced individuals. Such defensive behavior probably also has
elements of a mass startle effect.
Mobbing, a type of group defense in which a predator is harassed by a number
of maneuverable prey individuals, has long been known among colonially nest-
ing birds. Many social and/or communal insects have also developed this effective
behavior. Bembix sand wasps, for example, nest in aggregations; when a potential
predator approaches, a score of male wasps may fly up at it, buzzing ominously
and loudly but harmlessly. While mobbing, prey animals may approach, threaten,
or occasionally physically attack a predator. Often, even though the predator is not
actually injured, it moves off. This is probably not only due to the harassment but
also to the fact that its probability of successfully capturing a prey is very low amidst
all the commotion.
Chapter 6
Chemical Communication
6.1 Introduction
Over a century ago, the French naturalist Jean-Henri Fabré confined an unmated
female giant peacock moth in a container hidden on his desk; the following evening,
over 40 male moths arrived, in Fabré’s words, ‘eager to pay their respects to their
marriageable bride born that morning.’ The container was not airtight, and because
even the empty container was of intense interest to the attracted males, Fabré sug-
gested that the attraction must be chemical, but he was puzzled because he could
not smell anything emanating from his female moth.
Communication through chemistry has been found to be a persistent theme
in almost all animal taxa. Chemical communication is both the oldest and the
dominant method of communication among organisms; early metazoans relied
extensively on chemical communication for timing gamete release and to mediate
the union of gametes.
Biologically significant chemicals have been found in both sexes and all life
stages of many species, not just of insects but spanning the zoological universe.
These are chemical hotlines, not polite conversational perfumes. They communi-
cate matters vital to survival, such as the presence of danger, the identification of
friends and foes, the call to arms or emigration, the availability of food, and the urge
to reproduce.
Today the role of odors in animal communication is so well established that it
is difficult to appreciate that it has been only relatively recently that advances in
chemistry and technology have permitted humans to eavesdrop on the private lives
of insects and other organisms where chemical communication is often the dominant
mode. To date, the largest number of communication chemicals in the animal world
has been found among the members of the largest class of animals, the insects. It
is here that they have been studied longest and here that science knows most about
their modes of action.
hangs from a wooden post and smiles with satisfaction: the sticky tray top is covered
with small rust-and-yellow moths. As he starts back toward his pickup truck, the
man pauses to watch a flying moth. It zigzags, comes closer, then hovers over the
tray, extending its genital claspers. As its wingtips brush the cardboard and are
caught by the gummy surface, its abdomen curves upward, throbbing convulsively
as the moth tries to mate with a polyethylene disc in the center of the tray.
What has happened here? The scene was a carefully chosen test site, the man
a field entomologist; the moths were male red-banded leaf rollers (Argyrotaenia
velutinana). The plastic disc was dipped in a compound that smelled like a secretion
produced by a virgin female red-banded leaf roller. Male moths were lured by the
smell to the trap where, instead of mating, these destructive apple orchard pests
were captured.
The observation that odors secreted by some insects function as stimulants, incit-
ing those of the opposite sex to mate seems, rather obvious today. In fact, the
German medical doctor and botanist Philipp Franz von Siebold proposed such an
idea about 170 years ago. But during the 1800s the phenomenon of chemical com-
munication was largely ignored or disbelieved. Even Fabré, who easily accepted the
idea that insects could detect the odors of other insects, could not believe that such
odors could operate over such long distances; it simply strained credibility to think
that a single female moth could inject into the vast atmosphere enough of a sub-
stance to be perceived by males kilometers away. This would be, he thought, like
tinting an entire lake with a single drop of dye.
to the abdomens and ignored the remainder. How were the odors being received?
Males with their abdomens cut off still responded; so did males whose spiracles
were covered with glue. However, males whose antennae were gummed up did not
seek females. In fact, they showed no excitement even when held within 2.5 cm of
virgin females.
Insects produce their odors in many ways; this variety and complexity is par-
ticularly striking among the social species (Fig. 6.1). Chemicals come not just
from the abdomen but often from the head or thorax, produced by exocrine glands
(clusters of secretory cells whose products are discharged to the outside of the
body). Sometimes, single chemicals are produced, but more commonly they are
complex blends.
Once produced, the chemicals are sent forth as liquid streams, droplets, thin
films, aerosols, or gases. Emission rates and concentrations may be controlled
through adjustable nozzles, retracting applicators, evaporation pads, or other equally
elaborate devices. Among the more spectacular are the male arctiid moths that pos-
sess feather-like coremata that can be pumped in and out to waft their chemicals
into the evening air (see Plate 16).
Producing chemicals is only part of the picture, of course; receiving and reacting
to them is the other part. Just as with vertebrate animals, one can say that insects
both taste and smell. Also like in most vertebrates, the chemosensory system of
insects seems to rely far more heavily upon smell than on taste. This is probably
no surprise. Taste involves the reception of waterborne compounds with a limited
range of qualities, often in fairly high concentrations, at close range. Odor, on the
other hand, involves reception of chemicals over a greater range and often at much
lower concentrations; though these chemicals are usually in gaseous form, they may
also be either airborne or waterborne. Because the two senses are so closely allied,
rather than considering them separately, many biologists simply speak of insect
chemoproduction and chemoreception.
How are tastes and smells received? The external surface of insects is cov-
ered in cuticle, so it is the place to begin. On various structures, particularly the
antennae, mouthparts, legs and ovipositor, chemoreceptor cells provide ‘windows’
to the outside world. These are surrounded by the characteristic cuticular sensilla
that comprise the most obvious external parts of every chemosensory organ. They
occur in at least four morphological forms: bristles or hairs, pegs, plates, and pits
(Fig. 6.2).
All chemosensory sensilla have one or more pores through which chemicals can
pass. Gustatory and olfactory sensilla are quite similar structurally, but the former
have only a single pore and the latter have many. Once an odor molecule passes
through the pore(s) it binds with specific proteins, triggering a cascade of reac-
tions that ferry it through the fluid media of the sensillum lumen to the dendrite
of a nerve where it activates receptors and is changed into electrical action poten-
tials that travel along axons to the brain. Axons from gustatory sensilla on the head
lead to the subesophagael ganglion; those from olfactory sensilla terminate in the
deutocerebrum. Sensory filtering occurs at multiple levels in the nervous system,
but initial specificity and sensitivity is achieved by the odorant binding proteins.
220 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.2 Perceiving odors. Scanning electron micrograph (3,900×), showing part of the surface
of segment 10 of a worker honey bee’s antennal flagellum. Four types of sensilla are evident: sf =
sensilla companiformia, sp = sensilla placodea, sb = sensilla basiconica, II = sensilla trichodea.
The arrow points to the pore at the tip of one sensillum. A single antenna may have more than
8,000 sensilla trichodea alone. The sensillae companiformia are likely contact chemoreceptors
To function, the system also requires a rapid enzymatic breakdown of the odorant
and other chemical flotsam that may impact it over time. The process by which the
receptor’s active site(s) are cleared, renewing its sensitivity to incoming molecules,
must happen very quickly (on the order of milliseconds) to permit neural integra-
tion of the rapid directional changes that are observed during an insect’s movements
along an odor gradient. This striking behavior depends on antennae, the best-studied
chemosensory receptors of insects. Use of two identical sets of receptors proba-
bly serves to maximize the sensitivity and efficiency of the system. Moths flying
upwind in an odor plume and ants following an odor trail characteristically trace a
zigzag route in which each antenna appears to be moved alternately in and out of the
odor field. The ‘out’ antenna suddenly stops sending signals to the brain; as though
seeking to restore a balance of input from the two sides, the brain causes a steering
change which soon results in overcompensation toward the opposite side, and so on.
If both antennae remained continuously in the odor field, habituation might quickly
occur. By passing in and out of the threshold concentration level, maximal sensitiv-
ity to the stimulus would be likely to persist. In addition, such a system imparts a
clear orientation axis to the signal path. Orientation experiments using ants or bees
with amputated or crossed antennae lend support to these ideas (Fig. 6.3).
The sensitivity of many insects to odor molecules is further enhanced by antenna
design, as is evident from a brief glance at one well-studied pair of antennae, those
of the male silkworm moth (Fig. 6.4). It appears that the spacing and arrangement
of the antennal hairs allows them to act like molecular sieves; the width of the mesh
they form is so small that chemical molecules, because of their fast thermal move-
ments, cannot pass through without contacting the hairs and being preferentially
absorbed. The physics of airflow over structures this small obviously also must fac-
tor into their design. The sensitivity of male gypsy moths is likewise so great that
scientists estimate that the 30 g of disparlure, a synthetic gypsy moth sex attrac-
tant, already on hand will be enough to bait some 60,000 traps per year for the next
50,000 years.
222 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.3 Following a chemical trail. Trail following by Lasius worker ants with normal and
altered antennae after trail odor was applied along the straight line. (a) Normal trail following, a
tropotaxis in which the ant zigzags evenly first to one side then to the other as its paired antennal
receptors alternately move in and out of the trail’s vapor space and perceive a diffusion gradient.
Each time an antenna leaves the odor tunnel, the ant swings back to the opposite side. (b) With
its left antenna amputated, the ant repeatedly overcorrects to the right side. (c) With its antennae
crossed and glued, the ant is disoriented and relocates the trail with difficulty; its overall progress
in the proper direction is probably mediated by visual cues
Fig. 6.4 Finding females. In response to the odor of a female, the male silkworm moth Bombyx
mori begins fluttering its wings at 40-50Hz. Males of the commercial strain, having lost the abil-
ity to fly, walk toward the odor source while continually vibrating their wings, an action that
increases the flow of air over the antennae about 15 times more than just walking. The male’s
plumose antennae each contain 60–70 branches. Each branch is in turn covered with about 17,000
olfactory hairs that average 100 μ in length. About half of these receptors are tuned to detect
the female’s sex pheromone, whereas the antennae of the female do not react to this scent at
all. Electrophysiological recordings indicate that when a single biologically relevant molecule
strikes a single receptor, it elicits a detectable response. However, about 200 molecules must arrive
simultaneously before the male begins an upwind searching movement
noise. For reliable efficiency, some of the better man-made electronic detection sys-
tems currently require input about twice the background noise (a signal-to-noise
ratio of about 2:1). In contrast, the male silkworm moth shows a positive behavioral
response at the incredibly low signal-to-noise ratio of 0.125:1.
While these techniques are useful for screening potential attractant and deter-
rent chemicals, full behavior-based laboratory and field trials are needed before a
chemical can be conclusively said to be communicatory (Fig. 6.5), because physio-
logical state, ecological context, and nervous integration are all intimately involved
in behavioral activation.
The story of Fabré and his peacock moths is generally accepted as the first defini-
tive evidence of communication by chemistry, but almost a century passed before
microanalytical techniques reached the point in development that would permit
identification of the minute quantities of chemical that were involved. Beginning in
the period after World War II, however, the field of insect chemical communication
grew quickly. As knowledge about chemical communication systems has grown,
the subject has rapidly become more complex, and so has the terminology to deal
with it.
224 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.5 Proving chemical communication. Example of the procedure leading to identification of
the aggregation attractant of the bark beetle Ips pini. The stages involve the collection, purification,
identification, synthesis, and field testing. At nearly every stage appropriately designed behavioral
bioassays are essential
6.2 Mechanisms of Chemical Communication 225
In 1959, the German chemists Peter Karlson and Adolf Butenandt and the Swiss
zoologist Martin Lüscher proposed a new name for some of these messenger chem-
icals to replace the contradictory term ‘ectohormone’ then in use. From the Greek
pherein, to carry, and horman, to excite, they derived the word pheromone—a sub-
stance secreted by an animal that affects the behavior of other animals of the same
species. Initially it was thought that a pheromone would be a single chemical, but
as detection and bioassay methods became more sophisticated, most pheromones
proved to be blends of two or more components (see below) that are typically active
in very low concentrations. That same year, the first pheromone was chemically iso-
lated and identified—the sex attractant of the commercial silkworm, Bombyx mori
(Case Study 6.1).
Today powerful new devices are routine in the chemist’s toolbox, revolutioniz-
ing the study of animal and plant odors, and a single insect may be sufficient
to identify and characterize an emitted odorant molecule. However, the first
time took 30 years and over 500,00 moths.
Bombyx mori is the most famous caterpillar in the world—the silkworm
whose cocoon, spun from a single strand 500 to 1,300 yards long, furnishes
the material for a thread and cloth not yet duplicated by any synthetic fiber.
The only truly domesticated insect, the silkworm moth can no longer maintain
itself in a natural environment but survives only under cultivation, where most
individuals live only until the pupal stage, then are plunged into boiling water
and their cocoons unraveled. However, in each generation a few moths are
allowed to survive to furry, greenish-white adulthood to furnish eggs for new
progeny. Neither sex can fly, and therefore the male cannot easily scout the
terrain to find a mate. Yet it had long been known that female Bombyx in
some manner attract males from extraordinary distances (see Fig. 6.4).
In 1939, the German chemist Adolf Butenandt, who had previously gained
fame for his discovery of the identity of human sex hormones, reasoned that
a biochemical lure in the bodies of virgin females was probably responsi-
ble. Although his available tools would now be regarded as primitive—he
lacked such now-standard equipment as the gas chromatograph—he began to
work on the isolation and identification of the substance. By 1959, Butenandt
and his associates had processed half a million female silkworm moths and
had extracted from the abdomen tips just twelve-thousandths of a gram of a
derivative of the active compound.
Next, the researchers used a combination of chromatography, infrared and
ultraviolet spectroscopy, and chemical structural analysis coupled with bio-
logical assays to see which fractions of this derivative elicited male wing
fanning. They obtained a substance which, in minute quantities, was as attrac-
tive to male silkworm moths as the most seductive virgin female. It was a
226 6 Chemical Communication
Within a few years after Butenandt’s remarkable feat, Edward O. Wilson and
William H. Bossert proposed dividing pheromones into two functional groups
according to their mode of influence: releasers and primers. Releaser pheromones
stimulate an immediate and reversible behavioral response mediated wholly by
the nervous system; these pheromones are thus by definition chemical ‘releasers’
in the terminology of the ethologist (see Chapter 2). They are widespread in
insects and serve a great many functions, sex attraction and alarm being especially
important ones.
Primer pheromones, on the other hand, act to physiologically alter the endocrine
and reproductive systems of the receptor animal, re-programming it for an altered
response pattern. In a sense, the receptor’s body is ‘primed’ for new biological
activity, although such activity may not appear until some future time and may
require triggering by another releaser pheromone. Primers are best known in social
insects where they mediate a range of social interactions; in a highly populous
colony where it would be impossible for the queen to physically control the work-
ers, they have obvious adaptive significance. Primer pheromones are also important
in desert locusts, mediating maturation and phase change that facilitates swarm
synchrony.
Other attention turned to substances that transmit external chemical messages
that affect individuals or populations of a species different from their source. These
were termed allelochemicals (Greek: allelon, of one another). Allelochemicals may
affect the growth, health, behavior, or population biology of other species. Two
categories of allelochemicals have been recognized: allomones and kairomones.
Allomones are chemical agents of adaptive advantage to the organism sending them;
kairomones (Greek: kairos, opportunistic), on the other hand, are of adaptive value
to the organism receiving them.
All pheromones fall under the broader umbrella classification of semiochem-
icals or infochemicals. In short, an infochemical is any chemical produced by
one organism that incites a response in another organism. Figure 6.6 puts some
semiochemicals into their behavioral context.
Even with so much terminology, some chemicals that affect behavior do not fit
neatly; for example, compounds that are released by fermenting and decaying fruit
attract parasitoid wasps that attack the larvae of certain fruit flies. A number of
other categories have since been proposed, but classifying the diverse responses of
organisms to chemicals does not necessarily enhance understanding. Furthermore,
the same compound may sometimes have multiple roles depending upon context,
and, as we have noted before, the very labeling of behavioral phenomena tends to
color subsequent interpretations of it.
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 227
Fig. 6.6 Classifying semiochemicals. Pheromone releaser effects are classical stimulus-response
reactions mediated wholly by the central nervous system. With primer effects, behavior is usually
induced not by the pheromone but by later external stimuli for which the pheromone has ‘primed’
the animal’s physiology
A pair of bright orange patterned butterflies cavort in the air above a patch of gold-
enrod flowers, performing loops and tight rolls, one occasionally hovering briefly
above the other, rising and falling on invisible air currents. Below them on the
ground several ants scurry along an invisible line weaving among the plants. Close
examination of the goldenrod reveals a group of yellow aphids along some of the
stems, some of whom have glistening droplets of liquid on the tips of their corni-
cles; near them a black spotted lady bug is contently munching on one less fortunate
group member. What do these different observations have in common? All are
228 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.7 Calling males from a distance. (a) Position adopted by a female Indian meal moth,
Plodia interpunctella, during sex pheromone release. During calling the abdominal tip is elevated
and glands in the intersegmental membrane between the eighth and ninth abdominal segments
are extruded. A similar stance is taken by most calling moths as well as by a number of other
unrelated insects. (b) A wingless thynnid wasp female releases her sex attractant in a characteristic
‘calling’ position atop a grass blade. However, competing for the winged male’s attention is the
flower of an orchid, Drakea (at left) that deceptively signals with the same odor and ‘feel’ as
the female wasp. Male wasps are poor discriminators visiting several orchid flowers that they
pollinate
A well-known prediction from sexual selection theory is that the limiting sex
(almost always, the female) should put less effort or risk into display and attraction,
not more, than the less-limited sex. In other chapters, we have noted that males are
the producers of most visual or acoustic calling signals, as this theory would pre-
dict. So why should it be the female insect that produces these pheromone signals?
There are some reasonable non-exclusive ‘arm-chair theories’ but few experimental
studies to address this question. Signals in the chemical sphere may be energetically
less expensive than producing sounds or active visual displays, and they may involve
less risk of predators homing in on their source. It is also quite likely that males are
actually carrying the larger burden of risk because their flight is energetically costly
and exposes them to a higher risk of predation than the generally hidden, calling
female.
It would be overly simplistic, however, to think of insect sexual attraction as
simply ‘females signal, males respond.’ In many cases, odors also come into play
once the sexes have been drawn within range of each other by other cues. They may
be produced by either sex, but are usually emitted by the male and often as only part
of a complex pattern of courtship behavior.
These male-emitted chemicals are delivered in several ways. The male monarch
butterfly and its relatives display one type of system (Fig. 6.8); hairpencils are
extrusible organs that function as tiny scent-filled brushes, wafting pheromone-rich
clouds at close range over the female’s antennae. Similar structures in male moths
are called coremata. In some butterflies, special glandular scales called androconia
occur on the male’s wings. A courting male of the grayling butterfly (Hipparchia
230 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.8 Delivering perfumes to nearby females. Paired extrusible scent organs (hairpencils) at
the tip of the abdomen of a hand-held male Lycorea ceres (Nymphalidae, Danainae), fully splayed
open
semele) will actually clasp the female’s antennae between his wings, bringing them
into contact with these scent scales.
From tergal glands on their abdomens, several species of male cockroaches
produce secretions on which the females feed prior to copulation (Fig. 6.9); the
substance has been given the whimsical name ‘seducin’. Other male-produced
Fig. 6.9 Maneuvering with lures. A courting male cockroach, Nauphoeta cinerea, raises his
wings to offer secretions from his tergal gland secretion to a potential mate. When a female crawls
upon his back to feed she is in the proper position for the male to connect his genitalia with hers,
and if successful they will copulate in an end-to-end position
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 231
chemically diverse substances even include secretions passed to the female with
sperm, which have the potential to affect many aspects of female behavior and
physiology.
Only a few insects reverse roles from the very beginning, with the males pro-
ducing long distance attractants. Most (but by no means all) of these are beetles.
Male callers tend to occur in one of two situations. The first is with insects that
gather in courting associations called leks (see Chapter 9); these males often use
pheromones to attract females, especially among tephritid fruit flies, anthophorid
bees, and various ants and social and solitary wasps.
The other situation is when males locate patchy resources needed by females
and call, as occurs with male papaya fruit flies that call from fruits that will be
oviposition sites. Sometimes, especially among beetles, both sexes may be attracted
by the same scent; at an appropriate location such as a suitable host plant or other
resource, they congregate and mate. Even in cases in which the original function of
the pheromones involved is thought to have been sexual attraction, when both sexes
are attracted these chemicals are generally called aggregation pheromones.
One or both sexes of some insect species use cuticular hydrocarbons as sex
pheromones. Cuticular hydrocarbons are relatively involatile molecules that func-
tion at very close range or require contact for detection. They often play fundamental
roles in species identification. In Drosophila fruit flies they are important in
maintaining reproductive isolation between sibling species. When a courting D.
melanogaster male taps a prospective mate with his front legs he receives con-
tact pheromone signals via chemoreceptors on his tarsi; if it is the correct cuticular
hydrocarbon for females of his species, he vibrates his wings and courtship proceeds
to the next stage.
Unsurprisingly, the majority of the research on insect sex pheromones has been
economically motivated and practically oriented. The specificity of such com-
pounds makes it possible in theory to single out one particular pest species for
detection, monitoring, and/or control. Manipulation of insect behavior by means
of sex pheromones is a promising alternative to the use of conventional chemical
insecticides, although in actual practice it often has been fraught with technical
problems.
always, the signals that call them together are unrelated in any direct manner to the
subsequent activity. Aggregation, the crowding together of individuals, is assembly’s
result. Usually, except in the social insects, aggregations are temporary groupings
and pheromones are only one of several ways in which they are promoted and/or
maintained. Among those insects possessing aposematic coloration (see Chapter
5), aggregations induced by pheromones are particularly prevalent. For example,
a male-produced pheromone attracts brightly colored lycid beetles of both sexes
to form prominent clusters, sending potential predators very conspicuous adver-
tisements of their distasteful nature, as do the large hibernation aggregations of
distasteful ladybird beetles (see Plate 5). Aggregations and other nonsexual asso-
ciations are classified in Table 10.1, and the costs and benefits of such behavior are
discussed in Chapter 10.
In conservation biology, aggregation size assumes particular importance in the
context of threatened and endangered species because under-population has a num-
ber of serious consequences. Collectively these consequences are broadly referred
to as Allee effects, after the pioneering animal behaviorist, Warner Clyde Allee,
who first elucidated them in detail. Allee effects are (broadly) defined as a decline
in individual fitness at low population size or density. If group size falls too low,
the reduction or loss of behavioral interactions between individuals can constitute a
severe threat to a species’ survival or even doom its very existence. (The extinction
of the passenger pigeon is a noteworthy example.)
One recurrent benefit of aggregations is to permit more effective exploitation
of the environment than would be possible for single individuals. For example,
the caterpillar-like larvae of the sawfly, Neodiprion pratti banksianae, feed in tight
groups upon jack-pine trees. When young larvae were experimentally isolated from
their companions they suffered 80% mortality, whereas among those allowed to
remain in groups, only 53% died. Why? Newly hatched larvae have considerable
difficulty chewing holes into the tough cuticle of the jack-pine needles. Each larva,
even in groups, individually attempts to establish its own feeding site. When finally
one cuts through into the inner tissues successfully, other larvae are quickly attracted
to the cut, where their feeding widens the breach still further until soon all the larvae
are able to feed. Another benefit, also illustrated well by Neodiprion, is enhanced
defense (see Plate 17).
In still other cases, gregarious behavior clearly functions to bring the sexes
together for mating, sometimes in combination with attraction to suitable host
resources and oviposition sites. Bark beetles are a good example (see Case
Study 6.2).
Small cylindrical scolytid bark beetles destroy millions of board feet of stand-
ing timber each year. Exactly how do they manage recruitment for their mass
attacks?
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 233
Typically, beetles of only one sex initially select the individual host tree.
(In monogamous Dendroctonus beetles the female excavates the entrance tunnel,
whereas in polygamous Ips the male performs this function.) Then as the pio-
neer beetles begin to attack, they discharge a long-range pheromone derived from
ingested host monoterpenes. This simultaneously serves two purposes: attracting
additional individuals of both sexes and inducing the opposite sex to enter the nuptial
chambers and mate (Fig. 6.10). In low concentrations, in synergistic combination
with other host- and beetle-produced chemicals, the pheromone attracts both sexes
and causes males to stop and stridulate. At higher concentrations, it prevents flight
aggregation of both sexes.
The communication system of bark beetles would seem to have high adaptive
value, promoting an even distribution of available mates, preventing overcrowd-
ing with subsequent brood mortality, and allowing the cooperative mass attack
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 235
Fig. 6.10 Starting and stopping an aggregation. Summary of the interaction of olfactory and
auditory stimuli in the aggregation response of the Douglas fir beetle and its subsequent inhibition
While both sexes are attracted, it does not necessarily follow that their inter-
actions denote cooperation (although there may be Allee effect benefits to the
signaler). Responders may be exploiting the senders’ message as a shortcut to host
location without assuming the risks of attacking resisting trees—in essence, cheat-
ing. Responding males may be eavesdropping, hoping to gain mating opportunities
by intercepting some of the females that are attracted. Context is also important.
Low densities of beetles would be unlikely to be able to subdue a living tree and
thus would achieve little or no per capita reproduction, but an already-dead host
would present little challenge.
If initial colonizers of live hosts experience high mortality, might their behav-
ior be regarded as altruism (see Chapter 10)? Or are they perhaps manipulating
responders to help them defeat the host? Depending on context, there are likely to
be some evolutionary trade-offs; the trick is to maximize the odds of successful host
colonization while minimizing the impact of competitors. Also, it should be noted
that conspecifics are not the only players. The same pheromones that serve as aggre-
gation attractants also are exploited as kairomones by a suite of generalist and spe-
cialist predators and parasites of the beetles and these predators and parasites may
disproportionately affect late-arriving beetles, increasing their costs relative to their
benefits. Finally, recall from Chapter 4 that plants are not passive in their interac-
tions with insects. They might be expected to counter by evolving ways to interfere
with the beetle’s communication system, possibly masking or inhibiting signals.
Many other examples of insect aggregations occur, many of which are
pheromone mediated. Cockroaches often congregate inside of kitchen cabinets.
When experimentally offered simple roosts made of filter paper, groups of Blatella
germanica cockroach nymphs invariably preferred to aggregate on those kept in the
stock cockroach culture for several days rather than roosts of fresh filter paper of
the same size (Fig. 6.11). When their antennae were removed, however, the cock-
roaches aggregated about equally on both roost types, thus implicating a role for
olfactory cues in their preferred choices. In order to determine the source of the puta-
tive pheromone, various cockroach body parts were isolated and washed in ether,
and fresh filter paper impregnated with the extracts. Abdominal extracts caused the
strongest aggregation, and eventually the pheromone source was determined to be
in the feces, presumably originating from rectal pad cells.
The adaptive significance of cockroach aggregations is still not entirely clear.
However, it has been shown experimentally that German cockroaches reared in iso-
lation grow at a slower rate and suffer greater mortality compared to those raised
in groups. Amelioration of the physical environment may also be involved, since
individuals in groups would be less prone to desiccation. In nature such aggrega-
tion could also benefit individual survival in the face of predation, a selfish herd
effect. Interestingly, cockroach aggregation pheromones do not appear to be species
specific; nymphs of several unrelated species in three different cockroach families
aggregate in response to each others’ pheromones. However, nymphs appeared more
responsive to their own species’ pheromones, and a few species appeared to be
repelled by fouling produced by other species. In addition, at least two species pro-
duce their aggregation pheromone in their mandibular glands, so one cannot simply
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 237
Fig. 6.11 Investigating roach aggregation. Sixty first instar nymphs of the German cockroach,
Blattella germanica, were introduced into a glass arena with three folded filter paper roosts: two
made of fresh paper, the other (at upper left) of paper left in a stock roach culture for 24 hours
prior to the experiment. Progressively greater preference for the conditioned roost occurred through
time: (1) start of experiment; (2) after 1 minute; (3) after 25 minutes; (4) after 45 minutes
assume that all species of cockroach produce a fecal aggregation pheromone simply
because other species collect on paper contaminated by them.
Although most aggregations are temporary, as we have mentioned, some are per-
sistent, most notably those forming social insect colonies. Those colonies whose
social lives are most highly developed exhibit extensive direct food sharing (trophal-
laxis, see Chapter 4). Also nearly universal among the social insects is clustering
behavior. Workers removed from a nest and placed in bare surroundings will quickly
gather in one or several little groups. If the mother queen and/or some larvae are with
them, this occurs even more rapidly and the grouping is even tighter.
Of all the types of pheromonally induced insect assembly, perhaps the most dra-
matic is exhibited by a fertilized social insect queen, continuously surrounded by
a retinue of crowding, licking, food-offering attendants (Fig. 6.12). The effect is
least marked in small colonies or where there are multiple laying queens. However,
tightly massed workers may number into the hundreds upon the distended bodies of
the ‘physogastric’ queens of army ants, fire ants, and termites. Several investigators
238 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.12 Regulating the workforce. (left) A honey bee queen is surrounded by a retinue of
workers attracted to her by queen mandibular pheromone. (right) As the schematics show, the
pheromone component called homovanillyl alcohol (HV) bears a striking structural resemblance
to dopamine, a biogenic amine that plays a central role in insect behavioral regulation and motor
control
have shown that pheromones are involved; with army ants, in fact, merely allowing
the queen to sit on untreated balsawood strips will transfer enough scent to make
the strips highly attractive to workers.
Whenever joint efforts are needed—whether for exploiting a food source, repair-
ing a breach in a nest, or moving to a desirable new nest site—individuals of a great
many insects, particularly among social species, are able to chemically summon
others of their kind. Although the category is admittedly a loose one, this special
case of assembly has been termed recruitment, that is, communication that brings
conspecific individuals, often nest-mates, to some point in space where work is
required.
The simplest cases of recruitment appear to border on the unintentional. Honey
bee workers, for example, can recognize food odors both from smells adhering to the
bodies of successful foragers and by the scent of nectar regurgitated by them. Even
in the absence of communicative dancing, workers that have previously encountered
a similar odor will search the site for it again. In a slightly more advanced form of
recruitment, some social insects leave chemical ‘footprints’ to attract others, such
as the odor trails laid by both walking honey bees and some wasps in the vicinity of
their nests. While honey bee recruitment to pollen and nectar resources is mediated
by the famous dance language (see Chapter 8), there is also a unique scent to the
dance. Waggle-dancing bees also release small amounts of four hydrocarbons into
the air space surrounding them, and these odors stimulate the dance followers to exit
the hive, thereby increasing foraging activity for the colony.
Recruitment undoubtedly has reached its highest development among the ants,
bees, wasps, and termites, though it certainly is not restricted to them. Although
trail following has long fascinated biologists, the mechanism behind this behavior
was unclear until 1959 when it was first elucidated in fire ants, Solenopsis invicta.
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 239
Fig. 6.13 Studying recruitment. (left) Apparatus for experimental analysis of recruitment to food
in the eastern tent caterpillar, Malacosoma americanum. Caterpillars roost in a silken tent to which
they continually add silk secreted by their labial glands. (right) At intervals, groups travel out along
a branch in search of food, laying silk along the route; if productive food patches are found, the
caterpillars feed and then return to the tent, this time heavily impregnating the silk trail with a
pheromone (principally 5β-cholestan-3-one) from a sternal gland at the tips of their abdomens.
Other hungry caterpillars selectively follow the trails back to the food source
240 6 Chemical Communication
The tent caterpillar system has feedback loops similar to those in the mass
communication system of the fire ant, in that recruitment is elective. Individual
caterpillars mark in response to the quality of the food site (which in addition to
nutrition, may also reflect density of caterpillars), and other individuals use this
information to evaluate food patch quality and preferentially move out only to
highest quality patches. The overall effect promotes a high degree of coordinated
synchrony among the entire group, increasing the efficiency not only of foraging,
but also of thermoregulation (important for food processing), and defense. Group
members are initially all siblings, but groups often merge when more than one egg
case was laid on the same tree. Apparently bigger is better—experimental sibling
groups of 100 caterpillars grew to 150% the body mass of their siblings that were in
groups of only 30 caterpillars.
Alarm and defensive behavior often go hand in hand, and in some cases a single
substance simultaneously releases both behaviors. For example, disturbed nymphs
of the pyrrhocorid bug Dysdercus intermedius secrete a fluid from the third dorsal
scent gland; the compound is used simultaneously for defense and for releasing
alarm behavior. In other cases, different components of a pheromone blend release
different aspects of the response. For example, one of the multiple components of
the sting-released honey bee pheromone has been identified as isopentyl acetate,
which smells like bananas; alone on a cotton ball it continues to attract and anger
other worker bees but does not release attack behavior.
Alarm and alert pheromones are alike in that both are produced under con-
ditions of immediate or potential threat, but the responses they elicit grade into
several functional categories; defense, dispersal, agitation, aggregation, and recruit-
ment are among the more common. For example, subterranean ants of the genus
Acanthomyops have populous colonies crowded into relatively small caverns. When
stimulated by alarm pheromone, they rapidly converge toward the source of dis-
turbance and meet the danger head-on. Related ants in the genus Lasius normally
nest in smaller colonial groups in more exposed situations under rocks or logs; their
response to disturbance is to scatter as if in a panic and run hurriedly about. Both
species employ undecane as the principal component of their alarm pheromones,
but Lasius is much more sensitive at lower concentrations, in effect, possessing an
‘early warning system’ for rapid evacuation when danger threatens.
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 241
Fig. 6.14 Spreading the alarm. As shown in this pheromone bioassay for the green peach aphid,
Myzus persicae, when a filter-paper triangle daubed with E-β-farnesene pheromone secreted from
the aphid’s abdominal cornicles is introduced next to a cluster of feeding aphids, it takes less than
five minutes before most aphids have left the area and some have dropped off. This alarm dispersal
behavior is characteristic of a variety of aphids, but depends on context. Ant-attended aphids tend
to remain feeding, relying on their ant guards for protection. Ants are attracted to E-β-farnesene,
as are some predators, such as the multicolored Asian ladybug. In their glandular leaf hair, some
solanaceous plants produce E-β-farnesene that may help to protect them from aphid attack
242 6 Chemical Communication
6.3.4 Host-Marking
After laying an egg inside or upon a host, bean weevils, various parasitoid wasps,
and a host of other insects leave a pheromone mark (Fig. 6.15); other female
Fig. 6.15 Marking a host. A female Trissolcus basalis (Hymenoptera: Scelionidae) draws her
ovipositor tip across the cap of the host egg, marking the spot where she has just laid her own egg.
Such chemical marking repels other conspecific females and minimizes the likelihood of multiple
oviposition on a single host
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 243
conspecifics usually avoid laying their own eggs in or on these marked hosts.
Ovipositing female Mediterranean fruit flies, Ceratitis capitata, readily discriminate
between infested and uninfested host fruits; in a graded response, they adjust their
propensity to oviposit accordingly, marking hosts after egg deposition and avoiding
or reducing egg clutches in already marked fruits. Apple maggot flies (Rhagoletis
pomenella) behave similarly.
The advantages of such a system seem obvious. Few hosts or host plants can
support an unlimited number of parasites or predators. Too many attacks, and a
host usually will die, often taking its attackers with it. Thus, there would be strong
selective pressure for attackers to develop some way to assess host suitability and
in particular, to detect the presence of preexisting eggs or larvae, whether they
be one’s own, those of a conspecific female, or another species entirely. Marking
pheromones confer advantages both to the signaler and to the receiver. For both,
they reduce competition; an ability to distinguish between occupied and available
hosts and selectively choose the latter enhances the survival of one’s offspring. In a
limited resource the earliest offspring would have a head start over any latecomers,
and likely would triumph in any competition.
Visual inspections can and undoubtedly do sometimes provide cues, but for
insects chemistry appears to be overwhelmingly favored. Host-marking pheromones
have independently evolved many times, and are now known to occur in more than
200 species of parasitoid wasps and more than 30 species of herbivorous insects
across eight families of Diptera, Coleoptera, and Lepidoptera. All lay their eggs in
or on hosts of restricted size that can support the successful development of only
a limited number of offspring. Where marking pheromones have been investigated
in detail, they generally have been shown to be non-volatile chemicals detected by
contact chemoreceptors. A study of this behavior appears in Case Study 6.3.
Why is this solitary ichneumonid wasp, noted for low, slow egg production,
still consistently one of the most common parasitoids on its sawfly hosts?
Many sawflies in the hymenopteran families Diprionidae and
Tenthredinidae feed in the trees as larvae, but drop to the forest floor
below to pupate. Here they may be attacked by any of a number of predators
and parasitoids. In the case of the sawfly Neodiprion swainei, a serious
defoliator of jack pine trees in North America, one of these parasitoids is
the ichneumonid wasp Pleolophus basizonus. Compared to other parasitoids,
this ichneumonid is known to have a rather slow rate of egg production and
oviposition; a typical female lays only two to four eggs per female per day.
Surprisingly, when Peter Price collected sawfly cocoons from different areas
in Quebec, Canada, he found that P. basizonus was consistently the most
abundant parasitoid species to be reared from them. How were these ‘poor
layers’ able to compete so successfully against 18 other species of parasitoids
244 6 Chemical Communication
also competing for the same host? The question was of more than purely
academic interest, since this wasp had been purposely introduced several
years previously for biological control.
Maybe Pleolophus was simply more efficient than her competitors in some
other aspect than oviposition rate, Price thought. For example, the wasp might
search more efficiently or regulate her oviposition more carefully. He placed
three mated female Pleolophus with varying numbers of sawfly cocoons
into each of several caged ‘arenas’ that simulated the sandy, lichen-covered
forest floor in careful detail. Six days later, he split open the cocoons to
count the number of eggs laid within. At all cocoon densities he tested, the
frequency with which he found only one Pleolophus egg per Neodiprion
cocoon was much higher than expected from a random attack. In fact, at
the highest cocoon densities, the oviposition pattern was extremely regular,
with almost no wastage of eggs through multiple ovipositions in the same
host. Apparently, searching females could discriminate between parasitized
and unparasitized hosts. There were no visual signs of parasitism, so the
discrimination presumably had some chemical basis.
A number of other investigations with other parasitoid species had uncov-
ered cases in which conspecific females would interfere with each other
during oviposition, particularly if they were confined in a small area at high
parasitoid:host ratios. Price reversed his experiment and varied the numbers
of wasps but kept the number of cocoons constant in each cage. Then, for two
days he watched the cages at hourly intervals and recorded the wasps’ posi-
tions. As crowding was increased, he observed more females climbing on the
cage sides in what looked like an escape reaction. It appeared as though rather
than interfering with one another, Pleolophus were doing their best to avoid
one another.
Was this behavior also chemically mediated? Placing a barrier across the
middle of an arena, he released a single female in one side and allowed her
to search for 6 hours while watching continuously and recording her search-
ing pattern. She was then recaptured and released again in the center of the
arena with the barricade removed, and the amount of time subsequently spent
in each half was recorded. As a control, Price similarly observed the behavior
of a female released in a fresh arena. Analyzing the results of several such
trials, Price found that 7 of 19 females tested exhibited a clear recognition and
avoidance of areas they had previously searched (Fig. 6.16). Several variables
remained uncontrolled, but avoidance was frequent enough to suggest that
females were depositing some ‘trail odor’ on the substrate as they searched
and that it served later to repel them, promoting dispersal. In fact, later stud-
ies showed that even different species of parasitoids attacking the cocoons
are able to recognize and avoid areas previously searched by Pleolophus.
Price’s studies thus were some of the first to report long-distance repellency
of possible competitors by a solitary endoparasitoid.
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 245
It is important to remember that insects also can detect chemical and/or physical
changes that eggs or larvae induce and use these as signals; simple feeding dam-
age would be an example. Thus, three types of studies are used as evidence that
a marking pheromone is actually involved: description of a putative host-marking
behavior; biological assays of behavioral responses to extracts of putative marking
pheromones; and identification of sources of marking pheromones and mechanisms
for their detection.
Modeling studies suggest that a major selection factor for the evolution of
pheromonal marking has been to avoid self-superparasitism, that is, a female laying
eggs again on a host where she already has offspring, and potentially reducing the
survival chances for both clutches as a result. The models suggest that when hosts
are sufficient, other conspecific females might gain from taking notice of her marks
and using them; when few unparasitized hosts are available, they might do better
to ignore the marks. There is, in fact, abundant experimental evidence that some
female parasitoids can adjust their rate of superparasitism according to the propor-
tion of parasitized hosts that they encounter, as well as avoiding host areas that they
have themselves previously searched.
Moving from host-marking behaviors to true territoriality requires only a short
conceptual jump. Territoriality can be defined broadly as any space-associated intol-
erance of others of one’s own species, or more narrowly as defensive behaviors
based on real estate holdings. To be a true territory marking pheromone, the mark
by itself must induce aversive behavior in conspecific intruders.
In both vertebrates and invertebrates, territories (defended areas) occur in a vari-
ety of contexts that most commonly involve feeding, breeding, or raising young.
246 6 Chemical Communication
6.3.5 Recognition
Male and female Nicrophorus carrion beetles cooperating to rear a brood of young
on a small vertebrate carcass can accurately distinguish partners from conspe-
cific intruders after touching their antennae; the pair’s individual chemical profiles
become strikingly similar after several days together, but partner recognition appears
to be based upon the reproductive status that develops after prolonged contact with
the carcass, rather than upon a specific personal identification odor.
‘Body chemistry’ is more than simply a catchy phrase. Chemical profiles can
be as distinctive as fingerprints, and in the insect world they facilitate the recogni-
tion and identification of familiar and unfamiliar, friend and foe. These chemicals,
known as surface pheromones or contact pheromones, are based on cuticular
hydrocarbons, but in many cases, they may be composed of a combination of
species-specific or sex-specific odors and environmentally derived ones (as, for
example, from food sources).
An insect’s body is covered by a cuticle with an outermost layer composed of a
complex mixture of lipids. Some of these lipids are waxy substances that function
as moisture barriers; many insects can change the amount of wax on their cuti-
cle in response to seasonal changes, and species in hot, dry environments typically
6.3 The Functions of Chemical Communication 247
Table 6.1 Recruitment in Oecophylla longinoda, one of the most sophisticated communication
systems known for any ant
Odor trail from rectal Antennation; head Signpost marking; Recruitment of major
gland; regurgitation waving, mandible looping trails laid workers to immobile
of liquid crop opening with food around food source; food source,
contents offering main trail directly to especially sugary
nest materials
Odor trail from rectal Antennation; Signposts and looping Recruitment of major
gland occasional body trails only after workers to new
jerking physical contact terrain
with terrain; main
trail directly to nest;
increase in
frequency of anal
spotting
Odor trail from rectal Antennation; tactile Main trail directly to Emigration of colony
gland invitation for nest; no signposts; to new nest site
transport; physical predictable carrying
transport of sequence of
nestmates nestmate categories
Short looping odor None Trails short, looping, Short-range
trails from sternal and limited to recruitment to
gland; gland surface vicinity of contact enemies
exposed as abdomen with enemy
is lifted
Odor trail from rectal Antennation; at higher Main trail directly to Long-range
gland intensities, body nest; no signposts recruitment to
jerking enemies; used with
short-range
recruitment,
especially intense
during territorial
wars with
conspecifics
have greater amounts of wax than closely related species that live in more moderate
surroundings. However, for some time it has been suspected that the hydrocar-
bons in this cuticular lipid mixture play another important role—that of recognition
cues when insects encounter each other. Over the last few decades, technological
advances allowed the studies necessary to confirm this suspicion for many insect
examples (Table 6.2), and appreciation for insects’ use of cuticular hydrocarbons
for chemical communication has grown tremendously.
For the vast majority of insects, recognition of one’s own species is necessary
only for copulation. Contact pheromones are certainly important even in these
cases. Certain cerambycid beetles attempt to mate with conspecific females only
after touching them with their antennae. When a male cricket contacts another
male with his antenna, he becomes aggressive, but when the same individual anten-
nates a female, he initiates mating attempts; if filter paper impregnated with female
248 6 Chemical Communication
Are you a member of my Beetles: Conophthorus, 8 species recognize each other through
species? CH profiles.
Are you the same gender as Flies: Drosophila, sexes recognize each other by relative
I am? abundance in same CH set; Glossina, distinctive CH set in
each sex.
Are you a member of my Ants: Leptothorax, queen-produced CH;
colony?
To which caste do you Termites: Reticulitermes castes differ in relative abundance of
belong? CH components.
What is your dominance Ants: Dinoponera, dominance recognized by relative quantity
status? of one CH out of 80+; Harpegnathos, dominance recognized
by variations in CH correlated with oogenesis in both queens
and workers.
Do you want me to do some Ants: Pogonomyrmex, tasks determined by CH proportions,
task? with appropriate environmental input.
Are you closely related kin? Bees: Apis, CH profile plus exposure to hydrocarbons in comb
wax.
Can you recognize that I am Beetles: Zyras, Diaritiger, CH mimics that of host ant Lasius.
alien? Parasitoid wasp: Lysiphlebus, CH mimics that of host aphids.
Social wasps: Polistes sulcifer, a social parasite, changes its
CH to that of closely related host after nest takeover.
Consider a single insect sitting on a leaf, releasing a pheromone into perfectly still
air. Obeying the law of gas diffusion, the pheromone expands at a calculable rate
in all directions surrounding the insect until it forms a sphere at whose outer limits
its concentration is zero. Nested within this sphere is a second, more behaviorally
interesting one, the active space—the zone in which the concentration is at or above
the threshold level needed to evoke a biological response from other insects. The
active space constitutes the pheromone signal itself.
Of course, perfectly still air is an oversimplification. If a breeze is blowing, the
pheromone expands to form a cone downwind of the insect. Within the cone, the
active space becomes not a sphere but a downwind semiellipsoid, rather like the
upper half of a giant blimp lying on the ground with the emitting insect sitting at
the tip of the airship’s nose (Fig. 6.17). The stronger the breeze, the shorter the
blimp (or ‘odor plume’) becomes. This simple model does not account for surface
drag nor for the fact that most attractant vapors, being heavier than air, will tend
to fall before being fully dispersed, thus effectively flattening the top of the active
space. In fact, most real odor plumes are turbulent and unpredictably meandering;
a swirling smoke cloud from a chimney provides an apt visual analogy. However,
the active space concept can be used in combination with linear measurements and
gas diffusion laws to generate some predictions and generalizations about the use of
pheromones in functionally different communication systems.
Suppose that one knows both the amount of pheromone released and the behavioral
threshold for perception for some insect. To quantify the amount of pheromone
250 6 Chemical Communication
Fig. 6.17 Modeling pheromone movement. A simplified depiction of the active space of the
gypsy moth sex attractant, as deduced from linear measurements and general gas diffusion models.
Height and width are exaggerated in the drawing. As wind speed increases, there is a contraction of
the space within which the pheromone from a single, continuously emitting female is sufficiently
dense to attract males
released, let us consider it to be the number of molecules the insect emits per unit
of time (or as a single puff), and call it Q. For purposes of brevity, let us call the
potentially responding insect’s behavioral sensitivity threshold K and measure it in
molecules per unit of volume. The interval between release of the pheromone and
disappearance of the active space is called the signal duration or fade-out time;
using the blimp-like model of Fig. 6.17, it is the time required for the longitudinal
axis of the active space to diminish to below the receiver’s perceptual threshold.
Behaviorally, variations in the active space of a signal would be expected when
pheromones serve different purposes. How might this be achieved?
Both intuitively and by studying the model, one can recognize two ways: vary the
rate at which the pheromone is sent or vary the receiver’s threshold of perception.
Basically, the characteristics of any pheromone system can be expressed as a Q/K
6.4 The Information Content of Pheromones 251
ratio. The highest Q/K ratio corresponds to a pheromone system with a great signal
distance and slow signal fade-out time. Conversely, low Q/K ratios characterize sys-
tems having rapid fade-out time and a relatively small active space. A theoretically
infinite variety of systems exist as intermediates along the continuum; they result
from changing the values of Q and K independently of each other.
Consider the respective information transfer strategies of mate-seeking moths
and alarm-sending ants. A virgin female moth may need to broadcast her desirability
over a span of miles for several hours before she attracts a male. Obviously, a slow
fade time for her pheromone would be advantageous; it would be desirable also to
increase the area in which a potential mate could encounter and respond to the scent,
i.e. to have a large active space. This necessitates a high Q/K ratio, accomplished by
increasing Q, decreasing K or both. Because there will be metabolic limits on Q (the
amount of pheromone the female moth could manufacture and continuously pump
into the environment) it is more efficient to change the threshold for perception. To
achieve a large Q/K, the value for K will have to be extremely low; in behavioral
terms, the male receiver must have a very low threshold of perception, an example
of which is the silkworm moth (see Fig. 6.4).
When an ant worker generates an alarm, it is advantageous that other workers
be able to locate it sharply in time and space. For this to happen, a signal must
have a relatively short fade-out time; correspondingly, it requires a much lower Q/K
value, which can be accomplished either by lowering the emission rate or raising the
threshold of concentration, or both. The citronella ant, Acanthomyops claviger, uses
an alarm pheromone with a Q/K between 103 and 105 cc/second; signals take about
two minutes to reach an effective radius of about 10 cm and eight minutes to fade
out. If the citronellal alarm pheromone had as high a Q/K as some moth sex lures,
a one-ant alarm might theoretically keep the colony in a perpetual unproductive
state of chaos and panic. On the other hand, were the citronella alarm pheromone
to have an even lower Q/K value, the signals would be relatively useless because
they would not travel beyond the distance within which other ants could perceive
the danger directly themselves.
Because they have tended to be more thoroughly studied and thus simpler to under-
stand, most examples so far have involved single chemicals as pheromones. Often
even such ‘simple’ systems are quite complex, communicating different information
at different concentrations or in varying contexts. For example, we have mentioned
(see Case Study 6.2) that males of the Douglas fir beetle stridulate when they
detect low levels of the pheromone MCH. However, under a high concentration
of MCH the chirps of the male beetles change from those of the female attract-
ing calls to a longer, interrupted chirp with quite different acoustic properties; this
latter call is characteristic of both courtship with females and rivalry contests with
conspecific males.
252 6 Chemical Communication
Intuitively, one would expect to find many insects using such multifunctional
single pheromones; being small animals, it would be particularly advantageous,
allowing economy in their receptor systems without sacrificing behavioral diversity.
This phenomenon, termed pheromonal parsimony, is particularly characteristic of
the class of pheromones releasing alarm in social insects. Among ants, some alarm
substances are produced by the pygidial gland, others by mandibular glands. For
example, both harvester ants (Pogonomyrmex badius) and leaf-cutter ants (Atta
texana) use the same alarm pheromone (4-methyl-3-heptanone) secreted from the
mandibular glands, and in both species, differing concentrations of this odor elicit
very different behavioral responses. Upon perceiving relatively low concentrations
ants are attracted toward the source; as they move they cross into a much higher
concentration of the pheromone. The respective active spaces can be visualized as
nested concentric hemispheres in what is actually an intensity gradient to which
the receiver can orient; within the inner of these, ant behavior changes from simple
attraction to frenzied aggression.
An alternative to the multifunctional single pheromone is to evolve different
pheromones that serve the different functions but produce them in different exocrine
glands. Recruitment pheromones in termites are apparently all produced in the
termite’s sternal gland, but despite considerable convergence in the trail-laying
behaviors of ants and termites, among the ants trail pheromones are produced from
at least eight different glands, and in some cases more than one gland is involved in
recruitment communication.
The insect pheromone language is clearly not composed of an infinite number
of chemicals different within each species and unique to each message. In order
to achieve and maintain chemical uniqueness and promote privacy during com-
munication with such single chemical systems, insects would need to synthesize
increasingly more complex molecules; this strategy obviously would have its upper
limits, dependent upon such factors as the biosynthetic capabilities (energy cost) of
the organism, physical characteristics (such as volatility) of the molecules used, and
the neurological complexity of the receiver. Thus, among distantly related species,
there has been considerable evolutionary convergence in pheromone chemistry, even
while the uses to which the pheromones are put remain quite distinct. For example,
both stingless bees (Trigona spp.) and Acanthomyops ants use citral as a pheromone,
for recruitment trails in Trigona, and for alarm in Acanthomyops. Asian elephants
even share the same sex pheromone with 140 species of certain small moths!
Despite the intuitive logic that it would seem adaptively advantageous for
insect chemical communication to be species-specific, in tests of single chemi-
cal compounds under laboratory conditions, many pheromones have appeared to
lack specificity even at the generic level. How can this apparent discrepancy be
reconciled?
First, what appears to be a lack of specificity may not be so after all. The odor
environment of an insect is enormously complex; an ordinary terrestrial community
may contain hundreds of thousands of animal species and scores to hundreds of
plant species each producing its own characteristic odors. Superimposed on this are
seasonal and diurnal cycles of production and multiple other factors operating under
6.4 The Information Content of Pheromones 253
Fig. 6.18 Interacting systems. Males of four North American species of giant silk moths
(Saturniidae) each have a differing time of peak flight activity. Seasonal separation also exists;
most cecropia males emerge in May; polyphemus moths, in late May to early June; cynthia and
promethea moths, mostly in June. While the respective female sex attractant pheromones are
known to be very similar, caged females of one species almost never attract males of another
species because of these temporal and seasonal differences
field conditions. For example, males of different species of giant silk moths differ in
the periodicity of their flight activity, and thus vary in their responsiveness to female
sex pheromones according to the time of day or night (Fig. 6.18).
Female silk moths also call only during certain time periods of the daily cycle,
and the periodicity of both male flight and female release of sex pheromones can be
influenced by ambient temperature during development. When pupae of one giant
silk moth species, Antheraea pernyi, were kept at 25◦ C, the adult females initiated
calling behavior about 6 hours after lights-off. However, when pupae were kept
at 12◦ C, the resultant females advanced the onset of their calling to 2 hours after
lights-off. Corresponding shifts in flight activity also occur in males under these
temperature regimes. Hormones (especially juvenile hormone, see Chapter 2) medi-
ate both male responses and female calling times by acting on the different parts of
the nervous system involved in processing olfactory information. Circadian rhythms
cued by photoperiod and perhaps by temperature or other environmental factors set
the hormone secretion cycles.
Second, tests of a single chemical seldom represent the natural situation. Insect
exocrine systems are extraordinarily diverse (see Fig. 6.1), with a multitude of
glands and wide variations in the ultimate uses of a given gland and its products.
Furthermore, the products themselves are often complex. For example, at least 50
254 6 Chemical Communication
volatile compounds are present in the Dufour’s gland of a carpenter ant, Camponotus
ligniperda.
In human language development, the use of a different word for every message
in every language has been superseded by the greater efficiency of coding systems
based upon combinations of words. While a single word can send a message, adding
others produces an almost infinite variety of meanings; consider the different behav-
iors elicited by ‘Help!’ ‘Help me,’ ‘Help me love,’ and ‘Help me kill.’ An analogous
strategy has been evolved by insects and allows them to send messages more
efficiently: pheromone blends or multi-component pheromones. Multi-component
pheromone blends are probably the rule in arthropods rather than the exception.
Such systems have arisen in two ways: by the use of specific blends of chemical
produced within a given gland and, perhaps less commonly, by the use of various
combinations of glands releasing pheromones simultaneously or in succession.
Honey bees have one of the most complex pheromonal communication systems
so far identified; they possess 15 known glands that produce an array of com-
pounds. A classic example of a pheromone blend is the so-called ‘queen substance’
or queen mandibular pheromone (QMP) secreted in the mandibular glands of the
queen honey bee. It affects social behaviour, maintenance of the hive, swarming (see
Plate 20), mating behaviour, and inhibition of ovary development in worker bees.
A principal QMP component is the fatty acid E-9-oxo-2-decenoic acid. On
its own, it is both a sex attractant and an aphrodisiac. Combined with E-9-
hydroxy-2-decenoic acid it acts as a primer pheromone and inhibits worker’s ovary
development, queen rearing, and rate of juvenile hormone biosynthesis; the latter
thereby regulates the onset of age-related tasks such as foraging.
In addition to these, QMP includes three more chemical components that act
synergistically to elicit retinue attraction. The queen’s retinue pheromone is now
known to have nine chemical components (not all synergistic components orig-
inate from the same gland) and is the most complex pheromone system known
in any organism. The complete blend is translocated to all her body surfaces
by self-grooming and elicits the distinctive retinue response among worker bees,
causing them to lick, antennate, and groom their queen (see Fig. 6.12). Virgin
queens produce significantly less (or none) of many of the nine components
and are thus much less attractive to workers than mated, actively egg-laying
queens.
It is not unusual to find a chemical blend produced within a single exocrine gland.
For example, in ten Myrmica ant species, a variety of natural products are used for
the same communicative purpose—alarm. Among eight of the species, the same
two chemicals (3-octanone and 3-octanol) are present in the mandibular glands,
but in highly varying ratios. In one, the two chemicals are combined with a third
major constituent. Two other species have none of the previously mentioned com-
pounds but shared a single compound characteristic of them alone. As an interesting
aside, it has been shown that many of these same ant alarm chemicals have signifi-
cant allomone effects, inhibiting the germination of grass seeds collected as dietary
staples by various myrmicine ants.
6.4 The Information Content of Pheromones 255
In general, the pheromone blends of closely related species have diverged and
diversified mainly by varying the ratios of chemical mixtures produced in the
same exocrine gland. In moths, for example, the multi-component pheromones
typically consist of one abundant component and several minor ones; differ-
ent related species tend to use the same components, probably because of
shared biosynthetic pathways, but in unique combinations or ratios. Synergistic
effects, in which a blend elicits stronger responses than any component alone,
are common.
One of the most complex chemical signaling systems occurs among weaver or
green tree ants, Oecophylla (see Plate 19); five different recruitment systems have
been identified, using pheromones from two different glands combined with vari-
ous tactile signals to mediate a range of tasks from foraging to emigration to new
nest sites to responses to enemy threats (see Table 6.2). This is a striking exam-
ple of signal economy, with specificity arising from the different combinations of
tactile and chemical elements. Recruitment to food and to new terrain are both
initially guided by pheromones from the rectal gland; subsequently, characteris-
tic head waving and jaw opening specify food whereas body jerking denotes new
terrain.
Allomones—the other major class of chemical releasers of behavior—often also
consist of chemical ‘cocktails’ and many of the above considerations apply equally
to them. In addition, as defensive secretions, multi-component allomones sometimes
may simply overload predator receptor systems, thereby effectively hiding the insect
chemically from its enemies.
Table 6.3 Classification of semiochemicals by benefit (+) and cost (–) to signaler and receiver
Pheromone + + Mutualistic
Allomone + – Deceit, propaganda
Kairomone – + Eavesdropping
steal the message. For example, the cuticular hydrocarbon ‘uniforms’ of host ants
are acquired by some of their obligate social parasites. Such camouflage exploits
the manner in which the ants themselves acquire their colony-specific odors, via
grooming and trophallaxis.
A second form of exploitation of the chemical system occurs when an insect
actively produces chemicals that mimic the chemical messages of others. Illicit
chemical signalers of this type abound in the insect world. The ‘adoption’ and
‘appeasement’ gland secretions of some species of rove beetles (see Fig. 4.11)
elicit host behaviors allowing the beetles to integrate themselves into ant soci-
eties. Among social species, a common method of gaining chemical congru-
ency with the host is to actively biosynthesize the host recognition odor. Still
another type of exploitation involves disrupting normal communication. Some
slave-making ants spread panic throughout a colony by mimicking the alarm
pheromones of their hosts; in the confusion, they take over the victims’ pupae
(see Chapter 10).
At least three factors appear to facilitate evolution of chemical code-breaking
among insects. One is the design of olfactory systems, with their broad sensitivity
to odors of all sorts. Another is the ability to evolve extreme tuning to any odor that
provides a selective advantage. A third is the relative ease with which many insects
can learn olfactory cues, an ability that has been widely reported for insects ranging
from crickets to parasitic wasps. Thus, it appears that the risk of exploitation is one
cost of reliance on chemical communication systems. Why does natural selection
not eliminate it? Perhaps if the deceiver is rare relative to the genuine signal sender
or receiver, then the cost of the deception can be tolerated, because the legitimate
use of the signal will be more selectively advantageous.
possess several of the following features as well: (1) qualitative and/or quantitative
specificity, (2) rapid rate of information dissemination, (3) efficiency over consid-
erable distance, (4) directionality of transmission, (5) wide range of information
content, and (6) persistence or ability to start and stop quickly.
As we have seen, chemical communication systems possess most of these
attributes and are certainly as fully versatile and sensitive as acoustic and visual
communication to be considered in the following two chapters. In addition, they
possess certain unique advantages. For one, they are the only major communicatory
mode to have signals capable of lingering in the environment. Second, for chemi-
cal signaling, time and space take on a special meaning, for the sender and receiver
do not have to be simultaneously coordinated as demanded by visual or acoustic
communication. As a third point, like sound, chemical signals are able to go around
environmental barriers although, unlike acoustic signals, chemical messages may be
difficult to modify on short notice.
In concluding this section it is appropriate to consider the odorant chemicals
themselves. Edward O. Wilson and William H. Bossert have convincingly argued
(Table 6.4) that airborne odorants should have a carbon number between 5 and 20
and a molecular weight between 80 and 300 due to volatility constraints. In addi-
tion, they predicted that chemicals used for purposes requiring a high degree of
specificity (such as sex attractants) should have greater molecular size than those
odor signals used to elicit behaviors (such as alarm) for which species uniqueness
is less essential. In the main, these predictive generalizations have been upheld by
accumulating empirical data although size restriction does not apply to waterborne
and contact odorants.
Fig. 6.19 Behavioral manipulation. Insect pest control approaches often incorporate semio-
chemicals that affect behavioral responses. Integrating chemical stimuli that act to make a potential
resource unattractive to the pests (e.g., repellants, alarm pheromones) while at the same time lur-
ing them to an attractant source (e.g., sex or aggregation pheromones) constitutes an appealing
‘push-pull’ pest management system
6.5 Chemical Communication and Insect Control 259
7.1 Introduction
Small black paper balls pulled through the air on a string with sufficient speed
will attract male flies, which fly after and grab them. When dragonfly nymphs are
presented with various still objects against a background of moving stripes, they
will snap at them. Backswimmers, Notonecta, placed in a tank with white sides are
unable to avoid being swept downstream in a current. What is going on?
In all of these cases, the insects involved are using visual orientation (see Chapter
3) in the context of behavior. However, the above are not examples of insect
visual communication, because to communicate there must be at least one intended
receiver. In the context of orientation, vision is extremely important to most insects,
as it is to most animals in general. However, for communication, insects tend to rely
more heavily upon their other sensory systems.
Working primarily with vertebrates, the early ethologists found visual commu-
nication to be an unusually appealing subject for behavioral study for at least two
major reasons—the tremendous amount of information that visual signals can carry,
and the fact that visual systems play a dominant role in the behavior of humans.
Therefore, it is not surprising that in the development of ethology over the last cen-
tury, processes of visual communication have played a starring role. Konrad Lorenz,
for example, derived many of his theories from the study of fish and bird displays—
structures and behavior patterns that function as visual signals between conspecific
animals. Niko Tinbergen’s landmark 1948 paper on social releasers also took most
of its examples from visual signals. Ethologists have found that at least two impor-
tant signaling phenomena are more readily studied in visual signals than in auditory
or chemical signals. One is the origin of signaling structures; the other is the evolu-
tionary transformation of signals from their original purpose to assume new roles, a
process known as ritualization.
In addition to their central role in the development of ethological theories, visual
communication systems have formed a fertile ground for development of two of the
behaviorist’s most powerful tools: the use of models and the use of disguise. While
neurophysiologists might manipulate relatively few stimulus variables with good
result, the types of behavior of interest to ethologists often could be elicited only
7.2 Bioluminescence
Larvae and larviform adult females of Phrixothrix beetles are sometimes called
‘railroad worms’ because, like twin lanterns, luminescent organs of their body glow
green while those on their head glow red. The predatory larvae of some small dipter-
ans called fungus gnats are unique among flies in producing their own light. Groups
of fireflies sometimes blink in unison.
For centuries scientific investigators and curious lay observers alike have been
fascinated by the production of light by living organisms, or bioluminescence.
The phenomenon is more widespread than most people realize, and apparently
has evolved independently many times (some say up to 30 times or more).
Bioluminescence occurs widely in vertebrates, invertebrates, bacteria and fungi.
Ninety percent of deep-sea marine life is estimated to produce bioluminescence in
one form or another. Even symbiotic organisms carried within larger organisms are
also known to bioluminesce. Among the Insecta, self-luminescent species occur at
least nine families in five orders: Collembola, Hemiptera, Diptera, Dictyoptera, and
Coleoptera. The intensity of luminescence varies greatly from one insect species
to another. In some species, it is so low as to be visible to human beings only by
the completely dark-adapted eye. (If humans had more sensitive eyes, they would
probably consider more insects luminous.)
Bioluminescence has been described in more beetles than any other group.
Fireflies, lightning bugs, or blinkies—adult lampyrid and elaterid beetles—are the
most well known probably because there are over 2,000 species and many are widely
distributed. Some fireflies are exceedingly bright for their size. The common eastern
United States firefly, Photinus pyralis, has a flash that varies from 1/400th to 1/50th
7.2 Bioluminescence 263
potatoes to fish to silkworms. Luciferase is also used in forensics and in human and
animal medicine.
When insect larvae glow, the light is on more or less continuously, but most adult
fireflies produce brief flashes of light that are vital for their courtship. How does
a firefly control its flash pattern? Pliny the Elder’s famous Natural History offered
the opinion that fireflies turn their lights off and on by opening and closing their
wings. Because this encyclopedic work covered almost the entire field of ancient
knowledge and was based on the best authorities of that time, this statement was
repeated again and again all the way into and through the Middle Ages.
Insect physiologists have now studied the system in some detail (Fig. 7.1), and
have shown that firefly flashes are controlled by gating the supply of oxygen to
the light-emitting cells. A nerve impulse that does not travel all the way to the
insect’s ‘lantern’ as one might expect, but instead via tracheal end cells to tracheolar
cells that are usually filled with fluid. The impulse results in changes in the cells’
osmotic potential, so that fluid levels drop and oxygen can reach the photocytes
for the oxygen-requiring light reaction. During the dark part of the flashing cycle,
the tracheoles refill with fluid, oxygen can no longer reach the photocytes, and the
flash turns off. Interestingly, flashing can be turned on and off experimentally. If
spontaneously flashing fireflies are placed in a chamber where the atmosphere can
be rapidly changed from oxygen to air, they being to glow continuously instead;
changing the gas stream back to air turns the continuous glow back to spontaneous
flashing.
Why do railroad worms have red and green lanterns? Do fungus gnats in the family
Keroplatidae use their light to lure potential prey? When fireflies sometimes blink
in unison, what do they accomplish?
Bioluminescence serves many purposes in the animal world in general, and
among insect species in particular, and its functions are often different for vari-
ous life stages. For immature fireflies, for example, bioluminescence appears to be a
warning signal to predators, because many firefly larvae contain chemicals that are
distasteful or toxic.
For most adult fireflies, the production of light is used to locate other individ-
uals of the same species for reproduction. Courtship typically takes the following
sequence: Males initiate flashing during flight at species characteristic times (often
around sunset) in a well-defined habitat area; their flight paths are also species char-
acteristic, especially during moments of light emission. Females remain stationary
and, upon perceiving a male flash, answer with their own flashes, which follow that
of the male after a brief, again species-characteristic delay. Repeated flash–answer
sequences soon bring the sexes together, with copulation following.
In eastern North America, the commonly seen fireflies belong mainly to two bee-
tle genera; the larger ones are Photuris, and the slightly smaller ones are Photinus.
7.2 Bioluminescence 265
Fig. 7.1 Controlling the flash. (above) Adult fireflies produce discrete rapid bursts of light by
changing the length of a column of tracheolar fluids and thus controlling oxygen access to the
light-emitting cells (photocytes). These changes are actively brought about by nervous stimulation
of the tracheal end cells, which share similarities to other cells that function as sodium pumps.
P, photocytes; NE, nerve end; t, tracheole; TF, tracheolar fluid; T, trachea; TC, tracheolar cell;
TEC, tracheal end cell. (A) Increased fluid length blocks oxygen to the photocytes, producing the
dark phase of the flash cycle. (B) When fluid length decreases, the photocytes have the oxygen
necessary to produce light. (below) Only about 40–60 ms pass between the arrival of the nervous
action potential at the insect’s ‘lantern’ and the start of light emission, with maximal light occurring
within 100–150 ms after action potential arrival
Both genera include several cryptic species that are morphologically distinguish-
able only in minor details of body color. In the 1950s, Herbert Barber showed
that flash patterns clearly distinguished otherwise-confusing species of Photuris
266 7 Visual Communication
fireflies. A decade later, building on this work, Jim Lloyd researched the flash pat-
terns of Photinus, using electronic devices to produce artificial flashes of known
duration and to accurately measure the female’s response delay (see Fig. 1.16).
By varying different signal parameters, Lloyd learned that female Photinus were
discriminating male light pulse length, the interval between pulses, and/or pulse
number. Furthermore, males were able to discriminate between differing female
answer delay times. In addition, Lloyd confirmed that the flash signals of sympatric
species differed significantly, but that those of species that normally did not occur
together were often very similar. In other words, refined isolating mechanisms had
evolved only where there was a possibility of reproductive mistakes being made.
Do Photuris speak a different flash language from Photinus? Female Photuris
had long been known to be carnivorous, but imagine his surprise when Lloyd
observed Photuris females attracting and devouring Photinus males by mimick-
ing the flash responses of Photinus females. This has become a classic example
of aggressive mimicry (see Chapter 4); conspecific males are not eaten.
As aggressive mimics, female Photuris are quite versatile and effective; they
can adjust their flashes to attract males of at least four species with distinctively
different flash patterns, and they generally succeed in capturing at least one male
for every ten attempts (see Plate 22). Later studies by others showed that a Photuris
female becomes a femme fatale only after having mated. Mating induces a suite of
behavioral changes, including locomotor activity, answering postures, predaceous
behavior, and response to flashes of males of different species.
The complex flash exchange systems found in Photinus and Photuris are not
typical of all fireflies, but instead probably represent a quite advanced level of evo-
lutionary development for the group. Some more primitive species known to use
pheromones have continuously glowing females, and in other species females turn
on their glow only in response to glowing males.
Many behavioral and ecological adaptations serve to enhance bioluminescent
communication efficiency. Females of most species climb up on perches during
hours of mating activity. Flashing males assume flight altitudes so that their light is
directed toward the ground ahead of them, and many species execute aerial maneu-
vers which enhance their chances of seeing or being seen by females (Fig. 7.2). In
addition, most fireflies exhibit habitat specificity and/or orientation to a mating site;
these further restrict the areas that males patrol, reducing ‘background noise’ and
the chances of interspecific interaction. Restricted periods of activity, seasonally or
diurnally, have similar advantages.
Perhaps the most extreme example of such ecological and behavioral restric-
tion is that of the ‘firefly trees’ found in parts of tropical Asia. At times the firefly
population in a grove of trees will flash on and off synchronously, hour after hour,
night after night, for weeks or even months. More than 30 similar reports have been
published describing these oriental firefly displays, occurring principally from man-
grove trees along brackish rivers. Such behavior is best known in tropical Southeast
Asia, but not restricted only to there. In the United States, one of the most famous
sightings of this phenomenon occurred near Elkmont, Tennessee in the Great Smoky
Mountains during the second week of June 2005.
7.2 Bioluminescence 267
Many reasons have been proposed for these massive synchronized displays,
including ‘a sense of rhythm,’ ‘an organic law of rhythmic appreciation,’ ‘sym-
pathetic telepathy,’ and a whole host of other similar non-explanations with
anthropological overtones. One writer even went so far as to attribute it to the twitch-
ing of the observer’s eyelids, remarking that ‘the insects had nothing whatever to do
with it!’
Through extensive fieldwork with oriental fireflies in Thailand and Borneo and
use of photographic and photometric analyses, John and Elizabeth Bucks showed
that synchrony of great numbers of individuals is indeed nearly perfect. They
hypothesized that flash synchrony is controlled by an internal resettable pacemaker.
Contrary to earlier reports, both males and females occur in these trees, although
the females do not participate in synchronous flashing. In some instances, aggre-
gations in a given tree may include more than one species, resulting in a complex
combination of flashes that is still presumably effective for each species involved.
The Bucks considered synchronous flashing to be part of a complex of behav-
ior patterns (congregation, selection of certain trees, flashing, etc.) for enhancing
mating under otherwise difficult conditions. The firefly trees appear to serve as
quasi-permanent rendezvous, and synchrony appears to increase their efficiency as
beacons. Adult fireflies in these tropical areas live for only a few days and therefore
268 7 Visual Communication
must find mates quickly if they are to reproduce before they die. A single prospective
mate is nearly invisible among the thick, tangled plants of these swampy areas, but
many males flashing synchronously are easily seen by other fireflies, both male and
female.
Do some or all insects really see the world as a mosaic of tiny images? Popular
works depict insect vision like that, and one can buy novelty viewers that allow
children to imagine that they ‘see as insects do’, but are they correct? What does an
insect actually see? And how? Taking into account both insect neurology and the
significant variation that occurs in this very large segment of the animal world, the
answers seem to condense down to one short statement—It depends on the insect—
but working out the details has taken over two centuries, and still continues today.
detail. (Nor do we.) What it sees, or rather, what it responds to, depends on what its
evolutionary history has preprogrammed it to ‘look’ for. An insect’s response is also
determined by properties intrinsic to the photoreceptors themselves and the manner
in which they relate to higher centers in the brain.
Insect eyes vary a great deal in form and location, but are of two basic types:
compound and simple. Simple eyes include the larval eyes, or stemmata, of
holometabolous insects and the dorsal ocelli present in most winged adult insects;
interestingly, the two are apparently not embryologically related. Larval stemmata
vary in size, number, and complexity. Fly maggots have only small pockets of photo-
sensitive cells in the cuticle. A lepidopteran caterpillar typically has six stemmata on
each side of its head, which look like simple white circles; with these, some caterpil-
lars can differentiate shapes and orient toward boundaries between black and white.
Other larvae, such as those of predaceous Cicindela tiger beetles, have stemmata
that apparently work as well as many compound eyes, making one wonder why the
insects did not retain eyes like this into adult life.
The winged adults of most orders and the nymphs of hemimetabolous species
usually have three true dorsal ocelli on the top of their heads. They appear to be
‘good’ eyes with a wide view and a lens; some also have a reflective layer and
some have a mobile iris. However, the retina is very close to the lens, so they are
apparently profoundly out of focus. Then what is their purpose? Currently the best
hypothesis is that they are horizon detectors that contribute to equilibrium when
the insect needs to make a fast correction during flight. Being unfocused allows the
receptors to respond to overall light distribution rather than every small leaf shadow.
The ocelli also are very sensitive to low light intensities. In certain bees and wasps
there appears to be a correlation between large ocelli and activity peaks at dawn
or dusk. (It is also known that insects can perceive light directly via the brain cells
as well as through their compound eyes and ocelli; some have other photosensitive
tissues as well.)
The best-studied and presumably most important visual organs in the majority
of insects are the compound eyes. With the exception of certain specially adapted
parasitic and cave-dwelling species, most adult insects have a pair of these promi-
nent organs, bulging to varying degrees from either side of their head so as to give a
wide field of vision in all directions. Composing each compound eye, and extending
inward like narrow columns, is a group of densely packed hexagonal units called
ommatidia. Most people think they know this from looking at insects, but in real-
ity this is modern knowledge gained from microscopic and photographic close-ups.
All we can view directly is a vague bumpiness, because the facets of an insect’s
compound eye are too small to be resolved with the naked eye. The number of
ommatidia in an insect eye varies greatly; while the common house fly has about
4,000, dragonflies may have as many as 28,000.
There is considerable diversity in the structural detail of ommatidia in differ-
ent insect groups. However, basically each ommatidium consists essentially of
two parts. One is an optical, light-gathering part (the lens or facet, and the crys-
talline cone). The other is the sensory part (retinal cells and their differentiated
margins called rhabdomeres or rhabdoms) that perceives the light radiation and
270 7 Visual Communication
transforms it into electrical energy for nerve transmission. Each crystalline cone
also contains pigmented shield cells that help protect its actual light-sensitive
chemicals.
Yet, while all compound eyes look superficially quite similar, their insides vary
in some fundamental ways that relate to an insect’s behavior. Like all other organ-
isms, insects depend on light energy for their vision, but too much can be damaging.
Sunlight is particularly hard on pigment cells, so insect eyes basically come in
two different optical types (Fig. 7.3) that let different amounts of light strike the
rhabdoms.
Adult insects that are active during the day generally have apposition eyes that
limit the amount of light reaching the retinal rods. Each retinal rod receives light
only through its own lens. Light from other facets is intercepted by pigments that
act like a screen. The field of view of each rhabdom occurs next to (apposes) the field
of its neighbor, thus giving the eye its name. This is the kind of insect vision mim-
icked by children’s toys and popular literature. It produces an overall erect image
that is made up of a mosaic of adjacent fields of view. How coarse or fine a partic-
ular part of the image appears will depend on the number of facets per unit area.
Many nocturnal and crepuscular insects have a different system that gives a much
brighter image at night. A superposition eye converges (or superimposes) light rays
from many lenses onto each retinal rod. Because the effective aperture now is no
longer the width of a single facet but may be ten to thirty facets or more, the image
is brighter but more diffuse. By design, a superposition eye is much more sensitive
at low light levels than an apposition eye would be, but in addition, many super-
position eyes also have a tapetum, a mirror-like cell layer in the back that reflects
light outwards so that it passes twice along the retinal rod, doubling its effect on the
light-sensitive retinal pigments. Instead of a mosaic, all superposition eyes produce
a single deep-lying erect image in the vicinity of the retina. (This distinguishes them
not only from apposition eyes, but also from camera-type eyes in which the image is
inverted.)
When light is reduced, a day-flying insect usually just becomes inactive. But
if too much sunlight is damaging, what happens when a night-flying insect is in
daylight? Remarkably, whereas the shielding pigments in apposition eyes are sta-
tionary, those in superposition eyes can migrate. At low light intensity, the pigments
move into the distal part of the cells, allowing light from adjacent ommatidia to pass
through and strike the rhabdom below. Upon exposure to bright light, the pigments
disperse to form a protective curtain that prevents the spread of light from adjacent
facets, and the eye functions like an apposition eye.
This migration also explains why the eyes of many moths seem to glow at night
but not during the day. At night, the tapetum near the base of the moth’s eye reflects
light but during the day, the shielding pigments that have moved into place absorb
most of the light before it is reflected back.
Although we know that sight differs among animals, we do not know what
animals actually perceive. There is an important distinction between having light
illuminate the retina, and understanding what is being seen. Perception across the
7.3 Light Reception 271
Fig. 7.3 Seeing the light. (above) Robber flies (such as the one shown here feeding on a solitary
bee) typically have such acute vision that they are able to catch their prey on the wing. Like most
other diurnal insects, the Asilidae have apposition eyes that screen light from adjacent rhabdoms,
so that each lens forms its own relatively sharp image. (below) Nocturnal insects such as this firefly
typically have superposition eyes that gather light from many lenses to form a brighter but more
diffuse image. In addition, adjustments in both the colored screening pigments and the middle-
wavelength pigments in photoreceptors optimize the firefly’s reception of intraspecific signals
animal kingdom is the subject of much ongoing research. Consider the simple ques-
tion of whether an insect can distinguish between one shape and another, the ability
called form perception.
272 7 Visual Communication
Much of our present knowledge of insect form vision comes from honey bee
behavior during training studies where choice of some shapes has been rewarded
with food. However, there are several limitations and difficulties implicit in these
studies. Perhaps the most striking has been that all such training studies have been
complicated by the bee’s spontaneous responsiveness to flicker (see Section 7.3.2,
below). Moving shapes of all sorts are more attractive than stationary ones, and
while bees could be trained to distinguish solid figures from broken ones, they could
not distinguish solid from solid or broken from broken. The adaptiveness of such
behavior is apparent if we consider a bee in its natural environment. Not only does
wind cause flower movement, but when bees fly low over the ground in search of
flowers they experience the passing of a radial cluster of flower petals as a burst of
flickering. The regularity and high frequency of such flicker patterns identify the
stimulus as readily as the total shape does to our eyes.
A second less immediately apparent limitation is that even these results are quite
possibly open to question because they may be confounded by stimulus filtering
appropriate to flower searching but not to other situations. Still a third limitation
is that honey bees may not be the appropriate ‘representative’ insects to generalize
from, any more than white rats are necessarily ‘representative’ mammals for all pur-
poses. Certainly some other Hymenoptera, such as the predatory wasps Philanthus
and Ammophila, use landmarks for topographic orientation (see Chapter 2) to an
extent which implies a better ability to discriminate forms than would be expected
from theories built upon honey bee observations.
Dance flies cruise just above the water surface, searching for drowning insect prey.
How do they keep their bearings? Males of the hoverfly Syritta discern and begin to
pursue a female while remaining beyond her visual detection range. What cues are
they discerning that trigger this pursuit?
The term visual acuity is used in various ways. It can describe how well two
close objects can be determined, or the smallest single object that an eye can detect,
or (as often used experimentally) the finest grating of equal light and dark stripes it
can resolve. A related term, resolution, is used in a loose way to mean the ability to
resolve fine detail or determine the parts that make up an image.
The better the acuity, the greater the distance at which prey can be seen, mates
can be located, or environmental structures can be used to navigate. The conditions
under which acuity is measured must be specified, of course, because the amount of
light available to the receptors affects the performance of any eye. As light becomes
increasingly dim, one eventually reaches a point where there simply are no longer
enough photons available to trigger the receptors to respond. In addition, contrast
becomes less and less.
Assuming the light is adequate, how acute is an insect’s vision in general? With
so many lenses, at first glance one might think that insects should have excellent
7.3 Light Reception 273
vision—yet it has long been clear that for stationary objects at a distance, the com-
pound eye is universally poor. Details of an object often fail to evoke responses
at all. With some insects such as the honey bee, the eye receives fuzzy images
even when the object is large and nearby. In 1894, a scientist named A. Mallock
described the compound eye vision like this: ‘The best of the eyes . . . would give
a picture about as good as if executed in rather coarse wool-work and viewed at
a distance of a foot.’
If insect eyes do not come even close to having the acuity and resolution of
human eyes, then why not? For one thing, form and motion perception are very
closely intertwined; just as in photography, image motion across the eye causes blur.
This is more than a theoretical consideration. Insects have been measured routinely
turning at speeds well into the hundreds of degrees per second, and in high-speed
maneuvers, rotating at speeds up to several thousand degrees per second.
Furthermore, just as we have noted for so many other phenomena, an insect faces
problems that arise from its relative size. Because each ommatidium has its own
lens, and an insect is small, the lenses are very small. Small lenses are diffraction
limited; to increase resolution by a factor of two requires doubling the diameter of
each ommatidium as well as doubling the number of ommatidia in a row. Another
way to state this would be to say that the eye would need to grow as the square of
the required acuity. For an insect compound eye to provide the same resolution as
ours, Mallock calculated, it would have to be huge—on the order of 38 ft (12 m) in
diameter.
As one might expect in a large group such as Insecta, many subtle changes in eye
structure have been found that increase visual acuity in specific ways that adapt their
owners to different environmental conditions. Nonetheless, the acuity challenges
inherent in the compound eye are great enough to have led one researcher, D.-E.
Nillson, to comment, ‘It is only a small exaggeration to say that evolution seems to
be fighting a desperate battle to improve a basically disastrous design.’
If the design is so poor, why has it persisted when single-lens alternatives were
and are apparently at hand? Researchers studying insect vision find this to be one of
the intriguing unsolved problems. Currently, the best guess seems to be that visual
acuity is less important than other advantages. One is that a compound eye excels
in processing speed. Because the images are processed in parallel, the design allows
for fast motion detection and image recognition.
For another, the compound eye excels in providing a panoramic view of the world
with a large field of vision. Even ants, which have a relatively small number of facets
on either side of their heads, can perceive almost the entire visual field above and
below the horizon. Their ‘blind spot,’ an area hidden below the thorax and abdomen,
has been estimated to be only about 10% of their total visual field.
A third major advantage is that some parts of the ommatidial mosaic can be
fine-tuned to be more visually acute than others. It has long been known that the
number of facets per unit area often varies in different directions in a given eye and
in different regions of the eye. Since the middle of the last century, major technolog-
ical refinements have allowed biologists to study insect visual acuity by noninvasive
methods rather than the older histological techniques. A major outcome has been the
274 7 Visual Communication
realization that insect eyes vary in ways that reflect their life habits and ecology. For
example, one might reason that for many swiftly flying insects acuteness of vision in
the vertical axis would be more important than in the horizontal; in fact, the curva-
ture of their eyes lends very different dimensions to the ommatidial angles in these
directions. Three broad patterns have been identified in apposition eyes. One is an
overall pattern associated with forward flight (perhaps particularly through vegeta-
tion). The second is the presence of acute zones concerned with the capture of prey
or mates. The third is horizontal acute strips associated with flat environments such
as water surfaces.
Compound eyes also provide a unique way to determine distance. The eyes of
an insect are fixed; it cannot move them independently of each other. However, as
an insect directly approaches the object that it is viewing (or vice versa) the retinal
image gradually appears closer toward the inner part of the two compound eyes,
thus affording a method of judging distance. As the visual angle of the ommatidia
becomes progressively less over the inner part of the eye, the object comes into
sharper vision. Experimental work with predatory insects such as preying mantises
and dragonfly nymphs (Fig. 7.4) demonstrates this well.
Compound eyes are better adapted to perceive motion than static form. In fact,
reliance on an object’s real or apparent motion (flicker vision) is so widespread
that it may be regarded as one of the normal concomitants of vision with the
compound eye. Because ommatidia recover very rapidly from light impulse stim-
ulation, the insect eye has a remarkable capacity for seeing successively different
images at very short intervals and thus for scanning a moving object. As a result,
an insect may be able to resolve a finer pattern when it is flying than when it is
at rest. Many predatory species, for example, respond only to moving prey; and
given choices of stimuli, most insects show a preference for the shapes that cause
the most flicker.
Fig. 7.4 Assessing prey distance. At the front of its head, a dragonfly larva (Epicordulia) has a
two-jawed labium which is jointed in such a way that it cannot be moved sideways nor used except
in the fully extended position (right); thus there is only one point in space where a prey can be
caught. With its labium in the retracted position (left), the larva faces the prey directly and moves
toward it, viewing the prey with increasing definition. When the prey’s image falls on certain inner
ommatidial elements, it is seen the most clearly. Here it is exactly within proper striking distance,
which corresponds to the intersection of the optical axes of these ommatidia
7.3 Light Reception 275
significance of ultraviolet phototaxis may be that such light signals ‘open space.’
Much of nature, especially green foliage, absorbs ultraviolet wavelengths; the open
sky, left as the only extensive source of ultraviolet rays, may signify room for free
flight and maneuvering.
In his pioneering work on insect color vision in 1914, Karl von Frisch showed
that honey bees were able to differentiate accurately between several major cate-
gories of color (see Plate 24): yellow, blue-green, blue (including violet), ultraviolet,
and ‘bee purple,’ a mixture of the spectral extremes, orange and ultraviolet. Von
Frisch’s techniques employed simple Pavlovian conditioning: marked bees were
allowed to feed at a sugar source while simultaneously being exposed to a particular
color stimulus, then tested to see whether they would be attracted to the color in
the absence of the food. Finally, if this proved successful, they were permitted to
choose between the original color and a different, closely similar one to see whether
the bees could discriminate between the two. In recent years these studies have been
supplemented with electrophysiological techniques.
Fig. 7.5 Sending ‘invisible’ messages. Like semaphore flags, some courting butterflies flash
UV-based wing patterns that are invisible to vertebrate predators. (left) Three species of pierid but-
terflies whose wings exhibit regions of high ultraviolet reflectance. The male is uppermost in each
photograph and often exhibits a pronounced dimorphism with respect to the ultraviolet reflectance
trait (right column, photographed in near-ultraviolet light, 300–400 mμ) which is not apparent to
our visual spectrum (left column, photographed in visible light, 400–700 mμ). (A) and (B) are
Colias eurytheme whose main visible color is orange; (C) and (D) are C. philodice, a predomi-
nantly yellow species; (E) and (F) are C. chrysotheme, another orange species. (right) Comparison
of the average reflectance spectra for a group of in-copula males (solid line) and free-flying males
(dashed line) of C. eurytheme from the same population shows no differences. Therefore such
signals are probably not used by females to discriminate differences in male quality
7.4 Functions of Visual Communication 279
Visual systems would seem to have several advantages over other communicatory
modes. The range of possible signal variations is theoretically almost limitless; one
has only to vary, independently and in combination, such basic signal aspects as
color, form or posture, movement, or timing. In addition, the rapid adaptation rates
of insect visual receptors could be exploited in development of a wide variety of
temporal patterns, developing a system that could be started or stopped immedi-
ately. Thus, an insect sensing a predator could freeze and need not communicate
its position by any lingering image such as those that might be left by a chemical
system. But in situations where it would be advantageous to do so, the insect could
make clear its exact position, so that the receiver could respond to it in terms of
precise location as well as general presence.
Functionally, any situation involving face-to-face interaction seems a potential
candidate for visual signals. For example, many visual signs are employed in inter-
specific contexts such as defensive behaviors (especially crypsis, threat displays,
and mimicry) and in pollination. In intraspecific contexts, the great majority of
currently known examples concern visual signaling associated with reproductive
activities (see Plate 46). This may, however, simply reflect the disproportionate
amount of scientific attention that has been directed toward courtship behavior as
compared to other behaviors in which visual cues may be functioning (Fig. 7.6).
280 7 Visual Communication
Fig. 7.6 Signaling status. Portraits of nest-founding females of the paper wasp, Polistes dominu-
lus, illustrate some of the naturally occurring diversity in the shape and size of their conspicuous
black facial spots. Experimentally altering spots with paints indicates that these wasps can use
visual signals alone to assess quality differences among unfamiliar rivals, but there is some
controversy as to whether they routinely use these badges to recognize and assess one another
7.4 Functions of Visual Communication 281
Fig. 7.7 Displaying aggression. Two males of the dragonfly, Plathemis lydia, mutually display-
ing over an oviposition site, their abdomens raised to display the white upper surface. Success in
aggressive display is correlated with abdominal whiteness (which develops gradually with age)
and therefore with sexual maturity. Males with abdomens painted black are sometimes ignored by
other males
insects, are miniscule, and their removal appears to make no difference in navigation
or prey capture. As these facts might suggest, dragonflies are indeed among the few
insects in which the sense of sight is greatly dominant over the other senses. In fact,
Odonata behavior is so visually mediated that some have facetiously dubbed them
‘the bird watcher’s bugs.’
All dragonflies are more or less selective in their breeding sites. In many cases the
male, who generally arrives at the site before the female, confines his activities over
a certain pond or stream area that he will defend against intrusion by other males.
Within this area, he will court females. On a smaller scale, damselflies will often
establish similar territories closer to vegetation or to the water surface. Sometimes
several different species of different size classes will exhibit ‘stacked territories’
over the same pond.
As with most territorial animals, the dragonfly’s territorial behavior centers upon
certain ritualized aggressive displays, backed up by physical combat as a last resort.
In aggressive display, two males of the same species recognize each other as such
and then indulge in a formalized ritual that usually concludes with the departure of
one of the pair. One example can be seen in one of the most common pond inhab-
itants in North America, a rather large dragonfly, Plathemis lydia. The males have
abdomens that are bright silvery white above, and, as with the bright colors found
7.4 Functions of Visual Communication 283
in many other male dragonflies, these play an important role in male interactions
(Fig. 7.7) The two males alternately dash at one another, flashing their silvery tail
patches in a pursuit display that continues until the new arrival finally restricts his
movements to the vicinity of another site.
Unlike the redundant stereotypy of most sexual displays, visual displays that
communicate aggression are often highly variable, ranging along a continuum from
attack to flight. Such graded displays make it possible to communicate slight
changes in motivation, an important advantage in facilitating the resolution of
territorial and other conflicts.
7.4.2 Alarm
In marked contrast with auditory and chemical communication systems, specialized
visual alarm systems have rarely evolved in insects. One explanation for this may
be that most predators place great reliance upon their visual sense while hunting. It
is most difficult for a potential prey to emit a visual alarm to its companions without
also making itself more conspicuous to the predator. Thus, the commonest visual
signals eliciting alertness, alarm, and flight tend to be provided not by specialized
systems but by the very actions of flight.
Adult butterflies of several families congregate, sometimes in large numbers,
around the margins of puddles of water or urine (see Plate 25) where they appar-
ently obtain needed sodium. To a predator such concentration of brightly colored
butterflies could represent a potential bonanza, but in fact it usually doesn’t. Upon
disturbance, masses of butterflies will suddenly fly up and around, surrounding
the predator with a whirling cloud of butterflies moving in unpredictable and
chaotic patterns, then gradually settle back again only as the source of disturbance
wanes. A predator finds it much more difficult to single out particular individu-
als among the swirling cloud than to pursue an isolated individual flying away
from the group. This holds true whether one or several species are involved. At
the same time, greater protection might be expected in a larger crowd. Thus,
there would be selective advantage in the convergent evolution of similar behav-
ior among many ‘puddling’ butterfly species. (Thus although several individuals
of the same species may be present, they are not necessarily closely related, and
such behavior is better interpreted in terms of survival benefit to individuals that all
respond similarly and simultaneously to threat of danger than as some form of alarm
communication.)
True alarm–alert systems are characteristic mainly of group-living organisms
such as social insects. Their evolution here forms part of the larger question of
the evolution of all types of altruistic behavior, that is, actions that result in self-
sacrifice of an individual benefiting others of its kind; this subject is covered in
Chapter 10. Even among social insects, though, visual signals are not the most
widely used channel for alarm communication, being upstaged by chemical and
sound systems. One possible exception may be found in paper wasps of the genus
Polistes. Close approach to a paper wasp nest will immediately alert some of the
284 7 Visual Communication
residents, which respond with a graded threat display in which front legs are raised
and wings are spread and vibrating (Fig. 7.8). Other workers detect this display
and respond by themselves showing an increased state of alertness and patrolling
activity, a mobilization to meet the potential threat. Although the possibility of
auditory and chemical cues being also involved in this system has not been ruled
out, to date no alarm pheromones are reported for any Polistes wasps. Moreover,
their relatively small colony size and their exposed comb nest environment make
it likely that visual and/or auditory alarm communication would be quite effective
here.
Perhaps nowhere else is the role of vision in insect communication as well studied
and amply documented as in sexual behavior, where visual signals often mediate a
chain of stimulus/response interactions between the partners (see Chapter 9). Such
signals are often highly redundant and stereotyped—there is strong evolutionary
pressure against making species-identification and sex-identification errors.
It is tempting to assume that Lepidoptera, with their wings of decorative colors
and patterns, are primarily visual communicators, making little use of odors and
touch in their sexual behavior. The validity of this assumption apparently depends
upon which species one is considering. The courtship of butterflies and moths ranges
along a continuum from those with distance responses in feeding and courtship that
are mainly evoked by airborne chemical stimuli and those responding mainly to
visual stimuli. The extremes are most easily recognized by differences in the behav-
ior of males. The ‘chemical type’ follows the scent upwind in a gradual zigzag flight
to find the female. The ‘visual type’ looks for the female and approaches quickly and
7.4 Functions of Visual Communication 285
Fig. 7.10 Modeling courtship. Two of the several motor-driven carousel versions that Magnus
used to present different stimulus patterns to wild male silver-washed fritillary butterflies, Argynnis
paphia. The carousel arms carried either (right) a flapping butterfly model or (left) revolving
cylinders with alternating colors
To test his initial apparatus, Magnus presented male fritillaries with mod-
els of the same general form and color as females and with flapping wings.
Males readily followed the moving dummies and flew off only when they
7.4 Functions of Visual Communication 287
were close enough to discover that the dummy did not smell like a female frit-
illary. Satisfied by this, Magnus methodically began to vary different visual
aspects of the dummies on the two arms of the carousel. He soon confirmed
that shape made no difference; a circle or triangle was just as effective as a
‘butterfly.’ A wide range of colors and patterns would elicit the first phase
of male courtship. Evidently the object only had to be moving and colored
somewhat like a female fritillary.
Size of the model mattered greatly, however (Fig. 7.11). Smaller models
were followed less than those as large as a real female. Surprisingly, models
that were twice the size of a natural female were even more effective; models
four times natural size were better yet.
What about wing beat? When he varied the wing-flapping rate, Magnus
discovered that the particular type of motion was less important than the
rate of flickering perceived by the male butterfly. On the ends of the arms
of his apparatus, he replaced the model butterflies with a much simpler type
of dummy—a mere rotating spool with alternating bands of color. At first, he
used segments of female wings on the spool but found that to a male fritillary,
a pattern of pure orange and black was even more attractive.
Fig. 7.11 Supersizing. Results of four different experiments comparing approaches of the male
fritillary butterfly, Argynnis paphia, to nonmoving colored paper dummies of different sizes. The
larger than natural size models are clearly more stimulating in each trial. Sizes were presented in
different combinations in each experiment; for example, the dashed line trial shows percentages of
113 males that responded to each of four sizes: 1/8, 1/2, normal, and 2×
288 7 Visual Communication
Modifying his apparatus once again, Magnus developed a way to vary the
rotation of the spools on each carousel arm independently. When he charted
males’ choices between spools moving at different rates, he learned that
increasing the rate of flicker above a female’s normal wing-flapping rate also
made the model more attractive. In fact, the improved attraction continued to
increase even at flicker rates far greater than those that a flying female frit-
illary would be physically able to attain. Only when the flicker rate climbed
above about 140 stimulus changes per second—the upper limit of resolving
power of the butterfly eye—did the attractiveness begin to decline.
Thus, Magnus concluded, for a male fritillary the ‘ideal’ female would be
up to four times his size, pure orange, and flickering her colors as rapidly as
he was able to detect them.
Not only Magnus’ work but a great many studies with models have shown that
when it comes to attracting the opposite sex, ‘size matters’. Of course, simple phys-
ical impossibilities and opposing selective pressures make it unlikely that a female
like that in Fig. 7.11 would evolve. However, supernormal stimuli (see Chapter 2)
have been demonstrated many times in behavioral studies, and their experimental
use can be very helpful in interpreting the underlying physiological mechanisms
involved in stimulus filtering.
Over the years since Magnus completed his work, others also have confirmed
that visual long-range orientation of male silver-washed fritillary butterflies only
serves to bring the potential mates together. At close range, female pheromones
are necessary to initiate male courtship behavior, an apparently unusual situation
in butterflies. Receptive Argynnis females release a pheromone and when males
perceive it, they no longer respond to color. Furthermore, although receptive females
react to approaching males by directing their abdominal glands toward them in what
looks like a visually oriented response, this behavior might be strongly influenced
by perception of male forewing pheromones.
engaged in mate choice. However, it now appears that mate choice may generate
and maintain multiple secondary sexual traits in many animals, and that it is actually
quite adaptive.
If female butterflies, for example, choose between males—and considerable evi-
dence suggests that they do—then evolutionary theory suggests that they may be
receiving some benefit from this choice. Benefits generally come in two types:
material (or direct) benefits, and genetic (or indirect) benefits; examples of female
choice on each basis have been found. In the case of many butterflies, the benefit is
material, and comes in the form of a spermatophore; along with their sperm, males
deliver accessory gland secretions that contain protein and nutrients a female is able
to absorb and use for oogenesis and body maintenance. This is a significant gift,
because butterflies are on a ‘sugar high’; they are incapable of ingesting proteins as
adults and must completely rely on reserves they packed away as larvae. Thus, for
a female, receiving a spermatophore results in a higher oviposition rate and lifetime
fecundity and longevity. For a male, it is a significant investment; recently mated
males produce only about 40% of the quantity of material produced by males that
have not recently mated. Thus, there should be considerable pressure on both sexes
to ‘get it right’ by using every discriminatory tool at their disposal, not only during
courtship but even before it begins.
Many studies of visual sexual communication in butterflies have agreed in finding
movement to be universally necessary to elicit male butterfly courtship; Colias eury-
theme is no exception. Like many other butterflies, males tend to be most responsive
to the general color of their females’ wings, but complex color markings and pattern
details seem to have little effect, at least in this context. What are important, though,
are bright iridescent ultraviolet (UV) patterns that are only found in males and can
be seen over great distances.
Vision is not the only sensory modality, however. Pheromones are usually dis-
cussed in the context of night-flying moths, and wing displays in the context of
day-flying butterflies, and while these are true, butterflies also produce cuticular
hydrocarbon pheromones during courtship. Because UV signals can be seen over
a distance and cuticular hydrocarbons require proximity, it has seemed quite rea-
sonable to think that the ultraviolet signals of butterflies are used primarily to avoid
interspecific mating encounters and contact pheromones are used for intraspecific
female choice.
Newer evidence from several studies with Colias eurytheme suggests, however,
that bright structural coloration in the form of high UV reflectance plays a major role
in female mate choice. Under field conditions using free-flying males with naturally
occurring individual differences in UV brightness, female C. eurytheme preferen-
tially chose to mate with relatively younger males based on the greater brilliance
of their structural UV coloration. (As another way of looking at the situation, the
brightness of UV reflectance also was the strongest and most informative predictor
of male courtship success.)
At the same time, many studies have shown that C. eurytheme males produce
lipid-based cuticular hydrocarbons that they release when they brush their wings
against the female’s antennae. Individual males vary in both the quantities and
relative proportions of the three chemicals that comprise the pheromone, and the
290 7 Visual Communication
8.1 Introduction
Male grasshoppers and crickets stridulate, rubbing body structures against one
another to produce their familiar clicking calls, to which female conspecifics
respond. Death’s-head hawk moths squeak a warning to interlopers by forcing air
out of the pharynx through a short muscular proboscis. Male stink bugs challenge
rivals and court females by drumming their bodies upon a host plant; females listen,
then respond with their own species-specific calls.
Among all terrestrial animals, only vertebrates and insects have a rich sense of
hearing—the ability to detect those time-varying changes in air pressure that we
familiarly experience as ‘sound’ and to respond behaviorally in a biologically rel-
evant context. If one restricts this definition further by requiring hearing must also
involve ears—specialized organs sensitive to fluctuating airborne sound signals—
hearing has evolved in at least seven insect orders, including all the major ones
but Hymenoptera. However, expanding the definition to include detection of sound
waves through water and solids, then the list of insects with hearing expands
enormously.
Like the Russian nested dolls called matryoshkas, mechanocommunication keeps
revealing new layers of complexity and subtleness as humans develop the technol-
ogy to discern it. Since antiquity, people have known that many insects produce
relatively loud sounds, but it has only been during the past 200 years or so that sci-
entists have realized that many insects can hear. Detailed studies of insect ears and
behaviors associated with hearing only began in the early 1800s.
The concept that insects produce and sense vibrations began, again, with rather
loud and obvious examples, such as the tapping sound of deathwatch beetles in
wooden beams. However, paralleling the trajectory of so many other areas of sci-
entific endeavor, the development of new instruments for detecting and recording
acoustic signals (particularly those outside the realm of direct human perception)
has led to rapid developments in the field of insect bioacoustics. It is now clear that
insects also communicate through very soft vibrations, a private language whispered
at close range.
There is still much to learn, but already it is clear that insect acoustic communi-
cation is immensely more diverse than previously realized. It includes airborne and
Fig. 8.1 Making sounds. Vibrational signaling occurs widely in the insects. The pie charts reveal
that whether one tallies the occurrence of such signaling by number of species or by number
of insect families, substrate vibration is by far the commonest modality. Members of the order
Hemiptera display the greatest diversity in the use of different acoustic modalities
underwater sound, substrate vibrations, and water surface ripples. Current estimates
are that over 195,000 described taxa use vibrational communication alone or in com-
bination with other forms of mechanical signaling (Fig. 8.1); because it is based only
on current published reports and named species, this number is probably low.
The song of a single African cicada, Brevisana brevis, has been measured at 106
decibels at a distance of 20 inches (50 cm); considered the loudest insect sound on
earth, the call is equivalent to hearing a gas lawn mower running three feet away.
8.2 Producing and Sending Signals 293
Striking the tip of their abdomens against a tree branch, nomadic sawfly larvae
make soft tapping noises that summon lone individuals back into the aggregation.
The male waterboatman, Palmacorixa nana, strokes his peg-laden forelegs across
a female’s serrated mouthparts during mating, and she responds to the sound by
remaining motionless during copulation.
Clearly, in examples such as these the insects are making sound on purpose, as a
way to communicate. However, the first insect sounds undoubtedly were accidental.
The noises made by a flying insect, the scratching of burrowing larvae, or the vibra-
tions that occur when an insect crawls across a leaf all make sounds. Because insects
have a durable elastic cuticle, chance sound production occurs during locomotion,
feeding, or cleaning movements.
For communication to take place, however, there must be a sender and a receiver,
and the way in which a sound is produced must relate in some way to the manner in
which it is perceived. Many insects today produce sounds in the process of defensive
behaviors, such as the explosive anal discharges of bombardier beetles (see Fig. 5.9)
and the buzz of stinging bees and wasps. No one really knows whether these insects
make such noises in order to add to the effectiveness of their other defenses, but
such behaviors suggest a way in which some of the earliest sound communication
among insects may have arisen.
Moving their forewings back and forth, katydids sing their familiar choruses; one
rainforest species sings in the extreme ultrasonic range, making the highest calling
note of any arthropod. Booklice, lacewings, stoneflies and many other insects have
taken a different route, tapping the substrate. Virgin queen honey bees produce a
‘piping’ sound by thoracic sclerite vibration; other queens still in their larval cells
respond with their own piping in short pulses at a lower frequency.
In some insect or other, almost every body part has become modified to produce
sound, and the majority of insect species produce communicative sound at some
stage of their life cycle, using an enormous variety of mechanisms. Various ways
to categorize these have been proposed. One is by the method by which they are
produced (Table 8.1).
Clicking and buzzing insects seem to be everywhere, so it is probably no surprise
to learn that friction-based methods of sound production predominate, particularly
among the Orthoptera, Hemiptera, and Coleoptera (Fig. 8.2). Rubbing of one body
part against another, or stridulation, is so common in fact that some authors have
broadened their definition of this useful term to include any sound produced by an
insect. One body part, the file, is usually a series of pegs, teeth or ridges; the oppos-
ing part, the scraper or plectrum, is generally a single edge or ridge. Associated
with many frictional sound production devices are various types of resonating sys-
tems that impart distinctive features to the resultant sounds. Frequency and pattern
may vary tremendously. Crickets produce some of the purest sounds known for
any insects and, in general, the simplest patterns; each movement of their wings
produces one pulse of sound. In other insects with more complex songs, such as
294 8 Mechanocommunication
Table 8.1 Classifying insect air-borne sounds by the way they are made
C. Friction (stridulation)
Many different body Widespread, especially in Sexual and aggressive
parts are used; Orthoptera, Hemiptera, Coleoptera behaviors
mechanisms are
essentially similar
for all.
D. Vibration of a membrane
Muscle-driven tymbals Cicadas, pentatomid bugs, arctiid Sexual and aggressive
moths behaviors, aggregation,
predator confusion
Thoracic sclerites Virgin queen honey bees Identification
tettigoniid grasshoppers, each wing movement can produce many sound pulses.
Because of the mass and viscosity of water, the sound-producing structures of
aquatic insects that stridulate tend to be heavier and more massive than those of
terrestrial insects.
A second sound production method—using muscles to drive a vibrating mem-
brane, or tymbal—is most common among Homoptera and also occurs in some
Hemiptera (Pentatomidae) and Lepidoptera (Arctiidae). When the tymbal muscle
contracts, the tymbal produces a single sound pulse; when it relaxes, the tymbal pro-
duces another. The mechanism is analogous to a rounded tin can lid pressed inward
with one’s finger and then released. The insect’s tymbal muscle contracts with such
rapidity (170–480 contractions per second have been recorded) that to human ears
the sound output appears continuous. In a variation on this method, other insects
vibrate still other body parts.
In a third common method, many insects make noises by striking some body
part against the substrate. The death-watch beetle, Xestobium rufovillosum, attracts
mates by bending its head down and banging it against the floor of its wooden bur-
row seven or eight times a second, creating a tapping or ticking sound that can
be heard in old building rafters during quiet summer nights. Associated with quiet,
8.2 Producing and Sending Signals 295
sleepless nights such as the vigil (watch) kept next to the dying or dead, they became
an omen of impending death.
In addition to these three ways of producing relatively loud sounds, one must add
the many methods by which insects produce a world of softer, closer-range sounds.
This brings us to the matters of distance and substrate.
The love song of tephritid and drosophilid flies depends upon short pulses of air that
are only detectable at ranges of about 10 mm or less, driven by piston-like motions
of their small wings. During courtship, males of the parasitoid wasp Nasonia vit-
ripennis produce acoustic pulses that coincide with wing vibrations, but curiously,
the wings are not the source of the pulses because altering their surface or removing
them does not alter the signal. Crickets such as Phaeophilacris that dwell in cavities
no longer use airborne sound for calling and courtship; their wings make sound-
less flicks that only fan the air, but conspecifics can detect these motions from ten
centimeters away.
Another way to look at sound production is to consider the physics of vibration
transmission. This results in three broad categories: near-field sounds, substrate-
borne sounds, and hearing (or acoustic far-field sounds, which includes both
airborne and waterborne sounds).
Near-field sound involves displacement waves in air or water that occur close to
a sound (generally, a few body lengths from the sound source). At this short dis-
tance and small scale, sound production pushes air particles back and forth, making
low frequency waves that do not travel very far. The particles that are displaced are
296 8 Mechanocommunication
highly directional, and displacement occurs in the same direction as the radiating
sound wave. Mechanoreceptive hairs are most often used to detect these particle
movements. Near-field communication is probably more common than scientific
literature would indicate, because technically, near-field communication is a diffi-
cult phenomenon to measure. Many good examples already have been identified.
The antennae of many insects function as near-field sound detectors. Specialized
dorsal hairs on some caterpillars can detect the near-field sounds produced by
the beating wings of a flying wasp; in response, the caterpillars freeze or drop
from a leaf, thus evading the potential parasite or predator. Similarly, cockroaches
can sense approaching predators through particle-sensitive setae on their cerci
(see Fig. 2.3).
Substrate-borne sound is vibration by its most restrictive definition. These sig-
nals have also been called seismic communication. Most insects can probably sense
vibration, and thus in this wider sense, one could say that most insects can hear.
(Section 8.3.1 returns to this subject.) However, airborne and waterborne sounds are
the sort we are most accustomed to thinking of as hearing. They involve ears that
are sensitive to traveling waves of changing pressure in air and water, known as the
acoustic far field.
Combining communication by air and water vibration is somewhat artificial,
because insects that produce sound under water must contend with the physical
problems of sound transmission in a relatively dense, viscous medium with sharp
boundaries. Aquatic sound fields also differ from those in air because the extent of
the near field is approximately four times greater in water, extending to a greater
distance than most aquatic insects communicate over.
In Kansas, great numbers of female cicadas are attracted to a tractor with its motor
running. In France, another cicada species responds to whistling. Female corixid
bugs of certain species come to the ultrasonic sound of a frequency generator.
Parasitic wasps that lay eggs within wood-boring larvae can locate their prey with
great accuracy even through an inch or more of bark.
What is happening? A simple answer easily comes to mind. These invertebrates
are hearing, and responding to, sounds. Probably the sounds are very similar to
those that would normally serve some communicative function for them. But if these
examples seem to clearly involve hearing, what should one make of the following
ones? When a noise is sounded near some types of caterpillars, they react by rear-
ing up either the anterior third of the body or sometimes the tail end; decapitated
caterpillars, or even isolated body pieces, show the same reaction. Whirligig bee-
tles normally swim about on the water surface film in freely moving swarms; when
their antennae are altered, individuals collide. Migratory locusts have hairs on the
fronts of their heads; stimulation of these always results in flying. How many of
these examples should be called hearing? Any? Some? All?
8.3 Receiving Signals 297
In theory, these receptors fall into two general groups, those yielding information
about an arthropod’s position in space (the subject of Chapter 3) and those involved
in communication with other living organisms, the subject of this chapter. In prac-
tice, however, the divisions are often less clear-cut. One reason is that the same set of
information can be used for many purposes; for example, touch receptors function
in such diverse activities as avoiding obstacles, fighting, and copulating. Another is
that similar-appearing sensilla in different locations clearly serve different purposes.
All mechanoreceptors fall into two functional classes: pressure sensitive and
velocity sensitive. Pressure-sensitive hairs show a repetitive neural discharge during
a static deformation. In simpler language, they continue to fire all during the period
of time in which they are bent. Most common on those body areas where position
is important, they are the proprioceptors. Because they adapt slowly, proprioceptors
are not suited to registering sudden stimulus change. However, they give an accurate
measure of stimulus intensity, transmitting information about the state of muscles
even when contracted for long periods of time. Proprioceptors help the insect to
maintain its position, both the relation of various body parts to each other and the
relation of its whole body with respect to gravity (see Fig. 3.6). Other receptors,
particularly visual and tactile, assist in this capacity.
Velocity-sensitive sensilla, on the other hand, fire only while the stimulus is
changing, such as when a sensory hair is deflected, moves back to its original
position, and then is deflected again. Such waves of deflection are produced most
commonly by oscillations—vibrations that cause ongoing alternate compression
and expansion of the adjacent medium. Not surprisingly, tactile and auditory senses
depend upon this class of receptors. Velocity sensitive mechanoreceptors usually
have a very rapid adaptation rate and thus provide less accurate information about
differences in stimulus intensity. They are well suited, however, to record temporal
patterns of stimulation.
Because sound is vibrations spreading through a medium away from a source of
mechanical disturbance, hearing is a mechanical sense much like touch. However,
hearing and touch differ in the types of pressure alternations involved. Sound stim-
ulation has a phasic nature, whereas touch stimulation has a non-fluctuating or
irregularly fluctuating nature. More simply, the two senses differ on the basis of
distance. Touch is mechanoreception that involves contact; hearing is mechanore-
ception that involves a distance between sender and receiver. The differences thus
are akin to those between taste and smell in chemical communication.
8.3.1 Vibration
Water striders and backswimmers read the water’s surface waves for cues to food
and mate location (see Plate 26); they orient to the source of the pulsation through
differences in arrival times of the waves at receptors located on different legs.
Female Ephippiger bush crickets orient both to the audible songs of conspecific
males and to the tremors they produce; if the songs halt but vibration contin-
ues, females are still attracted, but only if the male is close by. Some solitary
8.3 Receiving Signals 299
plant-dwelling stink bugs find and recognize their mates by quivering their
abdomens to send signals through their host plants.
If the term ‘hearing’ is restricted to the detection of airborne sounds by special-
ized receptors, all other sound reception becomes relegated to a ‘vibration sense’ but
in the insect world sound detection and vibration detection often overlap. As a good
example, consider the chordotonal sensilla that are widely distributed in the insect
body, occurring in mouths, legs, wing bases, halteres, antennae, abdomens, and tra-
cheal systems. These sensilla are unique to the insects, but they have been found in
every insect in which they have been sought. Subcuticular, with no external evidence
of their presence, chordotonal sensilla were originally thought to be exclusively
audioreceptors. However, it now appears that they are derived from proprioceptors,
and at least some have a mixed function.
Upon the antennae, chordotonal sensilla are grouped into the Johnston’s organ
that detects movements of the shaft of the antenna. In culicine mosquitoes where
it was first discovered, and in midges as well, the Johnston’s organ is enormously
developed and has a clearly auditory function. On the other hand, in most other
insects where it has been studied, it appears to act primarily as a tactile organ that
relays several sorts of mechanical information. In aphids it is used in the control
of flight. Cutting off the antennal flagellum beyond the Johnston’s organ causes an
aphid’s flight to become erratic, but when an artificial antenna is reattached, normal
flight resumes.
Generally, insects also have chordotonal organs at various locations along their
legs, located in bundles in such a way as to sense vibrations of the surface upon
which they are standing. One set of particular importance are the ‘below the knee’
or subgenual organs. These are extremely sensitive at their optimal frequencies
because the amplitude of displacement required to stimulate the organ is very small.
Since they can localize a source of vibration that is not in contact with the insect,
subgenual organs could be called organs of sound perception as well as touch per-
ception. They may be particularly important among the wide variety of borers that
stridulate, transmitting vibrations through their wooden tunnels.
The fact that insects can detect vibration does not necessarily mean that they
communicate among themselves with it, of course. Most studies of vibrotaxis,
movement in relation to a substrate-borne source of vibrations, have involved par-
asites and predators honing in on and exploiting vibratory signals given off by
potential hosts, often unintentionally in the course of life activities. Thus, for exam-
ple, larvae of the leafminer Phyllonorycter produce wriggling vibrations that differ
in temporal pattern from the vibrations they make as they simply move through an
apple tree leaf; their pupae wriggle as well. One of their parasitoids, the eulophid
wasp Sympiesis, hones in on these signals to locate both life stages of their host.
Examples in which vibration is used for communication within a species have
been reported less commonly, but seem particularly to include insects that exhibit
some degree of sociality and regularly share some common substrate. Often, as
in the honey bee dance language detailed at the end of this chapter, the vibration
component of the communication is part of a larger picture that includes other
communication modes. However, a particularly well-studied example of primary
vibrotactic communication involves treehoppers, a group of membracid bugs that
300 8 Mechanocommunication
feed on the sap of trees and shrubs (see Plate 27). Some inventive research with
Umbonia crassicornis (Case Study 8.1) provides a useful model for studying natural
mechancommunication behavior in the laboratory.
To find potential mates, capture prey, or avoid predators, it’s not enough to just
notice vibration; an insect needs to be able to locate the vibration’s source.
Can it be done? If so, how?
Shaped somewhat like little thorns, treehoppers spend most of their lives
relatively motionless on host plant stems (a behavior that reinforces this
resemblance), leaving only to disperse or find mates. Fascinating in their vari-
ation and complexity, treehoppers have become the subject of quite a bit of
study. They exhibit the entire range of social behavior that ranges from asocial
individuals, to aggregations of nymphs and adults, to highly developed mater-
nal care with parent-offspring communication. The nymphs and new (teneral)
adults of some gregarious and subsocial taxa are colored aposematically (see
Plate 27); ant-treehopper mutualisms also are widespread.
Working with the thorn bug, Umbonia crassicornis, Reginald (Rex)
Cocroft found that when he simulated a predator’s approach, aggregated
nymphal offspring of this little treehopper produce substrate-borne vibration
signals that elicit their mother’s anti-predator behavior. Vibration was, he
showed, the primary communication mode; blocking vibration transmission
between signaling nymphs and their mother abolished the mother’s response.
The nymphs also communicated to one another, spreading the alarm
throughout the aggregation. It was clearly a complex system rather than a
simple response to random patterns of vibration because nymphs in otherwise
undisturbed aggregations signaled only in response to signals coordinated into
synchronized group displays. When signals from their siblings were played
back to undisturbed nymphs, they reacted as though they had received an
alarm. However, it was unclear whether they actually knew the direction from
which the threat originated.
A full-grown Umbonia is only a centimeter long. Could animals as small
as this send and receive vibration signals that encode location? To some scien-
tists it seemed unlikely on theoretical grounds. They pointed out that vibration
interactions take place at close range. Insect legs are close together anyway,
and the high conduction velocity of vibration waves will further minimize
arrival time differences between receptors. Cases of localization that had been
reported were in relatively larger arthropods such as orb-weaving spiders,
scorpions, and locusts, and in some cases, it also seemed that direction cues
were being gained from amplitude differences along a gradient. Whether the
same results would apply directly to smaller scales seemed questionable.
8.3 Receiving Signals 301
short length of dowel and two piezoelectric actuators (see Fig. 8.3). The actu-
ators were driven with two time signals that created the relationship between
slope and displacement that are characteristic of steady-state bending wave
motion. This would allow him to precisely control the frequency, intensity,
and direction of the vibration stimulus without moving the free-standing,
stationary bug.
To what degree can results like those in Case Study 8.1 be generalized?
Treehoppers have a very expanded pronotum, but it does not appear necessary for
generating a basic pattern of resonance and attenuation. The transfer function for
the legs and for the thorax was similar, and when Cocroft removed most of the
pronotum from three treehoppers, mechanical directionality remained unchanged.
Therefore, it seems as though other insects with different morphologies would show
similar patterns in mechanical response to vibration.
The bigger problem with generalizing lies in the many ways that environmen-
tal constraints on vibrotaxis can occur. For a treehopper on a plant stem, orienting
to a vibration source is essentially a one-dimensional problem. However, for other
insects such as a beetle searching for a mate on a leaf, localization is a two-
dimensional problem; results of studies on two-dimensional surfaces have given
mixed results, leading some scientists to question vibrotaxis as an explanation.
Alternatively, they say, insects may triangulate, sampling and comparing vibrations
at multiple points before orienting. Another possibility is that they may follow a
simple searching rule, making large turning angles whenever the amplitude of the
vibratory signal drops below a certain threshold. Further research will be needed
before all this can be straightened out but there is no reason, of course, to make such
explanations mutually exclusive; all insects need not follow a single rule.
8.3.2 Hearing
Some hawk moths have hearing organs in their mouthparts; while feeding on flowers
at dusk, they can listen for bat echolocation calls by inflating their palps. The South
African bladder grasshopper, Bullacris membracioides, has six pairs of ears running
along its abdomen.
A diverse group of specialized auditory organs has arisen in insects from organs
serving originally for proprioception. These organs are the ones most similar to
what most people might call insect ears. They show a great deal of morphologi-
cal diversity, and from one taxon to another, they can be found in many different
body locations (Fig. 8.4). Sometimes they are conspicuous; many other times, they
are not. Vertebrate ears are always on the head, behind the eyes and above the jaw.
How can insect ears appear in so many places? The answer seems to lie in evolu-
tionary and developmental differences. Vertebrate ears arise from the gill arches,
neural crest, and optic capsule, which confines them to the head. Insect ears, of
the other hand, have arisen through a few simple modifications to existing proprio-
ceptors and their surrounding cuticular tracheal structures. Because the chordotonal
proprioceptors are widely distributed, ears can be as well.
Despite this great diversity, each ear typically consists of the same three units:
a tympanal membrane, a tracheal air chamber, and a chordotonal sensory organ.
304 8 Mechanocommunication
Fig. 8.4 Finding insect ears. A generalized insect showing 15 body locations where tympanal
organs have been identified as evolving independently among members of seven different orders.
Key: Auc = Hemiptera: Auchenorrhyncha, C = Coleoptera, D = Diptera, Het = Hemiptera:
Heteroptera, L = Lepidoptera, M = Mantodea, N = Neuroptera, O = Orthoptera. Locations of
hearing organs of Lepidoptera are most diverse, but all tympanal organs probably evolved from
proprioceptors
With the development of ears, the chordotonal sensilla have increased in number
and become attached to a thin, taut membrane that is set into vibration by waves
of airborne sound. This membrane, or tympanum, functions as a sort of eardrum. In
many nocturnal Lepidoptera that are ultra-sensitive to sounds, the typanum is so thin
that it is transparent and occurs in a protected location. By contrast, many Orthoptera
have a much thicker tympanum conspicuously positioned on the cuticle surface (see
Fig. 8.2). Tympanic organs are usually paired and they often interconnect through a
series of tracheal chambers.
In still another variation on insect hearing, hair sensilla that are primarily used
for tactile purposes often react to high-intensity airborne sounds. As a result, many
insects that lack tympanic organs are still quite sensitive to sound waves in air. Most
if not all caterpillars, for example, can detect sound and will react by becoming
immobile or thrashing as a protective response. It does however appear that those
sensilla types most highly developed for airborne sound reception differ from tym-
panic organs in several ways. Compared to tympanic organs, these hair sensilla
respond over much lower frequency ranges and fatigue fairly rapidly. They exhibit
only limited frequency discrimination, responding synchronously with stimulus
frequency over certain ranges. Finally, they tend to habituate or equilibrate, that
8.3 Receiving Signals 305
is, as a sound continues through time, the amplitude of the massed spikes in the
responding nerve declines.
The very large and complex nests of many social insects present an enigma. No
single colony member can oversee more than a small piece of the construction work
or envision the nest in its entirety. Sometimes, a nest may require a number of worker
lifetimes to complete, and each new part must be brought into balance with the old.
How can the workers communicate so effectively over such a long time period?
Who has the nest blueprint?
In the 1950s, termite researcher Pierre Grassé suggested a key to this puzzle.
Perhaps the insect nest-builders were responding to previously accomplished work
on the nest itself rather than to direct communication from their nest-mates. Then,
even if one could constantly renew the work force, the nest would be completed
‘according to plan’ because the nest structure already finished would determine
what further work should be done. In the years since, many researchers have chal-
lenged the completeness of Grassé’s explanation, but not its basic insight. The
most durable signals that communicate information between insects of the same
species are those incorporated in structures built by the insects such as nests. In his
groundbreaking book Sociobiology, Edward O. Wilson termed such communication
sematectonic, from the Greek words for sign and builder, and defined it as the evo-
cation of any form of behavior or physiological change by the evidences of work
performed by other animals, including the special case of the guidance of additional
work.
Sematectonic communication is not limited to social species. For example, in
the nests of many solitary bees and wasps the cells are arranged end to end in a
linear series. At emergence, adults always chew outward in the direction that they
are facing, which is almost invariably toward the nest entrance. In general, the nest
tunnel is not broad enough to permit the adult to turn around inside should it emerge
from its cocoon facing the wrong way. How does the mature larva correctly orient
itself with an accuracy that far exceeds that expected by chance alone?
As the wasp nest is constructed, the mother builds partitions between successive
cells. As a result of the techniques of construction, the inner and outer faces of
each partition differ in texture and concavity. The mother wasp works from the
outside, and the partition tends to become concave on the outer surface. It is also
smoothed on the outside, while the inner surface has an irregular, bumpy texture on
its convex face. By testing independently the effects of the four possible cues on
larval orientation, Kenneth Cooper was able to clearly show that concavity was the
primary cue used by the spinning larvae (Fig. 8.5); they always oriented their heads
away from the concave surface. Moreover, Cooper found that completely unrelated
306 8 Mechanocommunication
Fig. 8.5 Getting cues from mother. The orientation of a solitary-wasp larva when spinning its
cocoon depends upon the way its mother built the linearly arranged cells. By facing the rough
convex side of the partition, when its development is complete the offspring will leave the nest in
the right direction. Artificially reversing partitions was the key to understanding this behavior; the
swollen end of each cocoon contains the head
wasps, including some parasitic species, orient correctly by the same cues. In twig-
nesting solitary bees, where the cell partitions tend to be rather amorphous walls of
resin or chewed leaves, the significant orientation cues seem to lie in the placement
of the pollen mass. Spinning larvae always orient cocoons facing away from the
pollen mass.
Theoretically, touch could form the basis for a complex and important com-
munication system, transmitting a great variety of messages by varying frequency,
pressure, and time of contact. However, tactile communication appears to be rela-
tively unimportant in comparison with the other communicatory modes. Most tactile
systems have one overriding limitation: the sender must be in contact with the
receiver. As a result tactile methods are usually restricted to close-range situations
such as courtship, mating, and occasionally alarm.
Except for the special case of sematectonic cues, touch communication as a
whole among insects is quite poorly known. The sense of touch is one of the most
difficult modalities to investigate in a communicatory context because it is hard for
an observer to interpose himself or his instruments in such a way as to register the
signals in the form that they are actually received without disrupting the system.
And because of this difficulty, even in cases where tactile signaling is involved,
other communicatory modes tend to receive the first attention.
8.4 The Acoustic Channel 307
The alternating songs of crickets, the long raspy choruses of katydids by nights,
and the intense shrill buzzes and rattles of cicadas by day—these are some of the
most familiar and conspicuous insect sounds that humans recognize. Their precise
choreography in space and time often gives rise to striking alternation or synchrony
between neighbors, and one cannot help but wonder how and why they evolved and
are maintained.
Of all the acoustic responses an arthropod may give, the most widely observed
and easily demonstrated is the phonoresponse—upon hearing a noise, the insect
replies by making one. Acoustic stimulation may produce two different types of
phonoresponse, depending upon the species. In some, the responding insect alter-
nates its emission with that of the stimulus. Such alternation is frequently displayed
between two males, either at the emission of the calling song or of the rivals’ song;
it is also observed in species where a male and female emit an agreement song.
8.4 The Acoustic Channel 309
Fig. 8.6 Measuring songs. Sonagrams and oscillograms enable a viewer to appreciate song
characteristics and differences at a glance. Sonagrams show the carrier frequency on the verti-
cal axis (ordinate) and time on the horizontal axis (abscissa). Oscillograms also represent time on
the abscissa, but the ordinate expresses fluctuations in the pattern of sound intensity (loudness),
without specifying the frequency; the degree of blackening correlates with sound intensity. (a)
Sonagram of an easily recognized human ‘wolf whistle’. (b) Song of the long-horned grasshopper,
Phlugis, displayed on an oscillogram (top) and a sonagram (bottom)
310 8 Mechanocommunication
Phonoresponses involving two or more individuals of the same sex are the most
common type.
Among other insects, particularly species living closely packed together, sound
emission by one individual may set off a collective and synchronous song of all the
population. Like their counterparts among bioluminescent species, these chorusing
displays of acoustically signaling insects are recognized as some of the great spec-
tacles of the living world. Chorusing most commonly involves males, and it may
result in large numbers of insects coming together in dense aggregations. In some
cases, individuals may synchronize or alternate calling phrases for indefinitely long
periods of time, as do Cicadidae and some Orthoptera. The forces that drive such
synchrony are undoubtedly many. The role of sexual selection has been a matter of
particular interest. Many play-back sound studies with various species have shown
that when females are presented with two male calls that are out of phase, they pref-
erentially orient toward the earlier call, a type of precedence effect. The importance
of this effect on mate choice in actual choruses and natural populations is still poorly
known, but there is some evidence that males strive to adjust their singing so as not
to lag behind others.
Studies of chorusing insects point to the powerful role of the social context in
influencing acoustic signaling, an area of research receiving increasing attention in
the context of communication networks. Chorusing of vertebrates has been most
thoroughly studied in various frogs and birds; chorusing of invertebrates, in periodi-
cal cicadas (Magicicada). In the latter, a three-species complex shows extraordinary
chorusing behavior. In each species, a calling or aggregating song produced individ-
ually but sung in chorus activates and assembles both males and females. Not only
do the songs of each species differ in their acoustic parameters, but each species has
a different time of peak chorusing activity as well (Fig. 8.7). These two factors result
in a slow but clearly defined grouping of each species in its habitat. In their first few
days of adult activity the three species may be intermixed, but after a week or two
the grouping has become so intense that a tree may contain hundreds of cicadas of
one species and only one or two of the other two species. Such sorting undoubtedly
helps female cicadas find a mate of their own species.
Fig. 8.7 Singing one’s species. Approximate diurnal times of maximal chorusing activity in the
three sympatric species of Magicicada found in the southeastern United States
8.4 The Acoustic Channel 311
Do female cicadas also choose between the males on some basis of some quality
difference signaled by the songs? Periodical cicadas have seemed to be strong can-
didates for participating in lek mating systems (see Chapter 9). However, a study
of patterns of mating success in actively chorusing males could find no consistent
differences between mated and unmated males on the basis of size and song pitch,
and the patterns of mating success in actively chorusing males were indistinguish-
able from random mating. Whether other insect choruses are leks is a matter of
ongoing debate.
A chorus is an extremely complex acoustic scene. What kind of information
do males and females acquire amidst this cacophony? How do they respond to
these signals? The reception of unintended signals—in a word, eavesdropping—has
become a particularly interesting area of theoretical study. Key conceptual questions
concern how listeners might use overheard information to their own advantage, and
how signalers might strategically direct their signal production to minimize possible
eavesdropping in certain contexts.
Acoustic messages are an unusually complete and efficacious way to impart infor-
mation. They diffuse easily, resist disturbances, and exhibit the potential for creating
a vocabulary. They convey information instantaneously but they leave no lingering
traces to alert a predator or parasite. In many cases, an insect would not even need
to suspend other activities in order to transmit or receive sound signals.
Sound can be distinguished at very low levels; it also may be raised above envi-
ronmental noise by pumping energy into the system. Sound waves can travel at any
time or day or night, and not only through the air but through solid surfaces, thick
vegetation or muddy water. Sound also can be transmitted over long distances. If
one considers the distance at which signals are no longer physiologically percepti-
ble, chemical messages have perhaps the widest range and visual messages probably
the shortest; acoustic messages fall somewhere in between.
If audible sound is such a good way to communicate, why don’t more insects
use this method? One set of reasons has its roots in insects’ relatively small size.
First, for mechanical reasons small size generally results in the production of rel-
atively high-frequency signals. These attenuate and degrade more quickly than
low-frequency signals, and this limits communication range. To get around this
problem, some small insects increase their broadcast range by behavioral tactics
such as using plants as acoustic baffles, calling from elevated positions, or call-
ing from within burrows that act like horns; others have structural modifications
that allow them to produce sounds at lower frequencies than others of comparable
size.
Second, sound production may be energetically more costly and inefficient for
a small invertebrate organism. The density and elasticity of insect cuticle differs
significantly from that of air, so it takes a great deal of effort to convert the mus-
cular energy controlling a vibrating structure into airborne sound. Thus, it probably
should not come as a surprise to find that many insects have taken the route of pro-
ducing vibrations in the substrate instead. The energetic costs of tapping on a surface
are much lower than the costs of calling through the air. Tubular structures such as
leaves and stems allow information to be transmitted as bending waves, which can
travel efficiently for some distance.
Third, the narrow separation of insect ears makes it difficult to nearly impossible
for most insects to localize the source of an airborne sound by using differences in
sound wave arrival times at their ears. Some insects have gotten around this prob-
lem by adopting a different mechanism, pressure-difference receivers. However, this
has other consequences for acoustic communication, such as limiting the frequency
ranges that can be effectively used, as well as interacting with mechanisms used in
sound pattern recognition.
314 8 Mechanocommunication
Another set of reasons arise from the ease with which potential predators and
parasites can home in on sounds. For example, Mediterranean house geckos ori-
ent to locations where male decorated crickets, Gryllodes supplicans, are calling.
Even though the male crickets call from burrows where they are protected from the
geckos, this behavior enables the geckos to intercept and consume female crickets
that also respond phonotactically to the cricket calls. Eavesdropping can occur with
any sensory system, of course, but acoustic messages are particularly apt to provide
clues to location.
Unlike bird songs that often feature a learned component, the song of arthro-
pods appears to be totally hereditary. What plasticity is present is preformed; it
cannot be modified through learning. Thus, song can be considered to be a species-
characteristic feature very much like morphological features. While external factors,
especially temperature, may considerably modify the song, these modifications are
only simple responses to changes of environment much like changes in pigmen-
tation or ornamentation. This means that with even a simple mutation, song may
become an important reproductive isolating factor. When new arrangements arise
(a rare but not impossible circumstance), they are capable of producing instant iso-
lation of these individuals from other members of their species. Sometimes, this
isolation may lead to speciation; more often it does not. Two sympatric grasshopper
species, Chorthippus biguttulus and C. brunneus, produce hybrids in the laboratory,
but few hybrids are found in nature. Because hybrid males produce hybrid songs
that are intermediate between those of either parental species, the females of both
species shun them. Thus although hybrid males are reproductively fertile, they are
behaviorally sterile.
When threatened, both sexes of the velvet ant Dasymutilla occidentalis (Fig. 8.8)
stridulate with enough volume to be readily detected by the human ear a meter
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 315
Fig. 8.8 Making a ruckus. Oscillogram of the stridulation produced by an unrestrained adult
female velvet ant, Dasymutilla occidentalis, when disturbed. The unpatterned sound is produced
by an in-and-out movement of the abdomen segments; this rubs a series of transverse striations (the
file) on the dorsal anterior margin of some abdominal segments across raised ridges (scrapers) on
the posterior dorsal margins of the preceding segments. The top recording represents one direction
of abdominal movement; the lower trace represents the opposite direction
away. After an encounter with the stridulating carabid beetle Cychrus, the common
shrew will not attack this insect again.
Insects of almost every order are known to react with some noise when picked
up, pinched, probed, restrained, or otherwise disturbed. In fact, more descriptive
papers have been published on this general kind of sound communication than on
any other aspect of arthropod acoustical behavior. For most arthropods, the only
known acoustic communication is such a protest sound, a high-intensity unpatterned
sound of a broad frequency spectrum. These signals are elicited primarily upon tac-
tile stimulation. Their function is still unknown, and whether they are even primarily
airborne acoustic signals is open to question. It has been suggested that the effec-
tive stimuli are tactile or vibratory and the air-transmitted portions of the signal are
incidental.
The emission of protest sounds is so widespread in the Arthropoda that it is most
tempting to consider these as predator-escape mechanisms. However, after actual
observations one finds it at least as easy to persuade oneself that such signals do not
upset predators at all; the literature is certainly full of documentary notes on insects
being eaten while emitting such sounds. Of course, failure in a certain proportion
of cases does not necessarily mean that a protest sound does not have a defensive
function. Some startled predators may still release the prey. Even if it only worked
to the prey’s advantage some of the time, such behavior would still have positive
selective value.
Another suggestion has been that these protest sounds should be regarded merely
as displacement activity, an outlet for the intense nervous excitement involved in
capture or cornering. A variant of this theory states that these sounds have no mean-
ing at all. Why, it is asked, does the possible meaning of this acoustic display even
have to be considered when no one thinks of giving equal consideration to the role
and efficacy of the cries of a mouse captured by a cat? Of course, on this subject
one is truly unencumbered by the facts. Until some perceptive experimental work
is undertaken, the situation will remain so. It is likely that various hypotheses may
316 8 Mechanocommunication
Fig. 8.9 Performing the play list. Sound spectrograms of a domestic cricket, Acheta domesticus.
The loudest and most commonly heard song is the calling song sung by males to attract sexually
receptive females to them. Once the sexes are together, a new song—the courtship song—facilitates
mating. A quite different song, the rivalry or aggression song, is sung when two males encounter
one another and serves to determine the relative dominance of an individual. In other species,
the male may sing a post-copulatory song that keeps the sexes together for a period after sperm
transfer
Fig. 8.10 Fighting for dominance. The stereotyped sequence of escalating performances char-
acteristic of aggressive encounters between male crickets, moving from mutual avoidance (level
0) through to all-out fights with biting and grappling (6). At any point in the sequence, the fight
halts if one opponent retreats. This establishes the winner, who typically bounces around singing a
rival song
Figure 8.10 outlines the interactions that occur when two male crickets encounter
one another. How can they be explained? One might assume that the acoustic signals
of the dominant cricket’s song inhibit the aggressive tendencies of the subordinate
cricket and that inability to hear these signals frees a deafened cricket from this inhi-
bition. This would account for the increased aggression that Phillips and Konishi’s
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 319
deafened individuals displayed. However, in the rematches, the deaf individual did
not always come out on top. Therefore, it seems likely that the probability of a
cricket winning an encounter is additionally influenced by the existence of other
factors as well. In fact, tactile antennal stimuli are also important. Research has
shown that a dominant male can be ‘defeated’ by repeatedly exposing him to artifi-
cially produced aggressive sounds while lashing him with bristles simulating cricket
antennae. Other studies with staged fights indicate that crickets use such antennal
fencing to assess the fighting readiness of an opponent, and high willingness to fight
may help crickets win fights even when they actually may have inferior fighting
ability because they are younger, less experienced, or weigh less than their oppo-
nent. This is in line with game theory prediction that at least some behavior patterns
displayed during aggressive encounters are used to assess asymmetries in variables
that indicate fighting ability and resource value.
Territorial behaviors often involve aggressive/alarm sounds and vibrations. As
is the case with many vertebrates, ritualized signaling seems to allow contestants
to resolve contests without physical harm. For example, caterpillars of the hook-
tip moth Drepana arcuata defend their silk nest sites from conspecific intruders by
drumming and scraping their mandibles and specially modified anal ‘oars’ against
the leaf surface in a ritualized display that sometimes goes on for several hours. As
acoustic communication has come under closer examination, it is becoming clear
that not only the hook-tip moth but a great many other Lepidoptera larvae (and
probably many other insects as well) use acoustic signals to settle territorial disputes.
Small insectivorous birds called tits will not approach stridulating Nicrophorus car-
rion beetles. Unless they are one of the few who are aware that male Hymenoptera
cannot sting, most people will not go near a buzzing male carpenter bee. Syrphid
flies hum like the bees and wasps that they mimic visually. Bats will avoid palatable
mealworms shot into the air if the sounds of unpalatable arctiid moths are played
concurrently.
It has long been noticed informally that many insects make warning sounds that
indicate some sort of special capacity for defense such as noxious secretions, an
offensive taste, or possession of chelicerae or a sting. A predator might, after one
or several experiences, associate protest sounds with earlier painful feelings. Once
thus conditioned, the predator would treat the sound as a warning to keep its dis-
tance. The ultrasonic clicks of certain moths (see Chapter 2) appear to serve as such
a device.
A major theme in evolutionary biology is the use of visual signals that warn of
special capacity for defense and the mimicry of these signals by other organisms,
both protected and unprotected (see Chapter 5). By comparison with the extensive
research attention these visually based phenomena have received, acoustic apose-
matism and mimicry remain almost uninvestigated but both may be more common
than scientists have recognized.
320 8 Mechanocommunication
resemblance. Interestingly, some tiger moths also fly during the day where they
are exposed to visually hunting bird predators. In the spring when birds are very
active and bats less so, tiger moths tend not to produce their ultrasonic clicks, and
those species active during the day are visually conspicuous, where their multimodal
defenses also serve them well.
Acoustic mimicry can occur in contexts other than predator-prey interactions,
of course. Courtship and mating provide another important set of behaviors open
to exploitation. For example, males and females of the Australian bush cricket
Caedicia normally sing in duets. However, when they cannot find a duet partner,
some males perform an extensive repertoire of acoustic mimicry tricks instead. They
make short-click calls that mimic the female’s reply within a duet. They disruptively
over-sing other males’ song elements that are critical for the female’s conspecific
recognition. Sometimes they even make a call that mimics the entire duet. As they
perform all this deception, they also search for the calls of any unmated female that
might be tricked into a response.
signals are used mainly for species recognition, whereas the courtship song may
contain information about an individual male’s quality. If so, a calling song might
be expected to be more stereotyped and less variable than a courtship song. Such
a difference has been demonstrated in the field cricket Teleogryllus oceanicus; the
higher variability in courtship songs did not depend on the response of the female
being courted, because similar results were obtained when males courted dead
females.
In long-range acoustic pair formation, one usually thinks first of a direct loco-
motory response of a silent female to a single sedentary male’s calling song, such
as occurs in most crickets (Fig. 8.11). Such oriented movements in response to
acoustic signals are called phonotaxes. Many variations in pair formation exist,
however. Some involve elaborate visual/acoustic displays by both sexes. Others
Fig. 8.11 Finding the singer. Positive phonotaxis used in experimental studies on the inheritance
of song in hybrid Teleogryllus crickets. Walking along a featherweight Y-maze, tethered recep-
tive hybrid adult females listened to different hybrid male calling songs played simultaneously by
equally distant speakers, and revealed their preference at fork points in the maze
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 323
depend upon alternating and distinctive songs between males and females. Habitat
ecology is undoubtedly a major determinant of the type of system that can be
employed. For example, diurnal desert grasshoppers that live in open habitats can
use visual–acoustic systems with ease, but tettigoniid grasshoppers that live in visu-
ally restricted habitats and have nocturnal and/or cryptic habits must rely more
heavily upon strictly acoustic elements.
During long-distance pair formation, one also usually thinks of each species
stridulating its own specific sex-related messages. If they do not, one immediately
assumes some other clear-cut isolating mechanism, but sometimes the case is less
well defined than this. One of the best examples of acoustically based reproduc-
tive isolation involves two sympatric grasshoppers, Chorthippus brunneus and C.
biguttulus. Morphologically they differ only by a very few small, nonoverlapping
characters. Although they have different songs, the two species are not separated
ecologically. Studies by A.C. Perdeck showed that the two species would cross-mate
and produce viable offspring in the laboratory, but very little hybridization occurred
in the field. How could this be? Careful field study revealed that the major difference
between the species lay in the degree of stimulating and orienting influence that their
songs possessed. In the presence of its own specific song, a male moved faster and
made more copulatory attempts; a female was also more ready to copulate in the
presence of the song of her own species. Hybrid females would answer the songs
of both hybrid males and parental males, but pure females of either parental species
would not respond to the song of hybrid males, which was intermediate between the
parental songs.
Long-range acoustic courtship signals are best known in cicadas, in some male
Orthoptera (Acrididae, Gryllidae, Tettigoniidae), and in some Diptera, but there is
suggestive evidence for their occurrence in males of many other arthropods. In the
classic case, a male insect stridulates sex-related messages and the female receives
them through her tympanal organs. However, sound production may also arise as a
by-product of other activities such as flight, and in some species the sounds used
in communication fall within the sensitivity range of hair sensilla and antennal
receptors. These sounds could, and do, also attract the opposite sex. Predictably,
the significant properties of these sorts of courtship sounds are rather different than
for the songs of Orthoptera.
Mosquitoes are a case in point. Many mosquitoes produce an audible hum during
flight, often so distinctive that some people can identify mosquito species solely by
their sound. Astute observers have long known that mosquitoes react to sounds as
well. Swarms of various species have been seen to alter their behavior following
acoustic stimuli as varied as pistol shots, locomotive whistles, musical instru-
ments, and the singing human voice. Furthermore, the antennae of male and female
mosquitoes of many species are structurally quite different. The female antennal
flagellum is about 2 mm long; the male flagellum, only about 1.6 mm. The flagel-
lum of the male antenna is plumose, with many more hairs of varying lengths, and
intriguingly, these hairs are absent in the males of some mosquito species for which
acoustic communication has not been demonstrated. As though to underscore the
hairs’ importance, males that have them can change their positions relative to the
324 8 Mechanocommunication
antennal shaft; most of the time the hairs lie flat, but they are raised erect during the
period of swarming and mating.
Despite such circumstantial evidence for mosquito hearing, however, some care-
ful studies were needed to prove its existence and unlock the details. The first
researcher to elucidate the basis of mosquito sexual communication in detail was
Louis M. Roth. Case Study 8.3 presents a few aspects of Roth’s complex, wide-
ranging investigation; the interested student may wish to peruse his original 1948
study for further detail.
Do mosquitoes listen to one another? If so, how? And what do they actually
hear? Before some perceptive research, the acoustic basis of mosquito sexual
communication was a matter of dispute.
In 1855, Christopher Johnston, a medical doctor in Baltimore, Maryland,
discovered a sensory organ in the second segment of the mosquito antenna,
and suggested it might play a role in mosquito hearing. Although the organ
was subsequently named after him, until about sixty years ago, scientists
were unwilling to give his observations a pivotal role in a behavior as vital
to survival as sexual attraction. Instead, one researcher claimed that males
were drawn to females by odor. Another postulated that the female’s motion
alone was sufficient for male recognition and attraction. A third suggest
that knoblike projections (halteres) behind the wings of the female produced
attractive ‘birdlike’ sounds both at rest and in flight.
As an entomologist in the U.S. Army during World War II, Louis M. Roth
had his own hunches. Observing yellow fever mosquitoes, he had noticed
that when a female mosquito was resting, she sometimes was surrounded by
males, with some so near that they touched her, but he never observed them
mating. It therefore appeared unlikely that odor or constantly produced sounds
were involved. It seemed more likely, he thought, that male mosquitoes were
attracted by the buzzing sounds produced by a female’s wings during flight
and that males received this sound through their enlarged antennal bases,
which have conspicuous Johnston’s organs. But how best to prove this?
Beginning with careful behavioral observations, Roth established that once
male Aedes aegypti became sexually responsive 15–24 hours after emergence,
they remained in a constant mating state through the rest of their lives. When
he forced caged females to fly continuously, males attempted mating with
them repeatedly; after a time, the females tended to resist further mating
attempts but as long as they continued to fly, males continued to be attracted.
However, when he killed female mosquitoes and presented them, the same
males that had readily mated with flying females were indifferent to these
freshly dead bodies.
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 325
work of Johnston, Roth, and others. Their studies have revealed a very complex
system, and the mechanical sensitivity of the male mosquito’s antenna has been
shown to exceed that of all other arthropod movement receivers studied so far.
Research on physiological aspects of mosquito acoustic communication was
hampered initially by technical limitations and the antennae’s morphological com-
plexity and small size, but in the last decade or two, new technology has allowed
new analyses. Studies show that because the antennal hairs are stiffly coupled to the
shaft of the antennal flagellum, at the appropriate frequencies, the flagellum moves
like a stiff rod rocking about in its socket. This stimulates the Johnston’s organ at the
flagellum’s base. In both male and female mosquitoes, the antennae are resonantly
tuned mechanical systems that move as simple forced damped harmonic oscillators
when they are stimulated. The tuning of female antennae does not match the flight
sounds of males or of females, but male antennae respond to a range of attractive
frequencies that brackets the fundamental frequency of female flight sounds for their
species. This bracketing occurs because hairs of different lengths have different best
frequencies, with the result that males respond to a wider frequency band than would
be possible if all the hairs were the same length. This may be an advantage in that it
assures that females remain attractive even if they produce slightly different sounds
because of such uncontrollable factors as size, wing damage, or extent of distension
of their abdomen with food.
Is the resonance of the hairs themselves essential for mosquito hearing? Probably
not. These same studies have shown that the hairs resonate at frequencies much
higher than the best frequencies not only of female flight sounds, but of all arthropod
sensory hairs known to serve as near-field sound receivers. For this reason, some
researchers contend that the main function of the hairs may just be to increase the
antennal surface. At this scale, they may also increase the drag force of air passing
between the hairs on the antenna, much like a boat paddle dragged through water.
Earlier we mentioned acoustic tuning by males of Toxorhynchites brevipalpis.
Despite the fact that the Johnston’s organ is only slightly less sensitive in female
mosquitoes than in males, most studies have investigated ways in which males are
attracted to female flight tones. Gabriella Gibson and Ian Russell have done some
of the first work to show that the auditory attraction phase of sexual recognition
in mosquitoes involves interactive behaviors, just as in sexual behaviors of other
insects. When they flew opposite-sex pairs of tethered T. brevipalpis, each mosquito
altered its wing-beat frequency in response to the flight tone of the other, so that
within seconds their flight-tone frequencies were closely matched or completely
synchronized (Fig. 8.12). The behavioral thresholds in both sexes are about ten
times greater than neural thresholds, differences that could be explained by neural
processing (see Chapter 2).
Before leaving the subject of mosquitoes, it should be noted that once again, a
vital activity such as courtship relies upon more than one sensory mode. Swarming
Aedes aegypti males have been shown to produce a volatile pheromone that
stimulates the flying activity of females at a distance. Females also produce a
volatile attractant that appears to act as an aggregation pheromone. The resul-
tant pheromone-mediated swarm has been described as a 3-dimensional lek (see
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 327
Fig. 8.12 Flying in tune. Responses of tethered flying Toxorhynchites brevipalpis mosquitoes
show that males and females interactively synchronize their wing-beat frequencies to one another.
(above) The flight tone of a flying male is shown as an oscillogram, with the frequency spectrum
of the sound pressure levels (SPL) for the area outlined in the rectangle directly below. (middle and
below)The lower two graphs depict spectrograms of a flying male and a flying female, respectively,
over a 10-second recording; each shows fundamental components of flight tones before, during,
and after a pure stimulus tone (solid line) was played. The male’s spectrogram illustrates that
his flight tone first exceeds and then converges with the stimulus tone. The female’s spectrogram
shows that her flight tone first drifts upward, then decreases without reaching the stimulus tone,
and finally returns to the original frequency. Numbers on vertical axis indicate flight tone at onset
of record, peak frequencies during the record (dotted lines), and final frequency. The dashed line
indicates the flight tone in the second before the stimulus
Chapter 10), though there is still some question as to whether female choice is
involved in these swarms.
Calloconophora forage cooperatively; scouts that locate a new feeding site vibrate
the leaf, and newly recruited individuals signal in concert with those that have
already arrived. Hornets produce and are sensitive to a variety of sounds inside their
nests. Honey bees initiate swarming in part in response to a contagious buzzing
sound started by a few workers running excitedly through the hive.
Group-living insects range from those existing in simple aggregations to those
that cannot survive outside of their complex societies (see Chapter 10). To be social
necessitates having a way to integrate the cooperative behaviors of group members.
Social communication is rarely restricted exclusively to one particular mode. Most
often such integrative communication is primarily chemical, but sound also serves
as an effective avenue of communication in social insects, often as a back-up and
sometimes as a primary channel.
The interlocking combination of odor, sonic, and tactile signals that character-
izes the communication system of the Douglas fir bark beetle (see Chapter 6) is a
case in point. However, one of the classic and best-studied examples of the interac-
tion of communicatory modes is provided by that classic social insect, the common
honey bee. The behavior involved is recruitment to food, one of the most important
adaptations of nearly all social insects. Case Study 8.4 outlines the story.
Honey bee dance language remains one of the most striking, best-studied
examples of mixed channel communication and of a highly complex transfer
of symbolic information.
Beginning in 1919 with a few paint-marked bees and a flower enriched
with a drop of sugar syrup, Austrian biologist Karl von Frisch began a series
of thorough observations of honey bees that has culminated in what is proba-
bly the best known example of complex insect communication in the entire
world. In recognition of his pioneering work, von Frisch shared a Nobel
Prize in 1973, the only entomologist to date so honored; behaviorists Niko
Tinbergen and Konrad Lorenz received it with him. He continued to research
honey bee communication for many decades and in the 1960s, published an
outstanding summary of the work he and his students had conducted. The
story continues to excite new research, and has become part of the shared
human experience as the premiere example of symbolic communication aside
from human language.
What was it that excited people so? Von Frisch had learned that a forager
bee that found a good food source would bring back a full load of pollen and
nectar to the hive. It would then fly out again to the food, thus establishing the
most direct outward route. Back in the hive upon its second return, the suc-
cessful forager communicated its vital information to others by performing
specific, repeated, stereotyped behaviors, which von Frisch termed ‘dances’,
upon the vertical face of the honey-comb inside the hive (see Figs. 8.13
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 329
Fig. 8.13 Sharing good news. The dance language of a successfully foraging honey bee. (a) A
forager locating a source of food close to the hive (within a distance that varies among strains)
moves in circles of alternating direction, performing a round dance. Followers of the round dance
obtain information that food is near. As the distance to the food increases, the round dance perfor-
mance gradually changes to an open figure-8, then to the waggle dance (b) performed by foragers
discovering food sources far from the hive. As the bee passes through the straight run portion of
the dance, she waggles her abdomen rapidly from side to side. At the end of each straight run, she
circles back to the beginning, to the left one time and to the right the next, so that the dance takes
a figure-eight shape
and 8.14). In response, other foragers accurately flew directly to the food.
The discovery captured the common imagination and excited the scientific
community. Although observers had repeatedly noticed that a bee in a colony
sometimes would perform repeated circular movements, no one had firmly
established the connection between these movements and recruitment, and
few had guessed the complexity of the communication.
One aspect of the forager’s message was distance, which correlates with the
type of dance the forager displays. Relatively short distances evoked a round
dance; longer distances, a waggle dance. Within the waggle dance, a number
of features also correlate with food source distance. Von Frisch’s standard
measure was tempo, which he measured as number of circuits (a waggling
run plus the loop that returns the bee to the beginning of the next waggling
run) per 15 seconds. Correlations also occur with the speed or length of the
wagging run, the duration of the semicircular run, the duration of a single
tail-wagging phase, and the number of tail wags during the straight run. How
did the bee know how far it had flown? For a foraging bee, distance could be
assessed in various ways. To von Frisch, the most important aspect seemed to
be the energy that the bee had expended in covering it, because if a bee had
to fly into a headwind or uphill or if it was forced to walk, it would report a
distance farther than what was actually involved.
330 8 Mechanocommunication
Fig. 8.14 Translating directions. When a forager waggle-dances to recruit others, her dance
velocity and number of figure-eight shapes per unit of time indicate distance to the food source.
Direction is more complex; whereas she learned the flight directions horizontally relative to the
sun azimuth, her dance takes place inside the dark hive on a vertical comb. To translate this to her
followers, the forager uses the direction upward as a reference for sun direction, and orients the
waggle portion of her dance at the same relative angle. Thus, a cross-run angle of 60◦ to the left
of the upward vertical axis as she dances means that the food was on a path 60◦ to the left of the
sun-hive line, going toward the sun. Interestingly, on sunny days a forager will sometimes dance at
the hive entrance; in this horizontal position, her tail-wagging run points directly toward the food
source
The other aspect was direction. The angle of the tail-wagging run in the wag-
gle dance with respect to the vertical is equal to the angle formed between the
sun’s position and the food source. Communication of this aspect is theoreti-
cally complex, because the dance is performed inside the hive out of sight of
the sky and (because it happens on a vertical comb) it must be translated to
occur with reference to gravity (see Fig. 3.6) rather than directly to the sun
itself. Furthermore, using the sun as a directional reference is complicated
by two problems—it moves, and sometimes it hides. Thus, both a celestial
compass and perception of polarized light are involved.
Some returning foragers do not dance at all; others may dance a hundred
circuits. As this would suggest, honey bee recruitment seems to be regulated
in a way that allocates outgoing bees to the resources that most benefit the
colony. The number of waggling runs a forager performs seems to indicate
the overall value of the resource, and bees dancing to good patches seem to
circle and dance more rapidly. Bees perform more dances to more sources of
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 331
nectar that are more highly concentrated or closer. When a colony is heat-
stressed, they perform more often and more vigorously to water sources than
to nectar sources.
A returning worker also changes her information in response to the activi-
ties of nest-mates with whom she interacts. When a successful forager returns
home, other workers greet her and, if she is carrying nectar, they induce her
to regurgitate her load to them. If many workers greet her enthusiastically,
she begins to dance a round or waggle dance. If, instead, she perceives a
long delay in unloading nectar or a shortage of receiver bees, she will per-
form a lesser-known variant called a tremble dance. Although von Frisch
described this dance in the 1920s, it has received less attention, and its func-
tion was unclear until 1993 when Wolfgang Kirschner discovered that the
tremble dance stopped nearby workers from flying to gather more nectar and
switched them to unloading nectar.
How do followers sense the dance? Given that the dance language is com-
munication, it is surprising how much remains undecided about this aspect
of the system. One reason is simply the complexity involved; the dances pro-
vide such a rich variety of potential information that it remains difficult to
know which information is actually being used and how it is being passed on.
Another reason is that the dance features help followers find and stay with the
dancer may differ from the features that carry the location signals.
Von Frisch emphasized the role of odors, specifically odors from flowers
and other environmental chemicals that might cling to the bees’ hairy bodies
and be detected by dance followers. He suggested that the dances got bees
into the flowers’ general vicinity, then odors from the forager guided them
in. In support of this idea, he noted that a powerful odor can even lead bees
to ignore dance information and instead find the food that the odor signals.
Foragers have in fact been found also to release an attractive pheromone as
they return to a familiar feeding place.
Might hearing also be involved? One of the first to tackle that question
experimentally with the aid of scientific instrumentation was Harald Esch,
who had spent some time studying bees with von Frisch. In the 1960s, Esch
built an artificial motor-driven bee capable of doing a waggle dance. When
it was introduced into the hive, bees clustered around it just as they would a
genuine forager, but no bees left the hive to search for food. Why? Perhaps
the robot didn’t sound right, Esch reasoned. So he placed a microphone in
the hive to record actual dancing bee sounds. To his surprise, not only did
the dancing bees produce strong sound signals with vibrations of their wings,
but the period of sound production coincided exactly with the duration of
the straight run of the dance. Since von Frisch had already found a correlation
between the latter and the distance to the food source, a logical conclusion was
that the bees might equally well be using sound as the cue to distance rather
332 8 Mechanocommunication
than using the visual or tactile cues of the waggle itself. About the same time,
Adrian Wenner independently discovered that sound was produced during the
bee’s dance. Produced mainly at 250 cycles/second, the sound occurs in bursts
and the number of bursts correlates directly both with the distance and with
the sugar concentration of the food. The pulse rate of these sounds is directly
related to the rate of waggling, but they are approximately 2.5 times as fast.
Because the followers can be seen to have antennal contact with the dancer,
other researchers noted that it is also likely that tactile cues are important.
The speed of waggling could be sensed this way as a measure of distance.
Contact could also be necessary in another way. By following the dancer and
closely imitating the path of her dance, as many potential recruits do, they
might obtain information on direction as their proprioceptors monitored this
imposed movement. Von Frisch also showed that foragers could orient their
dances either to celestial clues or to gravity through proprioreceptive bristle
fields between the major body segments and the segments of the legs.
In general, as one moves closer and the insects involved become smaller,
hearing grades into vibration and near-field signals, and finally to touch. Many
studies have now provided evidence that dancing bees produce sound with
their wings and followers detect these with their antennae. They also produce
vibrations in the substrate that could be detected via the subgenual organs.
At very close inter-bee distances, near field airflows are generated by the
dancer’s vibrating wings and by its waggling abdomen. Observations show
that most follower bees place their heads directly into this zone of intense
airflows. During the waggle run, there is substantial physical contact between
dancers and followers. However, at all these levels, questions remain unsettled
as to whether the information is precise enough and/or is used beyond some
restricted circumstances.
What about vision? Though humans find it natural to describe what the
dances look like, in the total darkness of the hive bees could not observe the
dance and visually interpret its tempo. However, von Frisch recognized that
vision might have an important role at an earlier stage: foraging bees might
measure distance by monitoring optic flow, the streaming of visual texture
across the visual field as an animal moves through the environment. He noted
that bees flying to a feeder over calm water (which provides a weak optic flow)
signaled a shorter distance in their dances than bees that had flown over more
visually textured surfaces such as a wind-disturbed lake. Von Frisch was never
quite convinced that optic flow was as important as his energy hypothesis
as a way to encode foraging distance. However, through an extensive series
of studies, Esch and others have shown that optic flow is probably not only
the most important, but perhaps the only, odometer for foraging honey bees.
Furthermore, they have shown that many of the experimental results that von
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 333
Frisch interpreted as supporting the energy hypothesis are also consistent with
the optic-flow hypothesis.
With so many senses interacting, one finally must ask what systems are
not involved in the honey bee dance. Even though bees can orient to magnetic
fields in other contexts, so far there is no evidence that they use magnetic
orientation in their dance. The evidence for orientation via landmarks is also
equivocal; honey bees can use landmarks if they were first given the opportu-
nity to see them in conjunction with celestial clues, but the role of this ability
in nature remains to be shown.
When Karl von Frisch showed that bees could use waggles and circles to tell
other bees where to find forage, he moved honey bees and their close relatives to
front and center as a model of animal communication and social behavior. Ever
since, biologists have been trying to figure out the details of how bees communicate
this information and attract recruits in the darkness of the hive. Probably the most
important lesson learned in that search for answers has been that important com-
munication nearly always involves redundancy—more than one sensory modality
acting simultaneously and generally reinforcing one another’s messages.
Failure to consider such possibilities when analyzing communication systems
can be a pitfall, as was illustrated by the so-called dance language controversy
that arose in the 1960s when some researchers (particularly Adrian Wenner, Patrick
Wells, and Dennis Johnson) directly challenged von Frisch’s theories. suggested
that even though the dances might contain spatial information, it was irrele-
vant to honey bee recruitment. No interplay of sensory systems was needed;
odors alone were sufficient to explain the phenomenon. In fact, the entire con-
cept of bee communication was unnecessary. Wenner, for example, asserted that
bees are recruited solely by a conditioned response to food or odor. He felt that
bees lacking experience with a particular food source have great difficulty find-
ing it. Only a relatively few seem to be successful, and these take much longer
to arrive than would be expected if they were flying a straight line between the
hive and the source. Inexperienced foragers, Wenner suggested, locate the source
simply by dropping downwind of the hive and then searching for the right com-
bination of odors from food, locality, and other bees. Johnson and Wenner’s
experiments, done with strong scents and short distances, seemed to confirm their
expectations.
Some of von Frisch’s experimental results couldn’t be readily reinterpreted in
terms of an odor-only hypothesis, however. For example, when a hive is turned on
its side so that bees cannot use gravity as a reference, they do disoriented dances.
Under these conditions, recruits are less well oriented, even though odor cues have
not been affected.
Normally, odor information and dance vector information are highly correlated.
To settle the controversy, scientists knew they must tease these factors apart. In
the 1970s, James Gould did this quite successfully in a set of experiments that
334 8 Mechanocommunication
unlinked the location and odor of the foragers’ food source from the directional
information in their subsequent dances. He knew that if one shines a bright light
from the side, dancing bees will use the position of this light as though it were
the sun, and recruits will interpret it that way. However, if a bee’s ocelli are coated
with opaque paint, the bee becomes less sensitive to light, and this shift in ref-
erence does not occur. Gould set up a situation in which dancing recruiters with
painted ocelli (and a reference of up, toward the sun) were followed by recruits with
unpainted ocelli who were reading those dances relative to whatever angle the arti-
ficial light produced. In this way, he was able to show that recruits could interpret
a direction from the dance that was independent of the direction to the food source.
When given the opportunity to hunt for the forager’s nectar source, these recruits
searched principally in the direction predicted by the modified dance information.
By the odor-only hypothesis, they should have still gone toward the true direction
of the feeder.
Many other imaginative investigations also took place as the scientific com-
munity subjected these two varying interpretations to critical examination and
experimental test, including development of a series of mechanical models and
robots capable of mimicking various aspects of honeybee dances (Fig. 8.15). One
of these was a computer controlled robot developed by Axel Michelsen, Martin
Lindauer, and Wolfgang Kirchner that mimicked the behavior of a dancing bee;
although it recruited rather imprecisely, changes in its dance caused significant shifts
Fig. 8.15 Mimicking a forager. An example of a complex model developed to simulate honey
bee foraging communication. (left) As an X-Y recorder (A) drives the model’s figure-eight and
wagging motions, a stepper motor (B) turns the model. Another motor (C) connected to a syringe
(S) pumps scented sugar water through a soft tube to the front end of a beeswax-covered model,
and an electromagnetic driver (D) from a bone-conduction hearing aid moves a wing (E) made of
a tiny piece of razorblade. (right) Potential recruits surround the mechanical model as it performs
a wagging run
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 335
in the search distribution of recruits in just the way that von Frisch’s dance language
theory predicted.
The odor search hypothesis has not yet been abandoned by its most loyal adher-
ents, and unfortunately some philosophers and science sociologists have greeted the
‘dance language controversy’ as a welcome example of what they see as severe lim-
itations of the scientific method. However, it is now clear that honey bee recruits can
use either the dance language and odor information or odors alone, and they most
probably can and do use other sensory inputs as well. The relative importance of
these various inputs undoubtedly varies from one situation to another. Furthermore,
as Gould has pointed out, the controversy might not have arisen but for the fact that
while von Frisch and Wenner were both studying situations involving exploitation
of an abundant food source, they focused on different stages of this process: von
Frisch on the early stages of discovery, Wenner on later stages.
The general consensus now seems to be that sounds, comb vibrations, tactile
cues from touching antennae, and odors all play roles in this elaborate transfer of
information, and that additional signals will probably continue to be discovered.
A study by Corinna Thom and associates in 2007 has added another component,
semiochemicals (see Chapter 6) in the form of four cuticular hydrocarbons emitted
by wagglers. Interestingly, the most energetic waggle-dancers produced the most
copious quantities of these compounds.
How did such a complex communication system evolve? Seeking an answer,
researchers such as Esch, Lindauer, and Kerr were among the first to focus on related
stingless bees (Meliponini) that occur in the tropics. All 11 species of these social
bees that they studied show some means of alerting their colonies to food, but the
efficiency and degree of development of these communication systems vary greatly.
In the simplest case, Trigona iridipennis workers that have found a food source sim-
ply fly back to the comb and excitedly run about, thus alerting other workers to the
existence of food but not to its location. In the most complex case, a Trigona pos-
tica worker constructs a trail system of marks left at short intervals by rubbing her
mandibles on convenient objects such as leaves to deposit mandibular gland secre-
tions. Back at the comb, the bee then runs noisily about, giving out food samples.
When a number of other bees have been acoustically and chemically alerted, the for-
ager leads these bees along the scent trail. Such a system is of special advantage to
these forest-dwelling species, because when living among trees vertical movements
may be as important as horizontal ones.
James Nieh has shown that another stingless bee, Melipona panamica, sends and
receives acoustic signals that allow inexperienced workers to communicate foraging
information. Foragers trained to a feeder at the top of the jungle canopy make short
pulses of sound while sharing food inside the nest, whereas those trained to a feeder
at the forest floor make much longer sound pulses. Immediately after unloading, the
forager dances briefly; sounds produced during this phase correlate with distance
of the food, with longer pulses given for greater distances. Nieh did various studies
in which he trained bees to a feeder, let these experienced foragers interact with
inexperienced bees, then released only the inexperienced bees to find a new feeder
336 8 Mechanocommunication
placed at the training location (Fig. 8.16). In this way, he showed that both dis-
tance and height of the food source are communicated inside the hive, but direction
appears to be communicated outside, perhaps by following the foragers outside the
nest for a brief distance.
Honey bees represent a very tiny fraction of the approximately 20,000 known
species of bees. Among the present members of the genus Apis, the more basal
species make single, exposed combs, while the more recently evolved species nest
in cavities and have multiple combs (a development that has greatly facilitated their
domestication). Martin Lindauer’s pioneering study of three Apis species that differ
in their nesting habits seemed to indicate a progression in complexity that appears
to correspond to the phylogenetic development of the dance during Apis evolution
(Fig. 8.17). On a horizontal surface, both A. florea and A. mellifera orient similarly
(Fig. 8.17a). However, on a slope, differences are apparent. Dwarf honey bee
dancers invariably orient to the sun’s azimuth by using the sky horizon as a refer-
ence; placed on a slope, they compensate by head rotation (Fig. 8.17b). In contrast,
true honey bees on a slope rotate both their body and head (Fig. 8.17c), so that the
slope’s plane serves as an apparent horizon reference for orienting to celestial cues.
To Lindauer this suggested that dancing first evolved in an ancestral bee that built
a single comb in the open and oriented to celestial cues but could not yet substitute
gravity for the sun, much as A. florea still does today. Perhaps the earliest dances
were simply excited, disorganized movements, but other bees were aroused by the
excitement and thus were more apt to leave and search for food. In a later evolu-
tionary stage, still on an open nest comb, dancers began to translate their solar flight
angle into a dance angle relative to gravity and added acoustic signals. Over time,
bees became able to use celestial cues when they were available and gravity cues
when they were not, setting the stage for bees to begin moving their nests into the
protection afforded by cavities. As intriguing as Lindauer’s theory may be, however,
there have been a number of objections to it, based both on taxonomic uncertainties
as to ancestry of the various bees and on behavioral studies that have revealed more
plasticity than Lindauer observed. Researchers’ hopes for discovering ‘missing
links’ in the shape of simpler forms of honey bee dance language so far have not
been fulfilled, but however the story plays out, it promises to be an interesting one.
Before leaving the subject, it is interesting to note that elements of the bee dance
also have been uncovered in a diversity of insects outside of the Hymenoptera. For
Fig. 8.16 Communicating food location. The tropical stingless bee Melipona panamica uses a
wide range of communication techniques including sound. Some aspects of the information are
shared inside the nest; other aspects, outside. In the three types of trials shown here, bees were
trained to a feeder, then allowed to interact with inexperienced bees inside the nest. When the
inexperienced bees were released to find food, (a) more found the training feeder high in the forest
canopy than found the control feeder at the base of the trees; (b) many more went to the training
feeder than to the control feeder that was closer to the nest; and (c) equal numbers went to the
control feeder and training feeder when they were at equal distances but in opposite directions
from each other
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 337
Fig. 8.17 Translating the sun. (above) The exposed nest of the dwarf honey bee, Apis florea,
(left) with and (right) without the curtain of bees. Dances are performed on the upper horizontal
surface of the comb and are oriented directly toward the source of the food. (below) Various bee
species orient differently on horizontal and sloped surfaces (see text)
example, blowflies will perform a crude sort of dance after eating, with a vigor and
persistence related to the distance the fly has flown after feeding. Some saturniid
moths rhythmically sway upon settling down after any movement; the number of
oscillations is closely correlated with the distance they have flown before settling. Of
course, there is no evidence that these activities have any communicative function;
they may be simple by-products of the physiological state of the insect concerned.
But they do share the important characteristic of persistence, that is, they continue to
be performed for a considerable time after the cessation of the activity that produced
them. Such persistence is of paramount importance in communication, especially in
recruitment of foraging bees. If such by-products were to come to signal the sender’s
state to another animal, then they would be ideal materials for the construction of
a communication language. However, recruitment to food is considered primarily
an adaptation to social life. Why should this be so? Sociobiologists point out that
being able to recruit nest-mates to food sources allows insect colonies to capitalize
on one of the major advantages of living in groups—the ability to harvest food
8.5 Functions of Insect Communicative Sounds 339
that would not be readily available to an individual foraging alone. At the same
time, recruitment overcomes an inherent disadvantage of group life. Were it not for
the compensation that recruitment provides, solitary individuals would have better
access to food if they did not have to compete with and share with other group
members.
Chapter 9
Reproductive Behavior
9.1 Introduction
Fig. 9.1 Going to extremes. Lovebugs, Plecia nearctica (Diptera: Bibionidae), in typical copu-
latory position (female on the right). Attracted in large numbers both to car engine exhaust fume
components and to engine warmth, mating pairs can be a serious problem for motorists in the
southern United States. In the laboratory, lovebug pairs copulate for an average of 56 hours
Douglas fir bark beetles and the tactile, visual, and olfactory interplay involved in
butterfly courtship.
These examples left several questions unanswered, however. Why are the
courtship rituals of some species so intricate, even bizarre, whereas many others
mate with apparently only minimal preliminaries? What factors influence the choice
of a mate? Why are some species territorial? Why do individuals of some species
mate but once, whereas others promiscuously?
Insect reproduction has been one of the most intensively studied aspects of insect
biology in the past century. One reason is quite practical, based on the intersection
between insect populations and our own interests in agriculture and human health.
Learning about the reproduction of problem insects offers the promise of disrupting
that reproduction and reducing the number of those insects in the next generation.
The other reason is more theoretical, based on the diversity in insect reproduction,
which provides a rich source of material for discovering underlying rules of biology.
As intensively studied model systems in both field and laboratory, insects are ideal
for addressing questions like these.
After an extensive aerial courtship, a male butterfly places a packet of sperm into
the female’s genital opening. Bed bug males inject sperm subcutaneously into the
female’s blood-filled body cavity. A male damselfly transfers sperm away from his
abdominal tip, freeing its claspers to grasp a female by the prothorax; the pair may
fly about for hours in this tandem position (Fig. 9.2, Plate 33).
Sexual reproduction, nearly all biologists agree, has evolved because of the much
greater speed with which new genotypes are assembled under this system. Such
diversity, they argue, is highly adaptive. Sexually reproducing populations are more
likely than asexual ones to adjust to changed environmental conditions through the
creation of new genetic combinations. Thus, sexual reproduction has increasingly
become the mode. In sexually reproducing animals, reproduction includes all those
events surrounding insemination (the transfer of a male’s sperm to a female) and
fertilization (the fusion of a sperm and egg to create a diploid zygote). We are
accustomed to the two events being closely linked, but in insects they often are
not, because insemination can be either internal or external and can either be direct
or through an intermediary step that involves a transfer container, or spermatophore
(see Plate 29). In addition, fertilization can occur as much as years later because
long-lived sperm can be stored in a special female pouch called a spermatheca.
Thus, rather than spanning all of reproduction, the narrower term ‘mating behavior’
is used to describe the more immediate actions that surround insemination itself,
from pair formation through courtship and copulation to the final breakup of the
mating pair.
The reproductive behavior of the primitively wingless (apterygote) insects has
received relatively little attention. In those that have been studied, an almost com-
plete dissociation of individual males and females occurs, each sex reacting to a
9.2 Courtship and Mating 343
Fig. 9.2 Indirectly transferring sperm. Prior to copulation, the male of dragonflies and dam-
selflies transfers sperm into a pouch-like structure on the underside of the second or third
abdominal segment. During copulation, the female bends the tip of her abdomen forward to con-
tact the male’s accessory genitalia. The male (above) uses claspers at his abdominal tip to grasp
the female’s prothorax, thus completing the “wheel position” characteristic of this group (right).
Pairs of damselflies or dragonflies often fly about for hours in tandem, the male holding the female
by his anal claspers (left); in some species the female oviposits during tandem flight
spermatophore rather than to each other. Such indirect sperm transfer seems to char-
acterize Archaeognatha, Thysanura, Collembola, Protura, and Diplura. In contrast,
in most winged (pterygote) species, sperm transfer is direct, through copulation. For
these insects, mating almost always involves chain-like interactions (see Fig. 7.9)
that begin with searching for mates, move on to pair formation and courtship, and
progress to copulation. For many, post-copulatory male behaviors culminate the
reaction chain.
Given the human interest in courtship and mating, it is perhaps no surprise to
find an enormous amount of published information on this aspect of insect behavior,
nor is it unusual that different approaches to the subject have arisen. The field has
become far too large to review comprehensively, but a brief overview can suggest
some of the directions that are being taken.
Early research on insect mating behaviors was almost solely descriptive. As
would be expected in a taxon as large as Insecta, the biological diversity in mating
behavior is enormous. Just to chronicle variation in the manner in which copulation
is accomplished has begun to produce an insect Kama Sutra. That task is still far
from finished and it is still a useful approach, not only in its own right but also in
providing the raw materials with which other analyses can proceed. Despite tremen-
dous diversity in the ways they may accomplish their goal, however, most insects
must solve a strikingly similar set of problems. They must find a partner, recog-
nize it as such, comport themselves appropriately so that insemination will occur
and be successful, and—for males—counteract potential sperm competition. Thus,
344 9 Reproductive Behavior
mating behavior studies increasingly are focusing upon broader and more theoret-
ical questions of causation and organization, fitness consequences, evolution, and
sexual selection in the context of costs and benefits to the individuals involved.
Male sexual behavior usually matures along with the metamorphosis of the ner-
vous system; following ecdysis, expression of full copulatory behavior awaits only
suitable stimuli. In most such cases, the corpora allata are not involved, although
sometimes juvenile hormone lowers the male’s behavioral threshold to the female.
The endocrine control of male sexual behavior has been best studied in the grasshop-
pers and locusts, where some species require the corpora allata for the maturation
of male sexual behavior. For example, allatectomy of newly ecdysed males of the
desert locust, Schistocerca gregaria, and the red locust, Nomadacris septemfasciata,
completely prevents the onset of male sexual behavior; if, however, several pairs of
active corpora allata are transplanted into an allatectomized male, sexual behavior
reappears.
Particularly among longer-lived insects that have a restricted breeding season,
male sexual behavior often is modulated through an interaction between the cor-
pora allata, the brain’s neurosecretory system, and environmental parameters such
as photoperiod. For example, under long-day conditions, males of some locust and
grasshopper species show little or no sexual behavior, apparently due to cessation of
neurosecretory activity in the brain. Sexual behavior resumes, however, when these
uninterested males receive active corpora allata implanted from other locusts reared
under short day conditions.
Female insects usually pass through three distinct behavioral states: young vir-
ginity, unreceptive to courtship; mature virginity, responsive to male advances;
and mated, typically unresponsive toward males but showing intense oviposition
behavior. The switch from one state to another is often abrupt and often hormon-
ally induced. Hormones regulate pheromone production and release so that mating
will occur at the appropriate stage in the reproductive cycle of the emitter and/or
in the proper environmental context. Though chemical systems have received the
most attention, neuroendocrine control of sexual signaling should apply equally to
nonchemical communicative modes.
Cockroaches provided some of the first evidence that hormones mediate the
onset of insect sexual behavior. About fifty years ago, Franz Engelmann reported
that when he allatectomized newly emerged females of Rhyparobia (=Leucophaea)
maderae, 70% of them never displayed sexual receptivity. When he reimplanted cor-
pora allata, normal receptivity ensued. However, when other researchers attempted
to duplicate Engelmann’s results, to their surprise their allatectomized females
became receptive and mated normally. To resolve the conflicting results, the
researchers repeated their experiments together, this time both using Engelmann’s
cockroaches. Nearly all the control roaches mated, usually within a few minutes
after the introduction of males. Allatectomy, on the other hand, once again led to
a high percentage of non-mated females, and those that did mate required over an
hour before males could induce them to copulate. The different experimental results
were due to differences in the two laboratory populations, underscoring both the
importance of heredity in behavioral studies and the dangers of generalizing from a
single strain of laboratory animal.
In many species of insects, females will no longer accept courtship advances after
successful mating. Female grasshoppers, for example, show secondary defensive
346 9 Reproductive Behavior
behavior rather than receptivity toward males once a spermatophore is placed in the
genital tract. For some species, such refractory behavior is temporary; in others it
lasts for life. A great variety of mechanical or chemical stimuli may be responsible.
In some cockroaches that respond to mechanical stimuli, inserting a glass bead into
the bursa of a virgin female will stimulate refractoriness. Other insects respond to
chemical cues such as juvenile hormone withdrawal or the release of chemicals from
the maturing ovaries. In many cases, secondary reproductive structures such as male
accessory glands or female bursae release agents that trigger the switch.
An interaction between mechanisms is probably common, as has been shown in
some Diptera; mating first results in a rapid neurally mediated refractory state, then
is followed by a more slowly developing but more persistent hormonally induced
response. As the bursa copulatrix of the yellow fever mosquito, Aedes aegypti, fills
with seminal fluid, an immediate (probably neural) switch-off of receptivity occurs.
However it is the action of a chemical (matrone) from the male accessory glands
passing through the bursa walls into the hemolymph that keeps females behaviorally
unreceptive to further mating for the rest of their lives. In fact, injecting matrone into
the haemocoel is an equally effective way to produce refractoriness.
The implicit assumption that ‘mated’ equals ‘inseminated’ should be carefully
evaluated in each case, because the two are different events. For example, although
new adult female Aedes aegypti mosquitoes appear to mate readily as soon as
they begin to fly, they actually are unable to accept the transfer of sperm for at
least 24 hours after they eclose. Certain tiny parasitic wasp females will mate only
once; thereafter they rebuff the overtures of the males that nevertheless persistently
attempt to court them; an occasional virgin female, having been intensely stimu-
lated by male courtship, reacts as though inseminated and thereafter behaves like a
mated female even though her actual physiological status is still virgin. Such exam-
ples underscore the importance of using insemination rather than copulation as the
criterion for a successful mating.
The ways in which insects reproduce vary in three important aspects: whether eggs
are fertilized, whether eggs are provisioned, and where embryonic development
takes place. The most common method of insect reproduction is by yolked eggs, fer-
tilized internally and laid outside the body. However, as one might expect in a taxon
as large as Insecta, other means are not uncommon, and some species approach the
task in quite unusual ways.
Take the matter of where embryonic development occurs. In Chapter 1 we con-
sidered oviparity, ovoviviparity, and viviparity among cockroaches, and it seemed
quite straightforward. Oviparous insects, by far the numerical majority, lay eggs.
Ovoviviparous insects do the same but retract their eggs back into a brood pouch
to develop, extruding them into the world again once they become nymphs. Going
a step further, viviparous insects ‘feed’ their developing young within the brood
pouch with nutrients and water.
9.2 Courtship and Mating 347
The overall picture is not quite this simple, however, because discrete terms can-
not adequately describe a continuum, and insect reproductive modes clearly form
a continuum. For example, several chrysomelid beetle species have embryos that
develop in the ovary so quickly that they hatch a few minutes after deposition.
Should they be considered oviparous or ovoviviparous? They do not differ essen-
tially from close relatives in which the embryos hatch within the genitalia just a
few minutes earlier. Furthermore, in addition to the reproductive modes mentioned
above, insects also show a wide variety of more unusual types of development.
In a smattering of insects (and quite regularly among endoparasitic ones) each
egg develops into a number of larvae rather than just one. This process, called
polyembryony, theoretically would have the effect of increasing the reproductive
potential of the insect involved, but because polyembryonic forms tend to lay fewer
eggs than related ‘normal’ species the actual net effect is not always much greater.
However, the metabolic costs are lower.
In other cases insect eggs develop without being fertilized, a situation known as
parthenogenesis. Because it has been recorded as an occasional event in nearly every
insect order, sporadic parthenogenesis is probably widespread when a female fails
to find a mate. Clearly the phenomenon should have short-term advantages. When
only female offspring are produced, a mother’s immediate reproductive potential is
much greater than if half the population is male, and a parthenogenetically reproduc-
ing female can spend all her time and energy in feeding and reproduction, without
having to locate a mate and/or court.
Then why is sexual reproduction the rule, rather than the exception? The classic
argument has been that over the long run asexual reproduction has an impor-
tant drawback—it lacks the genetic recombination that normally occurs at mating.
Sexual reproduction is favored because it allows selectively advantageous inter-
genomic recombination that promotes adaptation to and persistence in novel and
rapidly changing environments; it also purges weakly deleterious mutations from
the genome.
Debate arises, however, with certain predictions that arise from this cost/benefit
theory. One is that the transition from sexual to asexual reproduction should occur
frequently because of the short-term success of asexual lineages. The other is that
asexual lineages should suffer rapid extinction as the disadvantages play out. From a
phylogenetic perspective, this means that obligate asexual lineages should be poly-
phyletic and young. However, ancient asexual lineages clearly exist, and in a number
of species, parthenogenesis has become the normal method of reproduction.
This is where cyclically parthenogenetic taxa fit in. A life history of alternating
parthenogenetic and bisexual generations looks like an evolutionary compromise,
and a successful one at that, for over 15,000 species in three animal phyla have
been discovered to utilize it. Cyclical parthenogenesis suggests that there must be a
short-term advantage to maintaining a sexual generation. If not, such taxa would be
expected to rapidly move to obligate asexual reproduction.
In most of the cyclically parthenogenetic arthropods that are known, multiple
asexual generations are punctuated by occasional sexual generations, with male pro-
duction often triggered by a change in environmental conditions. Among Andricus
348 9 Reproductive Behavior
Fig. 9.3 Alternating diversity. Some cynipid wasp species produce galls on two alternate host
plants at different stages of the wasps’ life cycle; among other cynipids, asexual and sexual gener-
ations produce different galls on the same host plant. In some species of Andricus, a large genus of
oak gallwasps, the adults of the sexual generation are morphologically quite similar, and the galls
they produce are small and structurally simple. However, asexual generation adults are morpho-
logically distinct and produce large, complex, structurally diverse galls such as those shown here
for (A) A. glutinosus, (B) A. coriarius, and (C) A. pictus. Scale bars are 1 cm
oak and sycamore gall wasps (Fig. 9.3), however, sexual and asexual generations are
produced in strict alternation. However, the adults and galls of the sexual and sexual
generations are so different morphologically that historically they have often been
placed in different genera. Furthermore, whereas the galls of the asexual generation
are large, complex, and long-lived, those of the sexual generation are small, cryp-
tic, and short-lived. Thus, it is not surprising that the sexual generations have been
under-sampled and misidentified, and many fewer species are known only from a
sexual rather than an asexual generation.
New light was shed on the debate in 2008, when an international team of
researchers led by Graham Stone used molecular techniques based on analyses of
allele frequency data to test for cryptic sexual generations in eight apparently asex-
ual European species in the gall wasp genus Andricus, and found that cryptic sexual
generations were widespread. Their results led support to the side of the debate that
suggests either that selection acts against the loss of sex or that constraints make loss
of sex difficult. The research team also found four Andricus species that appeared to
reproduce only sexually; because no known oak gall wasps have such a life cycle,
these discoveries imply the existence of unknown asexual generations in a genus
that scientists had thought were quite well known.
Aphids provide another example of relatively well-known taxa that continue to
yield surprises. Some aphids lay eggs. Others give birth to their young (see Plate
30). Still others are some of the few cases among insects in which immature insects
are able to reproduce. Apparently as a result of a hormone imbalance, development
of the offspring usually begins while the parent is still a larva itself. Most insects that
9.2 Courtship and Mating 349
show such paedogenesis are also parthenogenetic and viviparous. When all of these
phenomena converge into a single life history, the situation can become incredibly
complex.
In tropical regions, most aphids are continuously parthenogenetic, but in tem-
perate zones they have developed a complex alternation of generations. Throughout
the summer, generations succeed one another so rapidly that while embryos are
developing in a mother’s egg tubes, they already have embryos developing in
them. Thus, the female aphid is not only an expectant mother but an expectant
grandmother! In autumn, males appear and normal sexual and oviposition behav-
ior are resumed. Eggs overwinter and, upon hatching the following spring, resume
rapid-fire parthenogenetic reproduction once again.
Overlaid upon this system there are usually also two other seasonal cycles: the
occurrence of several aphid morphs (physiologically and morphologically distinct
forms of the same species) and an alternation of host plants. For example, in Europe
members of the first spring generation of bean aphids, Aphis fabae (see Plate 10),
emerge from overwintering eggs on spindle trees, a species of Euonymus. This gen-
eration is entirely female and parthenogenetically produces either a winged female
generation or several wingless generations of females which themselves give rise
to winged forms. These migrate to bean plants, there to produce another series of
wingless morphs. Ultimately, these produce both winged and wingless sexual forms.
The winged aphids return to spindle trees and produce females; the wingless ones
go on to produce winged males that then join those females to mate and produce
winter eggs.
Two closely related sympatric species of Melittobia parasitoid wasps differ strik-
ingly in the occurrence and sequence of different elements of their courtship. In
M. australica, males pump the female’s antennae slowly maintaining continuous
contact and only periodically lift their middle legs; complete courtship requires an
average of ten minutes. Males of morphologically similar M. megachilis pump the
female’s antennae discontinuously in rapid alternation with an up-and-down move-
ment of the hind pair of legs; courtship duration for this species is slightly less
than two minutes. The two ecological homologues may parasitize the same host
in a single back yard, but the different courtship patterns will not permit them to
cross-mate.
As this example illustrates, insect courtship displays have at least three common
functions: to promote the meeting of solitary individuals, to facilitate species and
sex identification, and to stimulate and maneuver females into copulation. Among
predatory insects such as robber flies and scorpionflies, an important additional
function is appeasement, the inhibition of the normal predatory instincts of the
participants, especially the female.
The traditional view of insect courtship and mating has been that these behaviors
are relatively constant within a species and that, by differing from one species to
350 9 Reproductive Behavior
ACTIONS CONTEXT
Emergence from hosts
O
O
Sperm competition
Off
Search for hosts
Host-quality assessment
Fig. 9.4 Seeing the big picture. A schematic representation of the course of events in a typical
mating behavior sequence. Shaded and unshaded parts of the boxes denote the contributions of the
female and male, respectively
another, they serve as isolating mechanisms that effectively maintain and define
discrete species. An attractive analogy (Fig. 9.4) states that insect courtship operates
like a ratchet regulated by innate releasing mechanisms or physiological filters; at
each stage, some signal on the part of one participant elicits a response in the other.
This in turn elicits a new action by the initiator, and so on. Thus, the pair clicks along
toward copulation while the ratchet mechanism precludes inappropriate mating or
steps being performed out of sequence. Reproductive isolation is generally assumed
to be a principal function of such a reaction chain.
9.2 Courtship and Mating 351
Does courtship actually perform this function? To answer such a question, one
must turn to analytical analyses of individuals reared in isolation in order to exclude
learning or imprinting. As an example, consider the investigation in Case Study 9.1,
which detailed the courtship reaction chain in crane flies.
Crane flies generally look alike and tend to occupy the same sorts of habitats.
What courtship cues do conspecific males and females rely upon?
Crane flies, which look rather like delicate, fragile, oversized mosquitoes,
include a great number of species that are generally similar in appearance and
tend to occupy the same sorts of habitats. While the males mate repeatedly,
females typically mate but once. Under such conditions, a complex series of
reciprocal signals would be predicted; in what specific ways might this be
accomplished?
In the early 1960s, a Canadian researcher, H.J. Stich, raised crane flies
in the laboratory and kept them isolated from one another as adults. When
the adults were five days old, he began mating trials. The crane flies mated
readily, in a rapid series of actions and reactions that sometimes took as lit-
tle as 15 seconds and usually lasted only 90 seconds. Though the female
often began the courtship by touching a male’s long threadlike legs, it
was apparent that the male took the most active role in courtship as a
whole. True to the ratchet analogy, when Stich interrupted the courtship
pattern by removing the female, reintroducing her never allowed the pat-
tern to resume. Each time, the males began courtship anew from the
beginning.
The first detectable step of the crane fly courtship occurs when two individ-
uals happen to contact each other’s legs. A male’s response was to grab the leg
with his own. Stich began amputating legs before introducing live females to
males; six, five, four, three, two, or one leg—it made no difference to sexually
active males. Only when a female had no legs at all did males fail to respond.
Next, Stich picked up amputated legs in his tweezers and offered them.
Males immediately grabbed them. Was any discrimination occurring at this
step? Apparently not in relation to sex, because dead males or their legs
evoked the same grabbing reaction as females did. In fact, any leg-like tac-
tile stimulus seemed sufficient—males even readily grabbed paraffin-stiffened
threads dangled near them. But when stiffened threads of different thicknesses
were presented, those of the ‘wrong’ diameter were immediately released.
Stich repeated the experiment again, this time using legs of different diame-
ters from other species of Tipula. Male T. oleracea grabbed at them all but
immediately let them go. Here was Stich’s clue to an initial discrimination
that probably helped to ensure that the potential mate belongs to the proper
species.
352 9 Reproductive Behavior
Stich observed that receptive females respond to the male’s grasp by lift-
ing one or more of their legs upward, and that afterward, the male assumed a
mounting position above the female’s body. What exactly was the female sig-
naling with this behavior? To answer this question, Stich constructed a number
of models. He found that males refused to mount the model made from a dead
female with artificially stiffened legs, but they would mount a variety of mov-
able models, even a simple 1 mm long wire if it were fashioned into a pair
of movable legs. So, if to a male moving legs signify a female, what signifies
another male? Stich repeatedly caged pairs of males together and watched
their behavior. Males grabbed each other whenever their legs touched, but in
over 100 trials neither of them ever responded by upward leg movement nor
did they ever exhibit a mounting reaction.
After male leg-grabbing and female leg-lifting, a male crane fly tries to
pin down the female’s raised legs. Sometimes, this is accomplished in less
than 3 seconds; at other times, the female may resist, and the ensuing struggle
over leg position can extend over a period of 3 minutes. On the hunch that
this great variability might be associated with sexual receptivity, Stich pre-
sented males with previously mated females; these readily elicited the males’
grabbing and mounting reactions, but all their attempts at pinning down the
females’ legs failed. Sexually unreceptive females kept at least one leg contin-
ually raised, eventually causing the male to leave. Thus, previously mated or
otherwise unreceptive females effectively terminate courtship attempts at this
point.
After successfully pinning a female so that she remains motionless,
a male begins to locate her head, by contacting her with his mouth-
parts while he moves progressively forward over her body. When he
reaches her head, he touches his mouthparts to the back of it and stops
his search. To determine the stimuli for this particular element in the
courtship sequence (which Stich anthropomorphically called ‘kissing’),
the researcher exposed males to variously altered females (see Fig. 9.5).
Male reactions to these models clearly indicated that both the female’s
body and her head were necessary as a stimulus at this point in the
courtship.
Fig. 9.5 Searching reactions. Male crane fly behavior toward experimentally altered females. (a)
Decapitated female: searching extends beyond the body until male contacts substrate. (b) Female
head glued onto paper strip: male uses strip as guide to reach head, but percent completion declines
as strip length increases. (c) Female head attached to end of abdomen, model presented so that male
and female heads point in same direction: male orients toward head, attempts copulation through
neck cavity
9.2 Courtship and Mating 353
The final step in the courtship sequence occurs when the male slowly steps
backward, sliding his abdomen back over the female’s, and bends his abdomen
in such a way that the male genitalia come into contact with the female’s. Does
the female head or the ‘kissing reaction’ release this behavior? Or does the
abdomen function as the sign stimulus? To answer, Stich removed the heads
from females and attached them in various ways. Male responses to reversed-
head females showed clearly that the head, not the abdomen, was the sign
stimulus.
Thus, Stich was able to show that crane fly courtship facilitates dis-
crimination with respect to sex, species, and physiological condition of the
participants. At each stage of their courtship, different behaviors and differ-
ent body parts are employed as specific signaling devices in a touch-oriented
chain ideally suited for such weak-flying, drab denizens of the woods, and
courtship proceeds like a rigid sequence of filters excluding all but conspecific
sexually active males and receptive unfertilized females.
As this study illustrates, the use of experimental models can be particularly help-
ful in teasing apart the roles of individual behaviors in the complex interactive chains
that characterize many cases of insect courtship.
Approximately 65% of all flowering plant species are known to be insect pollinated.
As a group, insect-pollinated plants generally have relatively showy and conspic-
uous flowers (which may however appear quite different to an insect’s eye than to
our own; see Plate 24). They produce pollen over a period of time and characteris-
tically produce nectar as well. In contrast, wind-pollinated plants have rather small
and inconspicuous flowers, generally lack nectar, and produce copious amounts of
pollen in concentrated flushes. Corn is a familiar example; most grasses and their
relatives are pollinated by wind, a not surprising fact since grassland habitats tend
to be relatively windy and open. An exception to this rule, certain bamboo-like
grasses that grow deep inside tropical forests where wind circulation is nil have
secondarily evolved insect pollination systems, an example that serves to reempha-
size the importance of ecology and especially population and community structure
in understanding insect–plant coevolution.
Pollination is often closely interwoven with other aspects of insect behavior, and
coevolved adaptations are common. One particularly striking case, the fig wasp, was
examined in Chapter 4. A few other examples will illustrate some of the variety that
occurs in the interrelationships between pollination and reproductive behavior.
Some of the strangest cases of insect pollination involve orchids. One of the
two largest families of flowering plants, the Orchidaceae includes some 880 genera;
there are said to be over four times as many orchid species as mammal species
on this earth. Although most are found in the tropics of Asia, South America and
9.2 Courtship and Mating 355
Central America, orchids are cosmopolitan, and occur in almost every habitat but
deserts and glaciers.
While some orchids are self-pollinated and a few reproduce vegetatively, most
require cross-pollination. However, orchids have developed highly specialized pol-
lination systems and the chances of being pollinated are often scarce. In addition,
orchid seeds are generally almost microscopic and very numerous, in some species
over a million per capsule. After ripening they blow off like dust particles or spores.
They lack endosperm and must enter symbiotic relationship with various mycor-
rhizal fungi that provide them the necessary nutrients to germinate, so that all orchid
species are reliant upon fungi to complete their lifecycle.
In response to such long odds, orchid flowers usually remain receptive for very
long periods and most orchids deliver pollen in a waxy mass called a pollinium
(plural, pollinia) Each pollinium is connected to a stalk-like filament that ends in an
adhesive pad (viscidium) that sticks to the body of pollinators; taken together, the
entire apparatus is called a pollinarium (plural, pollinaria). In functional terms, this
means that the whole pollen content of a flower is removed during a single pollinator
visit and each time that pollination succeeds, a great many ovules can be fertilized,
making pollination a high-stakes game.
In orchids that produce pollinia, pollination happens as some variant of the fol-
lowing. When the pollinator enters into the flower, it touches a viscidium, which
promptly sticks to its body, generally on the head or abdomen. While leaving the
flower, it pulls the pollinium out of the anther, and the springy stalk bends, mov-
ing the pollinium forward and downward into a (generally orchid species-specific)
position such that when the pollinator enters another flower of the same species the
pollinium will stick to the stigma of the second flower, pollinating it (Fig. 9.6). Bees,
wasps, flies, ants, and moths are all exploited for orchid pollination in this manner.
Orchids go to great lengths to attract pollinators, guide them into the proper
positions, and assure that pollinia attach to an insect body location where they
will successfully be removed during the insect’s next visit to their particular orchid
species. The strangely contorted flower shapes that humans find so interesting are
actually adaptations that guide insect visitors in ways that promote pollination.
Sometimes, such promotion can grade into coercion. The bag-shaped bottom petal
of one group of orchids traps visiting insects; the only exit leads to the anthers
that deposit pollen on the visitor. Catasetum, a genus discussed briefly by Darwin,
actually launches its viscid pollinia with explosive force when an insect touches it.
For nearly 700 specialized orchids in the Neotropics, the pollinarium system
is carried even further. These flowers lack nectar, the usual reward for bees that
visit flowers, but produce such strong species-specific fragrances that they are often
called ‘perfume orchids.’ A bee attracted by the odor approaches a blossom from
downwind and upon landing, alternately brushes the surface of the basal flower
petal with his front legs, then hovers in front of the flower while scrubbing his legs
together as if to transfer some substance to his hind tibiae. Individual bees may stay
with a given blossom for up to 90 minutes, with repeated bouts of brushing and
hovering–transferring. Toward the end of prolonged visits the bee becomes much
less wary and its overall behavior suggests nothing less than increasing intoxication!
356 9 Reproductive Behavior
Fig. 9.6 Sticking to carriers. Outlines of Neotropical male euglossine bees showing the pollinia
of 11 different species of orchid flowers, each deposited in a precise location on the bee’s body.
Upon entering a flower, the bee either squeezes down a narrow pathway and brushes against the
sticky viscidium or triggers a mechanism that causes the pollinia to be ejected (sometimes with
rather great force) onto its body. On a visit to another conspecific flower, the same sequence occurs,
except that as the pollinator leaves, the attached pollinia are forced into the sticky stigmatic cavity
on the flower and are removed before the insect receives its new pollinia
bees suggests that the orchid bee/perfume orchid mutualism may be facultative for
the bees, even though it is obligatory for the orchids.
As in other orchids, orchid bees collecting volatiles pick up pollinia specifically
placed to ensure that cross-pollination only occurs between orchids of the same
species, but for the orchid an added dimension of specificity has been obtained
because each orchid species has a characteristic fragrance spectrum, and different
orchid bee males are attracted to different chemicals. This can be particularly advan-
tageous in the tropics, where plants generally do not grow in groups, and individual
plants of the same species may be miles apart. Orchid bees are believed to forage
on specific plants along set routes, a behavior known as trap-lining.
Over 60 distinct attractive chemicals have been isolated; one of the most com-
mon is cineole (Fig. 9.7). When tested alone under field conditions, cineole attracted
35 of 57 different species of male euglossine bees native to the area. But when one
or more of the other compounds was combined with the cineole, the number of
attracted species declined dramatically. Taking advantage of the specificity of this
mutualism, scientists commonly use single synthetic compounds as bait to attract
and collect euglossine males for study. The attractant compounds include many
familiar flavorings and odors considered appealing to humans (such as methyl sali-
cylate, eugenol, benzyl acetate, and methyl cinnamate), and others (such as skatole)
that are not. The euglossine male Eufriesea purpurata is highly unusual in actively
collecting the insecticide DDT in huge amounts from houses in Brazil, without
suffering any harm from it.
Whereas perfume orchids simply offer chemical attractants, other orchids appear
to offer the promise of sex—they have evolved to resemble female versions of cer-
tain insects. This sexual deception strategy is practiced by various Australian orchids
and dainty Mediterranean orchids in the genus Ophrys. Known since the days of
Pliny the Elder (23–79 A.D.), Ophrys flowers are commonly called bee orchids
Fig. 9.7 Smelling attractive. A male euglossine bee attracted to cineole daubed on the label of
this commercial chemical
358 9 Reproductive Behavior
because even to human eyes the flowers of some species resemble the furry bodies
of bees and other insects. Like the orchids above, these do not secrete nectar, nor
is their pollen available to most insects. Female aculeate Hymenoptera are never
seen visiting these flowers, but males of certain species of wasps, bees, and beetles
approach the flowers, their mating instinct stimulated by chemicals exuded from
the orchid’s basal petals. At close range, the flower petals mimic female insects,
attracting and duping the right pollinator. As they shift about, attempting to copu-
late (Fig. 9.8), the ardent males loosen the pollinia, which then stick to their bodies
in positions specific to the orchid species involved.
Fig. 9.8 Tricking pollinators. Flowers of the orchid, Ophrys, trigger copulatory behavior by
males in a number of unrelated genera of aculeate wasps (a, b) and bees (c, d). After a chemically
mediated attraction, vital tactile stimulation is provided by the form and construction of the orchid
labellum and by characteristics of the hairs upon it, including their direction, length and grouping.
Each of the approximately 30 Ophrys species depends upon different Hymenoptera species for
pollination accomplished by entering the flower frontwards or backwards, depending on the insect
species: (a) Campsoscolia ciliata on O. speculum, (b) Argogorytes mystaceus on O. insectifera,
(c) Colletes cunicularius on O. “arachnitiformis-sphecodes” of sphecodes type, (d) Andrena sp.
on O. fusca
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 359
site. When the pupal cuticle begins to split, a frantic and violent scuffle breaks out
between the males as each attempts to establish possession. The victor monopolizes
the emerging female by standing over her and holding her in his legs; mating is
established by the time she is free from the cuticle.
When the product of an interaction has the potential to yield different optima for
different individuals or classes of individuals, conflict is the usual result, because
each party in the interaction will be selected to manipulate the transaction in
ways that move the result closer to its own optimum. Generally, these manip-
ulations take one of three forms: persuasion, coercion, or force. In persuasion,
one individual offers a fitness incentive that would increase the benefits provided
to the other party. In coercion, one party (generally the one furthest from its
optimum) attempts to gain control through actions that exert a cost to the other
party. Force, of course, involves taking control away from the other individual
by physical, behavioral or physiological means. The processes are not mutually
exclusive, of course, and individuals often switch from one approach to another
through time.
Sex would seem to be the ultimate social act. Yet, as Edward O. Wilson has
pointed out in the context of social insects, in many ways sex can quite prop-
erly be considered an antisocial force in evolution. The ‘ideal’ society would lack
conflict and would have a high degree of selfless behaviors and coordination, a
situation most likely to evolve where all members of the group are genetically iden-
tical. Within the invertebrates, the highest forms of sociality are found in groups
that create new colony members by budding—the sponges, tunicates, etc. Sexual
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 361
Male and female insects sometimes differ so dramatically that they look like two
different species; in some aculeate wasps, in fact, matching has been possible only
when males and females have been discovered in copula. Other males are armored
(Fig. 9.10). Males of Hawaiian Drosophila, euglossine bees, Asian fireflies, and cer-
tain dragonflies repeatedly gather at specific sites where they display and compete
for the attention of females.
Why do males and females differ in their appearance? Why are males so often
the more elaborate sex? Why, for that matter, do individuals differ in appearance?
And why do members of the opposite sex care? Dimorphism may be driven by
362 9 Reproductive Behavior
Fig. 9.10 Wearing weaponry. An 1869 illustration by Émile Blanchard, a prominent early
French zoologist and entomologist, shows sexual dimorphism and the life cycle of a stag beetle
Lucanus cervus, a well known species across much of Europe. Male stag beetles use their stout
bodies and elaborate jaws to wrestle with each other for favored mating sites
The sex that courts, ordinarily the male, plans to invest less reproductive effort in the
offspring. What it offers to the female is chiefly evidence that it is fully normal and physio-
logically fit. But this warranty consists of only a brief performance, so that strong selective
pressures exist for less fit individuals to present a false image. The courted sex, usually the
female, will therefore find it strongly advantageous to distinguish the really fit from the
pretended fit. Consequently, there will be a strong tendency for the courted sex to develop
coyness. That is, its responses will be hesitant and cautious in a way that evokes still more
displays and makes correct discrimination easier.
Fig. 9.11 Choosing a male. Types of mate choice open to females. (a) No choice, female accepts
any available conspecific mate. (b) Female accepts most valuable mate. (c) Female accepts any
victor of a competition among available mates. (d) Female chooses on basis of comparison against
some absolute standard. (e) Female selects an extreme phenotype, simultaneously rejecting other
equally available but less flamboyant males. (f) Female chooses among territories or position
in a lek
At first glance, Darwin’s sexual selection theory might seem a simple answer to
a simple set of questions, but its implications are far-reaching. The subtle ways in
which mate choices can influence male and female fitness are only beginning to be
more fully appreciated.
Explaining how female mating preferences have evolved has been relatively
straightforward in those situations in which a female’s mate choice clearly influ-
ences her immediate reproductive success (fecundity). For example, in mating
364 9 Reproductive Behavior
aggregations based on real estate, males generally (but not always) defend ter-
ritories, wait for arriving females, and perform courtship prior to mating. Many
produce long-range signals to attract females. In these resource-based mating sys-
tems, direct selection has always been considered to have an important influence on
mate preferences.
In other cases, the logic of female mate choice has been less apparent. Non-
resourced based leks are a prime example. A lek is a gathering of males for the
purposes of competitive mating display. In lek or lek-like mating systems, males’
only contribution to females is their gametes; they are not involved in parenting, and
if they have territories, they do not regulate a female’s access to any resources that
the territories may contain. Most matings occur in a mating arena, where females
freely select a mate. Females have a wide choice of males with which to mate, and
clearly prefer some to others. (Trivia-lovers note: A lek when females come together
to compete for males is called a mung; this role reversal is rare, but occurs in the
yellow-spotted millipede.)
Classical lek species were first identified in birds and mammals, and most lek
research still focuses upon vertebrates. At first, it seemed that species could be
strictly classified as to whether they had leks or not, but as more and more diver-
sity has been catalogued, it has become obvious that lek-like behaviors should be
considered as continuous variables that can differ independently with ecological
conditions, taxonomic affiliation, and other factors. This consideration is particu-
larly worth keeping in mind when confronted by the immense diversity in a group
as large as the Insecta. In addition, some researchers consider leks to include mating
swarms, whereas others see leks to include only substrate-based systems. Certainly,
swarms occur in many non-mating contexts, and even mating swarms differ from
substrate-based systems.
The so-called paradox of the lek has posed particular problems for evolutionists.
Because females seem to gain no material benefits or parental care from males, it
appears as though direct selection would have little relevance here. Furthermore,
one would expect that persistent female choice for particular male traits such as
elaborate ornamentation should erode genetic variation in them and thereby remove
the benefits of choice, Paradoxically, both variation and choice persist.
A number of competing theories have been proposed to settle the paradox. Some
emphasize male roles; others, female roles. The notion that females prefer clus-
tered males because clustering facilitates mate choice is not consistently supported
by field observations. In general, field and laboratory studies have failed to conclu-
sively support one theory over another. The paradox might be somewhat alleviated
by the occurrence of mutations introducing potential differences, as well as the pos-
sibility that traits of interest have more or less favorable recessive alleles. Moreover,
if leks are composed of closely related males and larger leks attract more females,
the paradox might be avoided because lower-ranking males could pass on their
genes indirectly by helping attract mates for their brothers. Heated debate arose over
two explanatory hypotheses: coevolution (and its famous extreme, runaway sexual
selection) and good genes.
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 365
The first of these hypotheses rests on the understanding that lack of direct selec-
tion on a character does not exclude that character from evolving; indirect selection
can occur on traits if they are correlated with traits under direct selection. In 1930,
R. A. Fisher presented a brief discussion of runaway sexual selection, which was
subsequently elaborated by others. The model postulated a sort of ‘self-reinforcing
choice’ in which an initial assortative mating generates a statistical association
between a trait and a preference that are both found in both sexes but only expressed
in the appropriate one. When this genetic correlation is high relative to the heri-
tability of the male ornamentation, as male traits evolve under the opposing forces
of natural and sexual selection a correlated evolution of the female preference
results. As a result, the trait and the preferences are exaggerated indefinitely as
they continue to evolve. If the genetic correlation is somewhat lower relative to
the heritability of the male ornamentation, the trait and preference will increase
only until viability selection against further trait elaboration balances sexual
selection.
Runaway sexual selection has been difficult to show empirically. Thus, it is
often relegated to consideration as a null hypothesis when other explanations can-
not be demonstrated. Evolutionary biologists note that the runaway process may be
so rapid as to be unlikely to be observed and it can be difficult or unfeasible to
conduct the experiments necessary to validate runaway. However, cases of genetic
correlation between traits and preferences are beginning to accumulate, as is evi-
dence showing that evolution of a trait can result in an evolutionary response in the
preference.
A particularly clear example has been shown in studies of stalk-eyed flies (see
Plate 31). When considered together, Wilkinson and Reillo’s results, outlined in
Case Study 9.2, provide very good support for a coevolutionary model of female
choice sexual selection. Female mate preference changed as a consequence of arti-
ficial selection on male relative eye span. Because females were caged separately
from males and selection was not exerted on females, the changes in female prefer-
ence appeared to result from a genetic correlation between male eye span and female
preference. However, Wilkinson and Reillo themselves admitted, their results did
not exclude another competing model for the evolution of male ornamentation—the
‘good genes’ hypothesis.
as handicaps that signal increased viability. Still another holds that preexisting
sensory biases for conspecific recognition lead to trait exaggeration. Gerald S.
Wilkinson and Paul R. Reillo decided to test these models with a Malaysian
population of a little fly that has impressive male ornamentation–stalked eyes
separated by spans that often exceed a male’s body length (see Plate 31).
Several aspects of the flies’ life history suggested they might prove espe-
cially suitable for such study. Male eye spans are extremely variable, and
such variation has been shown to be heritable. Previous field observations on
Malaysian diopsids by other researchers suggested that eye span might influ-
ence mating success. During the day, the flies foraged alone, grazing on rotting
vegetative matter. However, each evening, they moved to streams and formed
aggregations on root hairs underneath overhanging stream banks. Each group
consists of a single male and several females, with males attempting to dis-
place one another from the root hairs as the aggregations formed. Males assess
the body size of competitors by their head projection, and males of similar size
engage in extended physical contests. Being the sole male in a roosting group
was important, because mating occurred at dusk or dawn in the aggregations.
A few years earlier, other researchers had published studies on a related
species showing that the male with the longest eye span usually won alterca-
tions between males, and that females preferred to alight on strings containing
model males with the longest eye span. This seemed to be a classic lek
situation—how, if at all, might a choice based on such a seemingly arbitrary
choice as eye span influence female survival or fecundity?
Wilkinson and Reillo approached the question quite systematically. In
January and October 1989, they counted male and female flies on root hairs
along a Malaysian stream bank. To quantify sexual selection, they collected
roosting flies at night, enclosing all flies on a root hair with a nylon stock-
ing. Flies were frozen in liquid nitrogen in the field and kept frozen until they
could be processed.
Analysis of their field data confirmed that females clustered, whereas
males dispersed beyond random expectation. But were the females sim-
ply returning to the same microhabitats night after night, irrespective
of the males that might be there? If so, the same sites should consis-
tently contain more females. Statistical analysis said no; female aggre-
gations did not appear to be determined by microhabitat choice alone.
In the field-collected aggregations the number of females per site varied
but the average number of females per male clearly increased with male
eye span.
With this background, the researchers decided to observe flies more
closely. They placed the insects in plastic mouse cages, fed them with ground
corn twice a week, and hung strings in their cages as substitute root hairs. The
flies lived well in captivity; groups of adults of both sexes could be sampled at
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 367
regular intervals up to 191 days old. Males could also be marked on the thorax
with typewriter correction fluid to identify individuals.
Would the number of females per male in a caged aggregation estimate
mating success? The researchers placed two males with different eye spans
and body lengths into each of 24 mouse cages containing an average of five
females. Then they counted and timed all the copulations for thirty minutes
after dawn on two successive days. Each morning within an aggregation, most
females mated repeatedly, but the larger male obtained 57% of the copula-
tions. In the field, they speculated, the skew in his favor would be even greater
because in cages females could not evade males and large males could not
exclude small males.
But were females really reacting to eye span itself, or to greater body size?
Wilkinson and Reillo scored frozen flies for eye span, body length, age, and if
female, fecundity. They measured eye span and body length with a video dig-
itizing system, and dissected each female to determine the number of mature
oocytes within her abdomen. Following procedures established by others, they
estimated fly ages from the amount of pteridine pigments in their eyes. Later,
this information would allow them to examine the body size/eye span relation-
ship and also female survival, fecundity, and other potential costs of female
choices.
What would happen if they could directly compare female reactions to
long and short eye-spanned males of the same body length? Measuring
the ratio of eye span to body length in their population, the scien-
tists selected ten males with the longest relative eye span and another
ten with the shortest relative eye span. They mated each with 25 ran-
domly chosen females. For thirteen generations, the scientists selected
for long and short male eye span distances in this way. At the end of
this time, they had two populations of males whose eye spans averaged
a difference of 1.57–1.47 mm, and whose body lengths averaged from
0.05 to 0.01 mm.
To quantify mate choice in the presence and absence of male interactions,
Wilkinson and Reillo decided to run four experiments. To make sure that
past association with males would not influence mate preferences, the females
from each selected line were caged together after eclosion without males until
tested two to three months later.
For the first set, groups of five females were placed into a cage with two
hanging strings and two selected line males. After two days of acclimation,
the number of females with each male before daylight was counted. In 24 of
31 cages, more unselected females roosted with wide-eyed males, suggesting
that male competition, female choice, or both occurred.
For the next three groups of experiments, male competition was excluded.
Clear partitions set up across the cages allowed the flies to see one another
368 9 Reproductive Behavior
but the partitions were perforated with holes that were just large enough
to allow females to move between the cage sides without allowing males
through. (Food was available on both sides of the partition.) For experiment
two, they used unselected females; as expected, the females preferred wide-
eyed males. For experiment three, they used wide-eyed females; these also
preferred wide-eyed male, but not to a statistically greater degree than in
experiment two. This was surprising; if genetic correlation exists between
male eye span and female preference, then selection for increased eye span
should produce more choosey females. Reviewing possible explanations, the
scientists decided it was quite possible that there may have been a difference
that went undetected because suitable test males with extra-long eye spans
were not provided. Supporting this idea were mate choice experiments with a
related species in which females preferred model males with longer eye span
than exist in natural populations. For experiment four, the researchers chose
females with short relative eye spans; these preferred not long- but close-eyed
males. In this way, they demonstrated a genetic correlation between female
preference and a sexually selected male trait.
Might it be more costly for females to select large eye span males than to
mate at random? If so, a negative correlation should exist between the relative
eye span of the largest male in an aggregation and either female age or fecun-
dity. In their laboratory analyses, Wilkinson and Reillo found instead that eye
span of the largest male in a field aggregation correlated positively with female
age, as estimated by amount of eye pigment. Thus, it appears that mate choice
did not appear to impair female survival, but actually improved female sur-
vival. However, because other explanations for this correlation are possible,
this conclusion must be approached with caution. One might also postulate
that a longer life could be expected to yield more offspring, but the data fell
short of demonstrating this advantage; the correlation between eye-span ratio
of the largest male and female egg count was not statistically significant.
controversy, but has come to gain much wider support since it has been shown that
female preferences can indeed evolve if they are genetically correlated to the ‘good
gene,’ which is taken to mean some heritable component for viability.
Evidence for the good genes hypothesis is lacking, but a number of studies pur-
port links between female choices, offspring viability, and paternal genetic effects
in a range of other animal species. Still it is important to recognize that costly dis-
plays by themselves cannot automatically be taken as evidence for female choice
for good genes. Because mate choice involves communication, it always involves
some sort of costs. The purpose of any signal is to increase the conspicuousness of
the signaler against background noise; it can easily become an unintended adver-
tisement to predators and parasites. There are physiological costs as well, either
in the growth of a structure used as a signal or in the behavior of displaying the
signal.
Increasingly, sexual selection studies are moving from antagonism between two
competing theories to consideration of a much broader range of choices. In some
cases, there is evidence that pre-existing sensory biases for conspecific recognition
lead to trait exaggeration. Direct selection on preferences and pleiotropic effects
can influence preference evolution. Furthermore, mate choice undoubtedly involves
female attention to a whole suite of sexually dimorphic traits that are under sex-
ual selection. Historical approaches have introduced a new dimension, as have the
modern phylogenetic methods.
Direct selection, runaway selection, selection for good genes, and such are often
portrayed as though they were mutually exclusive hypotheses, but in fact they may
represent something more akin to the ancient tale of the blind men’s encounter with
an elephant, with each describing a different aspect of the same reality. Thus, former
contentiousness between the holders of mutually exclusive hypotheses seems to be
yielding to a new emphasis on the relative strengths and influences of these different
forces when they act simultaneously.
Unlike vertebrates, female insects can store viable sperm from a single insemina-
tion for use during the remainder of their reproductive lives (several years in the case
of some social species). At the junction where the female’s pouch-like spermatheca
opens to the genital tract, a sphinctor muscle parcels out sperm as eggs are laid.
Scientists theorize that in cases where sperm are deposited in packets and maintain
their cohesion during storage, they presumably would be used in the reverse order
from which they were obtained. This ‘last in, first out’ principle, called sperm prece-
dence, has become a widely accepted tenet of sexual selection theory, and there is
evidence for it in many insect species. Its importance lies in the fact that it sets the
stage for continued conflict because if the sperm of the last male to mate are the first
to be used in egg fertilization, a late-arriving male would still profit by attempting
to mate with an inseminated female. A second strategy is sperm removal. Odonata
provide some of the best examples. The male of a pair of copulating Calopteryx
damselflies (see Plate 33) spends most of his copulation time physically extracting
sperm of other males from the female’s spermatheca using his relatively elaborate
penis containing recurved spines, hooks, and a scraper to accomplish this task.
Obviously, it is not to a male’s advantage to have his own sperm subsequently
replaced. How can he ensure that his sperm will be the most likely to fertilize the
female’s eggs? One way is simply to monopolize the female so that other males do
not have access to her. Such a situation occurs in Scatophaga stercoraria dung flies.
Continuing to maintain a territory has a similar effect, as does merely staying in
physical contact with one’s partner without actual genital contact; the tandem flight
of many Odonata is a familiar example (see Fig. 9.2). So does prolonged copulation,
which occurs widely among insects such as the lovebug (see Fig. 9.1), walking
sticks, the queen butterfly, and the burnet moth (Fig. 9.12). Still another widespread
device is the mating plug, formed by coagulation of male accessory gland secretions
in the female’s genital tract; such plugs prevent sperm leakage and/or physically
prevent subsequent mating with other males.
Fig. 9.12 Prolonging copulation. The day flying burnet moth, Zygaena filipendulae, is
widespread in Europe, but the majority of zygaenids are tropical. The moths have red spots on
black, a warning to predators that they are distasteful; burnet moths contain hydrogen cyanide
throughout all stages of their life cycle
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 371
After members of the courting sex have aggressively excluded a portion of the
competition, classical intrasexual selection theory states that the more passive sex
simply chooses a potential mate from among the elite group of winners or the single
winner. In the process of acquiring a vigorous partner, he or she often also acquires
something else of great value—a share in his or her set of resources. How members
of the more actively courting sex partition these resources among themselves brings
us to the twin topics of territoriality and dominance.
Male carpenter bees vigorously chase a variety of small objects tossed near them.
Male dragonflies drive off other conspecific males. Males of the ectoparasitoid
wasp Nasonia defend host fly puparia from which females are starting to emerge.
Mated females of the ectoparasitoid Melittobia megachilis aggressively fight other
females that they encounter upon their host, ultimately partitioning the host into
microterritories that are actively defended by amputating the legs and antennae
of rivals.
The phenomenon of territoriality—broadly, any space-associated intolerance
of others and, more narrowly, an intolerance based on real estate holdings—has
long been well known in vertebrates, often in association with aggressive behavior.
Among insects it has received less emphasis, but it is a surprisingly common and
widespread occurrence.
Territoriality is almost always associated with competition for mates or food. Its
function is to partition priority of access to resources that are limited and tied to a
particular area. Territoriality is not always associated with overt aggression; some
types of space exclusion can be settled solely through display. Nor does it even
always require the owner’s presence; some territorial insects such as female apple
maggot flies depend upon persistent chemical marks that have a repellent effect on
conspecifics.
Because territorial activities incur an expense of energy, selection for territorial
behaviors tends to operate only under a limited set of conditions. One condition
is that the desired but limited resource should be relatively localized in a readily
defensible situation. Another is that animals should stand to obtain more of the
resource by defending the area against competing individuals than by searching for
new resources to exploit. For example, males of the cicada killer wasp, Sphecious
speciosus, establish themselves on perches that overlook highly clumped nesting
areas containing emerging females; investment in defense of these perches may be
rewarded by extra copulation opportunities for the territory owner. Dragonflies and
damselflies establish similar perches along the shores of ponds and streams (see
Plate 33).
Territorial fights among vertebrate conspecifics are rarely fatal; when underlings
assume various submissive postures, the combatants slow or halt their battle. In
insects, in contrast, murder and even cannibalism may be routine phenomena. One
of the more striking examples of aggression occurs among males in Melittobia, a
372 9 Reproductive Behavior
absorbed nutritive material to meet a mate’s caloric needs for a full two days. Males
of the beetle Neopyrochroa flabellataduce ingest toxic cantharadin while feeding;
cantharadin transfer during mating has been shown to enable a female to use the
chemical to protect her eggs from predation.
As examples like this make apparent, during courtship the ‘salesmanship’ of
many male insects includes significant gift-giving, and their potential mates seem
quite open to such bribery. Nuptial gifts—male material donations transferred to a
conspecific female during mating—are delivered by two basic routes. One route
includes oral gifts that are donated by a male and ingested by the female dur-
ing mating (Fig. 9.14; see also Fig. 6.9 and Plate 29). The other type comprises
seminal gifts that are ejaculated into the female reproductive tract and absorbed
from there. Either way, the gifts may include nutrients and/or defensive chemi-
cals. These gifts are obtained from sources as diverse as leftover prey, carrion,
purposively collected foodstuffs, and secretions from male salivary or reproductive
glands.
The published literature holds many examples of nuptial gifts. When the imma-
tures of many insects feed, they accumulate various toxins, particularly alkaloids,
in their tissues that subsequently carry on to adulthood; in other cases, males gar-
ner the toxins as adults. In various species it has been shown that males that offer
toxin-bearing secretions either strengthen the female’s toxic protection to her eggs,
replace toxins she may have depleted during previous oviposition, or protect the
female herself.
The moth family Arctiidae seems particularly prone to host-derived chemical
defenses. The bella moth, Utetheisa ornatrix, is the most thoroughly studied in this
regard (Fig. 9.15), thanks to perceptive studies by Thomas and Maria Eisner and
Fig. 9.14 Courting with an oral gift. Nuptial feeding in a scorpionfly, Bittacus apicalis. Through
this habit, widespread in the Bittacidae, females gain increased reproductive success as measured
by increased oviposition
374 9 Reproductive Behavior
Fig. 9.15 Passing along alkaloids. The rattlebox moth, Utetheisa ornatrix, (left) feeds on the
toxic legume Crotalaria spectabilis (right) as a larva. Unaffected by the plant’s pyrrolizidine alka-
loids, the larva stores these toxic chemicals and carries them along into adulthood. Thus, females
are able to pass the alkaloids to their eggs; they also obtain an additional dose of the alkaloids from
males as a seminal gift during each copulation
their associates. This day-flying moth gains its alkaloid from its specialized larval
diet of rattlebox, Crotalaria, a legume that contains toxic pyrrolidizine alkaloids
(PA). The chemicals are stored systemically, retained into adulthood, and passed on
to eggs, thus providing all life stages of this moth with protection against spiders,
beetles and wasps. Moths of both sexes actively transmit PA to the next generation.
Females do it directly, in the process of laying hundreds of eggs. As a result, their
PA levels can drop over time. However, they can obtain more PA by mating, because
males gift them with a sizeable sperm package. Sometimes amounting to over 10%
of the male’s body mass, it contains both PA and nutrients.
Another example is the arctiid moth Cosmosoma myrodora (Case Study 9.3).
Males in this species produce fine cuticular filaments in abdominal pouches, and
discharge them in bursts during courtship (see Plate 34). If males feed upon plants
that produce PA, the filaments are laden with these alkaloids; a female laden with
these toxic filaments is protected against predators such as Nephila clavipes, a com-
mon spider, and she passes some of this protection on to her eggs. Males deprived
of alkaloids produce filaments that confer no such protection.
During courtship, a male of the scarlet-bodied wasp moth envelops its future
mate in a cloud of gauzy filaments that cling around her body. What is the
value of such a strange behavior?
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 375
Small Cosmosoma moths (see Plate 34) are not particularly abundant in
Florida, but their aposematic coloration and strange habits caught the atten-
tion of William E. Conner, Ruth Boada and their associates. For her master’s
thesis, Boada studied the courtship of these moths. Like many other moths, a
male C. myrodoa is attracted to a pheromone emitted by the female. However,
once he nears the female, he does not alight. Instead he flutters nearby, then
suddenly discharges bursts of fine filaments that swirl in the air around the
female like an explosively delivered net. These filaments cling all over her
body as the pair copulate, a lengthy process can take up to nine hours in this
species.
Unlike most adult Lepidoptera that are only known to feed on nectar,
Cosmosoma moths are attracted to juices from plants such as dogfennel
(Eupatorium capillifolium), a common roadside weed throughout the south-
ern and eastern United States; as its name suggests, the leaves and flowers of
this plant smell unpleasant when crushed. It also belongs to a plant group with
members known to produce strong pyrrolizidine alkaloids (PA). Watching the
moths closely, one might see them probing the plant’s surface with their pro-
boscis, feeding or attempting to feed from excrescent material. Because the
larvae of these moths feed upon climbing hempvine (Mikania scandens), a
relatively common and fast-growing southern weed that appears to have no
alkaloidal defenses, the researchers wondered if the males might be obtaining
PA as they probed about. If this were so, did the filaments they released during
courtship also contain pyrrolizidine alkaloids? Might the filaments offer any
protection against predators?
For answers, Conner would need many individuals of these relatively
rare moths. Happily, he was aware of a collecting technique that many
old-time naturalists had used to collect rare butterflies and moths. In
nature, a number of Lepidoptera visit flowers, stems, and leaves to obtain
alkaloid-rich juices, but roots of the plants contain the highest concen-
trations, and thus can be used as a lure. Collecting roots of E. capil-
lifolium, Conner hung them in moistened clusters about 1.5 m above
the ground. That night, Cosmosoma moths began arriving; all but one
was a male. Using mercury vapor light traps, he collected female moths
as well.
In the laboratory, the researchers raised the offspring of these field-
collected individuals on their natural larval food plant, climbing hempvine.
While they grew, he ran chemical analyses of both M. scandens and E.
capillifolium, and confirmed that the larvae’s natural food was PA-free, but
the Eupatorium roots were loaded with alkaloids, mostly intermedine and
lycopsamine but also four other PA, two of which apparently have not been
described from elsewhere in nature.
376 9 Reproductive Behavior
When adult males eclosed in his laboratory cultures, Conner presented one
batch with a crystalline offering of PA mixed with sugar, upon which the
males fed readily for an hour each day over the next five days. Then he killed
some, extracted flocculent out of their abdominal pouches with forceps, and
ran chemical analyses of their bodies and floss. Both contained PA, but it was
about 20 times more concentrated in the flocculent than in the moth’s body.
What protection, if any, did males gain from being PA-laden? As a test
predator, Conner decided to use Nephila clavipes, a common spider that coex-
isted with Cosmosoma, particularly along the edges of lakes and streams. The
researcher flipped a half dozen PA-fed males individually into the spider’s
web. Then he did the same with males that had no access to alkaloids. As
each became entangled, he watched the spider to see whether it ate the moth
or cut it free from the web. Every one of the PA-fed males were cut free, live
and uninjured. In contrast, five of seven PA-free males were eaten.
Would flocculent alone protect females that received it, or were PA neces-
sary as well? Conner placed conservative amounts of flocculent (about half
that present in a male’s pouches) in plastic containers with young virgin
females. As the confined females fluttered about, they stirred up flocculent
and coated themselves with it. Some females received flocculent from a male
that fed on the alkaloid/sugar mix; others received flocculent from a male
that had no access to alkaloids. Then Conner flipped each one individually
into the web of a N. clavipes spider. As they became entangled, he watched
the spider once again. All eight females coated with PA-free flocculent were
eaten. In contrast, the spider rejected and cut free over half the females whose
flocculent came from PA-fed males.
Wondering whether females might also gain PA during the lengthy copu-
lation process, Conner confined various combinations of males and females
overnight in cages; copulation typically began at dusk and lasted most of
the night. When he analyzed their bodies afterward, he found that all of the
females that mated with PA-fed males now contained alkaloids as well, but in
amounts that differed depending on whether they had laid eggs. This seemed
to indicate that the females not only received PA, but passed some of it onto
their eggs. Chemical analysis confirmed that the eggs did, in fact, contain PA.
Was enough PA passed to a female and her eggs in this way to be protec-
tive? Strikingly so. While five of seven virgin females and five of six females
mated with alkaloid-free males were eaten, all seven virgin females mated to
alkaloid-fed males were cut from the web by the spiders, without sustaining
injury. Furthermore, the amount of alkaloid in these protected females’ eggs
(on the order of 1 μg per egg) was shown in a study with another arctiid moth
(Utetheisa ornatrix) to be effective in egg defense against ants, coccinellid
beetles, and chrysopid larvae.
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 377
Oral gifts are particularly evident in different predatory taxa, where they appear
to have evolved independently. Thus, they were initially viewed wholly as a mech-
anism for appeasement, that is, inhibition of the normal predatory instincts of the
participants, especially the female. However, other explanations for this behavior
have also been put forth. One is that males with prey are more conspicuous to
females. Females probably also enjoy a selective advantage by consuming prey at a
point where they need extra protein and calories to convert into eggs; male fitness is
also increased by providing such prey because the success of his genetic contribu-
tion to the next generation is intimately tied to the survival and reproductive success
of each of his mates. In addition, the very act of feeding upon a gift tends to keep a
female occupied, making it likely that a male would benefit from longer copulation
that results in transferring more sperm.
Sexual cannibalism would seem like the ultimate oral gift, but this has been a
matter of some debate. Mantises generally begin feeding upon a prey insect by biting
off its head, and females sometimes do the same with males (Fig. 9.16), at least
under laboratory conditions. If mating has begun, the male’s copulation motions
may become even more vigorous.
Early researchers noted that ganglia in the abdomen, not the head, control
the male’s movements, and proposed that sexual cannibalism was a strategy that
females had developed to enhance fertilization while simultaneously obtaining
sustenance. Others have proposed that submissive males should be evolutionarily
Fig. 9.16 Living dangerously. With good reason, mantis courtship has sometimes been described
as a ‘sneak attack’. In this pair of copulating mantises, photographed in the field in Costa Rica, the
female has eaten the head, prothorax, and prothoracic legs of the male while their genitalia remain
coupled. Scientists are still debating whether males passively submit to such sexual cannibalism
because a well-fed female would optimize the number and condition of their jointly-produced
offspring, or whether males actively try to stay alive to mate again
378 9 Reproductive Behavior
favored because they achieve a selective advantage in their offspring when a female
copulates longer and is better fed; this theory is supported by the observation that
both copulation duration and chances of fertilization are as much as doubled when a
male is cannibalized. However, still other research with well-fed Tenodera sinensis
mantises indicates that the behavior of these extremely visually sensitive insects is
very open to human disturbance; when the mantises were given the opportunity to
copulate in a vacant room, with only a running video recorder to witness the event,
instead of sexual cannibalism they displayed elaborate courtship behaviors that
changed a female’s interest from feeding to mating. Other studies similarly suggest
that males mate repeatedly and do not merely submit to cannibalism; rather, they
appear able to assess a female’s hunger level and adjust their behavior accordingly.
Seminal gifts often appear to provide similar caloric or nutrient benefits to oral
gifts but simply delivered by a different system. However, evolutionary biologists
have been intrigued by an additional possibility. If mating is as much conflict as
cooperation, gifts that are directly produced by the male might contain manipula-
tive substances that enhance his potential at the expense of hers. For example, a
seminal gift might contain a hormone that induced female refractoriness (resistance
to further mating), thus reducing the chances that a later rival would overtop his
own sperm with theirs; in some cases, this has been demonstrated. However, it is
important to remember that there is no compelling reason to believe that all nuptial
gifts must function in the same way, nor is it necessary that the costs and benefits
to each sex play out in the same way for every species. Clearly, in many species the
females gain significant benefits from their gifts, and prefer males that bear larger
gifts. Likewise, in cases where gift-bearing males directly increase the number and
fitness of offspring as a result, they are making a contribution similar to those in
species that practice parental care (see Chapter 10).
To date, the concept of nuptial gifts as manipulation has been supported more by
theory than by empirical evidence. However, it has given rise to a number of predic-
tions that are potentially testable in various gift-giving species. The best known of
all gift-givers undoubtedly are the dance flies. This group of small Diptera received
its name from the flies’ habit of circling about in complex patterns as they search for
prey on the bark of trees. Dance flies present an enormous diversity in nuptial gifts
ranging from clearly useful items such as bits of prey to more puzzling tokens such
as elaborate silk balloons to inedible twig fragments. Why should females require
what appears to be a worthless gift as a condition of mating?
In many textbooks, dance flies are used as a model system to support an evolu-
tionary progression toward ritualization in courtship; a proposed sequence outlines
a decline in male investment in the mating effort from prey to a silk-wrapped dried
insect fragment to an empty silk balloon. Sexual conflict theory suggests a second,
simpler model in which token-giving arose in species that once gave nutritional
gifts, and males exploited female preferences by substituting more easily obtainable
but worthless items. Both models rely on male cheating and female inability to dis-
tinguish between worthwhile and worthless gifts, suggesting an evolutionary race
analogous to those that occur with other systems such as insect-plant coevolution
and predator-prey relationships.
9.3 Courtship and Conflict 379
Fig. 9.17 Cheating pays. Dance flies with oral gifts. (A) Rhamphomyia longicauda, male above,
mating with female while she feeds upon the gift of a small tipulid crane fly (B) Females of
Rhamphomyia sulcata usually accept a nutritious gift, but researchers substituted a cotton ball,
males presenting the token were rewarded with a copulation equivalent in duration to what they
would have received bearing a small genuine gift
gift in other species. Third, during copulation the females tried to feed upon the
cotton ball as though it were a genuine prey. (Similar behavior has been observed
in other species where males regularly present inedible tokens, suggesting that in
those species as well, males previously gave nutritious gifts.) However, copulation
duration was not significantly different for large versus small tokens, suggesting that
females take a set time to determine a gift’s edibility.
During her nuptial flight, a honey bee queen mates with several males in succession,
each of which dies after copulation. Sand fly males defend small arenas where they
emit pheromones; females choose freely, and a single male sometimes obtains all
the matings. A female of the hippoboscid fly Lynchia nurtures her single egg inside
her body until it has passed entirely through the larval stage and is about to pupate;
if a male encounters a female in this condition, he will dig his hook-like tarsal claws
into her back and ride about like this for hours before finally copulating with her
after she deposits her prepuparium.
In sociobiology and behavioral ecology, a mating system is any of the ways in
which animal societies are structured in relation to sexual behavior. These mat-
ing relationships also may or may not be associated with social relationships, in
which the sexual partners stay together to become parenting partners. Thus, there
has been some debate as to whether mating systems should be classified in a way
that reflects pair bonds and the rearing of offspring. The most straightforward clas-
sification simply considers the number of copulatory partners. The mating systems
most commonly recognized in animals are monogamy, polygamy, and promiscuity.
In monogamy, a male and female mate only with each other either during a particu-
lar breeding cycle or throughout their lives. Often called ‘pair bonding,’ monogamy
is rare among insects. Where it occurs, it is generally an evolutionarily derived
condition. Fidelity tends to evolve only under certain ecological circumstances, usu-
ally when the advantages of cooperation in rearing offspring outweigh the personal
advantages to both partners of seeking extra mates.
Polygamy is the state in which an individual has two or more mates, none of
which mates with other individuals. It has two forms. One is polygyny, where a
single male mates with several females; this is the most common system in insects.
The reverse case, polyandry, which occurs when a single female mates with several
males, as happens in honey bees, is relatively rare in insects, as in animals generally.
Thus, the terms polygamy and polygyny are sometimes used synonymously.
Promiscuity also takes two forms. In simple promiscuity, both males and females
mate repeatedly, but no pair bonds are formed. In polygynandry, two or more
males have an exclusive relationship with two or more females; this has been
best described among bird species in which males jointly help rear and/or protect
nestlings. The numbers of males and females need not be equal in a polygynandrous
system; in vertebrate species studied so far, the number of males is usually less than
9.4 Mating Systems and Parental Investment 381
Fig. 9.18 Differing viewpoints. The central principle of sexual selection reformulated in terms
of parental investment. In the common situation illustrated here, the optimum number of offspring
(Of for the female, Om for the male) differs for mothers and fathers. Because the female must
expend a greater effort to create offspring, her greatest net production comes at a lower number
than in the case of the male. This sets the stage for a conflict between the sexes
Fig. 9.19 Ovipositing underground. The migratory locust, Schistocerca, (see Plate 4) digs a
shaft by opening and closing her dorsal and ventral ovipositor valves like a clam-shell posthole
digger, then lays a pod of eggs. After deposition, the eggs absorb water via a special pore in one
end and swell to twice their initial size. Successful breeding depends on rainfall patterns, and final
choice of site is positively influenced by the presence of other ovipositing locusts
be longer and more difficult than is generally assumed. Laying eggs is a signif-
icant expenditure of body energy reserves, as well. Because oviposition mistakes
may severely lower reproductive success and hence individual fitness, the stimulus–
response sequences tend to be quite complex, minimizing chances for error. In
general, the behavioral chain of events leading to oviposition closely parallels that
used in food location (see Chapter 4).
Adult female mosquitoes are attracted to water, influenced by the presence of veg-
etation and amount of light reflected from the surface, but do not automatically
oviposit once they have reached it; when they land upon the water’s surface, some
first judge salt content and others assess the pH of the water through their tarsal
sensilla. Buprestid beetles of the genus Melanophila are attracted to burnt trees over
distances of several kilometers through an oriented response to infrared radiation
emanating from forest fires (see Fig. 3.7).
Site selection generally has two or more phases that vary in their sensory involve-
ment. First, there is a general reaction to the environment; this is followed by much
more specific final selection stages. For example, site selection by the tobacco horn-
worm moth includes both approach and landing. Approach is largely mediated by
384 9 Reproductive Behavior
Fig. 9.20 Tasting by foot-touch. Butterflies in the genus Pieris sense mustard oils in cruciferous
plants with their tarsi. Thus, in the laboratory they can be induced to lay their eggs on other surfaces
as long as their feet contact the proper foliar cue
visual cues and is not particularly discriminating. The decision to land, on the other
hand, involves subtle olfactory responses to smells emanating from host plants.
After landing, contact chemical stimulation elicits egg deposition. Similarly, female
cabbage butterflies are attracted to blue or yellow when searching for nectar, but not
when ready to lay eggs; an oviposition-ready female lands upon a plant and drums
upon it with her forelegs, sensing through her tarsal receptors whether the plant
contains mustard oil before she hangs upside down and oviposits her eggs on the
underside of its leaves (Fig. 9.20).
Among species that do not practice any form of parental care following egg
deposition—a situation that describes the vast majority of insects—proper egg
placement is particularly crucial. Considerable time and energy may be spent on
fine discriminations during site selection. Some stimuli that influence the behavior
of egg-laden female insects are negative, course. For example, certain plant-derived
chemical factors inhibit boll weevil oviposition (in one cotton species, by 40%). In
other cases, the stimuli for oviposition may be only indirectly related to actual larval
feeding preferences, a situation that may have unusual consequences (Fig. 9.21).
Many species have evolved a fine-tuned ability to assess host suitability fac-
tors such as the egg or larval load already upon a host. Female bean weevils,
Callosobruchus maculatus, do not deposit their eggs in mung beans at random but
compare the present bean with those previously encountered; a bean that is larger or
bears fewer eggs than the last one they came across is more likely to receive an egg.
The resultant nonrandom pattern of oviposition increases larval survival by 70%
9.5 Oviposition Behavior 385
Fig. 9.21 Differing cues. Although the larvae have a marked feeding preference for dandelion,
adult females of the noctuid moth Autographa precationis clearly prefer soybeans over dandelions
for oviposition, apparently because the shape of the dandelion leaves is a less effective oviposition
stimulus
over what would occur if eggs were laid randomly. Likewise, some species of hover
flies (Syrphidae) whose larvae feed on aphids are sensitive to aphid density and lay
their eggs in numbers proportional to the abundance of the aphid population. In a
similar vein, females of the parasitic wasp Tiphia popilliavora control the sex of
eggs they lay according to the size of their beetle host, Popillia; in third instar larval
hosts, the parasites lay fertilized female eggs, but in smaller second instar hosts they
lay smaller unfertilized male eggs.
At the same time, there is strong selective pressure for discrimination of poten-
tial danger from parasites and/or predators. Among cannibalistic species such as
preying mantises, even one’s own young may be a threat to siblings. For exam-
ple, the caterpillars of female heliconiine butterflies in the Neotropics will eat eggs
and one another. Ovipositing females often spend considerable time inspecting the
host plant prior to oviposition; it has been suggested that this is a visual search
for other Heliconius eggs or larvae. Probably the search is for other egg preda-
tors and parasites as well; one study estimated that over 90% of these butterflies’
eggs were killed by parasites. Such pressures may have led to uniformity in egg
386 9 Reproductive Behavior
Fig. 9.22 Rolling one’s own. Construction of a leaf roll from a single aspen leaf by a female
weevil, Byctiscus populi. After notching the leaf petiole (arrow), the beetle walks around on part
of the leaf blade (stippled area), perforating the epidermis with its tarsal claws and sometimes its
jaws. Then Byctiscus positions itself parallel to the leaf edge, sinks in its claws, and draws its legs
up toward each other, using the snout to help guide the developing roll. Creeping inside the roll,
the weevil chews a longitudinal slit through one layer before reversing position to deposit a single
egg. Only then does she complete the roll and glue the last edge in place with anal secretions
388 9 Reproductive Behavior
closely related species. Instead, one must turn to ecological factors and consider
theories with an applicability that ranges beyond insects.
In an ideal environment, one with no predators and no intraspecific competition
for resources, a population would increase at its maximum rate, r, a value simply
obtained by subtracting the population’s death rate due to old age from its birth
rate. The whole reproductive thrust of every species is directed toward maintaining
r as close to maximum as possible. In the non-ideal real world, two very differ-
ent strategies for doing this are possible, depending on the amount of competition
and rigor of the environment. One may increase birth rate or decrease death rate.
Uncrowded and nearly ideal environments tend to favor increases in birth rate, rais-
ing r in the process. For this reason, selection that acts to raise the maximum rate of
population increase has been called ‘r-selection’ (more properly, ‘r max selection’).
Species that are r-selected are opportunists, able to quickly discover new habitats.
However, they are rarely able to persist successfully for long in such habitats, since
they tend to be poor competitors. They often have what may be termed ‘big bang’
reproduction, good colonizing ability, and short adult life spans. They can be termed
‘r-strategists’.
Other species live in approximate equilibrium with each other; their densities do
not fluctuate much. Their population levels persist at just about carrying capacity, K,
the number of individuals that the environment can support. There are no uncrowded
habitats to find and exploit; however, being able to survive where one is, particularly
for a long period, takes on new importance. There is little advantage to producing
large numbers of young, but behaviors that increase the survival of one’s young
become important. These species can be termed ‘K-strategists’.
It must be appreciated that r and K are theoretical constructs that represent the
endpoints of a continuum, and that a given organism may display some traits typical
of one strategy and other traits more representative of the other. Related to all of this
is the matter of energy allocation. Any organism has only a limited lifetime with a
limited amount of time and energy to partition among the various activities of its
life. Each animal must divide its time and energy among three major requirements,
which are (in their usual order of descending importance) food, defense against
predators, and reproduction. For each species this total time and energy will be
divided in a different manner depending upon environmental and evolutionary con-
straints. However, just as in any other budget, to the extent that one priority is easily
satisfied, more expenditure can be devoted to activities of the other priorities.
Chapter 10
Parental Behaviors and Social Life
10.1 Introduction
Bumblebees are attracted to those flowers where other bumblebees are already for-
aging. Given a choice, male swallowtail butterflies prefer to land at artificial puddles
with a butterfly decoy. Female Mediterranean fruit flies reared in groups visit and
oviposit in more host fruits than solitary raised females do.
Most insects come together at least temporarily for mating; many also come
together for other reasons, forming temporary to relatively permanent groups. Some
of these are based on relatedness such as parent-offspring groups; others are not.
Some groups undoubtedly result simply from a common response to some partic-
ular environmental factor, such as presumably occurs when aggregations of bush
flies cluster on any available space (Fig. 10.1). At other times, mutual attraction is
clearly involved; some of the communicative devices involved have already been
mentioned in Chapters 6, 7, and 8 .
Might these be called ‘social responses’? The answer depends upon whom you
ask. Biologists use the term ‘social’ in a wide variety of ways. Some might speak
of the social relations between sexes during courtship and mating; others speak of
social interactions among animals in a herd, birds in a flock, or fish in a school.
Ecologists sometimes refer to all of the organisms in a habitat or community as a
society. However, a single thread is common to all these varied uses—the indication
of adaptively significant (often cooperative, but sometimes exploitative) interactions
between two or more individuals. Such interactions, mediated by reciprocal commu-
nication, produce effects that are qualitatively different from the mere summation
of the independent activities of the individuals.
Rather than getting mired in a bog of definitions, let us recognize that we are
dealing with a continuum of relationships. Attempts to classify the various kinds of
animal associations and interactions have been based on their form, basis for associ-
ation, degree of interactions of members, and the nature and duration of association.
One useful simple system is that represented in Table 10.1.
By flashlight, one can sometimes find clusters of bees or wasps assembled together
to pass the night (Fig. 10.2). Up to 200 or 300 individuals may be involved, usually,
10.2 Social Organization 391
Fig. 10.1 Feeling crawly?. An aggregation of Australian bush flies, Musca vestustissima, clus-
tered on a man’s back. The harmless flies thrive in the vast semi-desert Outback, constituting a
continual annoyance by their sheer numbers and tenacity
but not always, of one species. Often they will return night after night to the same
location, but they do not cohabit a nest or rear young together, and the basis of their
individual attraction to the sleeping roost is unknown.
When an assemblage is composed of conspecific individuals including more than
just a mated pair or family, all gathering temporarily in the same place but not inter-
nally organized or engaged in cooperative behavior, it is termed an aggregation.
Many kinds of these ‘uncoordinated’ groups are common among various insects
(see Plate 5).
The causal factors behind aggregations are often unknown. Some result only
from limitations of suitable habitat. In other cases, aggregations grade into slightly
392 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Table 10.1 A simple classification of insect associations, excluding those involving sexual
activitiesa
Interaction
among Nature of Selected
Association Basis members Duration association examples
Fig. 10.2 Sleeping aggregations. (left) A cluster of males of the solitary bee, Chalicodoma, from
Australia ‘sleeping’ on a clothespin. These males returned every evening to the same site for about
three weeks. (right) A group of male Actinote surima surima butterflies roost at night in Brazil.
Similar sleeping aggregations of various butterflies and solitary bees and wasps occur commonly
worldwide
Fig. 10.3 Burrowing battalion. Australian sawfly larvae of the genus Perga crawling on the
forest floor prior to cocooning. In order to successfully construct cocoons and pupate they must
first penetrate the crusty soil. Although individuals are poorly adapted for such burrowing, in larger
aggregations at least one larva usually succeeds in breaking through and the others are then able
to follow. If disturbed, the writhing mass of larvae rear up and regurgitate sequestered droplets of
liquid rich in the essential oils of their eucalyptus host plants, a behavior that gave rise to their
common name, spitfires
where an individual’s very survival depends upon it. Females lay their eggs in pods
within the tissue of eucalyptus leaves; after hatching the young must first gnaw an
exit hole before they can begin feeding. This is a difficult task, but if one succeeds,
the others follow. There is a strong correlation between pod size and larval mortality.
In one study, pods with fewer than ten eggs suffered 66% mortality, but among those
containing more than 30 eggs, mortality was only 43%. Social facilitation is also
involved when mature Perga larvae leave the tree to pupate (Fig. 10.3).
Certain female sawflies guard their eggs and young (Fig. 10.4). Young Gargaphia
solani bugs orient to their mother and follow her from place to place. Scarab beetle
pairs cut out a chunk of freshly deposited manure, roll it off, and place it in an
elaborate underground burial vault where eggs are eventually deposited. The parents
of Brazilian Phloeophana longirostris pentatomids apparently provide nourishment
for their nymphs in addition to protecting them.
More than a dozen orders of insects include at least some species that care for
their young. Parent-offspring groups are the most common form of social assem-
blage in the insects and most often parental care equates to maternal care. Next
most common is biparental care, followed by exclusive paternal care, and finally
10.2 Social Organization 395
Fig. 10.4 Guarding young. Maternal care in the Brazilian sawfly, Themos olfersii. Throughout
the approximately 20 days required for incubation, the conspicuous orange and black female strad-
dles her eggs. If disturbed, she displays one of a series of at least ten different types of defensive
or threatening reactions. Later, even when the fully sclerotized gregarious larvae migrate together
to the basal portion of their host leaf and begin feeding, their mother continues to guard them,
sometimes accompanied by other females that happen to be nearby. Apparently because of their
distastefulness and warning coloration, the adults are only rarely subject to predation, and eggs and
young also benefit from this protection
That the male chafes under the burden is unmistakable; in fact, my suspicions as to the sex
of the egg-carrier were first aroused by watching one in an aquarium which was trying to
free itself from its load of eggs, an exhibition of a lack of maternal interest not expected in
a female carrying her own eggs.
A few years later another entomologist, while mentioning the ‘indignity’ of such
male servitude, noted the ‘peculiar fact’ that copulation took place in connection
with oviposition in the species he observed. However it took almost 75 years before
Robert Smith’s pioneering study (Case Study 10.1) began to make the situation
understandable, and even today studies of male parental care in these bugs continue
to yield new insights.
10.2 Social Organization 397
Fig. 10.5 Reversing roles. The copulation-oviposition cycle of the giant water bug, Abedus her-
berti. After preliminary sparring between the sexes, receptive males perform vigorous pumping
(a). Females respond by climbing on the male’s back (b) as if to oviposit. With one hind leg, the
male manipulates the female off into a copulatory position (c). About one minute after intromis-
sion, the male abruptly ‘scrubs’ one hind leg on the female’s hemelytra. Immediately the female
repositions herself (d) and begins to lay eggs, starting at the apex of the male’s hemelytra. After
about five minutes, although the female has laid fewer than four eggs, the male’s temporary qui-
escence ends. Forcing the female out of position with his hind leg, the male begins a new bout
of vigorous pumping. This cycle continues until the female’s total egg clutch is deposited on the
male’s back (e)
398 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
vegetation or the stream bottom, they rocked their bodies repeatedly forward
and backward, pivoting on their middle legs while remaining in a fixed posi-
tion. He also noted that before they began to rock or pump, males of both
species patted or touched the eggs with their hind legs.
Was patting an essential precursor to pumping or stroking? Smith quickly
peeled off egg pads after a pumping bout had been initiated but while the bug
was still under water. The males all continued to pump. Later, when they felt
about for the eggs and found them gone, they no longer would pump. Thus,
Smith concluded that male water bugs used patting and stroking to inform
themselves of the status of their egg pad.
Why should males take on such a task? It seemed evident that a brood-
ing male is exposed to extra risks. The additional weight of developing eggs
would impair his swimming and perhaps reduce his ability to escape preda-
tors. Smith measured swimming speeds of egg-carrying and unencumbered
males and found that the latter could swim considerably faster. The behav-
iors involved in the father’s parental care undoubtedly also increase risk of
predation. Because water bugs normally rest motionless, protected by their
cryptic coloration, pumping or stroking behaviors probably make them more
vulnerable by betraying their location.
Perhaps males were merely being tricked into doing this work. If so, it
seemed likely that they could be tricked into carrying any female’s eggs with
equal likelihood. Were male A. herberti carrying eggs that they had not fertil-
ized themselves? Through detailed observations of Abedus mating behavior,
Smith showed that courtship and oviposition were under overwhelming male
domination. The male waterbug commanded the female to copulate, guided
her into the copulatory position, signaled his desire to uncouple, and deter-
mined the length of time she was given to oviposit. He allowed no oviposition
to take place at all until after the female had mated with him at least once
and usually several times. Then, as the female began laying her eggs, he inter-
rupted her frequently to insist upon additional copulations. As predicted by
sperm precedence, by insisting on copulation prior to oviposition, the male
water bug ensures that the eggs he receives will have been those that he has
fertilized. By limiting the period of oviposition he also minimizes the chance
that he will mistakenly accept eggs fertilized by another male. Does this mean
that belostomatids are monogamous? Apparently not. Smith was able to show
that males are able to simultaneously brood and fertilize multiple clutches
of eggs from different females; bug’s strict control over the situation would
assure that these offspring were his own, as well.
Thus, Smith established that male giant water bugs have developed a whole
set of courtship and mating behaviors that maximize individual male fitness by
ensuring paternity and maximize female fitness by placing brooding behavior
under male care and thus freeing themselves to capture the prey necessary to
make additional clutches of eggs.
400 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Smith proposed that male brooding in water bugs arose by ancillary selection,
a form of natural selection that produces characters supported by primary traits. In
this case, the primary trait was selection for large body size that occurred more than
150 million years ago. Larger bugs were able to take advantage of vertebrate prey,
but larger size also meant larger eggs. Because of the way that insect eggs develop,
those beyond a certain size cannot develop unattended under water, and thus the
stage was set for parental care in these species.
Why should it not be the female water bugs, rather than males, that brood the
young? An enhanced fecundity hypothesis that was originally proposed for birds
may provide an answer. Basically, this model suggests that if one of the costs that a
female incurs by brooding is a reduced ability to obtain nutrients needed to produce
eggs, selection will favor male traits that reduce this cost and improve the number
of offspring the pair can produce. Because males require only enough nutrition to
maintain their body, rather than to maintain it and produce energetically costly eggs,
they can invest in parenting behaviors at a lower cost than females can.
Research suggests that this theory applies very well to giant water bugs. In sev-
eral feeding studies in which adults have been given generous prey opportunities
immediately after mating, female water bugs have consumed many more prey than
males have eaten. (In one case with goldfish, females killed five to nine times as
many fish as males did over a seven to nine day period.) Males seldom feed while
brooding, and also appear to be inhibited from feeding on prey in the size class of
first-instar nymphs, an adaptation that may minimize the risk of males consuming
their own hatchlings. Even stronger evidence was provided by a study that compared
the effects of food limitation on fitness of males and females. One group was fed
every day; the other received one prey every ten days. The results were dramatic.
Both well-fed and poorly fed males sired statistically similar numbers of offspring.
However, well-fed females laid 3.5 times as many viable eggs as poorly fed females
did. Thus, as a result of freeing females to pursue prey while males assume the task
of parental care, both sexes benefit through an increase in offspring numbers.
Because Abedus males insist on a copulatory bout after every few eggs, it can
take hours before a female finishes depositing her full complement of eggs. Is the
threat from other water bug males really so great as to require going to such lengths
for paternity assurance? Smith mated a female to a male that was homozygous for
a dominant genetic marker. Then he paired that female to another male that he had
‘ductectomized’ so that it was unable to fertilize eggs. When this sterile male’s
brood hatched, all bore the genetic label from the female’s previous mate (Fig. 10.6),
demonstrating both sperm precedence and a very real threat of cuckoldry in this
species.
All stages of the house cricket, Acheta domestica, spend the day concealed in nooks
and crannies. As soon as they hatch, the larvae of bagworm moths (Psychidae) begin
10.2 Social Organization 401
Fig. 10.6 Brooding about baby. This male giant water bug has been made an experimental cuck-
old; the stripe on the first hatched nymph’s back identifies its father as a genetically marked male
that copulated with his mate before she was placed with him
making a mobile case of silk and environmental materials; extending the front as
they grow, and excreting waste out the back, they move about, consuming vegeta-
tion. Adult males leave to mate, but females remain in the bag; birds that eat them
pass the hard-shelled eggs unharmed, spreading the population. Across much of
Australia, homeowners are familiar with the massive nests constructed in unusual
locations by the continent’s largest mud dauber wasps, Abispa (see Plate 35).
Many larvae take advantage of refuges, from naturally occurring crevices to cav-
ities formed as a side effect of their own feeding. Others construct their own shelters
as feeding contrivances, such as the funnels made by the ant lions and worm lions
(see Fig. 4.6) and the cases constructed by caddisflies. Only the broadest definitions
consider these to be nests, however. A nest is more often defined to be a structure
that an adult animal builds to shelter its young, using available natural materials that
it modifies in some way. When adults build nests for their offspring, the behavior is
considered to be a hallmark of a high level of parental care. A colony, on the other
hand, is the social unit that occupies the nest, consisting of brood as well as adults.
(This distinction will become quite useful in discussing fully social insects later in
this chapter.)
Leaving aside the complex nesting behaviors outlined in the ‘social register’ later
in this chapter, let us consider here the solitary nesters, in which one adult or at
most a pair of adults build the nest and in some species, defend it. These are not
numerically common behaviors, but they have appeared in a taxonomically diverse
array of insects, often occurring in those groups closely associated with the soil. For
example, nesting is found in certain earwigs, burrowing crickets (Fig. 10.7), some
cockroaches, various solitary bees and wasps, and a variety of beetles, including the
dung beetles.
As an example, let us consider the latter. The term ‘dung beetle’ has commonly
been applied to three subfamilies of the large beetle family Scarabaeidae. Only
402 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Fig. 10.7 Burrowing crickets. A female of Anurogryllus arboreus in her brood chamber. To the
right is the exit burrow; to the left, a filled defecation midden. A single newly hatched nymph may
be seen touching antennae with the mother, while an egg pile lies beneath her abdomen partly
covered by soil. The chamber floor has bits of grass she has gathered, upon which nymphs feed. In
this species, special miniature trophic eggs laid by the mother also serve as baby food, an unusual
trait; these nonviable eggs are very attractive to the offspring, which crowd around them and fight
for their possession
in the Scarabaeinae, however, are the vast majority of the roughly 4,500 species
coprophagous, most feeding on the excrement of large mammals. No known adult
Scarabaeinae have mouthparts capable of chewing or cutting solid food, hence when
dung begins to desiccate it becomes unusable to them. This, plus the clumped
discontinuous distribution of dung pats of many grazing animals, creates a fierce
competition. Food relocation to constructed nests is a key behavioral feature of this
subfamily and is thought to have evolved to reduce predation, parasitism, and com-
petition, especially from coprophagous flies. Nest construction in this subfamily is
nearly always the cooperative endeavor of a male–female pair. The association is a
close one, and once a pair bond is formed, it endures, at least until after the dung
has been lodged in its final resting place.
Gonzalo Halffter and his colleagues, the team responsible for the most com-
prehensive studies available on the natural history of dung beetles, recognize two
major behavioral groups, burrowers (Fig. 10.8) and rollers. (A third small group are
the dwellers.)
Burrowers, which include the well-known genera Copris and Synapsis, compact
a piece of dung, cut it into spheres and place them in a nest they have excavated
below or adjacent to the dung pat. The female beetle attends the offspring until
they pupate, cleaning mold growth from the surface of the spheres and applying
antimicrobial chemicals derived from her sternal glands. Males help dig the nest,
10.2 Social Organization 403
Fig. 10.8 Burrowing dung beetles. In burrowers such as Onthophagus gazella, couples excavate
burrows below a dung source, then make a series of brood balls that the beetle’s larvae consume as
they complete their development
provision it, and compact the dung. During this period of participation, they also
actively defend the nest using (in many species) the prominent horns on their head
or prothorax.
Rollers (also called tumblebugs) (see Plate 36) are the second major group; they
include most of the tribe Scarabaeini. The sacred scarab of ancient Egypt belonged
to this group. The ability to roll a ball of dung overland (sometimes up to 40 m from
the source) sharply distinguishes this group and results in a quite homogeneous
group behaviorally. Generally the male pushes and the female pulls or rides on top,
walking in place like a hamster on a treadmill. Because morphological adaptations to
dung rolling (especially longer and more slender legs) often mean a partial or com-
plete loss of digging ability, there are no elaborate nests or impressive excavations
in this tribe, and some do not bury the brood ball at all.
The Australian continent lacks native large mammalian herbivores. Conse-
quently, native dung beetles have evolved as specialists on the small bits of
excrement characteristic of marsupials such as wallabies and kangaroos. With the
advent of sprawling cattle ranches in northern Australia, cattle dung became an
404 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
end, several inches below the surface, one finds a chamber with a legless, helpless
yellow-white grub feeding on the flies brought in by its mother. After eating about
two dozen, the grub will spin a cigar-shaped cocoon inside which it will eventually
metamorphose into an adult.
Like these sand wasps, the vast majority of wasps live solitary lives. Their nests
belong to three broad types: those dug in a substrate such as soil, rotten wood, or
plant pith; those constructed in preexisting cavities such as hollow twigs; and those
constructed wholly of foreign materials such as plant pulp, mud (see Plate 35), or
resin. Many of the details of solitary wasp nesting behavior appear to have been
molded by biotic factors, notably various natural enemies and nest associates. For
example, among ground-nesting crabronid wasps several species construct acces-
sory burrows, blind-end false tunnels close beside the true nest burrow. In every
case the accessory burrows are left open and the true nest entrance closed off.
There is evidence that such accessory burrows divert the attention of parasites; bom-
byliid flies may lay their eggs in them (Fig. 10.9), and velvet ants often spend time
investigating and digging in the bottom of such holes.
Species nesting in the soil have another problem relative to pests, namely, soil
removal. A mound of dirt at the nest entry can be a dead giveaway of the presence of
that nest. So while some species simply let the displaced soil accumulate outside the
nest entrance, others carry out the soil a bit at a time in their jaws, flying off a short
distance before dropping it to the ground. Still others push or rake the dirt back-
ward out of the nest and then by a variety of behaviors remove or at least disperse
Fig. 10.9 Deceiving enemies. A bombyliid bee fly hovers over an accessory burrow of a solitary
wasp. With repeated flicks of her abdomen she flings several eggs into the hole, which is actually
a fake nest entrance made by the wasp
406 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Fig. 10.10 Hiding the evidence. Patterns created by mound-leveling behavior of three Australian
Bembix sand wasps during nest construction. Throughout both digging and leveling, synchronous
motions of the long-spined front tarsi rake the sand, producing a species-characteristic pattern
the conspicuous mound. These leveling behaviors, which are a species characteris-
tic, typically consist of repeated passages over the mound while scraping with the
front legs and turning slightly from side to side (Fig. 10.10). The enigmatic resul-
tant patterns, highly diagnostic of the species that made them, are often elaborate
but ephemeral due to the homogenizing action of wind and weather. In addition to
dispersing the mound, it has also been postulated that leveling may serve to disperse
or cover any wasp scent that might be detected by foraging ants or other predators,
parasites, or scavengers.
Solitary nesting species sometimes clump their nests into aggregations with con-
specifics, in this way gaining some measure of support in defense, but many solitary
bee and wasp species have evolved another strategy. Each female provides the food
for her own offspring, but several females occupy a communal nest together. Such
nest sharing takes various forms that differ in duration of association and degree
of cooperation between females. In some species, females co-occupy nests soon
after emergence and simultaneously provision different cells for several days, but
10.3 The Insect Social Register 407
finally all but one of the females is forced out. In other species, females stay together
apparently amicably for prolonged periods.
Several advantages for this communal behavior have been proposed, including
increased efficiency at nest building, reduced parasitism, and improved nest defense.
However, because communal nesting is uncommon, there must be substantial, if as
yet largely unidentified, costs to this system as well. One almost certainly is the
ever-present temptation for kleptoparasitism (see Section 4.2.5) and prey theft.
Fig. 10.11 Going places. The Rev. H. C. McCook, an early American myrmecologist, wrote this
illustration’s original caption, ‘Winged female ants at play on the plaza.’ Actually, these are virgin
females preparing to leave on their mating flight
408 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
The name of this section is drawn from an old custom apparently specific to the
United States; the Social Register is a directory of names and addresses of prominent
American families who form the social elite in a number of major cities. These tra-
ditionally were the ‘members of polite society’ (i.e., those with ‘old money’); recent
editions now also include those with ‘newer’ wealth, the political or corporate elite.
Like that human social register, the insect social register includes the well-
established examples, with a nod to newcomers. The level of social organization
recognized as a society includes most instances of parental care discussed in the
first part of this chapter. It also embraces some groups of developing individuals
of the same generation (see Plate 37). Certain tent caterpillars, for example, spin
a home web at which they gather gregariously to sleep and a feeding net which
also serves as a protective community shelter; successful foragers deposit chemical
trails that elicit preferential following by unfed individuals (see Fig. 6.13). In most
insect species parents die before progeny mature, or else offspring disperse and do
not remain associated with the parent. In some, however, at least some young per-
sist with their parent(s) for one or more generations as an extended family living in
some type of domicile (Fig. 10.12).
As in so many areas of biology, many of the first serious scientific efforts to
understand social behaviors involved attempting to classify them. However, trying
to cram the ever-expanding number of examples of insect social groups into existing
Fig. 10.12 Living in a log. Horned passalid beetles, Odontotaenius disjunctus, sharing a common
tunnel system in a decaying oak log during the fall. During this nonreproductive season, both adult
sexes are present but young are absent. In the following breeding season, pairs cooperate to rear a
brood of larvae, sometimes with the help of other adults. Both adults and larvae produce a diversity
of audible sounds in one of the richest auditory repertoires known for any insect, but their functions
remain poorly understood
10.3 The Insect Social Register 409
classification frameworks that date back to the early 1900s has led to such confusion
as to call into question the value of such an activity. Even the widely cited tradi-
tional lexicon from the 1960s (pre-, sub-, quasi-, semi-, para-social) is cumbersome,
somewhat teleological, and has difficulty accommodating the range and complex-
ity of cooperative interactions of insects. Beginning in the mid-1990s a number of
alternative classifications were proposed that ranged from conceptual expansion, to
narrowing or redefining, to total abandonment.
Today there is a growing consensus in favor of a return to a broader, more
inclusive interpretation of social behavior, with a shift in focus to the case-by-case
examination of natural histories and a focus upon particular ecological constraints
that promote group behavior. Thus, we retain only the term applied to the most
advanced societies: eusocial.
While it seems that all levels of intraspecific interaction from completely solitary
behavior to highly complex coordinated societies are found in insects, compar-
atively few species are eusocial. They number a few thousand species and are
included in five orders, Hemiptera (bamboo and gall-forming aphids), Thysanoptera
(gall thrips), Coleoptera (a bark beetle), Hymenoptera (ants, bees, and wasps), and
Isoptera (termites). Molecular phylogenetic study suggests that the termites should
properly be considered as a family of cockroaches (Blattodea), an idea that has a
long and often contentious history (see Chapter 1). To minimize confusion we will
mostly avoid using either ordinal name and simply refer to this group as the termites.
Eusocial insect species are traditionally considered to include only those whose
societies meet three criteria: they live as groups of adults of different generations,
with cooperative activity, and with different individuals obligatorily performing dif-
ferent roles essential for the success of the group. The last of these three is the
essential defining criterion that sets the bar relatively high for admission to this
exclusive club. As originally applied, it refers to the presence of a reproductive divi-
sion of labor, in which there are sterile workers and one or more fertile queens.
A broader but still succinct definition of eusociality has been offered by Edward
O. Wilson, ‘care across generations of the offspring of a reproductive caste by a
non-reproductive or less reproductive worker caste’.
Castes are defined as a subset of the colony or group that is both morphologi-
cally distinct and behaviorally specialized. The physiology of caste determination
is beyond the scope of this book, but appears to involve changes in regulation of
highly conserved molecular pathways interacting with life history attributes in the
different social lineages.
Division of labor is a prominent feature across all the eusocial insects, and worker
and queen castes are the most common job descriptions. Among eusocial bees,
wasps and ants, workers are female adults and usually sterile, although some may
occasionally produce eggs. Among honey bees, highly developed task-related spe-
cialization has evolved; despite both being genetically female, the worker caste and
the queen caste differ in at least 53 quantitatively distinct morphological features in
addition to their obvious physiological and behavioral differences.
Caste polymorphism reaches its most spectacular development in termites, the
other large eusocial group. Workers are of either sex and include immature stages as
410 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
well as adults. Queens of some species develop huge abdomens so distended with
developing eggs as to resemble a small sausage (Fig. 10.13), and soldiers specialized
for defense may exhibit either enlarged heads with powerful jaws or conical nozzle-
like heads that eject a sticky substance (see Fig. 5.10).
In addition to caste polymorphism, as colony complexity increases and the range
of duties surrounding the care of the young becomes correspondingly greater, in
some cases an age-related division of labor also develops. For example, in the
ant Myrmica scabrinodis, worker individuals that have emerged during the present
season function as nurses; those that became adults during the previous season
are builders, and even older individuals act as foragers. Such temporal polyethism
reaches its greatest development among the honey bees. On emergence, a young bee
works as a cleaner for about three days. After this, coincidental with labial gland
development, she becomes a nurse, producing secretions with which she feeds lar-
vae. About the tenth day of her life her abdominal glands begin to produce wax
and her labial glands atrophy; she becomes a builder. At about the sixteenth day she
begins to receive nectar and pollen loads from foragers and stores them in the comb.
On about the twentieth day, she fills the post of guard. Finally, she becomes a for-
ager, working at this for the rest of her life. Age polyethism, though well developed,
is not inflexible. If a hive is divided so that one half contains only young bees and
the other only older foraging bees, after a few days of adjustment, workers in each
half carry out all normal hive tasks (see Case Study 1.1).
Fig. 10.13 Social insect castes. (above) Two castes of the African termite Macrotermes. The
soldier’s huge head serves in colony defense, but soldiers must depend upon the smaller workers
to feed them. (below) The extreme physogastry of this termite queen dominates the picture; her
sausage-like abdomen greatly overshadows her tiny head and thorax. Her abdominal tergites have
become pulled widely apart by the stretching of the intersegmental membranes and appear as
crescent-shaped islands on a sea of whitish membrane. Attending the queen are numerous workers
412 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Fig. 10.14 Blocking entry. A colony fragment of the myrmecine ant Zacryptocerus varians in a
hollowed-out stem of red mangrove. Three female castes are shown: the queen rests on the floor
of the nest to the left, while on the right a large major worker blocks the nest entrance with its
saucer-shaped head. Behind the queen another major worker receives regurgitated liquid from a
minor worker
Any first-time visitor to lowland rainforests of the New World tropics will
quickly note the ubiquity of ants patrolling nearly every surface. Periodically,
conspicuous hordes of army ants ominously swarm over every piece of vege-
tation driving hapless arthropods before their onslaught. How is this system
maintained?
Each day within the tropical forest, Eciton workers stream out at dawn to
begin their raids anew, their branching columns quickly overrunning areas up
to 100 m from their nest. At the front line, biting, stinging ants attack insects
and other arthropods, tearing their prey apart and carrying the softer pieces
back to the nest so that the forest floor soon has a series of two-lane highways
traveled by steady streams of advancing raiders and returning victors. So effi-
ciently does the massive raiding progress that one colony may haul in more
than 100,000 other arthropods in a single day. Then, as night begins to fall,
the whole colony begins to emigrate along one of the day’s principal raiding
trails. Moving in solemn procession, sometimes through most of the night,
the colony finally settles in a new bivouac, or temporary camp, often under
a low-hanging branch or vine. After about two weeks, however, the colony’s
behavior abruptly changes. It appears to quiet down and enter a statary phase.
Few workers go out on raids; when they do, the forays are much smaller. No
longer do nightly migrations occur. Remaining at the same site for about three
weeks, the colony acts as though it had gone partially dormant. Then, just as
abruptly, a new nomadic phase of intense foraging activity begins.
What factors underly such spectacular phasic behavior? Walter Schneirla
decided to investigate. At the time, most scientists accepted a straightforward
and seemingly logical explanation: depletion of food supply. The army ants,
they argued, simply stay in one place until the food supply is exhausted and
then move on to new hunting grounds. However, an alternative explanation
also had some appeal. Perhaps the cycles were cued to some environmental
phenomenon, such as phases of the moon, or perhaps changes in temperature,
humidity, or air pressure.
Detailed observational data on colonies in the field were sorely needed.
Schneirla began following a single colony, then another and another, through
one or more complete cycles. Painstakingly, he logged a dozen armies through
whole cycles and more than a 100 more through partial cycles, taking down
data in the field and repeatedly sampling the internal colony composition at
various stages of the behavioral cycle. One thing quickly became clear. Within
a single environment, several colonies were often present; of this number,
some were generally in the nomadic phase, others in the statary phase. This
seemed to rule out major environmental factors as the determinant of the
414 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Fig. 10.15 Raiding ant cycles. The alternation of statary and nomadic phases in the colony cycle
of the army ant, Eciton burchelli. Nomadic phases are triggered by worker–callow interactions
and maintained by worker–larval interactions. As cocooning of mature larvae begins, intensity of
mutual stimulation between adults and brood declines temporarily and the colony lapses back into
the statary condition until the pupal brood emerges
10.3 The Insect Social Register 415
being forged. Colonies from which he removed all callow brood appeared
lethargic in contrast and invariably failed to initiate nomadic behavior at the
expected time.
But while trophallaxis between workers and callows was appealing enough
in theory, Schneirla’s observations at this point did not rule out a second pos-
sibility, namely, that the necessity to feed developing larvae and new adults
was causative. In fact, during later stages of the nomadic phase when the lar-
vae had grown and developed voracious appetites, they seemed to become the
source of the colony ‘drive.’ To test the role of larval brood in stimulating the
workers’ activity, Schneirla split a colony into two parts of comparable size.
In one group he left the larvae intact; in the other he removed them. The work-
ers in contact with larvae continued to show considerable activity, but those in
the broodless portion were much less active. In another experiment, Schneirla
removed the entire larval brood from a colony that was in the nomadic phase.
True to expectations, the colony stopped emigrating, and the intensity of its
daily raids diminished.
among social wasps, but among dragonflies, crickets (see Fig. 8.10), and passalid
beetles, among others.
In the life of a social wasp nest, a colony cycle is the period of development that
lasts from the end of one reproductive episode to the end of the next. In temperate
species nests are normally annual; one foundress or a group of them starts a nest,
which grows and expands until late summer or autumn, when a reproductive phase
culminates in mated females overwintering to start the cycle anew the next spring.
Polistes paper wasps provide a good example. Sometimes, a single queen must find
a new nest location on her own, and bear the risk of nest building and foraging for
herself and her first batch of young. In other cases, groups of females join together
in the spring to begin their nests, but each fertile potential queen then competes in
laying eggs, and soon relatively rigid hierarchies are established. Dominant Polistes
maintain their reproductive superiority by three means: laying the greatest num-
ber of eggs, physically removing and eating the eggs of any subordinates that may
have succeeded in ovipositing, and demanding and receiving the greatest share of
food. Interactions between dominant and subordinate individuals are often matters
of posture; the dominant individual rises on her legs above the subordinate, which
crouches and lowers its antennae (Fig. 10.16). The dominant female also performs
a conspicuous side-to-side vibration of the abdomen (tail wagging) more frequently
than other individuals do. Polistes castes are difficult to distinguish morphologically
but they are behaviorally clear. During the first half of the summer, only workers
are produced. In late summer and early fall, sexual forms (males and reproductively
competent females called gynes) start to appear; concurrently, the colony slowly
declines, finally abandoning the nest entirely.
Fig. 10.16 Bullying nestmates. Dominance and subordinance behavior in the paper wasp Polistes
fuscatus. The female on the left shows the relatively elevated posture characteristic of a dominant
individual. The subordinate (right) has been seized at the hind leg and crouches with antennae
lowered
418 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
For tropical social wasps, on the other hand, seasonal constraints may be less rig-
orous, and in many species the components of the colony cycle seem to have become
dissociated. Several cycles of brood may be produced before a reproductive brood
is produced, a nest may be used for variable lengths of time of up to several years
before it is abandoned, or a colony may routinely occupy two nests successively
before producing any reproductives. Many of these tropical species start their colony
cycle with swarm-founding. A swarm can be thought of as a reproductive unit that is
essentially a colony without a nest or brood. In honey bees, it consists of a hive’s old
queen and about half of the workers; the remainder stay behind with a new queen
and the brood that are already in the hive. In swarm-founding wasps, it consists of a
group of reproductives that disperse, leaving a fraction of the population back at the
nest to repeat the colony cycle.
There are many advantages to swarm-founding, including reduced risk for the
queens. Rather than scouting for a new nest site, they remain in the security of
the natal nest while workers take on this job, and are exposed only while flying to
the site. In addition, they never have a period where they must take on the risks
of foraging. Finally, having a large worker force from the start provides the strong
defense that is needed against predators, in particular ants, a formidable enemy force
in tropical regions (Fig. 10.17).
Nests covered by envelopes are universal to the second group of eusocial
wasps, the vespines, a predominantly temperate group that includes the hornets and
yellowjackets. Their annual nest cycles (Fig. 10.18) are basically similar to that of
Polistes, except that the queen and worker castes are morphologically distinct. Most
species of yellowjackets construct subterranean nests; hornet nests are typically
aerial or built within hollow trees. Most wasps feed upon a variety of arthropods
Fig. 10.17 Defending the fort. (left) Polybia occidentalis workers rear back and buzz their wings
in short bursts, directing intermittent blasts of air at two fire ant (Solenopsis) foragers that are
attempting to climb on the nest. Ants that face this windstorm usually retreat, If an ant does
pass the defenders, a wasp will grasp the ant in her mandibles, carry it away from the nest, and
drop it. (right) ‘Ant guards’ made by the hover wasp Parischnogaster on a rootlet above the nest
attachment. The substance is secreted by the Dufour’s gland and applied to the substrate
10.3 The Insect Social Register 419
Fig. 10.18 Seasonally changing. The annual colony cycle of a yellowjacket wasp (Vespula)
includes solitary and colonial phases. In early spring fertile queens awaken from hibernation and
alone initiate nests by constructing a few paper cells hung from the top of a preexisting soil cavity
such as an abandoned rodent burrow (upper left). After the first workers emerge the colony grows
rapidly through the summer, with the nest ultimately reaching the size of a basketball. By late sum-
mer, cells of the bottom combs (here two) are distinctly larger than those in the uppermost combs.
These bottom cells give rise to males and virgin queens, while the cells in the upper combs produce
only workers. From this point the nest declines. Almost always, only the newly produced queens
overwinter to repeat the cycle the following year
caught alive and carried back to the nest piecemeal in their jaws, but some yel-
lowjacket species become nuisance pests, scavenging around picnic and recreation
areas, especially in late summer.
The Stenogastrinae, the third social vespid group, are delicate slender wasps
with hovering flight that are denizens of rainforests of the Indo-Pacific region. The
420 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
approximately 60 species are poorly known, and their colonies never become very
populous.
Outside the Vespidae, almost no other wasps are eusocial. However, a group of
four genera in the family Crabronidae are apparently unique in producing silk from
abdominal glands that have not been found elsewhere in this wasp family. The silk
is used in nest construction, and more than one adult is commonly found in these
nests, suggesting the possibility of social behavior. The most completely studied
species, a tiny Central and South American wasp called Microstigmus comes, preys
exclusively upon Collembola. Inside its pendent bag-like nests (Fig. 10.19) as many
as 11 females cooperate in nest defense and cell provisioning; genetic evidence
indicates that colonies include a mother and her daughters, with one female having
well developed ovaries.
Fig. 10.19 Crafting with silk. Thumb-sized nest constructed by the eusocial crabronid wasp,
Microstigmus comes. The nest is formed of plant fibers bound together by silk thread produced
by unique glands found in the tip of the adult female’s abdomen (arrow). The plant fibers are first
loosened and scraped from an area on the underside of a palm leaf (a, b), then gathered together
to form an undifferentiated mass which is bound with the silk (c–e) and then slowly lowered on
a silken thread pedicel (f). The pedicel coil and entrance hole at the base of the pedicel are then
added and the upper portion of the interior hollowed and prepared with the first cell (g–i)
Within certain sweat bee colonies, even if there are only two individuals, one is a
female that has mated, has developed ovaries, lays eggs, eats others’ eggs, dominates
others, rarely forages for pollen and nectar, and is the oldest or largest; the other
has not mated, has underdeveloped ovaries, lays few eggs and has these eaten, is
dominated, frequently forages or guards the nest, and is younger and/or smaller
than others.
422 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Whether halictid bees were once fully social and are evolving toward solitary
behavior or vice versa continues to be a matter of academic debate. As might be
expected with such a large group, the majority of sweat bees are still unknown
behaviorally, and information is limited for many others. Furthermore, most of the
species that have been studied are from the temperate zone, where environmental
correlates such as flowering synchrony may influence social biology.
The family Apidae includes many eusocial members. The true honey bees in the
genus Apis are the best known, but another group shares many behavioral similari-
ties with them. The Meliponini, or stingless bees are among the most conspicuous
and numerous bees in these tropical regions. Like honey bees, these bees live in
perennial colonies that produce new colonies by swarming; individual queens or
workers cannot survive alone for long. Furthermore, both have queen and worker
castes that differ, behaviorally, physiologically, and morphologically. They both
develop large populations, up to 180,000 workers in some stingless bee species.
Colony integration and communication are necessarily complex, and both of these
bee groups have evolved elaborate means of communicating the location of food
sources (see Chapter 8). Both honey bees and stingless bees also build nests with
wax secreted from abdominal glands. Larvae of both are reared in individual cells
arranged in combs within the nest, while separate cells are used to store honey
and pollen (Fig. 10.20). However, honey bees and stingless bees belong to differ-
ent clades, and do show other differences. One major difference is that meliponine
larvae are mass provisioned; those of Apis are fed progressively. Another is that
honey bees defend their nest by stinging; meliponines defend theirs by aggressively
biting intruders.
Another social group familiar to almost everyone is the genus Bombus, which
includes the robust hairy bees commonly called bumblebees. Bumblebees are char-
acteristic of cool north temperate climates around the world; with few exceptions,
they have not succeeded in invading the tropics, except at high elevations where
they may become very abundant locally. A behaviorally cohesive group, all of the
more than 250 Bombus species have relatively small annual colonies (Fig. 10.21)
founded by a single overwintered queen. Abandoned rodent burrows are preferred
nest sites; inside, a queen fashions her nest from wax secreted by abdominal glands.
Rather than directly feeding her brood as do other eusocial groups, the bumblebee
queen characteristically stocks pollen and honey separately from the brood cells in
special wax storage pots. She then lays several eggs together in a single distensi-
ble wax brood cell; thus, the bumblebee larvae are reared in groups but since they
are contained in a capped cell with their entire food supply, there is no trophallaxis
contact between parent and offspring.
Finally, some unusual eusocial behaviors are found among the so-called allodap-
ine bees. Unlike the nests of all other known bees, those of allodapines have no
cells. Larvae are kept together on the nest floor like ant larvae, and like the young
of ants they are moved about the nest by the mother and arranged in groups accord-
ing to age (Fig. 10.22). Allodapine larvae are fed progressively, being given more
frequent pollen meals as they grow, a behavior that promotes mother–offspring con-
tact. In most species, the mother dies before her young emerge, but in some species
10.3 The Insect Social Register 423
Fig. 10.20 ‘Beeing’ stingless. One of the species of stingless honey bees of the genus Trigona
from Costa Rica builds large aerial nests (right) of secreted wax mixed with large amounts of resin
to form a tough material called cerumen. Inside the nest, pollen and honey pots (left, below and
above respectively) fill the brood chamber. While Trigona cannot sting, they actively defend their
nests by vigorously biting intruders
the mother’s life overlaps the adulthood of her offspring, who often remain in the
parental nest and help rear a second generation.
Fig. 10.21 Living in a mouse nest. A colony of the European bumblebee, Bombus lapidarius.
The larger individual at the lower center is the queen, resting on a cluster of pupae-containing
cocoons. At the upper left are several communal larval cells. Open cells contain pollen or honey
Fig. 10.22 Developing progressively. A nest of an allodapine bee, Exoneura, showing eggs and
developing brood grouped together in an opened pithy plant stem. Larvae are fed progressively on
small pollen balls brought in by cooperating female bees
10.3 The Insect Social Register 425
Fig. 10.23 Ventilating the fort. (left) Environmental control in a termite nest is evident in this
cutaway view of the interior of the nest of Macrotermes bellicosus, an African fungus-growing
termite, that shows the temperature and percentage carbon dioxide concentration at different posi-
tions. Metabolic heat from the huge biomass of colony members is concentrated in the central
core of the nest, rises by convection to the large upper hollow cavity, then diffuses toward the
sides where it flow into a network of narrow channels close to the surface; here the air is cooled,
and gaseous exchange occurs by diffusion through the thin dry walls. Refreshed air then sinks to
the lower passages of the nest and eventually recycles. (right) Cathedral mound of Nasutitermes
triodiae from northern Australia
typical colony contains eggs, nymphs, workers, soldiers, and reproductive individ-
uals of both genders, plus one to several egg-laying queens. Nests are punctuated
by a maze of tunnel-like galleries that effectively ameliorate air temperatures
(Fig. 10.23) and control carbon dioxide/oxygen balance, as well as allowing the
termites to move through the nest; foraging is typically done along radiating cov-
ered tunnels built out from the nest. Constructed of soil, excrement, and saliva, nests
vary in location from completely subterranean, to inside wood, to entirely arboreal.
The most conspicuous nests are begun underground and as they grow begin to pro-
trude progressively, eventually assuming impressive species-characteristic shapes.
Although termites are sometimes called ‘white ants’ they are not at all related to
ants. In a very literal sense, termites are ‘social cockroaches’ that have attained their
eusociality from a base extremely remote in evolution from the Hymenoptera, yet
surprisingly the differences between these eusocial groups are not as great as one
426 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Similarities
1. Caste number and kind are analogous, especially between termites and ants.
2. Trophallaxis occurs, and is an important mechanism in social regulation
3. Chemical trails are used in recruitment; comparable trail laying and following behaviors
4. Inhibitory caste pheromones exist, and act in similar ways
5. Frequent grooming between individuals transmits pheromones
6. Nest odor and territoriality are of general occurrence
7. Comparably complex nest structures have precise temperature and humidity regulation
8. Cannibalism is widespread, but not universal
Differences
1. Castes in ‘lower’ termites are 1. Castes are determined chiefly by nutrition; pheromones
determined chiefly by sometimes play a role
pheromones; sex and other
factors are involved in some
‘higher’ termites
2. Worker castes consist of both 2. Worker castes consist of females only
sexes
3. Larvae and nymphs contribute to 3. Larvae and pupae are helpless and almost never
colony labor, at least in later contribute to colony labor
instars
4. No dominance hierarchies occur 4. Dominance hierarchies are commonplace but not
among individuals in the same universal
colonies
5. Social parasitism between 5. Social parasitism between species is common and
species is almost wholly absent widespread
6. Anal trophallaxis occurs in all 6. Anal trophallaxis is rare, but trophic eggs are exchanged
‘lower’ termites, but trophic in many bees and ants
eggs are unknown
7. Primary reproductive male stays 7. One or more males mate with the queen during the
with queen after nuptial flight, nuptial flight and die soon afterward; female stores
helps construct first nest. No gametes in spermatheca for up to several years
mating during nuptial flight;
mating occurs periodically as
colony develops.
8. Diploid sex determination 8. Haplodiploid sex determination
might expect (Table 10.2). The most striking differences in the two groups con-
cern the nature and care of their young. The Hymenoptera are holometabolous,
and because the larvae of eusocial Hymenoptera are helpless grubs that must be
nursed continuously, their care requires a great deal of adult labor. Termites are
hemimetabolous, so the young stages of termites, on the other hand, are active crea-
tures quite similar to the adults and quite able to fend for themselves, particularly
among the so-called ‘lower’ termites. Many termite workers are nymphs that may
eventually develop into winged adults. Others, termed pseudergates, are workers
that for chemical reasons will probably never grow up, although they retain the
10.3 The Insect Social Register 427
potential for developing into any of the castes. Thus, up to a point it may be said
that termites rely upon ‘child labor’ for the maintenance of their societies. Another
difference is that the male sex in termites participates in colony labor, which is never
true for male hymenopterans.
recruit soldiers to confront a threat. Galls clearly are resources that are generally
worth defending; in some cases, tending ants are also part of the ecological mix;
one study showed that excluding ants dramatically increased the aphids’ soldier to
non-soldier ratio.
One more unusual social insect that deserves mention is the Australian beetle
Platypus (=Austroplatypus) incompertus. Unlike most bark beetles that attack dead
or dying hosts, this ambrosia beetle attacks the heartwood of living Eucalyptus trees,
forming galleries that sometimes persist for several years. Each gallery houses a
cooperative family group composed of a single fertilized female, accompanied by
several unfertilized females that maintain and patrol the galleries and process the
mycelia of ambrosia fungi, ensuring a rich food supply.
eggs on themselves. Furthermore, there is little reason for golden egg bug
females to show preferences in terms of the sex of the carrier or the degree
of relatedness between egg and carrier simply because any conspecific car-
rier probably increases the chances that eggs will survive by about the same
amount. However, unlike the situation in water bugs, golden egg bug males
did not appear to be carrying their own offspring either. Samples sent to
Stockholm University for molecular genetic studies showed that egg-laden
males had fathered only a very small percentage of the eggs they carried.
Furthermore, hundreds of hours of observation did not reveal any type of
parental nurturing analogous to the behaviors of male water bugs.
Thus, Smith and Kaitala concluded, although it has obvious benefits, egg-
carrying in Phyllomorpha laciniata appears to be a purely passive behavior,
rather than parental care, at least at this point in time. Perhaps, however, its
imperfection is a clue that we are being given a rare glimpse at something we
almost never see—an evolutionary work in progress.
Over the past seventy years, it has become increasingly apparent that the evolution of
parental care—an important behavior both in and of itself, and as a key component
of more comprehensive social life—correlates with many aspects of an organ-
ism’s ecological milieu. A number of general aspects of the physical and biological
10.4 Implications and Correlates of Social Life 431
environment have been identified as being particularly important, not only among
insects but also among a wide variety of vertebrates that practice parental care. It has
been suggested that two extremes are likely to push animals toward parental care:
a very stable environment and an unusually harsh one; let us follow the reasoning
behind this idea.
The first of these, a stable, predictable environment, favors the evolution of
parental behaviors because the K-selection (see Chapter 9) that tends to prevail in
these conditions has certain demographic consequences: the animal tends to live
longer, to grow larger, and to reproduce at intervals. As an example, consider rotting
logs on a forest floor; these would seem to fit the stable, relatively constant environ-
ment criterion. Two lineages of cockroaches have evolved extended bi-parental care
of slow-growing, highly dependent offspring in such an environment; the most thor-
oughly studied exemplars are members of the genera Panesthia and Cryptocercus
(see Plate 39).
Extended bi-parental care has also evolved independently in Odontotaenius
(=Popilius) disjunctus, a large, glossy black beetle in the family Passalidae (see Fig.
10.12). The young of most cockroaches and beetles can forage independently from
an early age; they are analogous to many types of precocial birds that begin to peck
and feed themselves soon after hatching. The young of these three wood-feeding
groups of insects are more like the altricial young of robins and wrens—reared in
small slow developing clutches, highly dependent on their parents to provide food
and protection, and unable to forage for themselves. Both of the wood-feeding cock-
roaches require the help of bacterial symbionts to digest their cellulose diet; the
parents produce bacteria-rich soups to inoculate the digestive systems of nymphs
that line up like pigs at the food trough to imbibe the hindgut fluids. In contrast,
unlike most other wood-consuming species, bess beetles apparently lack digestive
symbionts. Instead, an important part of their diet is fecal frass, which acts as a
substrate for bacterial and fungal development and is reingested. Adult bess bee-
tles live two or more years; between rearing their yearly generations of young,
they are somewhat gregarious, many adults being found in the same tunnel sys-
tem. However, during the reproductive season, each pair of beetles maintains its
own tunnel system in which 20–60 eggs are laid. The developing young feed on
material prepared by their parents, and when ready to pupate they cooperate with
the adults in construction of pupal chambers.
A second circumstance that may propel an animal toward parental care is pen-
etration of a physically stressful environment where offspring would require some
sort of protection, at least during the most vulnerable period of their development.
In extremely harsh environments, many insect species guard their offspring. For
example, the staphylinid beetle, Bledius spectabilis, occupies intertidal mud; the
female of this beetle constructs a bottle-shaped burrow in which eggs are laid. She
remains with her eggs and young, twice each day plugging the burrow entrance to
prevent the tide from entering, then reopening it to prevent the young from suffo-
cating; this care continues until the larvae complete their first instar and disperse to
establish their own individual burrows. Eggs in burrows from which the mother was
removed all perished, either to mold or to predation by other beetles.
432 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Many ecological factors are probably involved in every insect’s life, and there is
often considerable challenge in finding what factors actually lie behind an observed
case of parental care. For example, adults of the tropical stink bug Antiteuchus
tripterus possess potent chemical defenses and are aposematically colored. They
generally live relatively long and are able to produce young two or three times dur-
ing their life. However, their offspring start life quite vulnerable to predation and
parasitism because their stink glands do not become functional until they reach their
second instar. William Eberhardt’s studies of maternal care in this species (Case
Study 10.4) showed how parental behavior and ecology are well correlated, as pre-
dicted, with risks of predation and parasitism acting as important selective factors.
Stink bugs are aptly named. Their distinctive odor, derived from abdominal
stink glands, is familiar to most everyone. Less well known is the extent to
which some mother stink bugs care for their young.
Antiteuchus tripterus is a tropical pentatomid bug common on a variety
of landscape trees and shrubs in urban areas of Cali, Columbia. It lays its
barrel-shaped, unusually thin-shelled eggs, in compact masses almost always
numbering 28, on the undersurface of leaves. For 15–16 days following
oviposition, females remain with their clutch without feeding while the eggs
develop and hatch and the nymphs grow to reach the second instar. Such
parental behavior is not uncommon in the Pentatomidae, and representatives
of several genera have been reported to have similar behavior. Why? William
(Bill) Eberhard reasoned that the parent bug was defending her eggs against
predators. His first experiment was simple. He removed brooding females
from some batches of eggs while leaving others nearby untouched to serve
as controls. Not one bug survived from 48 unguarded egg masses, most of
which vanished, whereas 50% of the protected eggs produced bugs! Later
observations revealed that several species of insects, especially foraging ants,
fed on undefended eggs. Thus, the females’ defense of their eggs against such
‘generalized’ predators appeared to be highly effective, and necessary.
Nevertheless, a 50% mortality of the guarded eggs seemed surprisingly
high, considering the time investment of the mothers. Eberhard presented arti-
ficial insect-sized models to brooding females. These elicited a repertoire of
defensive behaviors, including waving antennae in the direction of the stim-
ulus, tilting the body to form a shield between the threat and the nymphs,
general body ‘shuddering,’ scraping of the front legs along the periphery of
the egg mass, and kicking backward with the middle and hind legs. Although
like all other stink bugs Antiteuchus possessed glands which could spray a
highly repellent chemical, this defense was never employed against the insect-
sized stimulus. Eberhard assumed that such chemical defense was reserved
10.4 Implications and Correlates of Social Life 433
for larger potential predators, for when the body was tilted for spraying, the
bugs also vividly displayed aposematic orange bands along the sides of their
abdomens.
Attempting to rear the eggs, Eberhard discovered that a number of them
yielded, instead of bugs, tiny scelionid parasitoid wasps of two different
species. Why were they so successful despite constant maternal vigilance?
Stereotypy in maternal orientation provided a clue. Antiteuchus females con-
sistently aligned their bodies parallel with the axis of the leaf upon which their
eggs were laid, generally facing toward the leaf tip. Observing them care-
fully when their overt behavior (kicking, scraping, etc.) indicated they were
aware of the presence of a wasp attacking their eggs, Eberhard was unable
to document a single instance when a mother bug turned and assumed a new
orientation over its eggs. The consequence of this constant maternal orienta-
tion behavior (Fig. 10.24) was that, while females’ defenses did effectively
deter many wasp oviposition attempts in eggs situated on the front and sides
of the egg mass, eggs to her rear were not effectively guarded. Analysis of the
distribution of parasitized eggs within the mass confirmed this.
Fig. 10.24 Defending against attack. A female Antiteuchus tripterus in characteristic guarding
position over her eggs. As the bug lowers her antennae and scrapes with her front foot along the
egg mass in an attempt to drive off Phanuropsis, a parasitic wasp which attacks from the front, a
Trissolcus wasp oviposits in an egg at the poorly guarded rear edge
Would wasps attack more if eggs were not guarded at all? To exclude the
foraging ants and other walking predators that had decimated unguarded eggs
in his previous experiment, Eberhard ringed the trunk and branches of the host
tree with a sticky barrier. One of the scelionid wasps, Trissolcus, attacked the
eggs at the same rate in the mother’s absence. However, removing the mother
434 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Because all living members of both the termites and the ants are totally eusocial,
they can tell us little about the origins of social behavior. Attention has therefore
centered about the bees and wasps, since both of these major groups exhibit a range
of stages that suggest a ‘progression’ to eusociality. The other orders with eusocial
species also display a range of solitary to cooperative behaviors, making them a
rich resource for comparative study as well. All of these suggest that social groups
can form either by cooperation among adults of the same generation or via parent-
offspring (family group) associations (Fig. 10.25). The latter is thought to be the
most common pathway, and formed the cornerstone of William Morton Wheeler’s
treatises on social insects in the 1920s.
Regardless of which evolutionary route has been taken, however, the resultant
society seems to present an evolutionary paradox in the existence of altruism, or
self-sacrificial behaviors. One example is the awesome willingness of many bees
to throw themselves into suicidal battle (Fig. 10.26). Charles Darwin wrestled with
another aspect of the paradox: if natural selection favors the individual able to pro-
duce the greatest number of viable offspring that live to reproduce, then how can
one explain worker sterility? Social insects must represent a special case, he rea-
soned, an example where natural selection was operating on the level of the colony,
or family group, rather than on the single organism.
Group selection is defined as the differential survival and reproduction of
entire cooperative groups. Perhaps the foremost later proponent of group selec-
tion was a Scottish scientist, Vera C. Wynne-Edwards. In an influential 1962 book,
10.4 Implications and Correlates of Social Life 435
Fig. 10.26 Injecting venom. The ultimate self-sacrifice is to be sterile and die defending one’s
home. Honey bees have evolved a barbed sting; upon delivery, it remains in the victim and
disembowels the bee, resulting in the bee’s death
Table 10.3 Maximal average coefficients of relationship (r) among close kin due to haplodiploidy
in Hymenoptera
more closely related to her full sisters than she is to her own mother, a biologically
unusual situation. Just as unusual is the male hymenopteran. He has a grandfather
but no father. Because he receives all his genes—in one of two alleles—from his
diploid mother, he shares an average of one half of his genes with his brothers
but possesses only an average of one fourth of his genes in common with his
sisters.
In theory, an individual’s own altruistic sacrifice in fitness could be counterbal-
anced by an increase in the fitness of some group of relatives. Hamilton proposed
that this increase must be by a factor greater than the reciprocal of the coefficient of
relationship to that group (i.e., greater than 1/r) if an altruistic trait is to spread. In
the majority of animals, when an altruistic female sacrifices her life or reproductive
success for a sister or daughter, there is one half chance that the latter shares the gene
for that altruistic trait. For it to be statistically probable that the altruistic gene will
be fixed, the reproductive success of the sister or daughter must be at least doubled
as a result of the sacrifice. However, among full sisters where there is a three quar-
ter chance that the gene is shared, altruistic behavior is statistically probable when
the recipient’s gain in fitness is equal to only four thirds, or 1.33, times the donor’s
loss. Hamilton’s Rule is usually stated as br-c>0, or more simply as br>c, where b
= benefit to the recipient, c = cost to the actor altruist, and r is the coefficient of
relationship between the two (which ranges from 0 to 1).
Thus, because of haplodiploidy, when a hymenopteran mother lives to reproduce
beyond the adulthood of her first female offspring, these offspring may increase
their inclusive fitness more by care of their younger sisters than by an equal amount
of care given to their own offspring. Since sisters are more closely related than are
brothers, altruism will be more favored among daughters than among sons. Haploid
males have more to gain by fathering daughters, with whom they share an average
of one half of their genes, than by assisting in the production of more sisters, with
whom they share an average of only one fourth of their genes.
Thysanoptera are also haplodiploid and since 1990 a number of eusocial thrips
have been discovered (see Section 10.3.5). They differ in that soldiers of both
sexes actively defend the colony and can also reproduce. Colonies of some
soldier-producing species have been found to have extremely high levels of inbreed-
ing; this reduces the relatedness asymmetry between brothers and sisters. Also,
unlike the hymenopteran sting that can only be possessed by the female gender,
438 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
spines and enlarged forelegs found in thrips soldiers are not sexually dimorphic
traits.
All other things being equal, it would seem that Hymenoptera should all tend to
become social! Yet the truth is that although apparently all of the thousands of soli-
tary and parasitic Hymenoptera species have this same haplodiploid reproductive
system, most show no trace of sociality. Why?
As new technology has made relatedness one of the easier things to quantify
among social group members, the striking conclusion from numerous studies is
that there is no overwhelming link between sociality and degree of relatedness.
Thus, while genetic relationships and hence genetic mechanisms may be involved
in furthering the initial evolution of cooperative behavior, other factors must also be
considered to be important.
What might such factors be? Searching for the earliest stages of social evolu-
tion has traditionally focused on groups such as halictid bees or the aculeate wasps,
because in these sociality has risen both relatively recently and repeatedly, whereas
in the termites or ants all known species are eusocial and are derived from stocks that
evolved eusociality long ago. However, when we step back and look at the distribu-
tion of eusociality, we find that it has arisen independently at least 13 times, mostly
among the arthropods (including once in a crustacean) but also at least once in a
vertebrate, the naked mole rat. At the same time it is rather striking that eusociality
is relatively rare, considering the huge diversity of arthropods. Pressures inhibit-
ing the attainment of eusociality must be extraordinary. On the flip side, one must
envision some equally extraordinary set of conditions favoring flexibility that could
foster cooperative breeding to overcome environmental contingencies and individ-
ual selfishness. Such a scenario seems to require selection to operate at multiple
levels—genes, individuals and groups—in context-dependent ways.
A revolution in thinking about the origin and evolution of eusociality has
occurred over the past decade. Facilitated by the tremendous progress in whole-
genome sequencing and comparative genomics through various large-scale tech-
nologies (proteomics, epigenomics, etc.), the revolution has been driven in part from
new knowledge about the interaction of genes and the environment. It is increasingly
possible to probe relations between genes and social behavior. Several examples of
social influences on gene expression in insects are now documented. One instance
(see Case Study 1.1) involves shifts in the expression of large populations of genes in
honey bee brains that relate to the point at which hive bees switch to foraging outside
the nest. In particular, differences in the foraging (for) gene expression are related to
social activity, rather than to genetic differences between individuals. Thus, emer-
gent traits arising from interactions among group members constitute a new frontier
for ultimately understanding the social evolution puzzle.
The significance of quantifying relatedness as it relates to the evolution of euso-
ciality is also being reevaluated. Emphasis has shifted to the other parameters of
Hamilton’s Rule: costs and benefits of group life. Besides haplodiploidy, several
other preadaptations are now thought likely to be equally or even more important
for the evolution of eusocial behavior (Table 10.4). Previously existing behavior
patterns, physiological processes, and morphological structures that are already
10.4 Implications and Correlates of Social Life 439
Table 10.4 Some preadaptations that are considered to be important for development of insect
sociality
functional in some other context may serve as bridges to new adaptations. For exam-
ple, the reproductive ground plan hypothesis proposes that the gene network that
once regulated the foraging and reproductive phases that coexisted in solitary ances-
tors was modified in the process of evolution to form the basis for the caste divisions
that are evident in today’s social insects. Thus, queens express genetic traits that
are characteristic of the reproductive phase of solitary insects, whereas workers
express traits characteristic of the foraging phase. A modification of this hypoth-
esis suggests that in some wasps at least, the co-opted gene regulatory networks
were instead from ancestors with two summer generations; workers evolved from
networks switched on in early-emerging first generation individuals, whereas queens
evolved from networks switched on in late-emerging, second-generation individuals
that diapause.
Certainly, many studies have illustrated that relatedness alone is not necessarily
the best predictor of colony success. For example, comparisons of Florida harvester
ant colonies (Pogonomyrmex occidentalis) revealed that colonies with relatively
low relatedness among the workers had strikingly higher colony growth and repro-
duction rates than those with high worker relatedness. Structured kin groups like
harvester ants illustrate that genetic variation between colonies may be greater and
of more evolutionary significance than within-colony variation. It is upon this inter-
colony variation responding differentially to environmental or genetic factors (e.g.,
disease resistance) that group selection processes can act, resulting in some groups
being better adapted than others (Fig. 10.27). Indeed, the principal selective regime
440 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Fig. 10.27 Selecting groups. The group selection model for the evolution of eusociality. Some
individuals in an initial population possess eusocial-prescribing alleles (black thorax) that induce
cooperation, but whose phenotypic expression is flexible (curved and knobbed antennae). Common
possession of eusociality alleles trumps relatedness in the early stages; subsequent selection based
on environmental factors sorts subsequent groups without requiring a kin network. Higher related-
ness in downstream populations can arise by further group selection with or without kin selection;
thus relatedness is more a consequence than a cause of eusociality
Scaptotrigona stingless bees make a living by plundering the nests of other species,
stealing the honey and provisions for their own nest. Queens of Vespula squamosa
intrude into established nests of Vespula maculifrons and either seize the queen’s
position or lay their own queen-producing eggs alongside those of the host, to be
raised by the host’s worker force.
In Chapter 4, we considered a number of symbiotic interspecific alliances in the
context of feeding strategy. Here, we focus on some exploitive interrelationships
between fully social species. Nest building and the care and feeding of an individ-
ual’s young are usually thought to be very conservative, physiologically deep-seated
10.4 Implications and Correlates of Social Life 441
behavior patterns. However, even these may be selected against if increased fitness
results.
For example, various solitary Hymenoptera species (see Fig. 4.8) have lost the
behaviors of nest building and provisioning and rely entirely upon the nests and
labors of others. Social parasitism occurs when the offspring of one species are
raised by another species. Among the eusocial Hymenoptera, it has evolved inde-
pendently many times and in many ways. Ants show a greater diversity of forms of
social parasitism than any other group of animals, with over 160 species of social
parasites, no two of which are exactly alike in their adaptations for this mode of life.
The term ‘social parasite’ is unfortunately somewhat misleading, because the
parasite does not need to be social nor does the label refer to every parasite of a
social species. Rather, a social parasite is a species that uses its host as a work force
(something social insect species are particularly suited for) rather than as a direct
source of food. However, because many social parasites are closely related to their
hosts, they often are social themselves. In its most extreme forms, social parasitism
is a condition of permanent and complete dependence, in which the parasite’s entire
life cycle is carried out within the host’s nest. Commonly, when the parasite is a
social species, its own workers are nonexistent or conspicuously degenerate. Such
extreme social parasites, or inquilines, appear to have arisen convergently via at
least three evolutionary routes.
The name Teleutomyrmex means ‘final ant,’ a name most appropriate for what
is probably the ultimate social symbiont. In the Swiss and French Alps, one may
find this ant in small isolated populations that include no workers. Although quite
an assemblage of ant species surround it, Teleutomyrmex is parasitic upon only
one—its closest phylogenetic neighbor, Tetramorium. It has never been found out-
side the nests of its hosts. Teleutomyrmex queens spend much of their time riding
upon the backs of the queens of their host colony. Very delicate, the symbionts sel-
dom move independently; they apparently feed only upon regurgitates passed from
workers to the host queen. Placed in an artificial nest they cannot survive, even
if host workers are present. Their brains, mandibles, nervous system, and skin, in
fact nearly all of their morphology, shows extensive degeneration, except in their
reproductive system—each of the tiny physogastric parasites lays an average of one
egg every 30 seconds. Having up to six or eight ectoparasitic ants riding upon her
back may slow down a host queen; infested colonies tend to be somewhat smaller
than non-infested ones. Significantly, infested colonies produce no sexual forms
of the host. Teleutomyrmex adults, especially older females, apparently produce
a very potent attractant; host workers lick them continuously. As it is circulated
throughout the colony this substance may impose ‘reproductive castration’ upon its
hosts. Such a castration phenomenon has been demonstrated in a number of similar
parasite–host colony relationships, though its physiological mechanism has yet to be
determined.
Two more examples illustrate the diversity of social inquilinism. One involves
slavery. Red Amazon ants (Polyergus rufescens) with their saberlike mandibles are
fierce fighters but totally inept housekeepers. In their home nest, their only activities
are grooming themselves and begging for food. Living as a pure colony, they would
surely perish, because they neither excavate nests nor care for their own young.
442 10 Parental Behaviors and Social Life
Fig. 10.28 Raiding the neighbors. Amazon ants of the species Polyergus rufescens (light) con-
ducting a raid upon a colony of the slave species Formica fusca (dark) nesting in dry soil beneath
a stone. Killing resistors by piercing them with their saber-like mandibles, the Amazons rush off
with captured brood. Such behavior occurs quite often among cold temperate ant species, where
slave labor is found in at least 35 species from six independently evolved groups
What keeps them alive is a unique slave-making habit. Periodically, the Amazon
ants swarm out of their nest, marching swiftly to the nests of Formica fusca to
launch a raid (Fig. 10.28). With feverish haste, they pour over the colony in a body;
any defenders who resist the attack are punctured and killed. Then, like pillaging
soldiers, the Amazons proceed to carry off fusca pupal cocoons.
Back in the Amazon nest, the pupae soon begin to hatch; genetically programmed
to perform various housekeeping tasks, they begin work. Some bring food into
the nest, while others tend the eggs, larvae, and pupae of their captors. Still oth-
ers actually feed the adult Amazons who have made them slaves, responding to
their begging by regurgitating liquid droplets. Throughout all this activity, the fusca
slaves make no distinction between their genetic siblings and the Amazons, fully
accepting their captors as sisters. Eventually, their numbers dwindle, because as
members of the worker caste they cannot reproduce. In response, the slave-making
Amazons set out to pillage alien colonies once again.
The nest-raiding techniques of slave makers are among the most sophisticated
behavior patterns found in the insect world. Some physically overpower their vic-
tims. Others depend on various chemical ruses. Workers of two species of Formica
spray acetate chemicals at resisting nest defenders; these chemicals act as ‘pro-
paganda substances’ that imitate the alarm pheromones of the slave species so
powerfully they throw the resistors into utter helpless panic. In addition to this
disruptive effect, the acetates serve as an attractant to workers of the slave maker,
quickly assembling them where fighting has broken out.
A second example shifts our focus to queen behavior. Sexual forms of most social
Hymenoptera leave their home colony for their mating flights. Some mated queens
fail to find their own colony again following such flights, or after hibernation fail
to successfully initiate their own nest the following season. Should they locate a
colony of another species and somehow eliminate the host queen, however, they
gain an established nest complete with a work force. For a brief period, when the
10.4 Implications and Correlates of Social Life 443
usurper queen has begun to reproduce but not all the original queen’s brood have
reached old age and died, the colony consists of workers of both species. Eventually,
however, the nest will come to contain only the usurper and her offspring. This
situation, termed temporary parasitism may evolve from a facultative affair to an
obligatory one where usurper queens depend entirely upon this method to found new
colonies. Temporary parasites have increased their chances of successfully entering
the host nest in a variety of ways, from stealth to aggression to deceptions involving
chemistry, morphology, or behaviors such as ‘playing dead.’
The apparent ease by which symbionts have repeatedly intruded themselves
into their host’s colony is somewhat surprising, particularly in view of the excep-
tional ability of social insects to defend themselves from larger predators. The
clue to understanding this paradox lies in the organization and integration of insect
societies. As stressed in Chapter 6, insect societies are integrated primarily by non-
personal colony odors, an easier code to break than the visually based individual
recognition or learned roles and dominance-based interactions characteristic of ver-
tebrate societies. Furthermore, organization into castes results in role specialization
in which the individuals of one caste largely lack a broad awareness of the roles
of other colony members. These combine to lend an impersonal nature to social
insect organizations, which has apparently made it relatively effortless for social
symbionts to insert themselves into the colony regime.
Credits
Chapter One
Chapter Two
Fig. 2.2 From Wilson, D. M. 1968. The flight-control system of the locust.
Scientific American 218:83–90 (May). Copyright © (1968) by Scientific American,
Inc. All rights reserved. See also Marder, E., D. Bucher, D. Schulz, et al. 2003.
Invertebrate central pattern generator moves along. Current Biology
15(17):R685–R699.
Fig. 2.3 (above) Redrawn after Roeder, K. D. 1967. Nerve Cells and Insect
Behavior, revised edition. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. (below)
From Camhi, J. M. 1980. The escape system of the cockroach. Scientific American
243:151–172. Copyright © (1980) by Scientific American, Inc. All rights reserved.
See also Levi, R. and J. M. Camhi. 2000. Wind direction coding in the cockroach
escape response: winner does not take all. Journal of Neurosciences
20(10):3814–3821.
Fig. 2.4 Modified from Dethier, V. G. 1971. A surfeit of stimuli: a paucity of
receptors. American Scientist 59:706–715.
Fig. 2.5 Roeder, K. D. 1970. Episodes in insect brains. American Scientist
58:378–389. Reprinted by permission, American Scientist, journal of Sigma Xi,
The Scientific Research Society of North America.
Fig. 2.6 From Alcock, J. 1975 Animal Behavior. An Evolutionary Approach. 1st
ed. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.
Fig. 2.7 (above) Redrawn after Rains, G. C., J. K. Tomberlin, and D. Salasiri.
2008. Using insect sniffing devices for detection. Trends in Biotechnology 26(6):
288–294. (below) From Olson, D. M., G. C. Rains, T. Meiners, et al. 2003.
Parasitic wasps learn and report diverse chemicals with unique conditionable
behaviors. Chemical Senses 28: 545–549. See also Salazar, B. A. and D. W.
Whitman. 2001. Defensive tactics of caterpillars against predators and parasitoids.
Chapter 8 In T. N. Ananthakrishnan, Insects and Plant Defence Dynamics. Science
Publishers, Enfield, NH, USA.
Fig. 2.8 Reproduced with permission from Lent, D. D. and H-W. Kwon. 2004.
Antennal movements reveal associative learning in the American cockroach
Periplaneta americana. Journal of Experimental Biology 207: 369–375.
Fig. 2.9 Tinbergen, N. 1951. The Study of Instinct. Clarendon Press of the Oxford
University Press, London. By permission of Oxford University Press. See also
Tinbergen, N. 1972. The Animal in its World. Explorations of an Ethologist,
1932–1972. Vol. 1. Field Studies (especially pp. 103–145). Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Fig. 2.10 Drawing by Charles Clare from Janzen, D. H. 1974. The deflowering of
Central America. Natural History 83:48–53.
Fig. 2.11 (above, center) Modified from Gandolfi, M., L. Mattiacci and S. Dorn.
2003. Preimaginal learning determines adult response to chemical stimuli in a
parasitic wasp. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B 270:2623–2629.
448 Credits
Chapter Three
Fig. 3.1 Drawings by Paul H. Matthews.
Fig. 3.2 Photograph courtesy of Poramate Manoonpong, Bernstein Center for
Computational Neuroscience, Goettingen, Germany.
Fig. 3.3 Drawing by the authors. See also Dickinson, M. 2005. Insect flight.
Current Biology 16(9): R309–314.
Fig. 3.4 Drawing by Joan W. Krispyn. See also Fraenkel, G. S. and D. L. Gunn.
1940. The Orientation of Animals. Kineses, Taxes and Compass Reactions. Dover,
NY.
Fig. 3.5 Redrawn and modified from Schöne, H. 1951. Die Lichtorientierung der
Larven von Acilius sulcatus L. und Dytiscus marginalis L. Zeitschrift Vergleichen
Physiologie 33:63–98, with kind permission of Springer Science+Business
Media.
Fig. 3.6 From Alcock, J. 1975. Animal Behavior. 1st ed. Sinauer Press,
Sunderland, MA. See also Lindauer, M.1971. Communication Among the Bees.
Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Fig. 3.7. Photographs courtesy of Prof. Dr. Helmut Schmitz, Universität Bonn,
Institut für Zoologie. See also Schmitz, H, H. Bleckmann, and M. Murtz. 1997.
Infrared detection in a beetle. Nature (Lond.) 386:773–774.
Fig. 3.8 Photograph by the authors.
Fig. 3.9 Redrawn after Dingle, H. 1972. Migration strategies of insects. Science
175:1327–1334. See also Dingle, H. 1996. Migration. The Biology of Life on the
Move. Oxford University Press, New York.
Credits 449
Chapter Four
Fig. 4.1 Photograph by the authors.
Fig. 4.2 Photograph by the authors.
Fig. 4.3 Stoffolano, J. G. Jr. 1974. Control of feeding and drinking in diapausing
insects. In L. Barton Browne (Ed.), Experimental Analysis of Insect Behaviour,
Springer Verlag, NY, with kind permission of Springer Science+Business Media.
Fig. 4.4 Weires, R. W. and H. G. Chiang, 1973. Integrated control prospects of
major cabbage insect pests in Minnesota-based on the faunistic, host varietal, and
trophic relationships. University of Minnesota Agricultural Experiment Station
Technical Bulletin 291, 42 pp.
Fig. 4.5 Diagram by the authors. See also Charnov, E.L. 1976. Optimal foraging:
the marginal value theorem. Theoretical Population Biology 9:129–136.
Fig. 4.6 From Alcock, J. 1975. Animal Behavior. An Evolutionary Approach.
1st ed. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland, MA.
Fig. 4.7 Photograph by the authors.
Fig. 4.8 Drawing by Joan W. Krispyn.
Fig. 4.9 Courtesy of Ulrich G. Mueller. From Mueller, U. G. and N. Gerardo.
2002. Fungus-farming insects: multiple origins and diverse evolutionary histories.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Science 99(24):15247–15249.
Fig. 4.10 Courtesy of Bert Hölldobler. From Hölldobler, B.1971. Communication
between ants and their guests. Scientific American 224:86–93 (March). Copyright
© (1971) by Scientific American, Inc. All rights reserved.
450 Credits
Chapter Five
Opening quote from Owen, D. 1980. Camouflage and Mimicry, p. 15. University
of Chicago Press.
Fig. 5.1 Photographs by Robert E. Silberglied.
Fig. 5.2 From Cott, H. B. 1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. Methuen, NY,
with kind permission of Springer Science+Business Media.
Fig. 5.3 Kettlewell, H. B. D. 1973. The Evolution of Melanism. The Study of a
Recurring Necessity, with Special Reference to Industrial Melanism in the
Lepidoptera. Clarendon, NY. See also Hopper, J. 2003. Of Moths and Men: An
Evolutionary Tale: The Untold Story of Science and the Peppered Moth. W. W.
Norton & Co., NY.
Fig. 5.4 Courtesy of Terrence D. Fitzgerald. See also Fitzgerald, T. D. 2008. Toxic
hairs enable some caterpillars to venture forth in conspicuous processions. Natural
History 117(7):28–33.
Credits 451
Fig. 5.5 (left) Photograph by Douglas W. Whitman. (right) From Greene E., L. J.
Orsak, D. Whitman. 1987. A tephritid fly mimics the territorial displays of its
jumping spider predators. Science 236:310–312. Reprinted with permission from
AAAS.
Fig. 5.6 Photographs by the authors.
Fig. 5.7 Photograph by Robert E. Silberglied.
Fig. 5.8 (left) Photograph by the authors. (right) Photograph by Douglas W.
Whitman.
Fig. 5.9 (left) Photograph by Abraham Hefetz. (right). Photograph courtesy of
Thomas Eisner. See also Aneshansley, D., T. Eisner, J. M. Widom et al. 1969.
Biochemistry at 100o C. The explosive discharge of bombardier beetles
(Brachinus). Science 165:61–63.
Fig. 5.10 Photograph by Robert E. Silberglied.
Fig. 5.11 (above) Photograph by the authors. (center and below) Photographs by
Robert E. Silberglied.
Fig. 5.12 Photograph by the authors.
Fig. 5.13 From Stradling, D. J. 1976. The nature of the mimetic patterns of the
brassolid genera, Caligo and Eryphanus. Ecological Entomology 1:135–138. By
permission of Wiley-Blackwell.
Fig. 5.14 From Cott, H. B.1940. Adaptive Coloration in Animals. Methuen, NY,
with kind permission of Springer Science+Business Media.
Fig. 5.15 Drawing courtesy of Daniel Otte. See also Otte, D. 1977. Acoustical
communication in Orthoptera. In T. A. Sebeok (Ed.), How Animals Communicate.
Indiana University Press, Bloomington, IN.
Chapter Six
Fig. 6.4 Photograph by Kevin Wanner, Montana State University. See also
Steinbrecht, R. A. 1999. Olfactory receptors. pp. 155–176 In E. Eguchi,
Y. Tominaga & H. Ogawa, Atlas of Arthropod Sensory Receptors. Dynamic
Morphology in Relation to Function. Springer-Verlag.
Fig. 6.5 Adapted from Birch, M. C. 1984. Aggregation in bark beetles.
pp. 331–354 In W. J. Bell and R. T. Cardé (Eds.) Chemical Ecology of Insects,
Chapman and Hall, London, with kind permission of Springer Science+Business
Media.
Fig. 6.6 Diagram by the authors.
Fig. 6.7 (left) Photograph by Eleanor Smithwick and U. Eugene Brady. (right)
Photograph by Rob Peakall. See also Schiestl F. P., R. Peakal, J. G. Mant, et al.
2003. The chemistry of sexual deception in an orchid-wasp pollination system.
Science 302(5644):437–438.
Fig. 6.8 Photograph by Robert L. Silberglied.
Fig. 6.9 Photograph by Patricia J. Moore. See also Moore, A. J. and P. J. Moore.
1999. Balancing sexual selection through opposing mate choice and male
competition. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B – Biological
Sciences. 266:711–716.
Fig. 6.10 Drawing by Lee C. Ryker. See also Ryker, L.C. 1984. Acoustic and
chemical signals in the life cycle of a beetle. Scientific American 250:113–124.
Fig. 6.11 From Ishii, S. 1970. Aggregation of the German cockroach Blattella
germanica L. pp. 93–109 In D. L. Wood, R. M. Silverstein, and M. Nakajima
(Eds.). Control of Insect Behavior by Natural Products, Academic Press, NY.
Fig. 6.12 From Beggs, K. T., K. A. Glendining, N. M. Marechal, et al. 2007.
Queen pheromone modulates brain dopamine function in worker honey bees.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the US. 104(7):2460–2464.
Copyright (2007) National Academy of Sciences, USA.
Fig. 6.13 (left) Drawing by Paul H. Matthews. (right) Photograph by Terrence D.
Fitzgerald. See also Fitzgerald, T. D. 1995. The Tent Caterpillars. Cornell
University Press, Ithaca, NY.
Fig. 6.14 Drawings by Joan W. Krispyn. See also Beale, M. H., M. A. Birkett,
T. J. A. Bruce, et al. 2006. Aphid alarm pheromone produced by transgenic plants
affects aphid and parasitoid behavior. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences of the USA 103(27):10509–10513.
Fig. 6.15 Courtesy of CSIRO Division of Entomology, Canberra, Australia.
Fig. 6.16 From Price, P.W. 1972. Behavior of the parasitoid Pleolophus basizonus
(Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae) in response to changes in host and parasitoid
density. Canadian Entomologist 104:129–140. Courtesy of the Entomological
Society of Canada.
Credits 453
Chapter Seven
Chapter Eight
Fig. 8.1 Courtesy of Rex Cocroft. From Cocroft, R. B. and R. L. Rodríguez. 2005.
The behavioral ecology of insect vibrational communication. BioScience
55(4):323–334. Copyright, American Institute of Biological Sciences.
Fig. 8.2 Photograph by Douglas W. Whitman.
Fig. 8.3 Courtesy of Rex Cocroft. From Cocroft, R. B., T. D. Tieu, R. R. Hoy,
et al. 2000. Directionality in the mechanical response to substrate vibration in a
trehopper (Hemiptera:Membracidae: Umbonia crassicornis). Journal of
Comparative Physiology A 186:695–705.
Fig. 8.4 Drawing by Paul H. Matthews, based on Yak, J. and R. Hoy. 2003.
Hearing. pp. 498–505 In V. H. Resh and R. T. Cardé (Eds.), Encyclopedia of
Insects. Academic Press, New York.
Fig. 8.5 Drawing by the authors. See also Cooper, K. W. 1957. Biology of
eumenine wasps. V. Digital communication in wasps. Journal of Experimental
Zoology 134:469–514.
Fig. 8.6 Drawings by the authors.
Fig. 8.7 From Alexander, R. D. and T. E. Moore. 1962. The evolutionary
relationships of 17-year and 13-year cicadas, and three new species (Homoptera,
Cicadidae, Magicacada). Miscellaneous Publications of the Museum of Zoology,
Credits 455
University of Michigan No. 121, 59 pp. See also Cooley, J. R., G. Kritsky, M. J.
Edwards, et al. 2009. The distribution of periodical cicada brood X in 2004.
American Entomologist 55(2):106–113.
Fig. 8.8 Photograph by Justin O. Schmidt. See also Schmidt, J. O. and M. S. Blum.
1977. Adaptations and responses of Dasymutilla occidentalis (Hymeoptera:
Mutillidae) to predators. Entomologia Experimenta et Applicata 21:99–111.
Fig. 8.9 Courtesy of Richard D. Alexander.
Fig. 8.10 Stevenson P. A., Dyakonova, V., Rillich J., et al. 2005. Octopamine and
experience-dependent modulation of aggression in crickets. The Journal of
Neuroscience 25(6):1431–1441.
Fig. 8.11 Modified from Hoy, R., J. Hahn, and R. C. Paul. 1977. Hybrid cricket
auditory behavior: evidence for genetic coupling in animal communication.
Science 195:82–83. Reprinted with permission from AAAS.
Fig. 8.12 Modified from Gibson, G. and I. Russell. 2006. Flying in tune: sexual
recognition in mosquitoes. Current Biology 16(11):1311–1316.
Figs. 8.13 and 8.14 Drawings copyright Emily A. Matthews, based on Frisch,
K. von 1967. The Dance Language and Orientation of Bees. Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Fig. 8.15 From Michelsen, A., Andersen, B. B., Storm J., et al. 1992. How
honeybees perceive communication dances, studied by means of a mechanical
model. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 30(3–4):143–150. With kind
permission of Springer Science+Business Media.
Fig. 8.16 From Nieh, J. C. 1999. Stingless bee communication. American Scientist
87(5):428–435. Reprinted by permission of American Scientist, journal of Sigma
Xi, The scientific Research Society of North America.
Fig. 8.17 (above) Photographs by Thomas D. Seeley. (below) From Dyer, F. C.
2002. The biology of the dance language. Annual Review of Entomology
47:917–949. Reprinted, with permission, from the Annual Review of Entomology,
volume 47 © 2002 by Annual Reviews www.annualreviews.org.
Table 8.1 Based on DuMortier, B. 1963. Morphology of sound emission apparatus
in Arthropoda. pp. 277–345 In R. G. Busnel (Ed.), Acoustic Behaviour of Animals,
and on Haskell, P. T. 1974. Sound production. pp. 353–410 In M. Rockstein (ed.),
The Physiology of Insecta, Volume II, 2nd ed., Academic Press, NY. See also,
Bailey, W. J. 1991. Acoustic Behaviour of Insects. An evolutionary perspective.
Chapman and Hall, London.
Table 8.2 Based on Schwartzkopff, J. 1974. Mechanoreception. pp. 273–352 In
M. Rockstein (Ed.), The Physiology of Insecta, Volume II, 2nd ed., Academic
Press, NY. See also Ewing, A. W. 1989. Arthropod Bioacoustics. Neurobiology and
Behaviour. Comstock Press of Cornell University Press, Ithaca, NY.
456 Credits
Chapter Nine
Fig. 9.1 Drawing by Joan W. Krispyn. See also Thornhill, R. 1976. Reproductive
behavior of the lovebug, Plecia nearctica (Diptera: Bibionidae). Annals of the
Entomological Society of America 69:843–847.
Fig. 9.2 Drawing by Joan W. Krispyn.
Fig. 9.3 From Stone G. N., R. J. Atkinson, A. Rokas„ et al. 2008. Evidence for
widespread cryptic sexual generations in apparently purely asexual Andricus
gallwasps. Molecular Ecology 17:652–665.
Fig. 9.4 From Hardy, I. C. W., P. J. Ode, and M. T. Siva-Jothy. 2005. Mating
behavior. pp. 219–260 In M. A. Jarvis (Ed.), Insects as Natural Enemies: A
Practical Perspective. Springer, Dordrecht Netherlands, with kind permission of
Springer Science+Business Media.
Fig. 9.5 Drawing by Joan W. Krispyn. See also Stich, H. F. 1963. An experimental
analysis of the courtship pattern of Tipula oleracea (Diptera). Canadian Journal of
Zoology 41:99–109.
Fig. 9.6 From Dressler, R. L. 1968. Pollination by euglossine bees. Evolution
22:202–210.
Fig. 9.7 Photograph by Robert E. Silberglied.
Fig. 9.8 From Kullenberg B. and G. Bergström, 1976. Hymenoptera aculeata
males as pollinators of Ophrys orchids. Zoologica Scripta 5:13–23. By permission
of Wiley-Blackwell.
Fig. 9.9 Photograph by the authors. See also Crankshaw, O. S. and
R. W. Matthews. 1981. Sexual behavior among parasitic Megarhyssa wasps
(Hymenoptera: Ichneumonidae). Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 9:1–7.
Fig. 9.10 Drawing by Emile Blanchard, In Figuier, L. 1869. The Insect World.
Chapman and Hall, NY.
Fig. 9.11 From Otte, D. 1974. Effects and functions in the evolution of signaling
systems. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 5:385–417. Reprinted, with
permission, from the Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution, and Systematics,
volume 5 ©1974 by Annual Reviews www.annualreviews.org.
Fig. 9.12 Photograph by Garry Wall.
Fig. 9.13 Photograph by the authors. See also Matthews, R. W., J. M. González,
J. R. Matthews, et al. 2009. Biology of the parasitoid Melittobia (Hymenoptera:
Eulophidae). Annual Review of Entomology 54:251–266.
Fig. 9.14 Photograph courtesy of Randy Thornhill. See Thornhill, R. 1976. Sexual
selection and nuptial feeding behavior in Bittacus apicalis (Insecta: Mecoptera).
American Naturalist 110:529–548. See also Thronhill, R. and J. Alcock. 1983. The
Evolution of Insect Mating Systems. Harvard University Press., Cambridge, MA.
Credits 457
Chapter Ten
Fig. 10.5 Modified from Smith, R. L.1976. Male brooding behavior of the water
bug Abedus herberti (Hemiptera: Belostomatidae). Annals of the Entomological
Society of America 69:740–747.
Fig. 10.6 Photograph by Robert L. Smith. See also Smith, R. L. 1997. The
evolution of paternal care in the giant water bugs (Heteroptera: Belostomatidae).
pp.116–149 In J. C. Choe and B. J. Crespi (Eds.), The Evolution of Social Behavior
in Insects and Arachnids. Cambridge University
Press, UK.
Fig. 10.7 From West, M. J. and R. D. Alexander. 1963. Sub-social behavior in a
burrowing cricket, Anurogryllus muticus (De Geer). Orthoptera. Gryllidae. Ohio
Journal of Science 63:19–24.
Fig. 10.8 From Gullan, P. J. and P. S. Cranston. 2004. The Insects. An Outline of
Entomology, 3rd ed. Wiley-Blackwell, Oxford. By permission of Wiley-Blackwell
See also Halffter, G. 1997. Subsocial behavior in Scarabaeine beetles. pp. 237–259
In J. C. Choe and B. J. Crespi (Eds.), The Evolution of Social Behavior in Insects
and Arachnids. Cambridge University Press, UK.
Fig. 10.9 Drawing by Joan W. Krispyn.
Fig. 10.10 Drawings by Sarah Landry from Evans, H. E. and R. W. Matthews
1973. Systematics and nesting behavior of Australian Bembix sand wasps
(Hymenoptera: Sphecidae). Memoirs of the American Entomological Institute
20:1–386. See also Evans, H. E. and K. M. O’Neill. 2007. The Sand Wasps.
Natural History and Behavior. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA.
Fig. 10.11 From McCook, H. C. 1909. Ant Communities and How They Are
Governed, Harper & Brothers, NY.
Fig. 10.12 Photograph by the authors.
Fig. 10.13 (above) Photograph by Robert E. Silberglied. (below) Photograph by
Carl W. Rettenmeyer.
Fig. 10.14 Drawing by Turid Hölldobler-Forsyth, In Wilson, E. O. 1976. A social
ethogram of the Neotropical arboreal ant Zacryptocerus varians (Fr. Smith).
Animal Behaviour 24:354–363.
Fig. 10.15 From Tophoff, H. 1972. The social behavior of army ants. Scientific
American 227:70–79 (November). Copyright © (1972) by Scientific American,
Inc. All rights reserved. See also Gotwald, W. H. 1996. Army Ants: The Biology of
Social Predation. Comstock Press, Ithaca, NY.
Fig. 10.16 From West-Eberhard, M. J. 1969. The social biology of polistine wasps.
Miscellaneous Publications of the Museum of Zoology, University of Michigan
no. 140, 101 pp.
Fig. 10.17 (left) Drawing by Amy Bartlett Wright from Jeanne, R. L. 1991. The
swarm founding Polistinae. pp. 191–231. (right) Photograph by C. K. Starr from
Credits 459
Table 10.2 Based on Wilson, E. O. 1971. The Insect Societies. Harvard University
Press, Cambridge, MA.
Plates
Plate 1 Proximal analysis of honey bee foraging. (above) A foraging worker depends on its highly
developed spatial navigation abilities to search for pollen and nectar outside its hive or nest, return
home, and communicate this information to others. (below) Clusters of neurons called mushroom
bodies (shown in red) located at the top front of its brain are involved in spatial learning; the yellow
globes are the optic lobes of the bee brain
463
464 Plates
Plate 2 Amplifying power for a remarkable jump. As the flea crouches before takeoff, a resilin
pad (insert) at the base of the leg is squeezed and two cuticular catches are cocked. When these let
go, all of the energy imparted via the leg muscles is released from the pad in about a millisecond,
thrusting the flea’s hind trochanters against the substrate
Plates 465
Plate 3 Two monarch butterflies, Danaus plexippus, on their host plant, the milkweed Asclepias
tuberosa
466 Plates
Plate 4 Swarming behavior. (top) Two morphs of Schistocerca gregaria; the gregarious form is
on the left, the solitary form is on the right; (bottom left) locusts swarming in Africa; (bottom right)
a group of adults and nymphs feeding on cabbage in a laboratory colony
Plates 467
Plate 5 Aggregating for hibernation. (above) Paper wasps, Polistes carolina, move to sheltered
sites in the fall, then eventually disperse to establish new nests in the spring. (below) Some
coccinelliid beetle species gather in large numbers before hibernating, as this aggregation from
Arizona illustrates
468 Plates
Plate 6 Stylopsized paper wasp. Three strepsipteran parasites protrude from between the abdom-
inal segments of this Polistes exclamans worker. The parasites modify the behavior of the wasp to
their benefit
Plates 469
Plate 7 Cuckoo wasps such as this one, Stilbum cyanura, attack their host, in this case a mud
dauber wasp, by first chewing through the nest wall to reach the helpless offspring. They then
lay a egg on the host using their telescoping terminal abdominal segments. If disturbed, the heav-
ily armored cuckoo wasp can retract its abdomen into a tight ball; its thick cuticle is relatively
impenetrable
470 Plates
Plate 8 Living in a thorn. (above) Nest of Pseudomyrmex ants in a swollen thorn of the bull’s
horn Acacia, which the ant hollows out to house its brood. The entrance hole near the tip is clearly
visible, as are extrafloral nectaries (three swellings on the adjacent leaf petiole). (below) Portion of
an Acacia leaf, showing the protein-rich yellow Beltian bodies on the leaflet tips being collected
by a Pseudomyrmex worker ant
Plates 471
Plate 9 The carrion plant, Stapelia, produces a strong odor like decaying carrion; this is highly
attractive to calliphorid and sarcophagid flies that are duped into depositing their eggs at the
flower’s base. Two flies are visible at the flower base here
Plate 11 Vivid aposematic colors characterizes these nymphs of Oncopeltus fasciatus, the
milkweed bug; this warning is backed by chemical defenses gained from the milkweed plant
Plates 473
Plate 12 Two adults of the willow leaf beetle, Plagiodera versicolora; their larvae practice both
group feeding and cannibalism
474 Plates
Plate 13 The two forms of peppered moths, Biston betularia, on foliose lichen, (above) as they
would look in normal ‘visible’ light, and (below) under UV illumination
Plates 475
Plate 14 Three examples of crypsis among insects of the tropical rain forests. Top: nymph of an
unidentified preying mantis. Middle: a well-camouflaged walking stick. Below: a katydid resting
on a moss- and lichen-covered branch demonstrates both crypsis and disruptive coloration
476 Plates
Plate 15 Four examples of apparently Batesian mimics resembling many tropical ‘tarantula hawk’
wasp species. (upper left) a carnivorous katydid from Panama; (upper right and lower left) two
different coreid bugs from Ecuador; (lower right) an assassin bug from Panama
Plates 477
Plate 16 The expanded coremata of a displaying male of an arctiid lekking moth, Creatonotos
478 Plates
Plate 17 A cluster of
Neodiprion sawfly larvae on a
pine branch exhibit their
characteristic defensive
posture, with the anterior
parts of their bodies tilted
backward and droplets of
regurgitated fluids exposed
from their mouths. Vigorous
jerking movements enhance
the effectiveness of this group
display. The secretion
contains primarily
plant-derived substances
Plates 479
Plate 18 The Japanese hornet Vespa mandarinia japonica preys on worker honeybees that it cap-
tures at the hive entrance; instead of attempting to sting or succumbing as most domestic honeybee
species would, workers of the native Apis cerana honeybees grasp the intruder, and dozens more
quickly surround and engulf it into a living, buzzing ball of warm bees, producing a temperature
the bees can withstand but the hornet cannot
480 Plates
Plate 20 Attracting a crowd. The secret to creating a bee beard lies in knowing that the honey
bee queen’s pheromones makes her the center of attention. To make the beard, the queen must be
located in a bee swarm and moved into a small screen cage with an attached string that can be
quickly hung like a necklace. Within minutes the workers sense that their queen is missing and
take flight in a huge cloud; when they discover her, the workers quickly settle as close as possible
to their queen. (She is under the author’s chin, hidden by the swarm.)
482 Plates
Plate 21 Dinner by glowworm light. (above) The predatory Australian glowworm Arachnocampa
flava inhabits caves, where the larvae spin silk hammocks adorned with glistening droplets. (below)
A time-lapse photo of a group of glowworms; the source of one glowworm’s luminescence is
spotlighted in the upper photo
Plates 483
Plate 22 Playing femme fatale, a female Photuris firefly has seized a male of another firefly
species in a fatal embrace after attracting him by mimicking the mating signal of females of that
species. Photuris are such significant predators on other fireflies in the Americas that they are
thought to be the driving force that has caused several firefly species to become diurnal
484 Plates
Plate 23 Perception of color by humans (top), bichromatic insects (center), and (bottom)
trichromatic insects such as honey bees
Plates 485
Plate 24 A bumblebee, here just leaving a Mexican zinnia, sees a very different color palette
than the human who planted the flower. (above, left), human view; (above, right), the same flower
under ultraviolet (UV) light. (below) Simulated bee vision shows the way a flower with a visiting
bumblebee would look to humans if our light sensitivity were like that of the bees. In this photo-
graph of a bee on a yellow flower, colors have been remapped so that UV reflectance is shown as
violet/blue and the whole image only contains UV-blue-green
486 Plates
Plate 25 Butterflies drinking at a mud puddle. When disturbed, they will swirl up together to form
a confusing mass of colorful forms
Plate 26 Using a thin layer of thymol blue on the water surface reveals the way in which a water
strider (Gerridae) propels itself across the surface of a pond or river by hemispherical vortices shed
by its driving legs
Plates 487
Plate 27 An adult female thorn bug treehopper (Umbonia crassicornis) guards her nymphs. They
will use substrate vibrations to signal her as a group if a predator such as a coccinellid beetle
should approach, and she will respond by blocking the invader, fanning her wings aggressively,
and sometimes buzzing
488 Plates
Plate 28 ‘Speaking’ to bats. (A) All possible combinations of palatability and sound production
traits occur naturally in four different species of sympatric North American tiger moths: C+ and
C– refer to presence or absence of defensive chemistry and S+ and S– refer to ability or inability
to produce ultrasonic clicks. (B) Four stages in prey capture. (graph) Predictions arising from three
alternative hypotheses for the function of moth sound (jamming, startle, or warning) are depicted;
any would result in a trained bat aborting its usual prey capturing sequence. Results were consistent
only with a hypothesis of acoustic mimicry (‘observed’ line on graph); apparently the sounds are a
warning to bats that the moths are unpalatable, and the bats quickly learn to ignore clicks that are
not associated with distastefulness
Plates 489
Plate 29 Mating pair of snowy tree crickets, Oecanthus fultoni. For as much as an hour, the female
(above) will remain in position, chewing on a thick glutinous liquid from the male’s metanotal
gland; later she will also consume the white spermatophore that is visible here. Mating in this
subfamily is entirely female-controlled
Plate 30 An aphid giving birth to live young, one of the ways in which aphids reproduce
490 Plates
Plate 31 Stalk-eyed flies, Cyrtodiopsis dalmanni, gather to roost. A male (above) will fight off
other males to be the sole male in such aggregations, in which mating occurs at dusk or dawn.
Males with wider eye spans usually win these altercations, and females prefer to group with males
with the longest eye spans
Plates 491
Plate 32 Heliconius hewitsoni butterfly male, guarding a pupa (attached to a Passiflora vine in a
Costa Rican rain forest) from which his future mate will soon emerge
492 Plates
Plate 33 Ebony jewelwing damselflies, Calopteryx maculata. (above), a male on his territorial
perch. (below), a mating pair in the classic “wheel” position; the female has white wing spots
Plates 493
Plate 34 Chemical defense in the arctiid moth, Cosmosoma myrodora. (A) An aposomatic male
rests on its larval food plant, a source of pyrrolizidine alkaloids (PA). (B) A courting male has just
ejected a flocculent cloud (arrow) that will festoon the female in PA-rich fibers
494 Plates
Plate 35 Mud nest of a large Australian potter wasp, Abispa ephippium. Strictly solitary, one
female builds this fortress and progressively provisions each of up to seven cells. The nest entrance
funnel, thought to play a role in parasite deterrence, is dismantled and constructed anew with each
added cell
Plates 495
Plate 36 A tumblebug (Scarabaeinae) rolls its ball of dung carved from manure. The flesh fly
(Sarcophagidae) riding on the ball also breeds in dung and is a competitor for this rich food
resource. After rolling the ball for some distance, the pair of beetles will cooperatively excavate a
burrow and bury it, thereby making it inaccessible to flies. Underground in their burrow, the pair
may spend long periods preparing the ball to receive their egg
496 Plates
Plate 37 Phelypera distigma weevil larvae in their ‘circle the wagons’ (cycloalexic) defensive
formation between feeding bouts
Plate 38 Mastotermes darwiniensis worker being attacked by three green tree ants, Oecophylla
sp. Worldwide, ants undoubtedly pose the single greatest threat to termites
Plates 497
Plate 39 Parental care in a wood-feeding cockroach, Cryptocercus punctulatus. The mother pro-
tects a clutch of offspring that infect themselves with needed cellulose-digesting symbionts by
feeding on the mother’s feces as well as on fluids from the mother’s hind gut
Plate 40 (left) Australian gall-making thrips, Kladothrips morrisi, display striking polymorphism,
shown by a stout foundress female and her more heavily sclerotized soldier daughter (right) A
foundress female (black) and her offspring inhabit a domicile (here opened) formed from gluing
together two phyllodes of an Acacia
498 Plates
Plate 41 Golden egg bugs, Phyllomorpha laciniata, carry one another’s eggs, keeping them safe
from ant predation. The odd, leafy spines help the bug blend in with dried parts of its host plant
Plate 42 A Manduca hawk moth paying a night visit to a moonflower, Ipomea alba. The flow-
ers have coevolved for moth pollination; they do not open until evening, and they close the next
morning
Plates 499
Plate 43 Maternal care in the earwig, Forficula auricularia. Here, a female retrieves displaced
eggs and returns them to her nest. In addition to guarding the eggs, she will bring food to her
nymphs while they are very young
500 Plates
Plate 44 After spending most of their life feeding on roots underground, cicada nymphs emerge
from the soil in great numbers, split their exoskeleton, and become short-lived adults whose sole
purpose is reproduction. Males produce noisy songs, using their tymbals, and receptive females
respond with timed wing flicks that attract males for mating
Plates 501
Plate 46 To a human observer, all bumblebees and carpenter bees appear quite similar, but to a
female Xylocopa virginica, this yellow face mask indicates a conspecific carpenter bee male
Index
503
504 Index
acacia, 171–173, 470 Attractants, sexual, 27, 83, 221, 225, 226, 229,
Amazon, 442 250, 253, 254, 257, 344
aphid-tending, 155, 171, 241, 349, 428, Austroplatypus, 428
471 Autographa, 385
Argentine, 19 Autotomy, 213
army, 116, 158, 237, 238, 412, 413–416 Axons, giant, 53
attine, see Ants, leaf-cutter
carpenter, 207, 254, 412 B
citronella, 251 Backswimmers, 261, 298
fire, 131, 204, 237, 238–240, 418 Ballooning, 125
formicine, 186, 208 Bats, 62–68, 99, 194, 303, 319, 320, 321, 461,
green tree, see Oecophylla 488
harvester, 27, 252, 327, 439 Bee beard, 481
leaf-cutter, 107, 124, 139, 154–155, 183, Bee fly, 405
252, 410 Bees, 3, 7, 26, 42, 59, 72, 73, 74, 102, 105,
myrmecine, 412 106, 116, 129, 143, 144, 151, 153,
shampoo, 255 163, 174, 181, 183, 202, 205, 221,
thief, 153, 154 231, 248, 269, 272, 275, 355, 359,
trail-following, 222, 239 391, 404, 409, 420, 424, 427, 434,
velvet, 308, 314, 315, 405 436, 485
weaver, 29, 108, 246, 255, 410 allodapine, 422, 424
wood, 159–160 bumble, see Bumblebees
Anuraphis, 155 carpenter, 319, 371, 501
Anurogryllus, 402 coevolution with orchids, 355–357
Aphids, 59, 121, 125, 140, 141, 144, 148, 155, dances of, 45, 59, 72, 105, 106, 238, 276,
167, 185, 194, 197, 227, 231, 241, 299, 323–338, 420
248, 299, 348, 389 euglossine, 72, 356, 357, 361
alternation of generations, 349 gravity perception, 104, 106
bean, 117, 471 halictid, 420, 422, 438, 440
birth, 348, 489 honey, 8, 20, 21, 42–44, 45, 59, 60, 69, 72,
gall-forming, 409, 425, 427–428 81, 87, 94, 102, 105, 106, 108, 109,
mutualisms, 171 114, 128, 133, 139, 151, 175, 182,
social, 428 221, 231, 238, 240, 242, 254, 262,
Aphis, 349 272, 273, 276–279, 293, 294, 299,
Aphodius, 321 328–339, 380, 389, 407, 409, 410,
Aphrodisiacs, 195, 254 418, 420, 422, 423, 435, 438, 463,
Apiomerus, 170 479, 481, 484
Apis, 42–44, 248, 336, 422 hygienic behavior, 20–22
See also Bees, honey mimicry of, 197, 199, 319
Aposematism, 152, 188, 198–204, 232, orchid, see Bees, euglossine
319–321, 433, 472 retinue behavior, 237, 238, 254
Appeasement, 160, 161, 256, 349, 377 sleeping, 390–393
Apterygote, 342 sociality in, 163, 299, 409, 420–423,
Arachnocampa, 263, 482 434–440
Argogorytes, 358 solitary, 149, 151, 271, 305, 306, 393, 401,
Argynnis, 58, 285, 286–288 406
Argyrotaenia, 218 stingless, 185, 241, 252, 335, 336, 422,
Arms race, see Coevolution 423, 440
Asobara, 145 sweat, 87, 420, 421, 422
Assembly, 231–240 Beetles, 3, 4, 6, 10, 19, 75, 81, 96, 97, 104,
Associations, classification of, 392 105, 110, 111, 113, 116, 121, 122,
Atemeles, 158, 159–161 125, 132, 150, 155, 158, 162, 163,
Atta, 107, 139, 156, 183, 252 165, 170, 186, 197, 201, 206, 208,
Index 505
Dragonflies, 4, 68, 98, 100, 104, 115, 116, 118, Eyes, insect
141, 261, 269, 274, 279, 281, 283, apposition, 270, 271, 274
341, 343, 361, 371, 417 compound, 49, 88, 91, 269, 270, 273, 274,
Drakea, 229, 359 275, 281
Drepana, 319 superposition, 270, 271
Drosophila, 12, 19, 60, 68, 69, 75, 87, 88, 94, Eye spot, 209, 210, 212, 262
97, 109, 113, 121, 132, 145, 231,
248, 361 F
Dysdercus, 101, 117, 240 Fables, insect, 6, 8, 26, 389
Facilitation, social, 393, 394
E Feedback, 51, 52, 56, 57, 59, 84, 138, 240
Ears, insect, 303–305 Feeding, 70–71, 77, 79, 111, 117–119, 122,
Earwigs, 387, 389, 401, 499 123, 124, 126, 131–184, 188, 196,
Eavesdropping, 236, 255, 256, 311, 314 203, 228, 232, 239, 241, 271, 303,
Ecdysone, 85, 168, 169 327, 338, 359, 373, 378, 384–386,
387, 392, 394, 395, 402, 404–405,
Ecdysteroid, 85, 86, 92
408, 415–416, 422, 466, 473, 497
Echolocation, 62, 65, 67, 303
Femmes fatales, 200, 266, 392, 483
Eciton, 116, 412, 413–415
Ficus, 175
Eggs, trophic, 184, 402, 426
Figs and fig wasps, 174, 177
Elaphrosyron, 154
Filter, sensory, 60, 219
Elasmucha, 214
Fireflies, 37, 147, 262–268, 271, 361, 392, 483
Electroantennogram, 222, 258
Fixed action patterns (FAPs), 57, 58, 83, 84
Eleodes, 186
Flash coloration, 209, 212
Endocrine system, 2, 46, 48, 83–85, 117, 226,
Fleas, 86, 94–97, 228, 464
344–345
Flicker vision, 272–275
See also Hormones
Flies
Endosymbiont, 157 bombyliid (bee flies), 405
Endothermy, 114–116 bush, 390, 391
Enhanced fecundity hypothesis, 400 crane, 209, 213, 351–354, 379
Ephippiger, 298 dance, 228, 272, 378–380
Epicordulia, 274 dung, 370
Eryphanis, 209 fruit (Drosophila), 12, 19, 69, 121, 132,
Escape behavior, 46, 52–56, 62–65, 84, 98, 231
101, 209, 212, 262, 290 fruit (Rhagoletis), 243
Ethogram, 13 glowworm, 263, 482
Ethology, 2, 13–15, 42, 53, 81, 261 house, 40, 99, 137, 153, 201, 269, 275, 276
Eucalyptus, 294, 428 Flight, 117, 119, 379
Eufriesea, 357 muscles, 51, 99, 100, 114–117, 212
Euplusia, 74 tandem, 343, 370, 492
Eupatorium, 375 Food web, 141, 142
Euschistus, 3 Foraging, 42–44, 72, 74, 87, 113, 124,
Eusociality, 409–428, 434–440 131–184, 239, 240, 254, 255, 275,
in aphids, 427–428 329–339, 390, 413, 416–418, 425,
in bees, 420–423 463
in thrips, 427 Foraging (for) gene, 44, 438
in wasps, 416–420 Foraging strategies, 140–163
Evolutionary convergence, 36, 187, 198, 252 Formica, 81, 158, 159, 160, 182, 207, 213,
Evolution by natural selection, 2, 9, 15–16, 24, 442, 471
27 Formicoxenus, 255
See also Selection, natural Form perception, 268–272
Exocrine glands, see Glands, insect Foulbrood, 20, 21
Exoneura, 424 Foundress, 151, 416, 417, 427, 497
Index 509
Mosquitoes, 12, 57, 84, 99, 121, 125, 132, Nuptial gifts, 372–378
148, 157, 277, 294, 299, 311–312, Nutrition, 131, 134, 181, 240, 386, 400, 426
323–327, 346, 359, 383, 386 Nymphalis, 262
Moths, 30, 60, 88, 103, 104, 111, 115, 121,
122, 140, 146, 164, 167, 169, 185, O
186–187, 190, 196, 197, 204, 205, Occam’s razor, 25
209, 217, 218–219, 229, 290, 319, Ocelli, 269, 334
344, 370, 373, 374, 400 Ochthera, 279
arctiid, 219, 294, 319, 374–376, Odontotaenius, 408, 431
477, 493 Odors, 60–61, 70–71, 75–81, 133, 134, 150,
cecropia, 90, 111, 218 160–162, 167, 172, 182, 206, 211,
codling, 75–80, 259 217–260, 331–335, 426, 471
gypsy, 85, 111, 125, 131, 164, 221, 250, See also Pheromones
259 Oecophylla, 246–247, 255, 480, 496
hawk (hornworm, sphinx), 110, 116, 133, Oenothera, 87, 279
174, 291, 294, 303, 383, 498 Oligophagy, 133
noctuid, 62–68, 120, 124, 148, 385 Ommatidia, 104, 269–275
peppered, 191–194, 474 Omophron, 320
polyphemus, 60, 167, 253, 344 Oncopeltus, 118–119, 121, 472
silk, 89–90, 132, 253 Onthophagus, 403
silkworm, 9, 89, 221, 223, 225–226, 251 Oogenesis-flight syndrome, 120–121
tiger, 194, 320–321, 488 Ophrys, 357–358
Motivation, 58–59, 68, 83, 283 Optimality theory, 41, 140–145
Musca, 137, 391 Orchid pollination, 34, 58, 229, 354–359
Mushroom bodies, 42, 43, 49, 60, 463 Organization, social, 241, 390–407, 408
Myrmecophiles, 158–163 Orientation, 73, 100–109, 126–129, 276, 297,
Myrmica, 160, 161, 254, 255, 410 305–306, 333
Myzus, 241 Oscillogram, 308–309
Oscinella, 121
Osmeterium, 204, 211
N Ostrinia, 290
Nasonia, 295, 344 Oviposition behaviors, 30, 243–244, 346,
Nasutitermes, 208, 213, 386 382–387, 396–400
Nature–nurture controversy, 82
Nauphoeta, 230 P
Navigation, 42, 101, 105, 109, 124, 126, 128, Paedogenesis, 349
129, 282, 463 Palmacorixa, 293
Nectar guides, 279, 485 Panesthia, 431
Nemobius, 72, 295 Paper factor, 168
Neobarettia, 213 Papilio, 198, 202, 211
Neoconocephalus, 114 Parasites and parasitoids, 31–33, 67, 75–81,
Neodiprion, 232, 243, 244, 478 124, 125, 134, 140, 142, 145,
Neopyrochroa, 373 148–152, 243–245, 248, 255, 295,
Nephila, 374 299, 301, 306, 349, 359–361,
Nerve cord, 49, 54–56, 67 371–372, 385, 386, 387, 405–406,
Nervous system, 46–68, 83–90, 121, 139, 146, 433–434, 441–442, 468
219, 226, 227, 253, 277, 345, 441 Parasitism, social, 426, 441
Nests, 105, 157–163, 401, 421, 470 Parasitism, temporary, 443
Neural inhibition, 50 Parental behavior, see Care, parental
Neuropil, 47, 49 Parischnogaster, 418
Nicrophorus, 246, 404 Parthenogenesis, 347–349
Nomadacris, 135, 345 Parsimony, principle of, 25
Noradrenaline, 86 Passiflora, 164, 170, 491
Notonecta, 261 Patterns, fixed action (FAP), 57–58, 83–84
512 Index