How Languages Are Learned4thedition
How Languages Are Learned4thedition
How Languages Are Learned4thedition
Teaching American English Pro nu ncia rio n The Oxfo rd ESOL H an d boo k
Peter Avery and Susan Ehrlich Philida Schellekens
Des igning and 'Analyz ing Language T~srs Exploring Learner Language
Nathan T Carr Elaine Tarone and Bonnie Sunerzbin
[ames Dean Broum and Theodore S. F.vdgen Exp laining Eng lish G ramma r
Ceo~fJ" rule Fourth edition
From Exp eri en ce [0 Knowledge
[uit.m Edge .tna )lIt ";'II'ron
Acknowledgements xi
Preface to the fourth edition xiii
Introduction
..,
Before we begin ...
Language learning in early childhood 5
Preview 5
First lan gu age acquisitio n "i
The first th ree years: Milestones and developmental sequences b
The p re-schoo l years I'::
The school years 13
Explai ni ng first lang uage acq uisi cion 1-1
The beha viour isr per spe ctive 15
The innatisr per spective 20
Inreracrionisr/developrnenral per specti ves l'!
Language di sorders and dela ys 29
C hild hood bilingualism 30
Summary 33
Suggestio ns for further read ing 34
,
2 Second language learning .3 5
Prev iew 35
Learner characteristics .36
Learning conditions . 38
Studying the langu age of seco nd la nguage learners 40
Contrastive analysis. error analysis. and inre rlanguage 41
Developmental sequences 45
More about first language influence 57
Vocabulary 60
Pragma tics 65
Phonology 68
Sampling learners' language -..,
Sum ma ry -.,
Suggesrions lor further reading - .3
viii Contents Contents IX
)~
Preview 7 5
Preview 123
Intellige nce 79
In natural acq uisition settings 124
Personali ty 84
Observatio n sche mes 129
Learner beliefs 90
Q uestio ns in the classroom 14 5
Intuitions of grammaricality 95
Rate oflearning 96
6 S~cond language learning in the classroom 153
99
Preview 153
Summary
Suggesrions for further reading 100
Proposals for teaching 153
3 Lee's talk 16 5
Preview 103
4 Get rwo for one 171
Second language app lications: Krashe n's 'Mcn iror Model' 106
Sum m ary 197
Summary 120
Bibliography 22 7
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We wish first to thank the readers who responded so pos it ively to the earl ier
editions of this book. W ith each edition. we have benefited fro m sugges
tio ns and feedback offered by colleagues and studen ts. Our thanks to Ahlem
Ant mar. Alexander Ary, Philippa Bell. Luz Celaya. Laura Collins. Maria
Frohlich , Randall H al ter. Z haohong Han. Marlise Horst. Jim Hu, Phillip
Hubbard, Youjin Kim , Roy Lyster. Alison Mackey, Kim McDono ugh. Shawn
Loewen , Paul Meara, Imrna M iralpeix, Vicki M urphy, Carmen Munoz, H elke
Neumann . Howard Nicholas. Paul Quinn. Katherine Reh ner, Mela Sarkar,
Raquel Serrano, Younghee Sheen. Wa taru Suzuki. and Yasuyo Tomita . Leila
Ranta. and Jude Rand made essent ial con tr ibu tio ns to th e first ed ition .
At Oxford Unive rsity Press. we owe a de b t to H en ry Wi d dowso n for h is
early enco urage ment and to C ristina Whitecross, who was o ur editor for
the first three ed itions. We are grateful to Cather ine Kneafsey, Julia Bell,
H azel Gearches, an d An n Hu nte r who have wo rked with us thro ugh the
development o f this new edition. We thank the English Speaki ng Union tor
conferring the 1993 Duke of Edinburgh book prize for Applied Linguistics
on the book.
l~ .. r
II
PREFACE TO THE
FOURTH EDITION
When new methods and textbooks for second and foreign language teach
ing are int rod uced . th ey are often said to be based on the latest research
in psychology, linguistics, or pedagogy. Teachers are told that the y will be
more effective than those that have gone before. In many cases, the new
approaches are prescribed for immediate implementation in a school o r
region. Sometimes, the new materials co me with opportunities for extensive
training in their implementation. Sometimes, they are simply ordered and
distributed to teachers who have co do their best to use them effectively.
Many approach es to language teaching have been proposed and imple
mented . O ne approach req uires stu den ts co learn rules of grammar and lists
ofvocabulary to use in translating literary texts. Another emp hasizes th e
value of havin g st udents im ita te and p ractise a set of co rrec t sente nces a nd
me mo rize entire di alogues . Yet ano the r encourages 'natural' comm unication
be twee n stude nts as the y en gage cooperati vely in tasks or projects wh ile using
the new language. In some classrooms, the second language is used as the
medium to teach subject matter, with the assumption that the language itself
will be learned incidentally as students focus on the ' ~cademic content.
How are teachers to evaluate the potential effectiveness of diffe rent inst ruc
tional practices? To be sure, the mos t important influence on teachers'
de cisions is their own experience with previ ou s successes or disappointments ,
as 'well as the ir understanding of th e needs and abilities of their students.
We believe that ideas drawn from research and theory in second language
acq uisitio n are also valuable in helping reachers-co evaluate claims made by
proponents ofvarious language teaching methods. The goal of this book is to
introduce teachers-both novice and experienced-c-eo so m e of the language
acq uisition researc h th at m ay help th em nor on lfm evalua te existing text
books and materials but also to adap t them in ways that are more consistent
with ou r understanding of how langu ages are learned.
The book begins with a chapter on language learning in early childhood.
This background is im portant because both second language research and
second language teac hing have been infl ue nced by our understandi ng o f how
children acquire their first language. Several theo ries about first language
(l 1) learn ing are presented in th is chapter and they are revisited later in the
book in relation to second language (l 2) lear ning.
2 Introduction Introduction 3
In C ha p te r 2 we look at second lan gu age learn ers' developing knowledge, learning and teaching. Th ink about whether you agree or disagree with each
th eir abili ry to use th at knowledg e, an d how th is co mpares with L1 learning. op inio n. Keep th ese sraternents and yo ur reactions to them in mind as yo u
In Chapter 3, we rum ou r att entio n to ho w indi vidual learner characteristics read about current research and the o ry in seco nd lan guage learn ing.
may affect success. In C hapter 4, several theories that have been advan ced
to explain second lan guage learning are pr esented and discussed . Chapter
5 begins wi th a co mparison of natural and instructio nal environ ments for ACTIV ITY Give you r opinion on. t hese statements
seco nd lan guage lear nin g. We then examine so me d ifferent ways in which Indicate t he extent to which you agree wit h each statement by marking an X
researchers have observed an d described teaching and learn ing pra ctices in in the box associated with your o pinio n:
second language classrooms. SA-strongly agree
In Chapter 6 , we examine six proposals that have been mad e for second A-agree somewhat
lan guage teaching. Examples of research related to each of th e proposals are D-disagree somewhat
pr esented, leading to a disc ussion of the evidence available for assessing th eir SD-strongly disagree
effecti veness. The chapter ends with a discuss ion of wh at research findi ngs SA A 0 SO
suggest abo u t the most effective ways to teach an d learn a second language
in th e classroom. I Languages are learn ed mainly through imitation.
In C hap ter 7, we will provid e a general sum mary o f the book by looking at 2 Par ents usually correct youn g children w he n th ey
how research can inform our response to some 'popularopinions' abou t lan make grammatical errors.
r -
guage learning an d teach ing that are introduced belo w. 3 Highly inte llige nt people are good language
learners.
A Gl ossary pro vides a qui ck reference for a number of terms char ma y be new
o r have specific technical meanings in the context of language acq uisition 4 The mos t important predictor of success in
research. Gl ossary words are shown in bo ld letters where th ey first appear in second language acquisition is motivation .
the text. For readers who would like to find ou t more , an annotated list of
,
5 The earlie r a second language is introduced in
"
suggestions for further readi ng is included at th e end of each ch apter. Th e school programmes, the greater the likelihood of
Bibliography pro vides full refere nce info rm atio n for the suggeste d readi ngs success in learning.
We have tri ed to present th e information in a way that does no t assume that learners make are due t o interference from. the ir
readers are already famil iar with research method s or the oretical issues in , first language .
seco nd language learn ing. Exam ples and case studies are include d th rough
7 The best way to learn new vocabulary is thro~gh
o ut the book to illustrate the research ideas. M an y of th e exam ples are tak en read ing. ~
used in the research that we review in this book. At the end of each ch apter pro no unce all the individual sounds in the ~~.J
are 'Q uestio ns for reflection' to help read ers consolida te an d expand th eir second language .
understanding of th e material. 9 Once learners know 1.000 words and the basic
abounds with remarkable videos of infams reacting to language sounds. In the 196005, several researchers focused on how children acquire grammati
Whether they are becoming mo nolingual o r bilingual children, however, cal morphemes in Eng lish. One of the best-known stud ies was carried o ur by
it will be many months before their own vocalizations begin to reflect the Roger Brown an d his colleagues and students. In a lqngitudinal study of the
characteristics of the language or languages they hear and lo nger still before language development of three ch ild ren (called Adam, Eve. and Sarah) they
they connect language sounds with specific meaning. However, by the end of found that 14 grammatical mo rphemes were acquired in a sim ilar sequence.
thei r first year, most babies understand quite a few frequently repea ted words The list below (adapted from Brow n's 1973 book) shows some of the mo r
in the language.or lan guages spo ken around them. They wave when someo ne phemes they studied.
says 'bye- bye' ; they clap whe n someo ne says 'pat-a-cake'; th ey eagerly hu rry presenr progressive -ing (Mommy running)
to the kitche n when 'juice and coo kies' are mentio ned. p lural -s (rwo boo ks)
At 12 months, most babies will have begun to produce a word or two that irregular past forms (Baby went)
everyone recognizes. By the age of two, mo st children reliably prod uce at possessive -s (D addy's hat)
least 50 differ ent words and some produce many mo re. About this time, they co pul a (Mommy is happy)
begin to combine words into simple sentences such as ' Mo mmy juice' and articles the and a
8 Langu age learnmg in early childhood
Language learning in earlv childhood 9
regular past -ed (she walkzz)
What similarities and diffe rences do you notice among the children at
different ages!
2 Which gra mmatical morphemes do they find easy and which ones are
Brown and his colleagues found that a child who had mastered the gram more difficult!
matical morp hemes at th e bottom of the list had also mastered chose at the
cop. bu t the reverse was not tru e. Th us, mere was evidence fo r a 'develop men
tal seq uence' or order of acquisition. However, the children did not acquire The acquisition ofother language features also shows how child ren's language
the morphemes at me same age or rate . Eve had mastered nearly all me mor develops systematically, and ho w th ey go beyond what they have heard to
phemes before she was two-and-a-halfyears old . while Sarah and Adam were create new forms and structures.
still working on them when they were three-and-a-half or four.
Negation
Brown's longitudinal work was confirmed in a cross-sectional study of 2 1
Children learn the functions of negation very early. Th at is, th ey learn to
child ren. Jill and Peter de Villiers (197 3) found tha t children who correctly
comm ent o n m e disappearance ofobj ects, to refuse a suggestion , or to reject
used the morphemes mat Adam, Eve, and Sarah had acquired late were also
an assertion, even at me single word stage . However, as Lois Bloom's (199 1)
able to use the ones mat Adam, Eve, and Sarah had acquired ear lier. The chil
longitudinal studies show, even though children understand th ese func
dren mastered me morphemes at different ages, JUSt as Adam, Eve. and Sarah
tions and express them with single words and gesrures, it takes so me time
had d one, but the o rder of their acquisition was very sim ilar.
before th ey can exp ress chern in sentences, using the appropr iate wo rds and
Many hypothes es have been advanced co explain why these gramm atical word orde r. The follow ing stages in' me develo pment o f negatio n have been
mo rp hemes are acq u ired in the observed order. Researchers have stu died me observed in th e acq uisi tion of En glish . Similar stages have been observed in
frequency with wh ich the morphemes occu r in parents' speech, the cognitive other lan guages as well (\Vode 1981).
factory explanation for me sequence, and most researchers agree that me order Negation is usually expressed by the word 'no', eith er all alone or as the first
To su pplement the evidence we have from sim p ly observing children , some No . No cookie. No co mb hair.
carefully designed procedures have been developed to further exp lo re chil
dren's knowledge ofgrammatical morphemes. One ofthe first and best known Stage 2
is me so-called 'wug test ' developed by Jean Berko Gleason (1958) . In this Utteranc es grow longer and me sent ence subject ma y be included . The neg
'test' , children are shown drawings of imaginary creatures wich novel names ative word appears JUSt before the verb. Sentences expressing rejection or
o r people performing mysterious acti ons. For example. they are told, ' H ere is prohibitio n often use 'do n't'.
'.'
a wug. Now mere are rwo of them. There arc rwo _ _' or ' H ere is a man who Daddy no co m b hair. Don't touch mat! i
knows how to bod. Yesterday he did the Same thing. Yesterday, he _ _ '. By
co m p leti ng these sentences with 'wugs' and 'be dd ed' , children demonstrate Stage 3 ~_,
that they know the patterns for plural and simple past in English. By gener The negative element is inserted into a more complex sentence. Children
alizing these patterns to words they have never heard before, they show that may add forms of me negative other than 'no', including words like 'can't'
their language is more man JUSt a list of memorized word pairs such as ' boo k! and 'do n't'. These sentences appear ro follow the correct English pattern of
books' and 'nod i nodded'. attaching the negative to me auxiliary or modal verb. However, children do
Even though the ir language system is by now quite complex, they may still sentences with rising intonation:
The challenge oflear ning complex language systems is also illustrated in th e Where's Daddy? What's that?
developmental stages through which children learn to ask questions.
Stage 2
There is a remarkable consistency in the way ch ildre n learn to fo rm ques As they begin to ask more new questions, children use the word order of the
tions in English. For one thi ng, there is a predictable order in which the declarative sentence, with rising intonation.
' wh- words' emerge (Bloo m 1991) . 'What' is generally the first wh- question
word to be used. It is often learned as part of a chunk ('Whassat?') and it is You like this? I have some?
some time before the child learns that there are variations of the form, such They continue to produce the correct ch unk-learned forms such as 'What's
as 'What is tha t?' and 'What are these?' . that?' alongside their own created questio ns.
'Where' and 'who' eme rge very soo n. Identifying and locating peo ple and
. Stage 3
objects are within the ch ild 's understanding of the world. Furthermore, Gradually, children notice that the structure of questions is different and
adults tend to ask children just these types of questions in the early days of begin to produce questions such as:
language learning, for example, 'Where's Mommy?' or 'Who's that? '
Can I go?
'Why' emerges around the end of the secon d year and becomes a favouri te Are you happy?
for the next year or two. Children seem to ask an endless number ofquestions
beginning with 'why', having discovered how effectively this lit tle word gets Although some ques tions at this stage match the adnlt pattern, they may be
adults to engage in conversation, for example, 'Why tha t lady has blue hair?' right for the wrong reason. To describe this, we need !co see the pattern from
the child's perspective rather than from the perspective of the ad ult grammar.
Finally, when the child has a better understanding of manner and time, 'how' We call this stage 'fro nt ing' because the child's ruleseems to be that questions
and 'when' emerge. In contrast to 'what' , 'where', an d 'who' questions, ch il are formed by putting some thing (a verb or q uestio n word) at the 'front ' of a
dren sometimes ask th e more cognitively dlfficult 'why', 'when', and 'how' sencence, leaving the rest of the sentence in its sraternent form .
questions without understanding the answers they get, as the following con
versation with a four-year-old clearly shows. Is the teddy is tired ? Do I can have a cookie?
Why you don't have one? Why you catched it? ',~'
The ability to use these question words is at least pardy tied to children's cog questions resemble those of Stage 3, but there is more variety in the auxilia
nitive development. It is also predicted in part by the questions children are ries that appear before the subject.
asked and the linguistic complexity of questions with different wh- words. Are you going to play with me?
Thus it does not seem surprising that there is consi stency in the sequence of
At this stage, children can even add 'do ' in ques tions in whic h there wo uld be
tion of word order in questions. This develo pment is not based on learning
new mean ings, but rather on learni ng different linguistic patterns to express Do dogs like ice cream?
meanings that are already understood. Even at th is stage, howeve r, childre n seem able to use either inversion or a uih
word, but not both (for exam ple, 'Is he crying?' bu t not 'Why is he crying? '
Th erefore. we may find in version in yes/no questions but no t in wh- qu es cajoling lan gu age th at is needed to defend their toys in the playground. Th ey
dons, unle ss they are formulaic units such as 'What's that?' show that they have learned the difference between how adults talk to babies
and how they talk to each other, and they use this knowledge in elaborate
Stage 5
pretend play in which they practise using these different 'voices'. In this way,
At Stage 5, both wh- and yes/no qu est ion s are formed correctly.
they explore and begin to understand how and why language varies.
Are these yo ur boo ts?
ness, the ability to treat language as an object separate from the meaning it
Does Daddy have a box ?
conveys. Three-year-old children can tell you that it's 'silly' to say 'drink the
Negative questions may still be a bit too difficult. chair', because it doesn't make sense. However, although they would never
Why the teddy bear can't go outside? say 'cake the eat', they are less sure that there's anything wrong with it. They
may show that they know it's a bit odd, but they will focus mainly on the fact
And even though performance on most questions is correct, there is still one that they can understand what it means. Flve-year-olds, on the other hand,
mo re hurdle. When wh - words appear in subo rdi nate clauses or embedded know that 'd rin k the chair' is wrong in a different way from 'cake the eat ' .
questions, child ren overgeneralize the inverted form that would be correct They can tell you that one is 'silly' but the other is 'the wrong way around'.
for sim ple questions and produce sentences suc h as:
Language acquisition in the . pre-school years is impressive. It is also note
Ask hi m why can't he go ou t. wor thy th at child ren have spent th ou sands of hours int eract ing with
lan gu age- participatin g in co nversatio ns. eavesdro pping o n ot hers' con
Stage 6
versations, bein g read to , watc hing television , etc. A quick mathematic al
Ar th is stage, childre n are able to cor rectly form all q uestion types, incl uding
exercise wil l show you just how many hours childre n spend in lan guage-r ich
nega tive and complex embedded q uestions.
enviro nme nts. If child ren are awake to r ten o r twelve hours a day, we may
Passage through developmental seq ue nces d oes no t always follo w a steady or
estim ate that the y are in contact with the language their environment tor
uninterrupted path. Children appear to learn new things and then fall back 20,000 hours or more by the time they go to school.,
on old parrerns when there is added stress in a new situatio n or when they
Although pre-school children acquire complex knowledge and skills for
are using other new elements in their language. But th e overall path tak es
language and language use, th e school setting requires ne w ways of using
them toward a closer and closer approximation of the language tha t is spoken
language and brings new opportunities for language development.
aro und th em .
One of the mos t imp ressive aspects of language developmen t in the school Since the middle of the 20th century, th ree main theoretical position s have
years is the astoni shing growth ofvocabulary. C h ild ren enter school with the been advanced to explain language development: behavlo urist, innatist, and
ability to understand and produce several tho usan d wo rds, and thousands inte ractional/ developmental perspectives.
more will be learned at school. In both the spoken and written language at
school, words such as 'homewo rk' or 'r uler' appear freq uently in situations
The behaviouristperspective
where their mean ing is either immediately or gradually revealed . Wo rds like
'po pulatio n' or 'latitude' occur less frequen tly, but they are made impo rtan t Behaviourism is a theory of lear ning that was influential in the 1940s and
by their significance in academic subjec t matt er. 1950s, especially in the United States. Wi th regard to language lear nin g,
the best-known proponent of this psychological theory was B. F. Skin ner
Vocabulary grows at a rate of between several hundred and more than a (1957). Traditional behaviourists hypothesized that when children imitated
thousand words a year, depending mainly on how much and how widely the language pro duced by those around them, their attem pts to reproduce
children read (Nagy, Herman, and Anderson 19 85). The kind of vocabulary what they heard received 'positive reinforcement'. This could take the fo rm
growth required for school success is likely to' come from bo th reading for of praise or JUSt successful communication. Thus enco uraged by th eir envi
assignments and reading for pleasure, whether narrative or non-fiction. Dee ronment, children would continue to imi tate and practise these sounds and
Gardner (2004) suggests that reading a variety oftext types is an essential part .patterns until they formed 'habits' of correct language use. According to this
of vocabulary growth . His research has shown ho w the range of vocab ulary view, the quality and quantity of the language the ch ild hears, as well as
in narrative texts ·is different from that in non -fiction . There are wo rds in the con sistency of the rein forceme nt offered by others in the environment,
non-fiction texts that are unlikely to occur in sto ries or novels. In addi tion, woul d shape the child's language behaviour. This theory gives great impor
non-fiction tends to include more opport unities to see a word in its different tance to the env ironment as the source ofeveryth ing che.child needs to learn.
forms (fo r exam ple, 'm ummy', 'm ummies' , 'mummified'). The importance
of reading for vocabulary growth is seen when observant parents report a Analysing children's speech: Definitions and examples
child usin g a new word but mispronouncing it in a way tha t reveals it has The behaviourists viewed imitation an d practice as the primary pro cesses in
been encountere~ only in wri rren form . language development. To clarif)rwhat is meant by these rwo ter ms, consider
Another important development in the school years is the acquisition of dif the following defin itions and examples. J ,.
ferent language registers . Children learn how written lan guage differs from Imitation : wo rd-fo r-word repetition ofall or part ofsomeo ne else's utterance.
spoken language, how the language used to speak to the principal is different
MOTHER Shall we play with th e dolls ?
from the language of the playgro und, how the language of a scien ce repo rt is
LUCY Play with dolls
different from the lan guage of a narrative. As Terry Piper (2006) and o thers
have doc umented, some children will have even mo re to learn if they come Prahice: repeti tive manipulatio n of form.
to schoo l speaking an ethnic or regional variety of the school language that is
C IN D Y He.ear carrots. The oth er one eat carro ts. They both eat
quite different from the one used by the teacher. They will have to learn tha t "[
carrots. !
another variety, often referred to as the standard variety, is requi red for suc
cessful acad emic work. Other children arrive at school speaking a different Now examine the tra nscrip ts from Peter, Ci ndy, and Kathryn. They were all
language altogether. For these children, the work oflanguage learni ng in th e about 24 months old when th ey were recorded as ~y played with a visiting
early school years presents additional opportunities and challenges. We will ad ult. Using th e definitions above, notice how Peter im itat es th e adu lt in the
retu rn to this topic when we discuss bilingualism in early childhood. followin g dial ogue .
Peter (24 months) is playing with a dump truck wh ile two adults, Patsy and
These descriptions oflanguage development from infancy th rou gh the early PETER Get more .
school years show that we have considerable kn owledge of what children LOIS You're gonna put more wheels in rhe du m p truck?
learn in their early lang uage development. More co ntroversial, however, are PETER Dump truck. Wheels. Dump tr uck.
questions abou t how this develop ment takes place . What abilities does the (later)
child bring to the task and what are the contributio ns of the environment?
PATSY What happened to it (the truck)?
Ld nguage learning in early childhood J
16 Language learning in early childhood
Cindy appears to be working hard on her language acquisition. She prac
PET ER (loo king under chair for it) Lose it. Dump truck! Dump
tises new words and structures in a way that sounds like a student in some
foreign language classes! Perha ps most interesting is tha t she remembers the
' lan guage lesson' a week later and turns st raigh t to the page in the book she
had no t seen since Patsy's last visit, What is most striking is that, like Peter,
(Un published data from P. M. Lighcbown) her imitation and practice appear to be focused on what she is currently
If we analysed a larger sample of Peter's speech, we would see that 30-40 per 'wo rking on' .
cent of his sentences were imitations of what someone else had JUSt said. We The samples of speech from Peter and Cindy seem to lend some sup po rt to
would also see that his imitations were not random. That is, he did not simply the behaviourist explanation of language acquisition. Even so, as we saw,
imitate 30-40 per cent of everything he heard. Detailed analyses of larg e the cho ice of what to imitate and practise seemed determined by something
samples of Peter's speech over about a year showed that he imitated words inside the child rather than by the environment.
and sentence structures that were JUSt beginning to appear in his spontaneous
Not all children imitate and practise as much as Peter and Cindy did . The
speech. Once these new elements became solidly grounded in his language
amount of imitation in the speech of other children, wh ose development
Unlike a parrot who imitates the familiar and continues to repeat the same cula ted at less than 10 per cent. Consider the examples of imitation and
things again and again , children appear to imitate selectively. The choice practice in the following conversation between Kathryn and Lois.
of what to imitate seem s to be based on so meth ing new that they have just
Kathryn (24 months)
begun to understand and use, nor sim ply o n what is availabl e in th e enviro n
ment. For example, consider how Cindy imitates and practises language in LO IS Did yo u see the toys I brought?
the followin g con versations. KAT H RY N I br ing toys? Choo choo ? Lois brought the cha o cho o train ?
C INDY Carrot. (po inting [Q each carrot on the page) The other ...
~TH RYN (taking out tWO cars of train) Do this . I want do this . (trying
carrot. The other carrot. The other carrot. to put train together) I do this. I do this .
LOIS OK. You can do it. You can do it. Look <
I'll show •vou how.
(A few minutes later, Cindy brings Patsy a stuffed toy rabbit.)
~
CINDY He (incomprehensible) eat carrots. The other one eat carrots. LOIS Inside. It's in the box.
They both eat carro ts. KATH RYN A choo choo? (taking out parr of train) This is a choo choo
(One week later, Cindy opens the book to the same page .) trai n.
CINDY Here's the carrots. (pointing) Is that a carrot? (from Bloom and Lahey 1978: 135)
PATSY Yes. Like Cindy, Kathryn sometimes repeats herself or produces a series of related
practice sentences. but she rarely imitates the other speaker. Instead , she asks
(Unpublished data from P. M . Llghcbown)
and answers questions and elaborates on the other speaker's questions or
statements.
18 LanguageLearning in early childhood Language Learning in early childhood 19
Thus, children vary in me amount of imitation they do . In addi tio n, many so hard on performing me fascinating new gesture and me formulaic expres
of me things they say show that they are using lan gu age creatively, not just: sion 'I'd like ro propose . . . ' mat he failed to realize mat the wo rd he though t
repeating what they have heard . This is evident in the followi ng examples. he kn ew- 'roast '- was not me same roast and could not be replace d wit h its
apparenc near-synon ym , 'a p iece of bread'.
Patterns in language
Th e first example shows a child in me process of learning parrerns in lan Questio n formation
guage, in. th is case me rules of word fo rmatio n, an d overgene ralizing them Randall (2 years, 9 mo nths) asked m e foll owing questio ns in various situa
Ran dall (36 months) had a sore on his han d. Are dogs can wiggle their tails?
MOTHER Ma ybe we need to take you to the doctor. Are those are my boots?
RANDALL Why? So he can doc my littl e bum p? Are this is ho t?
Ran dal l forms me verb 'doc' from the nou n 'docto r', by analogy with farme rs Randall had concluded m at me trick of asking questions was to p ut 'are' at
who farm , swi mme rs who swim, an d acto rs who act. me begin n ing of me sentence. H is questions are goo d examples of Stage 3 in
question development.
Order of events
Randall (3 years, 5 months) was loo king fo r a rowel.
You took all the rowels away because I can't dry my hands.
He meant 'I can't dry my hands because yo u w o k all m e towe ls away', b ut
he made a mistake abour which clause com es first. C hildre n at th is stage of
~)
language develop ment ten d to men tio n events in rp,e order o f th eir occu r
ren ce. In th is case, me towels disa ppeared before Randall arternp red to dry
his hands , so char's wh at he said first. He d id no t vet.understand how a word
like 'before' or 'because' changes the order of ca~eind effecr.
/C:~~
'Si~oo~~;)
--/r These examples of chil d ren's spee ch provide us witha window on th e p rocess
of lan guage lear ning. Imi ta tion an d practice alone canno t exp lain some of
m~ forms created by chil dren . The y are not merely repetitions of sentences
that they have heard from ad ults. Rather, children'appear to pick out patterns
and generalize them to new contexts. They create new forms or new uses of
words. Th eir new sentences are usually comprehensible and often correcr.
Focus on meaning Behav iourism seems to offer a reasonable way o f un4,gstanding how children
Even olde r childr en have to wo rk out some puzzles , for example, whe n famil learn some of the regular an d routine aspects of langifage, especially at m e ear
iar language is used in unfamiliar ways, as in m e example below. \JVhen David liest stages. However, children who do lit tle overt imitation acquire language
(5 years, 1 m o nt h) was at his older sister's birthday parry, toasts were pro as fully and rapidly as those who imitate a lor. And although behaviourism
posed with grape juice in stemmed glasses: goes so me way to explaining me SOrts of overgeneralization mat childre n
make, classical behav iourism is not a satisfactory exp lana tion fo r me acquis i
FATHER I'd like to p rop ose a to ast.
tion of m e more complex grammar th at chil d ren acquire. These lim itations
Several minutes late r, David raised his glass: led researchers to look for d ifferent explanations for lang uage acq uisition.
DAV ID I'd like to p rop ose a piece of b read.
O nly when laug h ter sent D avid slinking from the table did m e group realize
mat he wasn't intenti o nally making a play on words! H e was concen trating
20 Language learning in early childhood Language learning in etzr/ychildhood 2l
7he innat~tperspective In (a) an d (b), it loo ks as if th e reflexive pronoun mus t follow the no un it
refers to . But (c) disproves thi s:
Noam Chomsky is one of the mos t influential figu res in linguistics, and
his ideas about how language is acquired and ho w it is stored in the mind c Looking after himse/fboresJohn.
sparked a revolution in many aspects of linguistics and psychology, includ If we co nsid er sent ences suc h as:
ing the stu dy of lan guage acquisition . The innatist perspective is related to
d John said th at Fred liked himself.
C ho ms ky's hypothesis that all human languages are based o n some innate
e "f ohn said that Fred liked himself.
universal principles.
f John told Bill to wash himself.
In his 1959 review of B. F. Skinner's book Verbal Behavior, Chomsky chal g «John told Bill to wash himself.
lenged the behaviourist explanation for lan guage acquisition. He argued that
we might conclude that the noun closest to the reflexive pronoun is the anre
ch ildren are biologically programmed for language and that language devel
cedent. However, (h) shows th at thi s rule won't work eith er:
ops in the child in JUSt the same way that other bio logical functions de velop .
For exam ple, every chi ld will learn to walk as long as adeq uate nourishment h John promised Bill to was h himself.
and reasonable freedom of movement are provided. The child does not have
And it's even more complicated than that. Usuall y the reflexive must be in
to be taught. Mo st childr en learn to walk at about the same age, and walking the same clause as the antecedent as in (a) and (d), but not always, as in (h) .
is essentially the same in all normal human beings. For Chomsky, language
Fur the rmo re, th e reflexive can be in th e subject pos ition in (i) but no r in OJ.
acq uisit ion is very simi lar. Th e enviro nme nt makes o nly a basic co ntri bu
tion-in th is case. th e availab ility of peopl e w ho speak to the ch ild. The i John believ es bimselfu: be intelligent (no n-fin ite clause).
child , o r rath er, the child's b iolog ical endowm ent , w ill do th e rest. j 'John bel ieves th at himself i s inte lligent (fin ite clause).
C ho msky"argued that the behaviourist theory failed to account for 'the logical In so me cases, more than o ne antecedent is possib le, as in (k) wh ere th e
pro blem oflanguage acq uisitio n'- the fact th at child ren co me to kn ow mor e reflexive co uld refer to eithe r Joh n o r Bill:
abo ut th e structure of th eir language than they could reasonabl y be expected k John ? showed Bill: a picture of himself.
to learn on the basis of the sam ples of language they hear. The language
children are exposed to includes false starts, incomplete sentences, and slip s When we loo k at thi s kind of complexity, it seems-it would be very hard
of the tongue, and yet they learn to distinguish between grammatical and to lear n , and ch ildren d o make errors along the way. Yet, most schoo l-age
ungrammatical sentences. He co ncluded that chi ldren's minds are not blank ch ildren would be able to co rrectly interpret the grammatical sent ences
slates to be filled by imitating language they hear in the environment. In stead , and recognize the ungrammaticaliry o f the othe rs. Researchers who study
he hypothesized, ch ild ren are born with a specific innate ability to discover language acq uisitio n from the innatist perspective argue th at such com plex
for themselves the underlying rules of a language system on the basis of the gram m ar could never be lear n ed purely on the basis of imitating and practis
samples of a natural language the y are exposed to . This innat e endowment ing sentences avai lable in the input. They hypothesize th at since all child ren
was seen as a SOrt of templat e, co nt ain ing the principles that are universal to acquire the language of th eir environment, theY
must have some innate
all human languages. This universal grammar (U G) would pr event the ch ild me chanism or knowledge that allows them to discover such complex synt ax
from pursuing all SOrtS of wrong hypotheses about how language systems in sp ite of limitations of the input. They hyp othesize furthermore that the
might work. If ch ildren are pre-equipped with UG , then what they have to innate mechanism is used exclu sively for language acquisition.
learn is the ways in which the language they are acquiring makes use of these The innatist perspective emphasizes the fact tha t almost all ch ildren success
princip les. fully acquire their native lan guage- o r more than one lan guage if the y live
Consider the follow ing sentences, from a book by Lydia White (1989). These in a multilingual community. Children who are profoundly deaf will learn
Engli sh sentences contain the reflexive pronou n ' h imself' . Both the pronoun sign lan guage if the y are exposed to it in infancy, and their progress in the
and the noun it refers to (the antecedent) are printed in italics. (An aster isk acquisition ofthat lan guage system is similar to hearing ch ildren's acquisition
at the beginning o f a sentence ind icates tha t the sent ence is u ngrarn m atical.) ofspoken language. Even children with very limited cognitive abiliry develop
quite complex language systems if they are brought up in environments in
a John saw himself. which people int eract with th em.
b "Him selfuuo J;hn.
22 Language learning in early childhood Language learning in early childhood 23
C hildr en acquire the basic syntax and morphology of the language spo ken to After she was discovered . Ge n ie was cared for and ed ucated with the par
them in a variery ofconditio ns, some of which wo uld be expected to enhance ticipation of many teache rs and therap ists, including Susan C urtiss (1977).
language development (for example, caring, attentive parenrs who focus on After a brief period in a rehabilitation centre, she lived in a foster home
the child 's lan guage), and some which might be expected to inhibit it (for and attended special schools. Genie made remarkable progress in becoming
exam ple, abusive or reject ing parents). Childre n achieve different levels of socialized and cogni tively aware . She developed deep personal relatio nships
vocabulary, creativity, social grace, and so on, bu t virrually all achieve the and strong individual tastes and traits, Neve rtheless , after five years of expo
abiliry to use the patterns of the language or languages spoken to them. Thi s sure to language, Genie's language was no t like'th at ofa typic al five-year old.
is seen as sup po rt for rhe hypo th esis tha t lan guage is somehow separa te from There was a larger th an normal gap between com prehension and prod uc
other aspects of cognitive development and may depend on a specific module tion. She used grammatical forms inco nsistently and overused formulaic and
of the brain. routine speech.
The Critical Period Hypothesis Although Victor and Ge nie appear to p rovide evidence in support of the
The innatist perspective is often linked to the C ritical Period Hypothesis CPH, it is difficult to arg ue th at the hyp o thesis is confirmed on the basis
(C PH)-the hypo thesis that animals , including humans, are gene tically of evidence from such unusual cases. We can not know wha t other factor s
pro grammed to acqui re certain kinds ofknowledge and skill ar specific times besides biological ma ruriry mig ht have contributed to their ina biliry to learn
in life. Beyond those 'critical periods', it is either difficult or impossible to lang uage. It is no t possible to de ter m ine whether eithe r of them suffered
acquire those abilities. Wi th regard to lan guage, the CPH suggests that chil from brain damage, developmental delays, or a specific language impair
d ren who are not given access to lan guage in infan cy and early childhood ment , even before they were separated from normal human int eractio n.
(because ofdeafness o r extreme isolation) will never acquire language if these
A more approp riate test of the CPH is the case of child ren who co me from
deprivation s go on for roo long.
homes where they receive love and care from thei r parents, yet do not have
It is difficul t to find evidence for or against the CPH, since nearly all ch ild ren access to language at the usual time. This is the case for some profoundly
are exposed ro language at an early age. However, history has do cumented a deaf children who have hearing parents. Only 5-~ 0 per cen t of the pro
few 'natu ral experiments' where children have been deprived ofco nt act with foun dly deaf are born to deaf parenrs, and only these children are likely to be
language. Two of the most famous cases are those of 'Victo r' and 'G en ie'. exposed to ASL from birth. Hearing parenrs may not realize that their ch ild
cannot hear because the child uses other senses to in teract in an apparently
In 1799, a boy who became known as Victor was found wandering naked
nor mal way. Thus, the early childhood period may be normal in most ways
in the woods in France . His Story was dram atized in a 1970 film by Fran cois
but devoid of lan guage that is accessible to the child . These children's later
Truffaut called L'enfantsauvage ( The Wild Child) . When Victor was cap tured,
exr.erience in learn ing sign language has been the.sub ject of some impo rtant
he was about 12 years old and completely wild, apparently having had no
research related to the CPH.
cont act with humans. Jean -Marc-G aspard hard, a you ng doctor accu stomed
to working with deaf children , devo ted five years to socializing Victor and Like oral and wri tten languages, American Sign pnguage (ASL) makes use
trying to teach him language. Although he succeeded to some extent in devel of grammatical markers to indicate such things-as time (for example, past
oping Victor's sociability, memory, and judgement, there was littl e progress tense) and nu mber. Th ese markers are exp ressed thro ugh specific hand or
in his language abiliry. body move me nts . ~.:
Nearly 200 years later, Genie, a 13- year-old girl who had been isolated, Elissa Newport (1990) and her colleagues srudied the abiliry of deaf users
neglecte d, and abused, was discovered in California. Because of the irrational of ASL to produce and comprehend grammatical markers. They compared
demands of a disturbed father an d the su bmission and fear of an abused Native signers (who were exposed to ASL from birth), Early signers (who
m o ther, Ge nie. had spent more than 11 years tied to a chair o r a crib in a began using ASL between fo ur and six years of age), and Late signers (who
small, darkened room. Her father had forbidden his wife and son to speak to began lear ning ASL after age 12). They foun d no differe nce between the
Genie and had himself only growled and barke d at her. She was beaten when groups in some aspecrs of their use ofASL, for example in vocabulary knowl
she made any kind of noise, and she had lon g since resorted to complete edge. However, on rests focusing on grammatical markers, the Native grou p
silence. Ge nie was undeveloped ph ysically, emotionally, and intellectually. used the markers more consistently th an the Early group who, in tu rn, used
She had no language. them more consisten tly than the Late group. The researchers co nclud ed that
24 Language learning in early childhood Language learn ing in early childhood 25
thei r study sup po rrs the hypothesis mat m ere is a critical period for first lan the child's experience an d cog nitive develop ment. Ind eed, researchers such
guage acquisition, whether mat language is oral or gestural. as Dan Siobin ( 197 3) have long emphasized me close relationship between
Anomer line of research mat has given new insight into me importance of ch ildren's cognitive development and their acquisition of language.
early language experience comes from stu d ies of ' intern atio nal ado prees.'
These are children who were adopted at an early age by Familieswho did Piaget and Vygotsky
not speak me language me child had heard during infancy. In their review One of th e earliest proponents of th e view mat children's language is built
o f studies of international adoptees, [ohanne Paradis, Fred Genesee, and on their cogn itive development was me Swiss psychologisr/eplsrernologist,
Martha Crago (20 11) concluded that cognitive and linguistic outcomes were Jean Piager (19 5 1). In me early decades of me 20th century, Piaget observed
generally very positive. Some com parisons of their language with that of infants and child ren in their play and in th eir interaction with obj ects and
children me same age who had always heard me same lan gu age showed that people, He was able to trace the development of the ir cognitive understand
su btle differences persist even after several years, but these are not me kinds ing of such things as object permanence (kn owin g that things hidden from
of differences that most people would notice. Here again, of course, one sigh t are still there) , me stability of quantities regardless of changes in th eir
can no t know whether so meth ing o th er man a late exposure to the language appearan ce (kno wing that 10 pennies spread Out to form a long line are
spoken in me adoptive environment also contributed to differences between not more numerous man 10 pennies in a tightly sque ezed line), and logi cal
th ese children and others who did not experience an abrupt change in their inferencing (figuring OUt which pr operties o f a set of rod s (their size, weigh t.
language en vironment. Nevertheless, with continuing researchon child ren's mat erial , erc.) cause so me rod s to sin k and Others to float o n water).
ling uistic behaviours an d intui tion s. as well as m e neu rological stud ies of It is easy to see how chi ld ren's cog nitive develop me nt wo uld par tly deter
in Fanr s' speec h pe rcepti on that we saw above, it is becom ing clearer that
mi ne how the y acq uire lan gu age. Fo r examp le, the use of certain ter ms suc h
langu age acq uisition begi ns at bi rth, an d poss ibly even befo re, as me ch ild 's as 'b igger ' o r 'more' depends o n the childre n's unde rsta nd ing of the co n
b rain is shaped by expo sure to m e lan gu age(s) in the environment.
cep rs rhey represe nt . The developi ng cog nitive u nde rstanding is bu ilt on the
The in natist perspecti ve is thus partly based on evid ence that m ere is a criti interacti on between th e child and the th ing s that can be o bserved o r manipu
cal period for language acquisition. It is also seen as an explanation for 'the lated. For Piager, language was one o f a number of.~ymbol systems that are
logical problem of language acquisition', that is, me question of how adult developed in childhood. Language can be used to represent knowledge that
speak ers come to know me complex structure of their first language on the child ren have acquired through ph ysical interaction 'with the environment.
basis of the limited sam ples of language to which they are exposed.
Anothe r influential student of child development'was the psychologist Lev
Vygorsky ( 197 8). He observed interactions among children and also between
Interactionistldevelopmentalperspectives child ren and adults in schools in me Soviet U nio n in the 1920 s and 1930s.
Developmental and cognitive psycho logists have focused on the interplay He concluded that language develops primarilyfrorn social interaction. H e
betwe en me inn ate learning ability of children and the environment in which argued that in a supportive interacti ve environm ent, children are able to
th ey develop. The y argue that me innatists place too much emphasis on me advance to higher levels of knowledge and perfJrmance. Vygotsky referred
'final state' (me competence ofadult native speakers) and nor enough on me to a metaphorical place in which children could do mor e than they would be
developmental aspects oflanguage acquisition. In their view, language acquisi capable ofdoing independently as the zone ofprox.iQ;tJ.1 development (Z PD ).
rion is but on e example of the human child's ability to learn from experience, Vygo tsky observed the importance of con versations that child ren have with
and they see no need to assume mat there are specific brain structures devoted adults and wirh other children and saw in these conversations the origins of
to language acquisition. They hypothesize that what children need to know is both language and thought. The conversations provide the child with scaf
essentiall y available in the language they are exposed to as they hear it used in folding, that is, a kind ofsupportive structure that helps them make the most
thousands o f hours of interactions with the people and objects around memo of me knowledge the y have and also to acquire new knowledge.
Psychologists artribu re considerably more importance to the environment Vygotsky 's view differs from Piager 's. Piaget saw language as a symbol system
than the innarists do even though they also recognize a powerful learning that could be used to express knowledge acquired through interaction with
mechanism in the human brain. They see language acquisition as similar the physical world. For Vygorsky, thought was essentially internalized speech,
to and influenced by the acquisition of othe r kinds of skill and knowledge, and speech emerged in social interaction. Vygotsky's views have become
rather man as something that is different from and largely independent of increasingly central in research on second language development, as we will
see in Chap ter 4 .
Langu age learni ng in early childhood 27
26 Language learning in early childhood
Cross-cultural research Researchers working in a 'lan guage socialization' framework have found
that the kind of child-directed spe ech observed in m iddle-class American
Since the 1970s, researchers have stu died children's language learning envi
homes is by no means universal. In some societies, adults do nor engage in
ronments in a great many differem cultural communities. The research has
conversation or verbal play with very yo un g children. For example, Bambi
focused not only on the development oflanguage itself, but also on the ways
Schieffelin (1990) found that Kaluli mothers in Papua New Guinea did not
in which the env ironment provides what children need for language acquisi
consider their children co be appro priate co nversatio nal partners. Martha
tion . Betwee n 1985 and 1'997, Dan Slobin edited five volumes devoted co
Crago (1992) observed that in tradi tio nal In uit society, chil dre n are expected
research on the acquisit io n of28 lan gu ages, provid ing exam ples an d an alyses
to watch and listen to adults. Th ey are no t expec ted or en couraged to par
ofchild lang uage an d th e language-learning enviro nm ent fro m com m un ities
ticipate in conversations with adults until th ey are older and have more
around the worl d. One of the most remarkable resources for ch ild lan guag e
developed language skills.
researchers is the C hild Language D ata Excha nge System (CHILDES) ,
where researchers have contributed child language data in dozens of lan Other researchers have observed that in some societies, young children inter
guages in recorded and transcribed forms that are available as electronic files act primarily with older siblings who serve as th eir caregive rs. Even wi thin
from the C H ILD ES website (M acWhin ney 2000) . the United States, Shi rley Brice Heath (1983) and oth ers have documented
subs tantial differences in the ways parents in differen t socioeconomic and
One feature of cross-cultural research is th e description of child-rearing pa t
et hnic groups interact with their ch ildren. Neve rtheless, in every society,
terns. Ca the rine Snow (199 5) and others have studi ed th e apparem effects
child ren are in situations in which they hear language that is meaningful
on language acq uisition of the ways in which adults talk to and interact
to them in their enviro nment. And they acquire the community language.
with young children. In middle-class North American ho mes, researchers
Thus, it is diffic ult to judge the long-term effect of the modifications that
observed that adults often modify the way they speak when talking co lime
some adults make in speech add ressed to chil dren. .
children. This child-directed speech may be characterized by a slower rate of
delivery, higher pitch, m o re varied intonation, shorter, simpler sentence pat
terns, stress on keywords, frequen t repetition, and paraph rase. Furthermore,
The importance of interaction
copies of conversation emphasize the child 's immediate environment, picture The role of interaction between a language-learnin&'child and an interlocu
books, or experiences that the adult knows the child has ha d. Adults often tor who responds co the child is illuminated by caseswhere such int eractio n
repeat the content of a ch ild's utterance, but they expand or recast it into a is missing. Jacqueline Sachs and her co lleagues (19$"1) studied the lan guage
grammatically correct sentence. For example, when Peter says, ' D um p tr uck! developmenr o f a child they called J im. He was a heari ng child 0 f deafparen ts,
Dump truck! Fall! Fall!', Lois responds, 'Yes, th e dump truck fell down. ' an d his only con tact with oral lan guage was th ro ugh television, which he
watched frequen tly. The family was un usual in. that the parents did not use
sigh language wi th Jim. Thus , altho ugh in other respec ts he was well cared
(~
MJ 3
0
p~-b.Je · '-.Ja~ She not-tk1e ~
CMs,'ttere.ti
for, Jim did not begin his linguistic developmen t in a normal en vironment
in which a parem com m unicated with him in either oral or sign language. A
~ effect5 of
hr:
SuC""
~-.--J/), ) Li""':f-eA. / /A.ltJm..'.t~. t lca.( language assessment at three years an d nine months indicated tha t he was well
below age level in all aspects oflanguage. Although he attempted to express
f,v ~ i"~ ? ~ ~ ideas ap propriate "co his age, he used unusual, ungrammatical word order.
~?()-
. ,~ ~
~
o
~, When Jim began conversatio nal sessions with an adult, his expres sive abili
ties began to improve. By the age of four year s and two mo nths most of the
un usual speech patterns had disappeared, replaced by language mo re typical
of his age. Jim's younger brother Glenn did not display the same type of
language delay. Glenn's lingu istic environment was different from Jim's: he
had his older b roth er-s-no r only as a model, but, mo re importantly as a con
versati onal parmer whose interaction allowed Gle nn to develop language in
a more typical way.
28 L.mguage learning in (arly childhood Language learning in early childhood 29
Jim showed very rapid acqu isition of English once he began co interact wi th an sam e kinds of creative 'm istakes' that child ren make, such as p u tting a regular
ad ult on a one-co-one basis. The faCt mat he had f.U1ed coacquire language nor -ed ending on an irregular verb , for example, eared.
mally prior co this experience suggesrs mat impersonal sources oflanguage such In a usage-based model, language acquisition involves not only associating
as television o r radio alo ne are nor sufficienr. One-co-one interaction gives chil words with elemenrs ofexternal reality. It is also a process ofassociating words
dren access co language mat is adjusted co their level of comp rehensio n. When and phrases with the other words and phrases chat oc cur with them , or words
a child does not understand. me adult may repeat o r paraphrase. Th e respon se with grammatical morphemes rhar oc cur with them. For example, children
of me adult may also allow children co find our when their own urrerances are learning languages in which nouns have grammarical gender learn co associ
understood. Television , for obvious reasons, does not provide such interaction. are the appropriate article and adjective forms with nou ns. So if children are
Even in children's programmes, where simpler language is wed and topics are learning French, they learn mar La and une go with chaise (chair) and le and
relevant co younger viewers, no immediate adjustment is made for me needs un go with li vre (book). Similarly, they learn co associate pronouns with the
of an individual child. Once children have acquired some language, however, verb forms that mark person and number-il aim e (he likes) and nous aimons
television can be a source oflanguage and cultural information. (we like). They also learn which temporal adverbs go with which verb tenses.
Usage-based learning Of particular importance co chis hypothesis is me fact that children are
exposed co many thousands of opportunities co learn words and phrases.
As more and more research has documented the ways in which children
Learning takes place gradually, as the number of links between language and
inreracr with the environment, developmental and cognitive psychologists
meaning and am on g language form s are builr op . For usage-b ased th eori sts,
find furth er evide nce that langua ge acq uisition is 'usage-based' . In thi s view,
acq uisition of lan guage. whil e im p ressive, is not th e o nly rem arkable fear
lang uage acq uisitio n is possible because ofchild ren's gene ral cog nitive cap ac
acco mplished by th e ch ild . They co m pa re ir co o the r cogni rive and percep
ities and th e vast n um ber o f op po rtuni ties they hav e co make co n nec tions
rual learning, in clud ing learn ing co 'see' . Th at is, th e visua l abilities tha t we
between the lan guage they hea r and wh at th ey expe rience in the ir env iro n
take for gra nre d, for exam ple, focusin g o n and inrerpreting o bjects in o ur
men r. Sop hist icate d electro nic recording devices have been used co track
visual field , are actually learned through experience.
and count words and phrases children hear in their daily lives. Deb Roy
do cumenred his son's acquisition of words, showing the frequency and the
co nt exts for the occurrence of language. Most remarkable, perhaps, is the Language disorders and delays
demonstration of the power of interaction between the child and the adults Although most children progress through the stages of language develop
and how adults focus on the language me child has begun co use (Roy 2009). rnenr without significanr difficulty or delay, there are some children for
The usage -based perspe ctive on language acquisition differs from the behav whom thi s is not the case. A discus sion of the various rypes of disabilities
io urist view in that the emphasis is more on the child's abiliry co create (includ ing deafness, articulatory problems, autism, dyslex ia, and so on) mat
networks of associations rather than on processes of imitation and habit for sometimes affect language development is outside me scope of this book. It is
mati on .·Referred co by var ious names, including cognitive linguistics, thi s essen tial mat parents and teachers be encou raged. to seek profe ssional advice
view also differs sharp ly from the lnnatists' because language acquisition is if they feel that a child is not developing language'norrnally, keeping in mind
not seen as requiring a separate 'mod ule of the mind' but rather depends on that the range for 'normal' is wide indeed.
th e child's general learning abilities and the contributions of the environ While most children produce recognizable first w~~ds by 12 months, some
ment. As Elena Lieven and Michael Tomasello (2008) puc it, 'C hild ren learn may not speak before me age ofthree years. In very young children, one way to
language from their language experiences-mere is no other way' (p.168) . determine whether delayed language reflects a problem or simply an individual
According to this view, what children need co know is essentially available to difference within the normal range is ro determine whether me child responds
them in the language they are exposed roo co language and appears to understand even ifhe or she is nor speaking.
So me of the early research in this framework was done in the context of con For older children , delays in learning co read that seem out of keeping with
nectionism and involved compuce r simulations in which language samples a child's overall cognitive functioning may suggest that mere is a specific
were provided as input to a fairly simple program. The goal was co show mat problem in that domain . Some children seem to begin reading almost by
the com p uter could ' learn' certain things if exposed to enough examples. magic, discovering the mysteries of print with little direct instruction.
The program was found to be able to SOrt out the paceerns from the input For most children, instruction that includes some systematic artenrion to
and even generalize beyond what it was actually exposed rooIt even made the
30 Language learning in early chi ldhood Language learning in early childhood 31
sound-Ierrer correspondences allows them to unlock me treasure chest of languages substantially slows down their linguistic development or interferes
reading. Both groups fall within a normal range . For some children, however, with cognitive development.
reading presents such great chal lenges that they need expert help beyond
Ind eed , many ch ildren attain high levels of proficiency in both lan guages.
what is available in a typical classroom.
Ellen Bialystok (2001) and other cognitive and developmental psychologists
have found convincing evidence m at achieving bilingual pro ficiency can
Childhood bilingualism have positive effects on abili ties mat are related to academic success, such as
Th e language development of chi ldren who lear n multiple languages during mecallnguisric awareness. Lim itations mat may be o bserved in me language
childhood is of enormous importance throughout the world. Indeed, the of bilingual individuals are more likely to be related to me circumstances in
majority of the world's children are exposed to more than one language. which each language is learned man to any limitation in me human capacity
Some children learn m ultiple languages from earlies t childhood; others to learn more than one language. For example, ifone language is heard much
acquire additional lang uages when they go to school. The acquisitio n and more often than the other or is more highly valued in me community, that
maintenanc e of more than one language can open doors to many pe rsonal, language may eventual ly be used better man, or in preference to, the other.
social, and economic opportunities. One aspect of bilingual language use is referred to as code switching-the use
Unfortunately, as Jim Cummins (2000) an d others have poi n ted out, ch il ofwords or phrases from mo re than one lan guage wi thin a conversation. Fo r
dren who already know one or mo re languages and who arrive ar their first example, a child who speaks bo th French an d English m ight say, 'I'm playing
day of school without an age-app ropriate knowledge of the language of the with le cnduau' . Such switching between lan guages may sometimes reflect
school have often been misdiagnosed as having lang uage delays or disorders. the abse nce of a particular vocab ulary word or expression, but it can just as
Th is includes immigrant and minority language children who do nor speak often be th e intentional use of a word from the otherlanguage for a variety
the school language at home and children who speak a different variety of ofin reracrio nal purposes. Highly proficient adul t bilinguals also code switch
the school lan gu age. These children's knowledge of a different language or when they speak to others who also know both languages. The use of both
language variety is often incorrectly interpreted as a lack of normal language languages within a bilingual context is not evidence of a lack of p roficien cy. It
development and a lack of background knowledge for school subjects. They may have many di fferent mo tivations, fro m expressJng solidarity ro making
may be placed in remedial or special ed ucatio n classes because schools are not a joke. Psychologists have shown that speakers of mo re th an one language
equipped ro provide an adequate assessme nt of children 's ability to use their are constantly making choices abou t how ro express themselves and that
ho me language o r of their general cogn itive abilities or their knowledge of code switching is patterned and often predictable. Indeed, this experience
school su bjects, learned through another language. Researchers have recen tly in making choices has been ide ntified as contributi ng to cognitive flexibility
rhro ughour life (Bialystok 2009).
mad e important pro gress in p rovid ing gu idelines that can help educators ,
distinguish between disability and diversity (Paradis, Genesee, and Crago As 'ch ild ren learn a second language at school, rhey need ro learn both the
2011), but much practical work remains to be done so that children can variety oflanguage that children use among themselves (an d in informal set
make the most of their cognitive and linguistic abilities. tings wi th familiar adults) and the variety that i5[used in academic settings.
Children who lear n more than one language from earliest ch ildhood are In his ear ly research on childhood bili ngualis m, Jim Cummins called these
referred to as 'simultaneous bilinguals', whereas those who lear n another lan two varie ties BlCS (basic interpersonal co mmuni2tion skills) and CALP
guage later may be called 'seq uential bilinguals'. We sometimes hear people (cognitive academic language proficiency). Charaderistics o f the two varie
express the opinion that it is roo difficul t for children to cope with two lan ties overlap ro a certain extent, but there are important differences, not JUSt
guages. They fear tha t the children will be confused or w ill not learn either in the range ofvocabulary that each requires but also in the way informatio n
language well. However, th ere is littl e su p po rt for the myth that lear ning is expressed. Mary Schlep pegrell (2004) and others have sought ro discover
more tha n o ne lan guage in ear ly childhood is a problem for ch ild ren who what exactly it is that characterizes th ese varieties oflanguage and the interac
have adequate op porrunities to use each one. Th ere is a co nsiderable body tio n pa tterns that tend to go with th em, and some aspectS of the distinction
of research on children's ability co learn more than one language in thei r remain controversial. It is widely agreed, however, that the lan guage need ed
earliest years. Al though some studies show minor early delays in one or both for academic discourse is mo re difficul t for ch ildren to acq uire than me info r
lan guages for simultaneous bilinguals, th ere is no evidence that learning two mal language of day-to-day interaction (Cummins 2000).
32 Language learning in ear(y childhood Language learn ing in early childhood 33
Children entering school with little or no knowledge of me language spoken yer mastered me school language. and they have not continued to develop the
there may acq ui re BICS wirhin a relatively sho rt time- as lime as a year or family lan gu age. During me transi tio n period, they may fall behind in their
two . They learn from wa tching and imitating interactions among their peers academic learning . Unfo rtuna tely, me 'solutio n' educators sometimes pro pose
and between teachers and students. They make connections between fre co parents is that they should scop speaki ng me family language at home and
que ntly heard wo rds and ph rases and th e routines and recurring even ts of m e co ncentra te instead on speaking me scho ol lan guage with thei r children.
classroom, cafete ria, and playgro und. For th is reason , students are so metimes
The research evidence suggests that a better approach is co strive for addi
perceived as ' fluent' in thei r seco nd language. This can lead teachers to assume
tive bilingualism-th e maintenan ce of the home language while the seco nd
mat any difficulti es in acade m ic tasks are nor du e to limi ted lan guage skills but
language is being learned. This is especially true if me parents are also lear n
to othe r causes- fro m lack of mo tivation to learning disabilities. More care ful
ers of the seco nd lan gu age. If pare nts continue to use th e language mat they
observa tio n shows mar th e students, while fluent in social setti ngs, do not have
know best with th eir ch ildr en" th ey are ab le to exp ress th eir kn owledge and
me CALP skills needed for academ ic tasks such as understanding a p robl em in
ideas in ways m at are rich er an d mor e elabo rate man th ey can manage in a
marhemarics, defini ng a wo rd , o r writing a science report.
. language they do not know as well. Using their own lan guage in fam ily set
Virg inia Collier (1989) found that, for most students, acquiring age-appro tings is also a way for parents ro maintain their own self-esteem, especially as
priate CALP takes seve ral yea rs. As the seco nd language learner rries to catch they may have th eir own struggles with the new language outside th e home,
up, th e children who came to school already sp eaki ng the school lan gu age at work, or in me community. Maintaining me family language also allows
are continui ng to learn hundreds of new words every yea r and to learn the children co rerain family co n nectio ns with gran dparents or relatives who do
concep ts char these words rep resen t , If second language learn ers have limited not speak the new language. They benefit from me o ppo rt un ity to continue
knowledge of the schoo l language and do not have opportunities to continue both cognitive and affecti ve development using a language th ey understand
learn ing academic content in a language they already know. it is not surpris easily while they are st ill learning the second language. .
ing that they fall behind in learning the academic subjecr matter th at their
Other positive effects of bilingual or multilingual development go beyond
Children need time to develo p th eir seco nd lan gu age skills. M any people one language can also increase opportunities for cross-cultural communica
assume that th is mea ns th at th e best app roac h is to start learn in g as early tion and econo mic cooperatio n amo ng people. As W~ have seen, develo ping
as possible and to avoid the use of me ch ild's p reviously learned languages. a seco nd language takes years. Bu t teachers. parents, and sru de nrs need
Certain ly, it is im portant for ch ildren to begin learn in g an d using th e school to know that the many benefits of additive bilingual ism w ill reward thei r
language as earl y as poss ible, but co nside ra b le research suggests th at co ntin patience and effort.
ued development of th e chil d's home lan gu age act ual ly co nt ribu tes in me
long term to m ore suc cessful acq uisitio n of th e scho ol language. Resear ch ers
an d ed uca to rs have exp ressed concern abo ut situatio ns whe re ch ild re n are
Summary
cut off fro m the ir fam ily lan gu age when they are very yo ung, spending long In this chapter we have focused on some of the.research on 'Children's early
h ours away from th eir fam ilies in sett ings whe re the ho me language is absent language develo pmen r th a t has inf!uenced research 0 n seco nd langu age acq ui
or even forbidden . Lily \'V'on g Fillmore (2000) obs erved that when children sitio n. We have described three b roa d th eo retical perspect ives for exp laini ng
are 'su b me rged' in a di fferent language for long periods in pre-s chool or first language acquisition. In Chap te r 2 we will loojeac some o f th e find ings
day care, th eir development of the famil y language may be slowed down or of research examin ing th e de veloping lan guage of seco nd lang uage learners.
stal led b efore they have developed an age-app rop riate p roficiency in m e new
language. Event ual ly th ey may sto p speaki ng the fam ily language altogether,
and this loss of a common language can lead to sign ificant social an d psycho
Questions for reflection
logical probl em s. Some researc h has found that the best predictor of children's voc abu lary
growth is the amount of language addressed to them by their parents and
Wallace Lambert (1987) called m e loss of one lan guage on me way to lear n
other caregive rs .What have you seen in this chapter that is compatible w ith
ing anomer subtractive bilingualism. It can have negati ve consequences for
that flnding !
child ren's self-esteem , an d their relationships w ith fam ily m embers are also
likely to be affected by such early loss of the family language. In these cases, 2 Go to the children 's section of a library o r bookstore and loo k at the
ch ildre n seem to continue to be caugh t betw een two lan guages: they have not vocabulary used in books that are pub lished for children between three
34 Language learning in early childhood
and six years o ld.Compare these to books for young readers. aged six to
eight.What does this suggest about the impo rtance of continuing to read
SECOND LANGUAGE
2
to children after they have begun to learn to read at school? Finally. look at
the language used in textbooks for children at age 10 or I I.What can you
conclude about the challenge faced by English language learners entering LEARNING
school at this age?
3 Ifyou are or may be te aching a second language to a gro up of school
aged learn ers with different first language backgrounds. can you think of
pedagog ical casks/activities in which children can display and use the ir LI
knowledge to help them learn the second language?
On th e one han d , cognirive maruriry and meralinguistic awareness allow C lassroo m learn ers not only spend less tim e in co nt act with th e new lan
older learners to solve problems and engage in discuss ions abour lan guage. guag e, they also tend to be exposed to a far smaller ran ge of discourse rypes.
This is particularly important for those who are lear ning language in a class For example, classroom learners are oft en taught language that is somewhat
roo m, with limited time in contact with the language. On the other hand, form al in comparison to the language as it is used in most social settings. In
some theorists have suggested that the use ofthese cognitive skills-so valua many fa reign language classes, teachers may even switch to their students' first
ble for many kinds of tasks- can actually interfere with language acquisition. lan guage fo r discipline or classroom management, thus depriving learners of
Th ey argue m at successful language acquisiti on draws o n different mental oppo rtu nities ro experience uses of the language in real communication.
abilities, abilities that are spe cific to lan guage lear ning. It has been suggest ed As we saw in C hapter 1, parenrs tend to respond to th e meani ng rather th an
that older learners draw on their problem-so lving and metalinguistic abiliti es co the grammatical accuracy of thei r children's language. Similarly, in second
precisely because they can no lo nger access the innate language acquisition language lear ning outside classrooms, errors that do not interfere with
abiliry they had as young children. We will have more to say about this in meaning are usually overlooked. Most people would feel they were being
Chapter 3, when we discuss the role o f age in second language acquisition. impolite if they in terrupred and corrected someone who was trying to have
In addition to possib le cognitive differen ces, th ere are also attitudinal and cul a conversation with them. Nevertheless, interlocutors may react to an error
tu ral differences berwee n children an d ad ults . Most ch ild learners are willing if they cannot understand what the speaker is trying co say. Thus, errors of
to try to use the language-even when th eir pro ficiency is quite lim ited . grammar and pronunciation may nor be remarked on, bur th e wrong word
. Many adults and adolescents find it stressful when they are unable to express choice may receive com m en t from a puzzled interlocutor. In a situation whe re
themselves clearly .and correctly. Nevertheless, even very young (pre-school) a second language speaker appears to use inappropriate language, interlocu
chi ldren differ in their willingness to speak a language they do not know well. tor s may feel un co mfortable, not knowi ng whether..the speaker intends to be
Some chil dren happily chatter away in their new lan guage; others prefer to rude or simply does not know the polite way to say ~h:i't is intended. In this
liste n an d participate silently in social interactio n with thei r peers. case coo, espec ially berween adults, it is unlikely that the second language
speaker would be told that something had gone wrong. The only place where
feedback on error is typically presenc with high frc;quency is the lan guage
Learning conditions classroo m . Even there, it is no t always provided consis ten tly. In C hapters
Young second language lear ners are often allowe d to be silent u ntil they are 5 and 6. research on the role of feedback in the classroo m will be reviewed.
ready to speak . They may also practise th eir seco nd language in songs an d
One condition that appears to be common to learners ofall ages-though not
games that allow th em to blend their voices with those of other ch ildren.
in equal qualiry or quantiry-is expos ure to modified or adap ted input. This
O lde r second language learners are often forced to speak from the earli
adjusted speech style , called child-directed speech in first language acquisi
est days of their learning, whether to meet the req uirem en ts of classroom
tioh, has sometimes been called foreigner talk .or teacher talk depending
instruction or to carry out everyday tasks such as shopping, medical visits, or
on the contexts of second language acquisition. Some people wh o interact
job interviews.
regula rly with lan guage learners seem to have an intuitive sense of what
Another way in which younger and older learners may differ is in the amou nt adjustments they need to m ake to help learners tlnderstand. Of course, not
of time they can actually spend learning a second language. We know that everyone knows what adjustments will be most helpful. We have all witnessed
first language learners spend thousands ofhou rs in contact with the language those pa inful conversations in which people seenj-to th in k that they can
or languages spoken around them . Young second language lear ners may make learners understand better if they simply talk louder! Some Canadian
also be exposed to their second language for many hours every day-in the friends told us of an experience they had in China. They were visiting some
classroom. on the playground. or in fronc of the television. Older learners, historic temples and wanted to ger more informatio n about them than they
especially students in foreign lan guage classrooms, receive far less expos ure could glean fro m a guidebook, so they asked their guide some questions.
perhaps only a: few hours a week. Indee d , a typical foreig n lan guage student Unfo rtu nately, their limi ted Chinese and his non-existent Englis h made it
will have no mo re than a few hundred hours of exposure, spread om over a difficult for them to exchange information. The guide kept speaking louder
number of years. Adu lt learners who are immigrants or minoriry language and louder, bu r ou r friends understood very little. Fina lly, in frust ratio n , rhe
speakers often continue to use the language they already know as they fulfil guide concluded that it would help if they could see the informatio n, so he
th eir daily responsibilities for work and family, and they may use the second rook a stick and began wri ting in th e sand-in Chinese characrers!
language only in limited situations.
40 Second IdnguugeLearning Second Language Learning 41
language can be q uite perplexing ifo ne does no t have an overall picture o f the
LVtf",.to.4/la.u~, -o.~ e"t.ire pl... ",b;"S ~~ t-e ..,
is. eu.rre n I:' ~ l..U1aer r.epce;.r . It ~ M. ros' ,1k
'tP ~ -tj.,e. ,.....bl.·c. ~"""",",,':~,es
Sita-H.,( .u>D....c f.·.e
1I.tocJ<,S -f.-o...,
iCA eve
I:c " ,.
hete .. ~:-~ ) ~
?:
/
f)
~~
steps learners go through in acquiring the seco nd language.
In p resenting some of the findings of second language research, we have
included a number ofexamples oflearner language as well as some additional
sam p les to give you an opportunity to practise analysing learner language. Of
~' ~ c'~ ~ e!~.~0' course, teachers analyse learner language all the time. They try co determine
~, ~ ,> whether students have learned what has been taugh t and how closely their
language matches the target language. But progress cannot always be meas
. ·;tE1~.a
ured in these terms . Sometimes language acquisition progress is reflected
in a decrease in the use of a correct form that was based on rote memoriza
'"
\.~
J1~ r Fl-t6HI M~,v...
I N FoP. M,q,T, ON
{,..C; -
tion or chunk learning. New errors may be based on an emerging ability
to generalize a particular grammatical form beyond the specific items with
.. wh ich it was first learned . In thi s sen se, an increase in error may be an indica
tion of progress. For example, like nrst language learners, second language
learners usually learn the irregular past tense forms of certain common verbs
before they learn to apply the regular simple past -ed marker. That means
t ha t a learner w ho says ' I bu yed a bus ticket' may know more abou t En glish
gram mar than o ne who savs ' I bo ug h t a bus tickct., Wit ho u t further infer
ma rio n. we cannot co ncl ude that th e one who says ' bo ugh t' wo uld use th e
regu lar past -ed marker w here it is approp riate , bu t the lear ner w ho says
This bri ef d iscu ssion places th e em p hasi s on how bo th th e cha racte ristics of ' buyed ' has p rovided ev id enc e o f develo pi ng kn owledg e of a syste ma tic
learners and the contexts in which they acquire a seco nd language m ay be aspect of English.
,
different. In the following pages , we will focus more on similarities in how
Teachers and researchers cannor read learners' minds , so they must infer
their knowledge of the new language develops over tim e.
what lear ners know by observing what they do. Like those who study first
language acquisition , we observe learners' spontaneous language use, but
Studying the language of second we also design procedures that help to reveal more about the knowledge
language learners underlying their ob servable use oflanguage. WitP.0ut these procedures, it is
oft~n difficult to determine whether a particular. behaviour is representative
We have seen that children's knowledge of the grammatical system of their of something systematic in a learner's current language knowledge or sim ply
first language is built up in predictable seq uenc es. For example, grammat i an isolated item, lear ned as a ch u nk . 0(
cal morphemes such as the -ing of the present progressive or the -ed of the 1
Like first language learners, second language learners do not learn language
sim ple past are not acquired at the same time, but in a sequence. Are there
sim ply through im itation and practice. They prod~c5 sentences that a re not
developmental seq ue nces for second language acquisition? How does the
exactly like those they have heard. These new sentences appear to be based
prior knowledge of the first language affect the acquisition of the second (or
on internal cognitive processes and prior knowledge tha t interact with the
third) language? How does instruction affect second language acquisition?
language they hear arou nd them . Both first and seco nd lan gu age acquisition
Are there differences in the development oflearners whose only contact with
are best described as developing systems with their ow n evolving rules and
the new language is in a classroom and those who use the language in daily
patterns, no t simply as imperfect versions of the target language.
lite? These are some of the questions researchers have sought to answer, and
we will address them in this chapter as well as in Chapters 5 and 6 .
Contrastive analysis, error analysis, and interlanguage
Knowing more about the development oflearner language helps teachers ro
assess teaching procedures in the light of wha t they can reasonably expect to Until the late 1960s, people tended to see second language learn ers' speech
accomplish in the classroom. As we will see , some characteristics of learner simply as an incorrect version of the target language. Acco rding to the con
trastive analysis hypothesis (CAH) , errors were assumed to be the result
42 Second language learning
Second language learning 43
of transfer from learners' first language. Detailed analysis of learners' errors
discover and describe different kinds of errors in an effort to understand
revealed , however, that not all errors made by second language lear ners can
how learners process second language data. Error analysis was based o n the
be explained in terms of first lan guage transfer alone. A number of studies
hypothesis that, like child language, second language learner lan guage is a
show that many erro rs can be explained better in terms of learners' develop
system in irs own right-one thar is rule-governed and predictable.
ing knowledge of the structure of the target language rath er than an atte mpt
to tr ansfer patterns of the ir first language (Richards 1974). Furthermore, Larry Selinker (1972) gave the name incerlanguage to learners ' develo ping
some of the errors are remarka bly similar to those made by you ng first lan second lan guage knowledge. Analysis of a learner's inte rlanguage shows that
guage learners, for example, the use of a regular -ed past tense ending on an it has.some characreristics influenced by previously learned languages , some
irregular verb . characteristics of the seco nd language, and some characteristics, such as the
omission of function words and grammatical morphemes, that seem to be
A simplified version of the CAH would predic t that, where differences exist,
general and to occur in all interlanguage systems. Interlanguages have been
erro rs would be bi-directional, that is, for examp le. French speakers lear ning
foun d to be systematic, but they are also dynamic, continually evolving as
Englis h and English speakers learning French woul d make errors on paral
learners receive more input and revise th eir hypotheses about the second
lei linguis tic featu res. Helmut Zo bl (1980) observed that thi s is not always
language. Th e path through language acquisi tion is no t necessarily smooth
the case. For example, in simple English sentences, direct objects, whether
and even . Learners have bursts of progress , then reach a plateau for a while
nouns or pronouns, come after the verb ('The dog eats the cookie. Th e dog
befo re something stim ulates furth er progress. Selinker also coined th e term
eats it.') . In French. direct objects that are no uns follow the verb (Le chien
fossilization to refer to the fact that so me features in a learner's language seem
mange Ie biSCUit-literally, 'The dog eats the cookie'). However, direct object
to Stop changing. This may be especially true for learners whose exposure to
pronouns precede the verb (Le chien le mange-li terally, 'The dog it eats').
the second language does not include instruction or-the kind offeedback that
The CAH would predict tha t a nativ e speaker of English might make the
would help them to recog nize differences between their interlanguage and
erro r of saying: ' Le chien mange le' when learning French, and that a native
the target language.
speaker of French might say 'The do g it eats' when learn ing English. In fact,
English speakers learn ing French are more likely to make the predicted
error than French speakers learning English . This may be due to the fact ACTIVITY Analyse learner language :.;
that English speakers learning French hear many examples of sentences with
The follow ing t exts were written by two learn ers of English. one a French
subject-verb-object word order (for example, Le chien mange le biscuit) and
speaking secondary school student, the other a Chinese-speakin g adult learner.
make the incorrect generalizatio n-based on both the word order of th eir
first language and evidence from the second lang uage- that all direc t objects
Both learn e rs were des cribing a cartoon film entitle~ The Great Toy Robbery
(National Film Board of Canada) . After viewing the.film. they were aske d to
come after the verb. French-speaking learners of Engli sh, o n the other hand,
retell the story in writing, as ifthey were telling it to someone w ho had not seen
hearing and seeing no evidence that English direct object pronouns precede ~fi~ '
verbs, do not tend to use this pattern from their first language.
Read the texts and answer the following questions:1'
The findi ng that many aspec rs of learners ' language could not be explained
by the CAH led a number of researchers to take a different approach to ana I Can you unde rstand what each learner is trying to say?
lysing learners' errors . This approach, wh ich developed during th e 1970s, 2 Examine the errors made by each learn e r. What kirtd's of errors interfere
became known as 'error analysis' and involved detailed descriptions of the most with yo ur ability to understand?
errors second language learners made. The goal of this research was to dis 3 Do both learners make the same kinds of errors?
cover what learners really knew about the language. As Pit Corder observed
in a famous arti cle published in 1967 , when learners produce correct sen 4 In what ways do t he two interlanguages differ?
tences. they may simply be repeating something they have already heard; Learner I: Fre nch first language , secondary school student
when they prod uce sen tences that differ from the target language, we may Du r ing a sun ny day, a cowboy go in t he desert with his ho rs e . he has a big hat.
assume that these sentences reflect the lear ners' current understanding of the His horse eat a flo ur. In the same time. Santa Clause go in a city to give some
rules and pattern s of tha t language. We saw this in the example of a learner surprises. He has a red costume and a red packet of surprises.You have three
who says 'buyed' instead of 'bought.' Erro r analysis differed from contras robbers in th e mountain who sees Santa C lause with a king of glaces that it
tive analysis in tha t it did not set out to predict errors. Rather, it sought to permitted us to see at a long distance. Every robbers have a horse.They go in
44 Second I.mguage learning Second I.mgufJge learning 45
the way of Santa Clause. not Santa Clause but his pocket of sur pr ises. After 'interference ' errors . Wha t is m ost clear. however, is thar ir is often difficult
they willgo in a city and they go in a saloon. [. . .] to determine the so urce o f errors. Thus , while erro r anal ysis has the advan
(Unpublished data from P. M. Lightbown and B. Barkman) tage of describing what learners actu ally do rather than what they might do ,
Learner 2: Chinese first language. adult it does not always give us clear insights int o why they do ir. Furthermore,
This year Christmas comes soon! Santa Claus ride a one horse open sleigh to as Jacquelyn Schachter pointed out in a 1974 article, learn ers so meti mes
sent present for children. on the back of his body has big packet. it have alot of avoid using some features of lan guage th at they perceive to be diffic ult for
toys . in the way he meet three robbers.They want to take his big packet. Santa them. This avoidance ma y lead to th e absence o f certain errors, leaving th e
Claus no way and no body help. so only a way give them, then three robbers anal yst without information about so me aspects o f th e learn ers' develop
r ide their horse dashing through the town.There have saloon, they go to dr ink ing intedanguage. Th e absence of particular erro rs is difficult to interpret,
so me beer and open the big packent.They plays toys in the Bar.They meet a and the phenomen on of 'avoidance' may itself be a part of th e learner's sys
cow boy in the saloon. tema tic second langu age performance.
(Unpublishe d data provided by M. J, Martens)
Developmental sequences
Perha ps the most striking thing her e is that man y erro r rypes are co m mo n ro Second language learners, like first lan gua ge learn ers, pass throu gh sequences
bo th learners. Furthermore. both make err ors of spelling and punctuation of development: what is learned early by one is learned early by others.
tha t we m ight find in th e writing ofa yo ung first lan gu age spea ker o f Eng lish. Amo ng first lang uage learne rs, the existence ofd evelopmen tal sequences may
Even th ough Fren ch uses grammatical mor phemes to indicat e person and not seem surp rising because their language learning is partly tied to the ir cog
number on verbs and C hinese does not, both these learners make errors of n itive de velo pm ent and to thei r expe riences in learn ing abo ut relation sh ips
subject-verb agreemenr-s-borh leaving off the third person -s marker and betwee n peop le, events, and objects arou nd them. But the cognitive develop
overus ing it when the su bject is plural Ca cowboy go' and 'th ree robbe rs in the ment of adul t or ado lescent second language learne rs is much more stable.
mo untain who sees' by Learne r 1 and 'Santa C laus ride' and 'th ey plays' by and thei r experiences wi th the langu age are likely to- be q uit e different, not
Learne r 2) . Such errors reflect learners' understanding of th e seco nd lan gua ge o nly from the experiences of a sm all child, but also different from each other.
system itsel f rath er than an attem pt to transfer characteristics of their first Furth ermo re, second language learners already knowanother language th at
lan gua ge. They are some tim es referred to as 'd evelop ment al' erro rs because has different partern s for creati ng sent ences and wo rd form s. In light of th is,
th ey are sim ilar to those made by children acquiring English as their first lan it is more remarkable that we find developmental sequences that are similar
guage. Sometimes these are errors ofovergeneralizarion , th at is, errors caused in the developing interlanguage of learners from ,different language back
by tryin g to use a rule in a conte xt where it does not belong, for exam ple, th e grounds and also similar to those observed in first langua ge acquisition of th e
-s end ing on th e verb in 'they plays'. Sometimes the errors are better described same language. Moreover, the features of th e language that are most frequent
as sim plification. where elements of a sentence are left out or whe re all verbs are not always learned firsr. Fo r exam ple, virt uallYFery Eng lish senten ce has
have th e sa me form regardless o f person , number, o r ten se. o ne or more arti cles ('a' or 'the'), but even ad vanced learn ers have difficulty
using these forms correctly in all contex ts. Finall y, altho ugh th e learner's first
O ne can also see, especially in learner 2's text , th e influ ence of classroom
experience. An exam ple is the use of formulaic expres sions such as 'o ne horse language does have an influ en ce, many aspects of thcr.o9 developmental stages
ope n sleigh' which is taken verbatim from a well-known Christmas song that are sim ilar among learn ers from different first langu;ge backgrounds.
had been taught and sung in his English as a Second Language (ESL) class. In Chapter 1 we saw some developmental sequences for English first lan
The vivid 'dashing through the town' probably comes from th e same source, guage acquisition of grammatical morphemes, negation, and questions.
with the substitution of 'rown' for 'snow'. Researchers in second language acquisition have also examined these, as well
as other features . They have found patterns in th e development ofsyntax and
For those who are familiar with the English spo ken by native speake rs of
French, so me of the errors (for exam ple, preposition cho ice 'in the same morphology that are sim ilar among learners from different language back
tim e') made by the first learner will be seen as probably based on French. grounds. Evidence for th ese developmental patterns first came from studies
o f learners wh ose primary learning en vironment was o utsid e th e classroom.
Sim ilar ly, tho se familiar with the English of Chinese speake rs may recog
n ize some word order patterns (for example, 'o n the back of his body has For examp le, JUrgen Meisel, Harald C lahsen, and Manfred Pienemann
big packet') as based on C hinese patterns . These may be called transfer o r
46 Secondlanguage Learning Secondlanguage Learning 47
plural
Subsequent research has shown that learners who rece ive instruction exhibit copula ('to be ')
.m o rphem e, that is, the places in a sentence where the mo rpheme is necessary
to make the sentence grammatically correct. Fo r example, in the sentence
'Yesterday I play baseball for rwo hours', the adverb 'yesterd ay' creates an
obligatory context for a past ten se, and 'fo r twO hours' tells us that th e required
•
reg ular past -ed
third person singular -s ""
form is a simple past ('played') rather than a pas t progressive ('was playi ng') . possessive's
Similarly, 'two' creates an o bligatory context for a plural-s on 'hours'.
For the analysis, ob ligatory co ntexts fo r each grammatical morpheme are Figure 2.1 Krashen 's (/982) summary of second language grammatical morpheme
counted separately, that is, one count for simple past, one fo r plu ral, one for acquisition sequence J!.
third person singular present tense , and so o n . Afte r co u nting th e n um ber of
The sim ilar ity among learners suggests that the accuracy o rder canno t be
obligato ry COntexts, the researcher counts the correctly supplied morphemes.
described or explained in terms of transfer from the learners' first lan guage,
The next step is to d ivide the number of correctly supplied morphemes by
and so m e researchers saw this as strong evidence against th e CAH . However,
the total n um ber of obligatory co ntexts to answer the question 'w hat is the
a thorough review of all the 'm o rp hem e acquisition' studies shows tha t the
percentage accuracy for each morpheme?' An accuracy score is created for
leafners' first language does have an infl uence on; acquisition seq uences. For
each morpheme, an d these can then be ranked from highest to lowest, giving
example, learners whose first language has a possessi ve form that resembles
an accuracy order for the morphemes.
the Eng lish s (such as German and Dani sh ) se1m to acqui re the English
The overall results of the studies suggested an order that was similar but not po ssessive earl ier than those whose first language 'h as a very different way of
ide ntical to the developmental sequence found fo r first language learners. forming the po ssessive (such as Frenc h or Spani sh). And even though art i
H owever, the order th e researchers found was quite similar among second cles appear early in the seq uence, lear ners fro m manylanguage background s
lan guage lear ners from d ifferent first language backgro u nds . For exam ple , (in cluding Slavic languages, Chinese, and Japanese) continue to struggle
most studies showed a higher deg ree of accuracy for plural -s than for posses with this aspect of English, even at advanced levels. Learners may do well in
s,
sive - and for -ing than for regular pas t (-etl). Stephen Kr ashen summarized supplying articles in certain obligatory contexts but not others. If the lan
th e order as shown in Figure 2. 1. The diagram sho uld be interpreted as guage sample that is analysed COntains only th e 'easier' obligatory co ntexts,
showing that ' lear ners will produce the morphemes in h igher boxes with the lear ner may have a m islead in gly h igh accur acy score.
higher accuracy than those in lower boxes, but that within boxes , there is no
Another reason why something as di fficult as English articles appears to be
clear pattern of difference.
acquired early is that the order in the diagram is based on t he analysis of
correct use in o bligatory co ntexts only. It does not take in to acco unt uses of
grammatical m orp hem es in places where they do not belong, for example,
when a lear ner says, 'The France is in Europe' . These issues led researchers
48 Second langu.lge learning Second I.mguuge learning 49
to que stion th e adequacy of ob ligatory co ntext anal yses as th e so le basis to r when the y are under pressu re. Th us, sirnilarirv to a learn er's first language
understanding developmental seq uences. Teresa Pica (1983) argued that may slow down a learner's progress th rough a particular developmental stage.
accuracy scores should take account of overuse and incorrect uses to deter
mine a 'score for target-like use rather than reflect only use in obligatory Stage 2
At this stage, 'no' and 'no t' ma y alt ernate with 'do n't' . H owever, 'do n't' is not
Con te-xts.
mar ked for person , numbe r, o r tense an d it may even be used before mod als
The morphem e acquisition literature raises other issues, not least of them the like 'can' and 'sho uld' .
question of why there should be an order of acquisition for these language
fearu res.So me of the similarities observed in different studies seemed to be He don't like it.
due to the use ofparticular tasks for collecting the dar a, and researchers found I don 't can sing.
of variables that contribute to the order. Salience (how easy it is to notice the He was not happy.
. morpheme), linguistic co mplexity (fo r example, how man y elements yo u She d on 't like rice.
have to keep track of), semant ic trans paren cy (how clear th e meaning is). At this stage, German speakers, whose first language has a st ructu re that
sirni lariry ro a first language form , and freque ncy in the input all seem to play places the nega tive after the verb may gene ralize th e auxiliary-negative
a role. pattern to verb-negative and produce sentences such as:
ten ce because the subject is not there. Manfred Pienemann, Malcolm Johnston, and CSeoff Brindley (1988)
No bicycle .
I no like it.
Not my friend .
Stage 6) come from French speakers who we re playing a game in which they
'N o' is preferred by most learners in this early stage, perhaps because it is had to ask questions in order to find out wh ich picture the orher player
the negative form that is easiest to hear and recognize in the speech th ey are (the researcher) was holding. As we saw for negation , the overall sequence is
exposed to. Italian- and Spanish-speaking learners may prefer 'no' because it similar to the one observed in first language acquis ition. And again , there are
co rrespo nds to the negative form in Italian and Spanish (No tienen mu chos some differences that are attributable to first language influence.
libros). They may conrinue to use Stage 1 negation longer than other learners
because of the similarity to a pattern from their first language. Even at more Stage 1
ad van ced stages, they may also use Stage) negativ es in longer sentences or Single words, formulae , or sent ence fragm ents.
50 Second language learn ing Second langu age learning 51
Dog?
ungrammatical to use inversion with a noun subject (" Pou rq uoi aiment les
Four children?
enfanrs le chocolat /).
What's that?
Stage 6
Stage 2 Complex questions.
D eclarative word order, no inversion, no fronting. question tag: It's better, isn'r it?
It's a monster in th e right corner?
negative q uestio n: Why can' t you go?
The boys th row the shoes?
embedded ques tion: Can you tell me whar the date is today?
Declarative order with rising intonation is common in yes/no questions in
Pienemann's developmental sequence for questions has been che basis for
informal spoken Fren ch . French speakers may hypothesize that in English,
a number of studies, some of which will be discussed in Chapter 6. Alison
as in French, inve rsion is optional.
Mackey and her colleagues have done a number of these studies, and she pro
vided the da ta in Table 2.2. These examples come from three"aduIr Japanese
Stage 3
learners of English as a second language who were interacting with a na tive
Fronting: do-fronting, wh-fronting without inversion, other fronting.
speaker in a 'Sp ot the differences' task. In rhis task, learners have similar but
Do you have a shoes on yo ur picture?
no t identical pictures and they have to ask q uestions un til they work OUt how
Where the children are playing?
the pictu re they can see is different from the one their interlocuror has. Note
Does in this picture the re is four astronauts?
char p rogress ro a higher srage does not always mean char learners produce
Is the picture has rwo planets on top?
fewer errors .
French has an invariant form est-ce que (literally ' is it that') that can be placed
. . Stage
Where is the sun?
\
Learner'
Is there a fish in the water?
inversion, it may also permit inversion with full verbs, as German does, leading
2 Is the room his room? I 2 3 4 5 6
them to pro duce questions such as 'Like you baseball?' (Magst du basebaip.)
3 Is he caking out his skate board? ,. I 2 3 4 5 6
,:'\J
Stage 5
4 What is he thinking? I 2 3 4 5 6
Inversion in wh- questions with both an auxiliary and a main verb .
French-speaking learners may have diffic ulty using Stage 5 questions in Learner 2
which the subject is a noun rather than a pro noun. They may say (and 6 Are they buying some things? I 2 3 4 5 6
acce pt as grammatical) 'Why do you like chocolate?' but not 'Why do chil
7 Is they bought present? I 2 3 4 5 6
d ren like cho cola te?' In th is, they are drawing on French, where it is often
-J Sa ond lungudge learn ing
) - Second language learning 53
8 Is they're retirement people! I 2 3 4 5 6 No te that when the object possessed is a body part, French rvp ically uses a
defin ite ar tic le rather than a possess ive de termine r.
9 Is this perfume o r .. , I don't know. I 2 3 4 5 6
Il s'esi casse le bras = He broke the [his] arm.
I 0 And it is necktie! I 2 3 4 5 6
Joanna Whi te ( 1998,2008) studied the acq uisitio n ofpossessive determiners
by Frenc h-speaki ng students, adapting a developme ntal seq uence that was
Le arne r 3
first p rop osed by H elmu t Zobl (1984). White found a to tal of eight stages in
I I Are there any shuttle! Space shuttle! I 2 3 4 5 6 the sequen ce, but th ey can be gro uped into three ma in stages. The examp les
12 Inside , is there any girl? sho wn below co me from Fren ch -sp eaking students learning English . They
I 2 3 4 5 6
are describi ng ca rtoo n draw in gs of famil y eve nts and in te rac tio ns.
13 You don't see? I 2 3 4 5 6
Stage 1: Pre-emergence
14 What are, what the people wearing! I 2 3 4 5 6
No use of 'hls' and ' her'. Defi n ite ar ticle or 'yo ur' used fo r all perso ns, genders,
I S And they are carrying pink box! I 2 3 4 5 6 an d num be rs.
The little boy play w ith th e bicycle.
Answer key
H e have band -aid on the ar m , th e leg, th e stomach,
Learner 3: Questions I I and 12 are Stage 4 questions. Questions 13 and The girl m aki ng hisself bea utiful. She p u c the ma ke- u p on h is hand , o n h is
I5 are Stage 2 questions. Question 14 shows the speaker apparently on head, and hi s fat he r is su rp rise.
The dad put her little gi rl o n h is sho ulde r, and aft er, o n his back.
Possessive determiners
At the end of th e po st- emergence stage, in what Whft't: (2008) calls Stage 8,
A developmental sequen ce for th e En glish possessive form s ' h is' an d 'h er' has learners finall y ach ieve erro r-free use o f'his ' and ' her' in all co nt exts includ
been o bse rved in th e inrerianguage o f French- and Spanish -speaking learn ing natural gender and body parts .
ers. In English , the cho ice o f''h is' or ' her' (or 'its') is determined by the na tural
ge nde r o f the possessor. In Fren ch and Span ish (and m any other languages), The little girl with her dad play together. And th e dad cake h is girl o n his
th e co rre ct form o f the possessive d eterminer matches the grammat ical shou ld er and he hur t his back.
ge nde r o f the o bjec t o r person tha t is possessed. Th is ca n be illustrated w ith \V he n Eng lish speake rs learn Fren ch , o r othe r lan guages that use grammati
th e fo llowing t ran slati on eq u ivale n ts for Fren ch and English: cal ge nder as the basis For choosing possessi ve determine rs, the y must also
Sa mere = h is mother o r her mother learn a new wa y o f determining th e ge nde r of the pos sessive determiner. Th e
Son chien = his dog or her dog need to learn che g ram ma rical ge nde r o f eac h and every no un furthe r add
Ses enfants = his ch ild ren o r her ch ild ren to the challenge.
54 Second language learn ing Second language learning 55
Relative clauses ir modifies ), they ma y avoid using relative clauses even when their interlan
Second language learners first acquire relative clauses that refer to nouns in the guage is fairly advanced. Third, first language influence is seen in the erro rs
subject an d direct object positions, and only later (and in so me cases, never) learners make. For example, Arabic speakers often produce both the relati ve
learn to use them to modify nouns in other sentence roles (for exam ple, indi marker and the pro noun it replaces (for example, 'The man who I saw him
rect object an d ob ject of preposition). A summary of the observed pacrern was very angry') as the y wo uld in Arabic.
of acquisition for relative clauses is shown in Table 2.3. It is referre d to as the
accessibility hierarchy, and it reflects the apparent ease with which learners Reference to past
have access to certain st ructures in th e target lan gu age. A n umber of researchers, including jurgen Meisel (1987), have observed th e
develop ing ability to use lan guage to locate events in time. The research has
Part of speech Relative clause
shown that learners from different first language backgrounds and acquiring
Subject The girl who was sick went home. a variety ofsecond languages, acquire the lan guage for referring to past events
in a similar pattern.
Dire ct ob ject The story that I read was lo ng.
Indirect object The man who[m] Susan gave the present to Like young chil dren, learners with limited language may simply refer to
was happy. events in the order in which they occurred or mention a time or place to
show that the event occurred in the pase.
Object of preposition l-found the book that John was talking about.
Viet Nam. We work too hard.
Possessive I know the woman whose father is visiting. My son come. He work in resta urant.
Object of comparison The person that Susan is taller than is Mary. Later, learners start to attach a gra mmatical morpheme marking the verb for
Photo co piable © Oxford University Press past , although it may not be the one that the target language uses fo r that
meaning.
Table 2.3 Accessibility hierarchy for relative clauses in English Me working long time. Now stop. j .-
(adapted from Doughty 1991)
Past tense forms of irregular verbs may be used before-the regular past is used
Unlike the study of grammatical morphemes , negation, and questions, the reliably. '
study of relative clau ses was no t inspired by research on child language.
We went to school every day. We spoke Spanish/
Rather, it cam e from patterns that Edward Keenan and Bernard Comrie
(1977) observed in a large number oflanguages. They found that those lan After they begin marking past tense on regular ver bs, learners may overge n
guages that included the structures at the bottom of the list in Table 2.3 era lize the regular -ed ending or the use of the wrong past tense form (for
would also have those at the top, but the opposite was not necessarily true . example, the present perfect rather than the simple past ).
.,'
Subseque ntly, Susan Gass (1982) an d others found that if a second language My sister cat ched a big fish. i
lear ner could use one of the str uctures at the bo ttom of th e list, he or she She has lived here since fiftee n years.
would probably be ab le to use any that precede it. O n the ot her hand, a J\.,1
learner who could pro duce sentences with relative clauses in the subject or Kathleen Bardovi -Harlig (2000) and others have found that learners are more
direct object positions (at the top of the list) would not necessar ily be able to likely to mark past tense in sentences such as 'I broke the vase' and 'My sister
use them in any of the clause types further down the list. fixed it with glue' than in sentences such as 'She seemed happy last week' or
'My father swam in thar lake'. These differences appear to be d ue to the 'lexical
Despite the similarity of the general pa ttern, several types of first lan guage aspect' , tha t is, th e kinds of meanings expressed by the different verbs . Learners
influence have also been observed in the acquisition of relative clauses. Firs t, seem to find it easier to mark past tense on verbs that refer to something whose
it has been observed that for learners whose first lan guage does nor have a end poi n t can easily be determined. These are referred to as 'accomp lishme nts'
particular clause type (for examp le, object of comparison ), it is more dif an d 'ach ievemen ts' ('I ran thre e m iles.' 'My brother took an aspirin and went to
ficult to learn to use that type in En glish . Second, where learners have a first bed '). For 'activities' that may continue fo r some period ('I swam all afternoon ')
language with a substantially d ifferent way of for ming relative clau ses (for or 'states' that may be perceived as constants ('He seemed ha ppy to sit by the
example, Chinese and Japan ese, whe re the relative clause precedes the noun lake') , learners use simple past markers less frequently.
56 Second I.mguage learning Second language learning 5
First language can have an influence here too. Laura C ollins (2 002) in vesti me?' we can see that the learner's ime rlang uage ru le really is so me thi ng like
gated the diffe rent English verb fo rms used by French speakers. The past tense 'Put a question word at the beginning of the sentence.'
tha t is most commonly used in spoken French and that is usual ly a transla
Another important observation about developmental seq uences is the
tion of a simple past form in English is a form that resembles th e p resent
way they interact with first language influence. Learners do not appear to
perfect in English. Thus, the equivalent of 'Yesterday he ate an apple' is H ier
assume that they can sim ply transfer the structures of their first language
il a ma ng~ un e pomme- literally..'Yesterday he has eaten an apple'. Teachers
in to the second. Rather, as Henning Wode ( 197 8) and Helmut Zobl ( 1980)
often comment on French speakers' tendency to overuse the present perfect.
observed , when they reach a developmental stage at which they perceive a
In Collins' study, learners completed passages by filling in blanks with the
'crucial similarity between their first lang uage and their interlanguage,
appropriate form of a verb. As expected, in places where English speakers
they ma y generalize their first language partern and end up making erro rs
would use the simple past, French speakers did so metim es use the perfect
that speakers of other languages are less likely to make. They may also have
(either present perfect or past perfect)" forms. Furthermore, they used them
difficulty moving beyond that stage if their errors do not interfere with
more frequently than a comparison group of Japanese speakers. However,
com m un ica tio n.
the French speakers were more likely to use perfect forms for achievement
and accomplishment verbs than for the states and activities. Collins observes,
'The [first lan guage] influence does no t appear to override the effect of lexical More aboutfirst language influence
aspect; rather it occurs within it' (p. 85). One reason that so me researchers rejected the hypothesis that 'transfer'
o r ' int erference ' wo uld best exp lain a learn er's d ifficulties wi th the target
M ovem en t through developmental sequences language was the fact char co nt rastive analysis was closely assoc iate d wit h
\Y/e have seen in thi s section that, as in first lan guage acq uisition , th ere are behavio urist views oflanguage acquisition. In rejecti ng beha viou rism, so me
systema tic and p red icta ble developmental seq ue nces in seco nd lan guage researche rs also disca rde d co nt rastive analysis. In d oing so, they potential ly
acq uisitio n. H owe ver, it is important to em p hasize that developmental stages lost an essent ial so urce o f information abo ut lan gua ge acquisition .
are not like closed rooms. Learners do not leave one behind when they enter Researchers at the European Science Foundation carried out a study that
another, In examining a language sample from an individual learner, one created some valuable opportunities to examine mc::, influence of the first
sho uld not expect to find behaviours from only one stage. On the contrary, language on second language learning. Adult language learners , most of
at a given point in rime, learners may use sentences typical ofseveral different w hom had little or no second language instrucrion; were followed as they
stages. It is perhaps better to think of a stage as being characterized by the learned anothe r European language. For each target language, learners from
emergence and increasing frequency of new forms rather than by the com rwo different first language backgrounds were co m pared , Also, for each first
plete disappearance of earlier ones . Even when a more advanced stage comes language background, the progress oflearners in their acquisition of the twO
to dominate in a learner's speech, conditions ofstress or complexity in a com target languages was studied. As \'YIolfgang Klein and Clive Perdue (1993)
municarive interaction can cause the learner to slip back to an earlier.stage. repOlt, there were substantial simi larities in the interlanguage patterns of the
In addition, as we have already noted, progress to a higher stage does not learners, in spite of the great variety in the first anH second language combi
always mean fewer errors. For example, a learner may produce correct qu es nations. The similarities were greatest in the earlies t stages ofsecond language
tions at Stage 1 o r Stage 3, but those correct forms are not necessarily based on acquisi tion , when learners produced similar simple Spntences.
underlying knowledge of subject-verb inversion . That is, correct questions There is no doubt that learners draw on the patterns of other languages they
at Stage 1 are formulaic chu nks, not sentences that have been constructed know as they try to discover the complexities of the new language they are
from the wo rds that mak e the m up. At Stage 2, learners have advanced, in th e learning. The patterns of those ear lier languages are firm ly established, and as
sense that they are forming o riginal questions, bu t the word o rde r of those learners have experience with the new language, there is an interplay between
questions is not grammatical in the ta rget language. At Stage 3, questions are the new and old patterns. As Nic k Ellis (2009 : 153) put it, 'Th e lan guage
formed by placing a question form (most often a tab- word o r a form of the calcu lator has no "clear" burton.' In learning so meth ing new, we build on
verb 'do') at the beginn ing of a sen tence with declarative word order, This wha t we already know.
may result in questio ns such as ' Do you want to go?' that conform to English
patterns. However, when the learner asks a question such as '0 0 you can help
58 Second language Learning Second language learn ing 59
We have seen some ways in which the firsr language interacts with develop
mental sequences. When learners reach a certain stage and perceive a similarity
S =Subject V =Verb 0 =Object A =Adverb
to their nrSt language, they may linger longer at that stage (for example, the ASVO
extended use of preverbal 'no' by Spanish speakers) or add a sub-stage (for Often, Mary drinks tea.
example, the German speaker's in version of subject and lexical verbs in ques Souvent, Marie bait du the.
do ns) to the seq uence which, overall, is similar across learners, regardless of SVOA
their firsr language. They may learn a second lan guage rule but restr ict its Mary drinks tea often.
app lication (for example, the French speaker's rejection of subject-auxiliary Marie bait du the souvent
inversion with noun subjects that we saw in Stage 5 questions on page 50).
SAVO
. The first language may influence learn ers' interlanguage in other ways as well. Mary often drinks tea.
As we saw earlier, the pheno menon of avoidance that Jac quelyn Schachter *Marie souvent bait du the.
(1974) described appeared ro be caused at least in pa rt by learners' perception SVAO
that a feature in the rarget language was so distant and different from their *Mary drinks often tea.
first langua!?e that they preferred not co try it. Marie bait souvent du the.
Other researchers have also found evidence of learners' sensitivity ro degrees Noce: The asterisk (*) means that the sentence is not grammatical.
of distance .or difference and a reluctance co attemp t a transfer when they
Phococopiable © Oxford University Press
perceive the lan guages as roo different. In one revealing study, Hikan
Ringbom ( 1986) found that the interference errors made in Engl ish by both
Table 2.4 Adverb placement in French and English
Finn ish-Swedish and Swedish-Finnish bilinguals were most often trace
able to Swedish, not Finnis h. The fact tha t Swedish and English are closely It seems fairly easy for French-speaking learners of English to add SAVO to
related languages that actually do share many characteristics seems to have their repertoi re and for English-speaking learners of French to add SVAO ,
led learn ers to take a chance that a word or a sentence structure that worked but both groups have difficulty getting rid o f a pattern that does nor occur
in Swedish would have an English equivalent. Finn ish, on the other hand, in the target language if it is similar co one in their first language. Englis h
belongs co a completely d ifferent language family, and whether their own speaking learners of French continue to accept SA'!.O as grammarical, and
first language was Swedish or Finnish, learners appeared reluctant to draw on French-speaking learners of English accept SVAO.:.AsWhite points OUt, it is
Finnish in learning English . difficul t to notice that something is not present in.the input, especially when
The risk-taking associa ted with this percep tion of similarity has its lim its, its translation equivalent sounds perfecdy all rightand communication is not
however. For example, Eric Kellerman ( 19 86) observed tha t learners often disrupted. This may be even more challenging when learners interact with
believe that idiomatic or rneraphorical uses ofwords are un ique co a particu Others from the same first language background. Their own errors are not
lar lan guage. Kellerman found that Dutch learners of English were reluctant likely to cause misunderstanding and, in fact, they may hear Others make the
to accept that certain idiomatic expressions or unus ual uses of words were same errors. We will return co this when we look ai the role of instruction and
also possible in Englis h. For example, rhey rejected 'Th e wave broke on the feedback on errors in Chapters 5 and 6. .
Jo, J
shore' but accepted 'He broke the cup ' even though both are straightforward Our understanding of the relationship between 'n rSt- and later-learned
translations ofsentences with the Dutch verb breken. languages has been refined in recent decades. The term cross-linguistic influ
Another way in which learners' first languages can affect second language ence is now often used, in part co reflect the fact that the relationship is by no
acquisition is by making it difficult for them to notice that something they means unidirectional . That is, as we acquire a second or third lang uage, the
are saying is not a featu re of the language as it is used by more proficient patterns that we learn can also have an impact on the way we use and under
speakers. Lydia White (199 I) gave the example of adverb placement in stand the language(s) we learned earlier. Current views of second language
French and English. Both languages allow adverbs in several positions in development emphasize the interaction between the firsr language (or other
simple sentences. However, as the exam ples in Table 2.4 show, there are some previo usly learned languages), cog nitive processes, and the samples of the
differen ces. English, b ur not French, allows SAVO orde r; Frenc h, but not targe t language tha t learners encoun ter in the input. As extensive reviews by
Eng lish, allows SVAO. Terence O dl in (2003) and SCOttJarvis and Aneta Pavlenko (2008) show, the
complexity of this relationship has ins pired scores of investigations.
60 St>cond Lmgu<lgt> Learning Second Languuge ft>urning 61
So far thi s chap te r has focu sed on th e acquisition o f morphology and syntax The challenge o f acq ui ring a large eno ug h vocab ula ry for succ essful comm u
in the second language. We now tu rn to the learning ofsome other irn po rran r nication in a variety of set tin gs has been the focu s of m uch recent resear ch.
co m po ne n ts of communicative competence: voca bulary. pragmatics, and Every lan gu age has an asronishi ng ly large n umber of wo rds . Eng lish, which
pron unc ia tio n . has built its vocabulary from a grea t var iety o f so urce languages, is variously
estimated ro have anywhere from 100 ,000 to one million words, depending
Vocabulary in part o n ho w wo rds are counted . For examp le, so me would treat 'teach .
teacher. teaching, and taught' as sepa rate words whil e others would count
In 1980, Paul Meara characterized vocabulary learning as a 'neglected aspect all of them as pan of o ne 'wo rd '- a single root from which the o thers are
oflanguage learning '. Researche rs in the 1970s and ear ly 1980s were drawn derived.
ro syntax and morphology because of the way error patterns and develo p
mental sequences of these features migh t reveal something about u n iversals An educated adult speaker of English is believed to know at least 20 ,000
in languages and language acq uisi tio n. H ow diffe rent things are now! J USt as words: some estimates suggest a number that is more than twice that. But
M eara was commenting on th e state of neglect, an explosion of research on most everyday conversation requires a far smaller member, something more
vocabulary learning was beginning, and the acq uisitio n of vocabulary has like 2,000 words. Sim ilarly. although Chinese and Japan ese have ten s o f th ou
become one of the mos t active areas in seco nd lan gu age acquisition research. sands of characters. mos t are rare , and no n- tech nical material can usually be
read with a knowledge of about 2.000 characters. Even so, acquiring a basi c
For most people, the importance of vocabulary seems ver y clear. As it has vocabulary is a slgnifican r accomplishment for a seco nd language learn e r.
o fte n been remarked , we can communicate by using words that are n ot placed
in the prope r order, pr on ounced perfectly, or marked wi th th e pr op er gram As we saw in C ha pter 1. children learn th ousan ds of wo rds in the ir firSt lan
matical m orphemes, but communicati on o fte n brea ks down jf we d o not guage wit h little observable effort. TIle task of acqu iring a large vocabulary
use the correct word . Although ci rcu m loc u tio n a nd gestures can sometimes is q uite d ifferent for second lan gu age learners. For one thing, they are likely
to be ex pose d to far smaller sam p les of th e language to be learned . Also, th e
com pe nsa te. the impo rtance of vocab ulary ca n ha rdly be overes timated.
co ntexts in which seco nd language learners enco u nte r new voc ab ulary may
not be as helpful as those in which ch ildre n learn die first one o r two thou
san d words of their first language. If they are olderchlldren or adults , the
J)oYou ~EA'f:::
words they are exposed to may be m ore diffi cult, refer rin g to meanings that
rRE:NC~ ?
are not easily guessed from context, Marcella H I.! and Paul Nation (2000)
<, showed that, in order to unde rstand a text witho ur frequent stops to co nsult
(~
(If;; WE 1-"', I )X)N'T
KNOW AL.l
-me woRDS if:T· ··
.J
a di cti onary, one needs to know more than 9 5 pe,r cenr of the words-a rare
case for second langu age learners at mos t srages of acq uisi tion. Altho ugh the
two or three th o usan d most freq ue nt words in English make up as much as
80- 90 per cenr of most non-technical texts, less f~equenr wo rds are crucial to
th e meaning of many th ings we hear and read. F6 r examp le, th e meaning o f
a newspaper article abo ut a court case m ay be lost without the know ledge of
words suc h as 'testim o ny', 'alleged', or 'acco m plice'e....
( The first step in know ing a wo rd is sim p ly to recogn ize that it is a word. Paul
Meara and his co lleagues (2005) have develo ped tests th at tak e ad vanrage
f of th is fact. So me of th ese tests ta ke the fo rm of word lists, and learners are
instructed to check 'yes' o r 'no' accordi ng to whe the r or no t they know the
wo rd. Eac h list also includes so m e items that loo k like E ng lish wo rds but arc
nor. The number of real wo rds that the learn er identifies is ad justed fo r guess
ing by a facror tha t takes acco u nt of th e n umber of no n-words th at are also
chosen . Such a procedure is more effective th an it m igh t so u nd. A ca refully
co nstructed list can be used ro esti ma te th e vocabulary size of even advanced
learne rs. For example. if shown the fol lowing list : 'Fro lip, laggy, scr u le. an d
62 Secondlanguage learning Secondlanguage learning 63
albeit' . a proficient speaker of English would know that only one of these there is naming in me w ritte n form or me pronunciation of me wo rds th em
words is a real English word, albeit a rare and somewhat odd one. On the selves mat gives a clue to their meaning. If students are to learn them , they
oth er hand, even proficient speakers mighr recognize none of me following must see or hear me words in contexts mat reveal their meaning, and, as a
items: 'go n io m erer, micel le, laminitis, rhrostle'. Even our computer's spell rule. they must do this many times before the link between a word and irs
che cker rejected twO our of fo ur, but all are real English words, according to meani ng is well established.
me New Oxford Dictionary ofAmerican English. On me other han d , some stu dents who .have never studied. English m igh t
Among m e factors that make new vocabulary m ore easily lear nable by already know words in List 2, beca use they are par t of an international
second language learn ers is m e freque ncy wi th which me word is seen, hear d, vocab ulary. Wim increasing internationalizatio n of co mm unica tio ns,
and understood. Paul Nation (200 1) reviews a n umber ofstudies suggesting many languages have ' bo rrowed' and adapted words from other languages.
mat a learner needs to have many meaningful encounters with a new word Studenrs chroughour che world may be surprised ro lear n how many words
before it becom es firmly established in memory. The estim ates range as high they already ~ow in me language they are tryi ng to learn .
as 16 ti mes in some studies. Even more encounters may be nee ded befo re a The wor ds in List 3 loo k difficult. They are rat her lo ng, not easily illust rated,
learner can ret rieve the word in Huen r speech or au tom atically und erstand and most are fairly infrequent in the lan guage. However, many students
me meaning of the word when ir occurs in a new COntext. The ability to would either 'know' chem on sigh t or learn them afte r a. single exposure
understand the meaning of most words without focused attentio n is essential because they look like the ir tra nslation equivalent in other languages that
for fluent reading as well as for fluent speaking. they already know. Some, such as 'natio n' and 'dictionary', are cognates
Frequency is no t me only faeror that de termi nes how easily words are lear ned , (words that have come fro m the same original roo t); others, such as 'deno ue
howeve r, as illustrated by the wo rds in me three lists shown in Table 2.5. ment' and 'en tent e' , are bo rrowed words (words that have been adopted from
other languages) . These words ma t look alike and have shared meaning can
friend hamburger gove rnment Teachers should not assume mat studen rs will always recognize bo rrowed
more Coke responsibility words or cognates in the ir second lan guage. Some eognates are identical in
form and mean ing, while others may require some knowledge of how spell
town T-shirt dictionary ing patterns are related in me two languages (fo r example, 'water' and Wasser
boo k Facebook elementary in English and German respectively or 'm usic' and musique in English and
French) . Even with differe nr spellings, words are likely to be easier to recog
sing pizza description need guidance in recognizing rhern , as illus trated in me following question,
hotel expression asked by an eight-year-old in a Quebec hockey arena: 'H i coach, comment on .
box
dit 'coach'en anglais?' ('Hey, coa ch. How do yo u s~ 'coach' in English?') . And
smile dollar international afte r a moment's reflecti on , English speakers may realize mat they know both
eye Internet denouement speciality items in a Japanese restauran t mat calls i~c:lf 'Sushi and Bisuteki.'
night disco entente On th e other ha nd, students may have particular difficulty with words that
look similar in the twO languages bu t have different meanings. These 'false
Phococopiable © Oxford University Press cognates' may come fro m different origins or they may have evolved differ
entl y from the sam e origin. For example, the En glish verb 'deman d ' has a
Table 2.5 English words that may be 'easy' or 'difficult' (or second language learners d ifferen t meani ng fro m its Fren ch cousin demander, which mea ns 'request'
All of me words in List I look easy because the y are simple one-syllable words or 'ask a q uestio n', even though they came from the same Lat in verb .
that refer to easily illustrated actions o r objects. They are also quire common Some th eo rists have argued that second language learners, like children
words in English, appearing amo ng the 1,000 most frequent words. And yet , learning their first language, can learn a great deal of vocabulary with little
they are not likely to be known ro stu dents who have not had p revio us ins tr uc inten rional effor t. Stephen Krashe n (1989) has asserted mat me best source
tion in English or exposure ro the language outside sch ool. Furt hermore, of vocabulary growth is readi ng for pleasure. There is no doubt mat reading
language and to judge whether a particular request is appropriate or inap Stage 5: Fine tuning
propriate in a specific context. Fine tuning of requestive force to participants, goals, and contexts.
Since the early 1990s more research has directly investigated the acquisition You could put some Blu Tack down there.
of second language prag matic ability. This includes lon gitu dinal and cross Is there any more white?
sectional studies describi ng the acquis ition ofseveral different speec h acts. O ne Learn ing how to make and reject suggestions has also been extensively inves
that has been the focus of consi derable atten tion is 'requesting'. Req uests are tigated. Kathleen Bardovi-Harlig an d Beverly Hartford (199 3) o bserved
an interesting pragmatic feature to exam ine because there are identifiable ways differences between the way in which native and non-native speakers of
in which requests are made within particular languages as well as differences in English communicated with their professors as they discussed their course
how they are expressed across different languages and cultures. selections in academic advising sessions. These differences contributed
In a review of longitudinal and cross-sectional studies on the acquisition to their greater o r lesser success in negotiating their academic plans . For
of requests in Englis h, Gabriele Kaspe r and Ken neth Rose (2002) outline example, the non-native speakers did not initiate suggestio ns whereas na tive
a series of five Stages of development. Stage 1 consists of mi ni m al language speakers initiated a grea t d eal. There was also a tendency on the part of th e
that is often incomplete and highly context-dependent. Stage 2 includes non-native speakers to reject suggestions made by the advisor in ways that the
primarily memorized routines and freque nt use of imperatives.. Stage 3 is advisors might find rude or inappropriate. For example, they would reject
marked by less use o f form ulas, more productive speech, and some mitiga an adv isor's suggestion to take a particular course by saying 'I think I am no t
tion of requests. Stage 4 involves more complex language and increased use interested in that course' , instead of saying ' My schedule conflicts with that
of mitigation, especially suppo rtive statem en ts. Stage 5 is marked by more course', or 'I th ink this other co urse would better meet my needs', which was
refinement of the force of requests. The five stages , thei r characte ristics and more typical of na tive-speaker rejection responses. 'The-non-native speakers
examples are given below. were also much less adept than the native speakers at using mitigation. For
example, native speakers were observed to say ' I think I would like to take this
Stage 1: Pre-basic
course' , whereas the non-native speakers said 'I will rake that course'.
Highly context-dependent, no syntax, no relational goals .
Over a period of four an d a half mon th s, the rese arc~irs observed progress in
Me no blue. some aspects of the non-native speakers' pragmatic ability. For example, th ey
Sir. learned to take a moreactive role in the advising interaction and to provide
reasons for rejecting suggestions that the advisors-were likely to perce ive as
Stage 2: Formulaic
more credible or acceptable. Even so, they continued to have some d ifficulty
Don't look.
speech acts. This was especially true in reacher-fronted classrooms where
the dominan t int eractio n pattern was 'teacher initiation-learner respo nse
Stage 3: Unpacking
Formulas incorporated into productive language use, shi ft to conventional teacher feedback' and where the emphasis was alm'~~t always on producing
full sentences that were grammatical ly correct (see fur ther d iscussio n of
in directness .
this in Chapter 5). In communicative, content-based, and task-based lan
Can you pass the pencil please? guage teaching, there are more opportunities not only fo r a greater variety
Can you do ano ther one for me? o f input but also fo r learners to en gage in different roles and participant
organization structures (for example, pair and gro up work). This enables
Stage 4: Pragmatic expansion lear ners to produce and respo nd to a wider range of communicative func
Addi tion of new forms to reperto ire, increased use of mitigation, more
tio ns. Furthermore, research o n the teaching ofpragmatics has demonstrated
co mp lex syntax.
that pragmatic features can be successfully learned in classroom settings and
Could I have another choco late beca use my child ren- I have five chil d ren. tha t expl icit rather than implicit instruction is most effective (Kaspe r and
Can I see it so I can copy it? Rose 2002). This is good news for fo reign language learners who do not have
(,8 Second language learning Second language It.'aming 69
exte nsive exposure to conversational in te raction ou tside the classroom. Th e exam ple). Such erro rs are sim ilar to the overge neral izario n erro rs tha t we saw
qu estion is no longer whe ther second lan guage pragmatics should be taugh t for grammatical morphemes. If they rep lace earlier correct pronu nciation o f
but ramer how it can be integrared into classroom instruction. t or d sounds, this may represem progress in learners' abil iry to notice and
produce the th so un d .
Phonology The relationship between perception and production of so unds is co m plex.
As noted earlier, grammar has been the focus fo r second language teac hers Evelyn Alcenberg (2 005) developed a se ries of tasks ro exp lo re Span ish
an d researchers for a lo ng time. Vocabulary and pragmatics have also received speakers' perceptions and production of English consonant clusters at the
more attentio n in recen t years. H owever, we know less about pronunciatio n beginning of a word . In one task, th ey had ro say whether certain invented
and how it is learned and taught. Pronunciation was a central component in words were possible 'new English words'. The Spanish speake rs were quite
language teaching when me audi olin gu al ap proach was dominant. Several good at recognizing w hat English words are supposed to sound like. They
techniques for teaching pro nu nciation were developed at mat time, and accep ted pseudowords like 'sp us' and rejected those like 'zban' , even though
mo st of th em focused on th e pronunciation of segmenrals , getting learners born words would be unacceptable as 'new Span ish words' . Sh e found th at
ro perceive and ro produce distinctions between single sounds in minimal they co uld usually wri te (from dictation) pseudowords with initial cl usters
pai r drills (fo r exam p le, 'ship' and 'sheep') . such as sp and sm . However, in their own production , the se same learners
might still in sert a vowel at the beginning o f words such as 'spoo n' and 'sm ile'.
When -rh e au d io lingu al approach was repla ced by other ways of teach
ing, atre nrio n [0 pron un ciation was min imized if not tot ally d iscarded . As we have seen with regard to gram mar and voca bulary, it is hypothesized '
Furthe rmo re, evidence for the critical period hypo th esis, sugges ting that tha t a grea ter d ifference between the learn er's nat ive lang uage an d th e target
native-like pronunciat ion was an unrealistic goal for older seco nd lan guage lang uage ca n lead to greate r difficul ry. The evide nce supporting [he hypoth
learners (see C hapter 3), led to the argument that instructi on al time woul d esis comes partly fro m the observation that it takes learn ers longer to reach a
be be tter spe n r o n teaching so me thi ng th at lear ne rs co uld learn mote success hig h level ofH ue ncy in a particul ar seco nd or fo reign lan guage if th at lang uage
fully. When communicative language teaching (CLT) was nrs t introduced in is su bstant ially different from the languages they alrefldy know. Fo r exam ple,
the late 1970s, little attention was given to me teaching of pronunciation. a speaker of C hinese faces a greater challenge in learn ing English than does
If it was taught, the emphasis was o n su p raseg m en tals (rhythm, stress, and a sp eaker of German or Dutch . Language distance. affects pronunciation
in rona rio nj-c-as pecrs of pronunciati on mat were considered m ore likely to as well as other language system s. Theo Bongaerrs '.(1999) collected speech
affect communication (C elce-M urcia, Brinton, and Goodwin 1996) . sam p les from highly proficient speakers who had learned Dutch in their
adulthood and who came from a wide variery o ffi ~s t language backgrounds.
Although research o n me teaching and learning o f pronunciation is not as W ~e n native speakers of Dutch were asked to judge the sp eech sam p les, o nly
extensive as m at in other language domains, there is theoretical and em pirical those learners who spoke a language mat was closely related to Dutch (fo r
wo rk ro help us understand the processes invol ved in phonological develop exam ple, English or German) were judged to have nat ive-like accents. None
m erit in a second language and the factor s tha t co n tribu te to it. For example,
of the speakers whose first languages were mor1' distant from Dutch (for
co nt rastive analysis helps ro explain some aspecrs offirst lan guage infl uence on exam ple, Vietnamese) were judged to h ave n at ive-like pronunciation .
second lan guage learners' pronunciation . We can all th in k o f examp les from
ou r own experiences or those of o ur st udents. Japanese and Korean lear n ers 1 he re has been little research to document (he dev;~wpmental seq uences of
of Englis h often have problems hearing and p roducing I and r because these ind ividual sounds in seco nd language ph onological a~quisi tion . Nonetheless,
sounds are no t d isti nct in th eir language. Spa nish speakers will often say ' I there is evide nce for similariry in the acq uisition o f some features ofstress an d
e-speak e-Spanish' because Spa nish words do not have consonant clusters rhythm and it also clea r tha t the learner's first lan guage pl ays an im po rtant
beg inning with s at the beginning ofa word. French speakers may p lace stress role. O rner factors suc h as the amo unt an d rype of exposure to the targe t
on the last syllab le of a word beca use French usually stress es the last syllab le. lang uage and th e degree of use of the first language have bee n identified
Few languages have the th sounds that are frequent in En glish, and learn as in fluen tial co ntrib uto rs to pronunciation. Thorsteri Piske, Ian MacKay,
ers may substitute sim ilar sounds from their first language (for examp le, t and James Flege (200 I ) have repor ted that lon ger periods of exposure to
a t d, s o r z). Some times, howeve r, learners overcompensate fo r so unds mat the seco nd lan guage ca n lead to improved pronunciatio n. It has also been
they know are d ifficult. Thus, learners may pronounce a th (as in 'th in' or observed that adults who continue ro make great er use of thei r first language
'this') where a t or a d so und belongs (saying 'th in' w hen they mean 'tin' for may have stro nge r accenrs in the seco nd lan guage (Piske 2007).
70 Second language learning Secondlanguage learning 71
Learners' ethnicaffiliation and their sense ofide ntiry are also related [Q how they than learn ers who received lesson s focused o n ind ivid ual sounds. Even though
pro duce the sounds and rhythms ofa second language. Elizabeth Garbo n ron, the learne rs wh o were given instruction on individ ual sounds were more
Pavel Trofirnovich, and Michael Magi d (2005) found a complex relation ship accurate in their use of those sounds, this did no t seem to increase listeners'
between feelings of ethnic affiliation and second language learners' acquisi perception of the intelligibility of their speech to others. Findings like these
tion ofpronunciation. Among other th ings, they found that learners who had support the current emphasis on su prasegmentals in pronunciation classes.
achieved a high degree of accuracy in prono uncing the seco nd language were One of the con troversial issues in pronunciation is related to the question
sometimes perceived as being less loyal [Q their ethnic gro up than those whose of whether the goal of secon d language acqui sition is to sound like a 'na tive
second language speec h retained a strong 'foreign accent'. Such percep tions speaker.' One obvious problem with the question is tha t it suggests that there
can affect learners' desire [Q achieve high levels of proficiency in the second exists a single correct variety of English, and th is is far from true. Not on ly
language , especially in contexts wh ere th ere are co nflicts between groups or are there many different pronunciations of English by American, Australian,
where power relationships imply a threat to one group's identity. British, Canadian 'native speakers' , the re are also many other varieties ofEnglish
Pavel Trofimovich (2005 ) has looked at learning pronunciation from a that have come to be used as a linguafranca arou nd the world. Jane Setter and
somewhat unusual perspective. His research raises questions about how well Jennifer Jenkins (200 5) and Barbara Seidlhofer (20 11) are among the many
learners perceive th e specific sounds of the new language wh ile their focus is scholars who stress the role of English as a linguafranca (ELF). Indeed, there
on meaning. Second language learn ers ofSpanish were asked to listen to a list are now far more speakers of ELF than of Englis h as a first language:
of familiar Spanish wo rds. For the purpose of comparison , they also heard A related question is whether intelligibility rather than native-like pro
a list of words in English, their na tive language. O ne group of participants nu nc iation is the standard that learners should strive toward. Stu d ies of
were told [Q 'just listen' to the words; the second group were asked to pay relatio nsh ips between Engl ish nati ve speake rs' perceptions of fo reign accent,
attention to how good the recording quality was; the third group were asked their perceptions ofcomprehen sibility, and their actual abil ity [Q understand
[Q rate the 'pleasantness' of the things the words referred to. Then they heard
what speakers are saying show that the th ree are related. However, research
another list, which included bo th the original words and some new words, by Murray Munro and Tracey Derwing (20 11) shows that the presence of a
an d they were asked to repeat each word as they heard it. Trofimovich then strong foreign accent does not necessarily resul t in r.educed int elligibility o r
compared how quickly each learner started to prono unce the words they had comprehensibility.
already heard and the new words . Th e d ifference in the time it took them
to react to 'old' and 'new' words is a m easu re of how easily words could be Unfortunately, research evidence does not changethe fact that some listen
retrieved fro m memory. ers respond negatively to second language speakers'<pronunciation. In some
situations, accent still serves as a marker of group membership an d is used as
As expected, the participants were always faster at retrieving the old words the.basis for d iscrim ination . Thus, some secondlanguage learners , particu
in their native language, and rwo groups oflearners also showed this pattern larly thos e who have achieved a hig h level of knowledge and performanc e
for their seco nd language. But the third group. who had been told to focus in other aspects of the target language, ma y be motivated [Q approximate a
. o n th e 'pleasantness' of the meanings, did not retrieve the old words faster. mor e 'native-like' accent for personal and professi~nal reaso ns. O ther second
Trofimovich suggests that when learners focus p rimarily on meaning. they language learners view this as irrelevant [Q their goals and objectives as users
may not be able [Q also pa y attention to the sounds that make up the words. of the second language (D erwing and Munro 2009); .... .~
words, syntactic patterns, pragmatic features-when their focus is on under already clear that decori rexrualized pronunciation instruction is not enough
Stand ing meani ng . and that a combination of ins tr uction, e.xposure, experience, and motiva
tion is required if learners are [Q change their way ofspeaking. Ro bin Walker
Few studies have investigated the effectiveness of pronunciation instruction , (20 10) provides guidance for teaching pronunciation in a way that recognizes
but the results of recent research suggest that it can make a difference, par the importance of preparing students fo r interacting with other speakers of
ticular ly if the instruction focuses on suprasegrnenral rather than segmental English as a lingua fran ca.
lecting samples of their speech or writing that are large enough to ensure Look ing at th e develop mental sequence that has been described for English
that an alyses an d their findings are based on more than JUSt a few learners o r negation. think about what learners seem to notice first. Is it word order?
fro m just a few examp les fro m a larger number oflea rne rs. Research ers often Special words?What features seem hardest for them to acquire?
find it d ifficult to recruit learners, to obtain th eir co nsent to participate in a 2 How would you collect samples of learner language for a study of the
stu dy. and to persuade them to remain available over the time peri ods that acquisition of grammatical morphemes?What kind of speaking or writing
are necessary to show development. It can also be challenging to sched ule task would be most effective in leading learners to create obligatory
sessions fo r recording sp eech or collecting writing sam p les, and to transcribe contexts for each of the morphemes listed in Figure 2.1? Do you think
o r digitize the speech sam ples for analysis . some morphemes would be relatively easy to create contexts for? Which
Co mp u te r-based tool s are making it possible for resear chers to ask and ones do you think would be difficult! Do you think the 'wug test' would be
answe r new ques tions an d to revisit so me o f th e tentative answers to q ues a useful tool?
tions th at have been around for a long tim e. For example. corpus linguistics J What aspects of learners' interlanguage are most likely to affect their
has provid ed us with large coll ections of naturally occ urring data that can ability to use language effectively outside the classroom? Word order?
be used to discover information about the frequency o f different language Grammatical morphemes? Vocabulary? Phonology! Pragmatics? Do you
featu res (wo rds. ph rases. gra mmatical patt ern s) in a variety o flang uage co n th ink pr iorities for class room int er action and instruction reflec t t he
texts and regist ers. Some of th ese co rpo ra co nt ain langu age sam ples that have importan ce of t he se different language features!
been co llected from newsp apers and conversatio ns. whe reas Othe rs are more
specific to part icu lar rypes o fla nguage. In additio n. the re are learner co rpo ra
and pedagogic corpora. More and mo re research ers are making th eir learn er
languag e data available to o the rs so that each corpus of learner language dat a Suggestions for further reading
can be used for a number ofdifferent studies. Some of th ese corpora are avail Ellis, R. and G. Barkhuizen, 2005 . Analysing Leamer La ngu age. Oxford:
able in CD or D VD format or online (Tarone and Swierzbin , 2009). The Oxford Universiry Press. "
links between corpus research and second language teaching are also becom Analysing Learn er Language introduces read ers co different approache s co
ing increasingly apparent (Bennet, 2010; Sinclair. 2004). For example, if a investigating learner language. It also serves as an extensive review of pub
co rp us o f classroom language reveals that certain features occur frequ ently lished research using a range of m ethods and techniques for gathering and
in classroom input, teachers might decide to focus on features that occ u r less an alysing data. Each ch apter is devoted co a particular anal ytic approach ,
frequenrly. Similarly if corpus research reveals that specific grammatical fea including error ana lysis, frequency anal ysis, and socio cultural anal ysis. as
tur es are rarely used by native speakers in conversati onal interaction , teachers well as a cha p ter by Michael Barlow o n how com pu ter cools can be used .
(and textbooks) may de vote less time to th e o ral pr acti ce of th ese features. Th e book will be of special interest to students who are em barking o n
seco nd language acquisition research at th e post-graduate level.
Summary Nation, I. S. P. 200 1. Learning Vocabulary in Anorhft,JLanguagt!.
The lan guage that second language learners produce and understand changes C am bridge: Cambridge University Press.
as they have more exposure to the language and as they use it in a greater This comprehensive book cove rs research and theory and their implica
var iety of situations. Describing those changes has been the focus of this tions for teaching and testing vocabulary development in a second or
chapter. \'(fe have seen that there are strong paceerns ofsimilariry across learn foreign language. Although many books on voca bulary teaching and
ers o f different ages, learn ing in different contexts, and start ing from differ ent learning have been published since thi s on e. it remains an essential and
first languagc backgrounds. The focus of this chapter has been mainly on accessible text for reach ers and post-graduate stu dent s who wish co under
these similarities. In Chapter 3. we will turn our attention to some of the stand both the chall enges of vocabulary learn ing and those involved in
ways in which learners differ from each other and how those individual dif reaching and assessing vocab ulary knowled ge.
ferences affect how quickly and how well th ey succeed in second language
acq uisitio n.
74 Second language learning
3
Th e au thors collecred speech an d wri rin g samples from a group ofEn glish
language learn ers fro m different L 1 background s par ticipating in the same
IND IVID UAL D IFFEREN CES
tasks. This created a data base show ing how each learner tried to achieve IN SECOND LANGUAGE
th e same com mun ica rion goals. Th e text issupplemen ted by a OVD o f the
learn ers engaged in th e oral tasks. Exercises focus on differem ap proaches LEARNING
to un ders ta nding the lear ners' emerging language syste ms , includi ng error '
analysis, developmemal sequences, learners' response to feedback, an d
commun icat io n strategies. Many of the ideas tha t are introduced in th is
chapter o f How Languages Are Learned are illust rat ed in th e examples of
learners' lang uage th at are presen ted in th is book.
Preview
As V-(e saw in C hap ter 1, child ren are almost always succe ssfu l in acquiring
the language or lan guages th at are spo ken (o r signed) to them in early child
hood, provided that they have adequate opportunities..[ Q use th e language
over time. This co n trasts with our expe rience of second lan guage learners,
whose success var ies grea tly. Both educators an d researche rs have an interest
in u nderstanding how the characteristics of individ uals are related to the ir
ability to succeed in learning a second language. ;: .
Many of us believe that individual d ifferences that are. inherent in the learner
can predict succes s or failure in lan guage learni ng. Such beliefs may be based
on our own experience or that of people we have known . For examp le, many
teachers are convinced th at extroverted students ~ho interact witho u t inh i
bit ion in the second language and seek opporrunities to practise language
skills wUI be the most success ful learners. In add ition to an outgoing per
sonality, other characteristics often believed to predict succ ess in language
lear ning are intelligence , motivation , and the ag9at which learning begins.
To what exte nt can we predict differences in the su ccess of seco nd lan guage
acq uisitio n if we have in fo rmatio n about learn ers' Rf:.!t50 nalities, the ir general
an d specific intellectual abilities, thei r mo tivation, or th eir age? In th is
cha pter, we will review some of the studies that have so ugh t to understand
th e relationships berween in d ivid ual diffe rences and learn ing outcomes.
to the questionnaire and refe r to them as you read this chapter about
individual differences in second language learn ing. How much time have I I
you spent living in a place
where the language is
I
I a Wha t language do yo u spea k best? Do you speak more t han o ne
language equally well? spoken? I
I b When did you begin to learn this language (these languages)? Have you used the I
2 Which second or foreign language(s) have you learned with the language to learn other
most success? subjects at school? At
wha t level (elementary.
3 Which second or foreign language(s) have you learned with the secondary. university)?
least succ ess?
Do you have personal or
4 For the languages you mentioned in response to questions 2 and 3. emotional attachments
answe r the following questions in the appropriate columns: to this language? For
Languages learned , Languages not example. do you have
successfully learned successfully peers or family members
who speak this language?
How old were you when . I Do/did you enjoy I
yo u first tried to learn
the language? studying the grammar of I
I I t his language ?
Did you have a choice I I I
"
Do you regularly Do you think of yourself as a person who learns anew language easily?
that those who repo rt th at th ey frequen tly do so are h igh ly mo tivat ed to U nde rstandi ng the rela tio nsh ip between individ ual characte ristics, social sit
lear n. Thi s seems reaso nable, but it is not so simple. If a learn er responds that uatio ns, and success in second lan guage learning is a' chal lenge, Nevertheless.
he or she frequen rly in teracts with sp eakers of the second language, it may research in this area is of great importance to both researchers and educato rs.
not be because he or she is mo re mo tivated to learn than o ne who repo rts less Researchets seek to know how di fferent cognitive and personality variables
interaction. Ra ther, it m ight be th at th is indi vid ual lives where the re are mor e are related and how they interact with learne rs' experiences, so th at they can
o pporrunities-or a grea ter necessity- fo r language p ractice th an th ose who gain a better un derstand in g of h um an learning. Edl)c.ators ho pe to find ways
report a low freq ue ncy of inte ract ion. Beca use it is usual ly im possible to of hel ping lear ners with different ch aracteristi cs to achieve success in second
separate thes e two variables (i.e. motiva tio n or desire to interact and oppo r lan guage learn ing . The larger community is also cdncerned because of th e
tu nities or the need to interact) , we cannot co nclude whethe r it is motivation, enormous impact second language learning has on'shaping op porruni ties for
necessity, or opportunity th at is bein g measured by th is q uestio n. ed uca tio n, em ployment, mobility, and other societal benefits.
Perhaps the most serious erro r in interp reting co rrela tions is th e conclusion Let's look at some of the in div id ual characcer lstlcsfhar have been investigated
that one o f the var iables cau ses the other. The fact tha t rwo things tend to in the effor t to discover explanations for differences in learning outcomes.
occur together or increase and decrease in a similar pattern does not neces
sari ly mean tha t o ne ca uses the other. While it may be that one variab le
Intelligence {
infl uences th e o ther, it ma y also be th at both are influenced by something else
enti rely. Researc h on mo tivation is perhap s the best co nt ext in which to illus Th e term ' intelligen ce' has trad itionally been used to refer to performance on
trate this. Learners who are successfu l may indeed be h ighl y motivated . But certain kinds of tests. These tests are oft en associatM-with success in school,
can we conclude th at they became successful because of the ir mo tivatio n ? It and a lin k between intelligence and seco nd language lear n ing has sometimes
is also pla usible that early success heigh tened th eir mo tivation, or that bo th bee n reported. O ver the years, some research has shown that IQscores were
su ccess an d motivation are due to their special aptitude for lan guage learn ing a goo d means of p redicting suc cess in second language learning. H owever,
o r th e favourable context in which th ey were lear n ing. as suggest ed above, IQ test s m ay be more stro ngly relate d to metali nguis tic
Another diffic ulty in assessing rh e relatio nship between in d ivid ual learner dents in French im m ers ion p rog ramm es in C anada, Fre d Genesee (197 6)
characteristics and second language learning is how lan guage proficiency is foun d th at, while intelligence was related to th e development of Fren ch
defined and measured . In the L2 learning liter atu re, so me studies repo rt th at seco nd language readi ng, gra m mar, and vo cab ulary, it was unrelated to oral
learn ers with a highe r intelligen ce q uotient (IQ) are mo re success ful language productio n skills. This sug gests that th e kind o f abi lity meas ured by tr adi
learners th an those with a lowe r IQ , w hile othe r studies report no suc h cor tio nal IQ tests may be a str ong pre dicto r when it comes to learning that
relatio n. One explanatio n fo r the se conflicting find ings is tha t the language
Individual dlffi7'ences in second Language learning 81
80 Individual differences In second langudge learning
invo lves lan guage anal ysis an d rule learning b ur may play a less important 'sho rr- rerm mem o ry' refe rs to th e active processing o f info rma tio n. Althoug h
role outside me classroom o r in classrooms where rhe instruction focuses long-term memory ca paci ty is very large indeed, working memory capacity
more on communication and interaction. Indeed, many students whose is limited. Thar means tha t only a certain amount of information can be
general academic performance is weak experience considerable success in processed at a given rime, and individuals d iffer in me amount of informa
second language learning if they are given me right opportunities. rion they can process in working memory. Pet er Skehan (1989) suggests mar
successfu l lan guage learners need not be stro ng in all of th e co m po ne nts of
Many ed uca to rs have been influenced by Howard Gardner's (I 99 3) pro aptitude. For example, some may have strong memories but onl y average
posal mat individuals have 'm ulti ple intelligences' and that traditional IQ abilities in language analysis.
rests have assessed only a limited range of abilities. Among me 'm ultiple
intelligences' Gardner includes abilities in the areas of music, interpersonal Lear ners' strengths and weaknesses in thes e different aptitude components
relations, and athletics, as well as me verbal intelligence that is most often may account for their ability to succeed in different rypes of instr uctio nal
associated with success in school. programmes. In a C anadian language programme for adult learners of
French, Marjorie Wesche ( 19 8 1) studied the progress of students who were
placed in instructional programmes that we re eith er compatible or inc om
Language learning aptitude patible with their aptitude profile. In the compatible groupings, students
Specific abilities thought to predict succes s in language learning ha ve been with high analytic ability but average memory were assigned ro teaching that
stu d ied under the titl e of language learning 'a ptitu de'. One of the pioneers focused on grammatical structures: learners with good memory but average
in th is area, John C ar roll (199 1), has cha racte rized aptit ude in te rm s of the analytic skills were placed in a class o rgan ized around th e Fu nctional use o f
abi lity to learn q u ickly. Th us, we may hyp othesize rhar a learner wirh high rhe second lan guage. In the inco m patible gro up i ngs~ students were placed in
ap titude may learn with greater ease and speed bu t th at o rhe r learners may classes that d id no t co rrespo nd to the ir aptitude profiles. \'V'esche reported a
also be successful if they perseve re. h igh level o f stu de nt and teac her sarisfaction whe n stude nts were mat ched
with co m patib le teach in g env iro nments . In ad d itio n, som e evide nce indi
Over several d ecades, me most widely used aptitude measures have been the
cated that matched students were able to attain sigpi fican tly high er levels
Modern Language Aptitude Test (M LAT) (Car roll and Sapon 1959) and the
of achievement than those who were mi smatched. Wh ile few schools could
Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery (PLAB) (Pimsleu r 1966). All me rests
o ffer such choices to their students, teachers may be able to ensure that their
are based on me view mat aptitude has several components, for example. the
teaching activities are sufficiently var ied to accommodate learn ers with dif
samp les, and remember new words . \'V'hileearly research revealed a su bstantial Furthe r su pport for the claim mat a particular. type of instruction can no t
relationship between performance on the MLAT or PLAB and performance benefit all learners in the same way co mes from a. study with seco ndary Stu
in foreign language learning, these studies were conducted at a time wh en lan dents of French as a foreign language in New Zealand. Rosemary Erlam
guage teaching was based on grammar translation or audiolinguaJ methods. (2005) explored whether there was a relarionship.berween aptitude and the
suggest that some of the abilities measured by aptitude tests are predictive
ent rypes of instrucrion on direct object pronouns in French.
analysing language (one componenc of aptitude that is targeted by the tests) grammar instruction followed by the opportunity to practise me rules they
were me most su ccessful lear ne rs in an English second lan guage programme had learned. Learners in me inductive group received no grammar instruc
in which activities almost never in volved direct attention [Q grammar. tion; instead they partiCipated in activities that encouraged them to figure
out the different meanings conveyed by direct object pronouns and men to
Nick Ellis (200 1) and orhers have hypothesized rhat working memory
produce them. Learners in the structured input instruction group received
(~I) capacity may be the mosr important variable in predicting success for
explicit rule-based grammar instruction but did not produce me target
learn ers in many language lear n ing situations. Working memory, also called
82 individual differences in second language learning
individual differences in second language learning 83
forms. Insread rhey participated in activities that exposed them to spoken
At th e orher end o f rhe aptitude continuum we find individuals wh ose
and wr itten examp les of direct ob ject pronouns.
achievements seem to defy every prediction about what is possible in seco nd
language learning. Lo rraine Obler ( [ 989) repo rted on the case o f one
American man who seemed ab le ro acquire oral fluency in a new language
in 'a matter ofweeks '. Neil Smirh and Ian thi-Maria Tsimpli (1995) followed
a polyglot savant who learned many lan guages wirh appare nt ease. This
achievement was part icular ly asto nishi ng in ligh t of rhe fact char h is overall
cognitive fu nct io ning and soci al skills we re q ui te lim ited .
Such exceptional learners suggest that an aptitude for language learning is
at least partly independent of cognitive, social, and personality characteris
tics that are often assoc iated wirh su ccessful learning. N evertheless, M ichael
Erard's (2012) review of the case s ofsome of h isro ry's mo st success ful learners
ofmultiple lan guages shows th a t thei r un us ual talent was also associated with
a w illingness to work hard at tasks that m any wo uld consider roo .bori ng or
diffi cul t, su ch as using word cards to study vocab ulary.
A learner's willingness to communicate (WTC) has also been related to Attitudes and motivation
anxiety. We have all experienced occasions when we tried to avoid commu
nicating in a second lan guage. wrc may chan ge with the number of people Robert Gardner and his coll eagues have carried out a programme of research
p resent. the topic of conversation, the fo rmality of the circumstances, and on the relationship between a learner's attitudes toward the second or foreign
even with whether we feel tired or energetic at a given moment. A collea gue language and its community. and success in second lan guage learning
in C anada, w ho works in the area of second lan guage learn ing an d speaks (Masgorer an d Gardne r 2003) . As is the case with other variables, it is not
sever al lan gua ges, recently co nfessed tha t he avoided the corner sto re in his easy to determine whether posi tive attitudes prod uce successfu l learn in g or
neigh bourhood because th e p ro p rieto r always spo ke Fren ch to him. He rec successfu l learning engenders positive attitudes, or whether both are affecte d
ognized the p rop rieto r's efforts to help him improve his skills in this new by other factors. Although the researc h canno t prove that positive attitudes
language, and was grateful for it, but, as he told us with embarrassment, it cause success in learning, there is ample evidence th at positive attitudes are
was just easier ro go ro the store where he coulduse Eng lish. associa ted with a willingness to keep lear n in g.
This is consistent with research car ried OUt by Richard Clement, Peter Motivatio n in second language lear nin g is a co mplex phenomeno n. It has
Maclnryre, and their colleagues, who argue that lear ne rs who willingly been defined in terms of rwo factors: on th e one hand, learners' communi
com m un ica te in a wide range of conversational interactions are able co do cative needs, and on me other. their atti tudes towards the second language
so beca use o f th eir comm unicative confidence. In a series' of studies they community. If learners need to speak the second language in a wide range
have shown th at communicative confidence is shaped by two variables: how of social situations or to fulfil professio nal ambitions, they will perceive th e
relaxed L2 1earners are and how com petent (or incompetent) th ey feel about comm unicative value of the second language and are therefore likely [Q be
their L2 ability. These factors are di rectly in fluenced by prev ious contacts motivated [Q acquire proficiency in it. Similarly, if lear ners have favourable
with L2 speakers and are considered to be the mai n conrribu ro rs to co m mu attitudes towards the speakers of the language, they ;""ilhiesire more contac t
nicarive co nfidence (Clement, Baker, an d MacIntyre 2003). with them. Robert Gardner and Wallace Lambert (1972) coined th e terms
instrumental motivation (language learning for im mediate or practical goals)
Several other personality characteristics such as self-esteem, em pathy, domi and integrative motivation (language learning for personal growth and cul
nance, talkativeness, and responsiveness have also been studied. The research tu ral enrichment through contact with speakers of th~ other lan guage). For a
does no t show a clearly-defined relat ions hip betwe en o ne pe rson ality trait long time int eg rat ive motivation was considered [0 bethe stronger predi ctor
and second lan guage acq uisition. And, as indica ted earlier, the majo r d if of suc cessful learning. In some contexts, however, instrumental motivati o n
ficu lty in inve stigating perso nality characteristics is that of idenrlfication and was found [0 be a bett er predictor. Thus, both types-of mo tivatio n have been
measurem en t. Ano ther explanatio n has bee n offered for the mixed findings . foun d [Q be related [0 success in second language learni ng, However, in some
Personality variables seem to be more consisten tly related to co nversational learn ing environments, it is difficult [0 distinguish' between thes e twO types
skills than to the acquisition of grammatical accuracy or academic language. of orientation [Q the target language and its community. Fu rthermore, early
Finally, most of the research on personality variables has been carried out research tended [0 conceptualize motivation as a 'st able characteristic of the
withi n a quantitative research paradigm, that is, an approach that relies learner. More recent work emphasizes th e dy na mi 4 nature ofmotivation an d
heavily on relating learners' scores on personality questionnaires to th eir tries [0 account for th e changes th at take place over time.
language test performance. Some research ers have argued that a more quali
tative approach to understanding an d investigating personality variables is Zoltan Dornyei (2001 a) developed a pro cess-orientqd model of motivation
needed to adequately cap ture th eir depth and complexity. especially as they mat consists of three phases. The first phase, 'choice motivation' refers to
emerge and evolve over time. getting started and [0 setting goals . the second phase, 'executive motivation',
Despite th e contradicto ry resul ts an d th e problems invo lved in carrying ou t third phase, 'motivation retrospection'. refers to students' appraisal of and
research in th e area of personal ity ch aracteristics . many researchers believe reactio n [0 th eir performance. An example of how one might cycle th rough
that personality will be shown to have an important influence on success in these phases would be: a seco ndary school learner in Poland is excited abo ut
lang uage learning. This relationship is an intricate one, however, in that it is an upco m ing tri p to Spain and decides to take a Spanish course (choice moti
pro bably not perso nali ty alone, but the way in which it combines with o ther vation). After a few months of grammar lessons he beco mes frustrated with
factors, tha t influences second language lear ni ng . the course, StOps going to classes (execu tive motivatio n) and fin ally decides
to dro p the co urse. A wee k later a friend tells hi m about a grea t Span ish
88 Indiuidual diffirem:esin second Lmgu.tge f.:llnz ing Individual diffirences in second language learning 89
co nver sation co u rse she is taki ng, and his 'cho ice motivation' is activat ed ln each lesson , th e learn ers' m otivation was m easured in terms of th eir level
again. He dec ides ro register in m e conversation course an d in JUS t a few ofengagemenr. The proportio n ofstudents w ho paid atte ntio n , who actively
weeks he develops so m e basic Spanish co nversatio nal skills and a feeling of participated, an d who eagerly voluntee red d uring activities was calculated.
acco m plishmen t. H is satisfactio n level is so positi ve (mo tivation ret rospec A three-level scale was used to measure engagement in each observed lesso n:
tion) that he decides (Q enrol in a more advanced Spanish co urse when he very low (a few students) , low (o ne third to rwo thirds of me students) and
returns from his trip (Q Spain. high (mo re man rwo thirds o f the stude n ts}. Learners also co m pleted a ques
tionnaire about their motivation levels specifically related co their EFL class.
In a book devoted (Q helping second language teachers generate and maintain
learners' mo tivation. Dornyei (200 I b) pr op oses and descri bes conc rete and The researchers fo und significant positive correlations between the teachers'
innovative rnethods and techniques that can help teachers motivate learners motivational practices, the learners' engagement behaviours, and me learners'
throughout these three phases. self- repo rt s on the questio nnaire. The resea rche rs acknowledge that corre
lation results do not indi cate cause-effect relationships , Nevertheless. the
M otivation in the classroom findings are important because this is me first study (Q provide 'any empiri
cal evidence concerning the concrete. classroom-specific impact oflanguage
In a teac her's mind, motivated studenrs are usually those who participate teachers' motivational strategies' (C uillo reaux and Dornyei 2008: 72) .
actively in class, express interest in the subject matter. and srudy a great deal.
Teachers also have more infl uence on these behavio urs and the motivation
they represent than o n students' reason s for study ing the second language
Identity and ethnic group affiliation
o r their arrirudes coward the lang uag e a nd its speake rs. Tea che rs ca n make a Social Fa ct o rs in the wider comm u nity can also affe.:: t m ot ivatio n . alt itu des,
pos itive co ntr ib ut io n co stude n ts' m ot ivati on co learn if classr ooms are pla ces and lan gu age learn ing success, One suc h facto r is th e social dynamic or
that st ude nts enjoy co m ing co becau se the co n tent is inte resting and relevan t powe r relarionship betw een lan gua ges. Fo r exa m ple, me m bers ofa m in ori ty
co th eir age and level o f ab iliry, the learning goal s a re cha lleng in g yet manage gro u p learning the language o f a maj oriry group may ha ve differen t attitudes
able and clear, and the atmosphere is supportive. Teachers must also keep in and mo tivation from those of majority group members learn ing a minority
mind that cultu ral and age differences wi ll de termine the mos t appropriate language. Even though it is impossible to predict the exact effect of such
ways for them (Q motivate students. societal factors on second lan guage learn ing , the facc'that languages exist in
social COntexts cannot be overlooked when we seek to unde rstan d th e vari
Little research has in vestigated how pedagogy interacts directly with rnori
ables that affect success in learn ing. Children as well as adults are sens itive to
va tio n in second/fo reig n lan guage classrooms. One excep tion is a study
social d ynamics and power relationships . .' .
by Marie Guilloteaux and Zolran Dornyei (2008) who explored the links
between teachers' motivatio nal practice and sruden ts' motivation for L2 A g60d example of how relations of power in the ~o cial world affect interac
learni ng. It was a large-scale study with 27 teachers a nd over 1,300 learners rion between second lang uage lear ners and ta rget language speakers comes
in English as a Foreign Language (E FL) classrooms in Korea, The teach from the work of Bonny Norton Peirce, Drawing from data collected in a
ers' mo tivat ional strategies were described using a classroom observation lo ngitu d inal case study of the lan guage lear n ing ;experiences of immigrant
scheme-the Motivation Orientation of Language Teaching (MOLT) . women in Canada. she arg ues that concepts such as instrumental and inte
MOLT iden tified 25 m otivat io nal p ractices used by the teachers that we re gra tive mo tivatio n do nor ad equately cap ture the co rfip lex relati ons ofpower,
relatively easy co de fine and co o bserve. They were divided into four ca t identity, a nd language learning, Instead, she uses the te rm ' lnvestrnen c' to
ego ries mat are descri bed below alo ng with exam ples o f the mo tivational 'cap ture th e relati o nshi p of the language lea rn er [an d his/ her idenrity] co the
behavio urs included w ithin each. chan g ing social worl d .' (Norton Peirce 1995: 10). All the participants in her
study we re highly motivated to lear n English . H o wever, the re we re social
1 Teac her d isco urse: arousing curiosity or atte ntion, promoting autonomy,
situations in which they were reiucranr to speak and these were typically o nes
stating co m m un icative purpose/ utility of activity
in which t here was a power im balan ce. The ir experiences in th ose situations
2 Participation structure: gro up wo rk/pai r wo rk
limited the opportun ities they had to practise and to conti nue to develop the
3 Activity design : individ ual competitio n, team competition , in rellecrual
seco nd language outside the classro om .
challenge. ta ng ib le task product
4 Encou raging positive retrospective self-evalua tio n an d ac tivity design: Worki ng w it h immigranr c hil dren in English-medi um ki ndergart en classes,
effective p raise, elici ta tio n of self/peer correction sessio n, class app lause. Kellee n Toohey (2000) observed th a t they were q u ickly assig ned iden tities
Individual differences and classroom crirical pe riod may no t be based o n th e innate biolog ical struc t ures believed
to co nt rib ute to first language acquisition o r seco nd language acquisition in
instruction early childhood. Rather, older learners may depend on more generallearn
There are many questions about how the existence of individual differences ing abilities-rhe same ones they might use to acquire other kinds of skills
sho uld influence instruction . On a simple practical level , it is not possible or information . It is argued that these general learning abilities are not as
to r a teacher wi rh 50 st ude n ts-c-o r even one with 10 stude nts-c-eo customize effective for lan guage learnin g as th e.m or e speci fic, inna te capacities that are
instruction to suit the abilities or preferences ofeach one. Nevertheless, there available to the yo un g child. It is mo st often claimed that the critical period
can be little doubt that an instructional approach that rigidly adheres to a ends so mew here aro un d puberty, but so me researchers suggest it co uld be
single way of teaching all studen ts and an expectatio n that all students can even earlier. Others find evidence that there m ay be multiple critical periods,
learn in the same way will deprive some students of learning opportunities. related to different aspecrs of language learning. For example, the ability to
Zo lta n Dornyei (200 5) has reviewed the research on individual differences acquire rhe pronunciation patterns of a new language may end earlier than
and proposes a number ofways for educators to help learners make the mo st the abiliry.ro acquire vocabulary.
o f th eir individual abilities and learning preferences. Of co urse, as we saw in C hap ter 2, ir is difficult to compare ch ild ren and
Learners' instructi onal preferences, whether due to inherent differences in ad ults as second language learners. In addition to possible biological di f
their approach to learning or to their beliefs about how languages are learned, feren ces suggested by the C ritical Period H ypothesis, the conditions for
will influence the kinds of strateg ies th ey use in try ing to learn new mar e language learning are often very different. Youn ger learners in informallan
rial. Teach ers ca n help learn ers expa nd th eir repertoi re of learni ng strategies guage learning enviro n m ents usually have m or e tim e to devo te to learning
and rhu s devel op g reater flexibility in t heir ways of approaching langu age lang uage. They o fte n have more opportunities to hear-and use the lan guage in
learni ng. enviro n me nts where th ey do not experience stro ng pre ssure to speak fluently
an d accu rat ely from the very beginnin g. Fu rthe rmo re, [heir ea rly im pe rfect
effo rts are of[en prai sed, o r at least accepted. O lde r learn ers are mor e likely to
Age and second language learning find th emselves in situations char demand more co m plex language and [he
We now turn to a learner characte ristic of a different type : th e age at whi ch exp ressio n of more co mp lica ted ideas . Adults are often embarrassed by thei r
learning begins. Age is easier to define and measure than personality, apti lack of mastery of the language and they may develop'a sense of inadequacy
tude, or moti vati on, but the relationship between age an d success in seco nd mer experiences offrusrration in trying to say exacrlywhac they mean. Such
language acquisition is hardly less complex or controversial. negative feelings may affect their motivation and willingness to place them
selves in situations wh ere they will need to use the new language.
It is frequently obs erved that most children from immigrant famili es even - -r,
tuall y speak the language of th eir new community with native-like fluen cy, Research based o n the CPH in addition to personal experience or informal
while their parents often fall short ofsuch high levels ofproficiency. especially observation of adult learners' difficulties has led some educators and policy
in th e spo ke n language. To be sure. man y ad ulr second language learners makers as well as many parents to conclude that second language instruction
achieve excellent language skills . One often sees reference to Joseph Conrad, is most likely to succeed if it begins when learners 'are very young. However,
a native speaker ofPolish who became a majo r writer in the English language, some studies ofthe second language development ofolder and younger learn
and it is not uncommon to find adult second language learners with a rich ers learning in similar circumstances have shown [ha$cHder learn ers are more
vocabulary, sophisticated syntax, and effective pragmatic skills, even though efficient than younger learners. By using their meralinguistic knowledge ,
there may be subtle differences between their language use and that of those memory strategies, and problem-solving skills, they make the most ofsecond
who began learning the language wh ile very young. or foreign language instruction. In educational settings, learners who begin
learning a second language at primary school level do nor always achieve
/\5 we saw in Chapter 1, the Critical Period Hypothesis is that there is a
grea ter proficiency in the long run than those who begin in adolescence.
time in human development when the brain is predisposed for success in Furthermore, there are countless anecdotes about older learners (adolescents
language learning. It has been hypothesized that there is a critical period
and adults) who achieve excellence in [he second lan guage. Does this mean
tor second language acquisition JUS t as there is for first language acquisi
that there is no critical period for second language acquisition?
tion. Developmental changes in the brain , it is a rgued, affect the natu re of
language acquisition , and language learning that occurs after the end of the
94 Indiuidua! differences in second Language Learning Indiuidual differences in secondLanguage Learning 95
The criticalperiod: More than justpronunciation? When Patkowski examined the other factors thar m ight be thought to affect
success in second language acquisition, the picture was much less clear. There
Most studies of the relationship between age of acquisition and second was, naturally, some relationship between those factors and learning success,
lan guage development have concluded that older lear ners typically have a but it often rurn ed out that age was so closely related to the other factors that
no ticeable 'fo reign accent' in the spoken language. But what abo ut other it was not really possible to separate the m completely. For example, length of
linguistic features? Is syntax (word order, overall sentence st ructure) as residence in the United States so metimes seemed to be a fairly goo d predic
depend ent on age of acquisition as p ho nological develo pment? What about tor, However, it was often th e case that those with longer residence had also
mo rphology? arrived at an .earlier age. Sim ilar ly, amo unt of instru ctio n, when separated
M ark Patkowski (1980) studied the relatio nship between age and the from age, did not pred ict success as well as age of immigration did. Thus,
acquisition of fearures of a second language other than pronunciation. He Patkowski found that for learners who acquire a second language primarily in
hypothesized that, even if accent were ignored, only those who had begun the 'natural' environment, age of acquisition is an important factor in setting
learn ing th eir second language befo re the age of 15 could achieve full, limits on the development of native-like mastery of a second language and
na tive-like mastery of th at language. Parkowski srudied 67 highly educated tha t this limitation does not apply only to pro n unciation.
im migran ts to the Un ited Sta tes. They had started to learn English at vario us
ages, b ut all had lived in the United States for more th an five yea rs. H e Intuitions ofgrammaticality
compared them to 15 native-born Americans with a similarly high level of
Jacqueline johnson and Elissa Newport (I989) conducted a study of 46
education, whose variety of English could be considered the second language
C h inese and Korean speakers who had beg un to learn English at different
speakers' target language.
ages. All were students o r faculty members at an American un iversity an d all
The main questio n in Parkowski's research was: 'W ill there be a diffe rence had been in the United States for at least th ree years. The sru dy also included
between learners who began to learn English before puberty and those who a comparison group o f 23 native speakers of English . The participants were
began learning English later?' However, he also compared learners o n th e asked to make grammaticali ry judgements of a large number of sentences
basis of o ther characteristics and experiences that some people have sug that tested 12 rules of English morphology and syntax. They heard recorded
gested might be as good as age in predicting or explaining a person's success in sentences an d had to indicate whe ther each sentencdwas correct. Half of th e
mastering a second language. For example, he looked at the total amount of sentences were grammatical, half were not. .>'
time a speaker had been in the United Sta tes as well as the amount of formal
Johnson and Newport found that age of arrival in the United States was
ESL instruction each speaker had ha d.
a significant predictor o f success on the test. Learners who began earliest
A lengthy interview with each person was tape-reco rded . Because Patkowski achieved the highest scores on the judgement task. Those who began later
wanted to remove the possibility that the results would be affected by accent, were less likely to judge the sentences correctly arid their performance on the
he transcribed five-minute samples from the interviews and asked trained test varied more widely, .~
native-speaker judges to place each transcri pt on a scale from 0 (no knowl
edge of English) to 5 (a level of Englis h expected from an educated native
of
Robert DeKeyser (2000) carried our a rep lication th eJohnso n and Newport
study, working with Hungarian immigrants to the United States. He also
speaker).
found a strong relations hip between age of immig~ation and performance
The findings were quite dramatic. The transcripts of all native speakers and on th e judgement task. In addition, he asked participants to take language
32 out of 33 second language speakers who had begun learning English aptirude tests and fou nd that, for participants who began learning English as
before the age of 15 were rated 4 + or 5. The homogeneity o f the pre-puberty adults, aptitude scores were correlated with success. However, there was no
learners suggests that, for this gro up, success in learning a seco nd language such correlation for those who learned English in childhood. These findings
was almos t inevitable. In contrast, 27 of the 32 post-puberty learners were appear to confirm the hypothesis that ad ult learners may learn language in a
rated between 3 and 4 , but a few learners were rated higher (4+ or 5) and one way that is different from the way young children learn,
was rated at 2+ . The performance of this group looked like the SOrt of range
one would expect if one were measuring success in learning almost any kind
ofskill or kn owledge: some people did extremely well; some did poorly; most
were in the middle.
96 Indiuidua] differ ences in second language learning Individual differences I
second language instruc tio n, it is essent ial to think carefully about the goa ls
Rate oflearning of an instructional programme and the co nt ext in which it occurs befor e we
So me researc h suggests that olde r lear ners may have an ad vantage in term s of jum p to conclusions about the necessiry-or even the desirabiliry-of the
the rate of learning. They appear to lear n faster in th e early stages of seco nd earlies t poss ible start (Lighrbow n 2008a) .
lan guage devel opment. In 197 8, C atherine Sno w and Marian Hoefnagel
As we have seen, there isstrong evidence that differences in learning outcomes
H ohl e published a study on a group of English speakers who were learning
are associated with age oflearning. We have also seen that, especially for older
Dutch as a seco nd language while living in the Netherlands. The lear ners
learners, reaching high levels of second language proficiency involves apti
included children as yo ung as three years o ld as well as older children. ado
tude, motivation, and the appro pria te social conditions for learning. Thus ,
lescents, and adults. On tests administered when learners had been in the
dec isio ns about the age at which instruction sho uld begin canno t be based
COUntryfor less than a year. adolescents were by far the mos t successful learn
solely on research on the critical period hypothesis, which focuses only on
ers. They were ahead of everyone on nearly all of the tests, Furthermore, it
age and on the attainment of native-like proficiency.
was the ad ults, not the children. whose scores were second best . In ot her
words, adolescents and adults learned faster than children in th e first few In educational settings. it is particularly important to assess th e goals and
months of exposure to Dutch. the resources avai lable for second lan guage development. In these sett ings,
researc h has shown that older children and adolescents progress more rapidly
By the end of the year, the children were catching up, or hac! surpassed.
than younger ch ildren particu larly in the early stages oflearning. The knowl
the adults on 'several measures. Nevertheless, the adolescents retai ned t he
edge and sk ills tha t o lde r learn ers are abl e to acqu ire in a relati vely sho rt
hig hes t levels of performance ove rall. The fact that the young ch ildren were
pe riod of tim e wi ll satisfy th e needs of many learners whose goal is to use
catc h ing up, toge ther wi th evide nce fro m o ther stud ies, sugges ts [hat [hey
the lan guage for everyday com m u nication, to succeed on foreign language
wo uld pr ob ably su rpass the old er learners if they co nt inued to have adeq uate
exam inatio ns. or to read texts for an academic course rather than to speak
oppo rtunity to use th e language. However, thi s stu dy shows th at adults and
with na tive-like p ron unciation.
adolescents can make co ns ide ra ble and rapid progress in their proficiency in
a second language in cont exts where the y use the language in social, personal . When the o bjective of seco nd language learning is nati ve-like proficiency
professional , or academic interaction , in the target language. it may indeed be desirable for the learner to be com
ple tely surro unded by the lan guage as earl y as pos sib le. However, as we saw
One view of critical pe riod research that has had an important impact on
in Chapter 1, earl y intensive exposure to the second lan gu age may entai l
th e way we look at studies of language acquisition has been exp ressed in the
the loss or incomplete development of th e child 's first language. When the
work o f Vivian Cook (200 8). H e makes a strong case for the inappropriate
goal is basic communicative ab iliry for all stu dent s in an ed ucational system.
ness of using th e crit er ion of Tndisringuishable from a native speak er' as the
an d whe n ir is assumed that the ch ild's native language will continue to be
basis for su ccess in seco nd language acquisition. Indeed, Cook argues that
an important part of the ir lives. it may be more efficient to begin second or
a second language speaker or bilingual person should not be compared to
foreign language teaching later.
monolingual native speakers because the real goal is 'm ulrico rn petence' , that
is. knowledge of m ultip le languages th at in form and enrich one another. In mos t second- and fore ign -language classrooms. learn ers receive only a few
hours of instruction per week. Those w ho start la ter (for example. at age 10 .
Resear ch on the long-term outcomes of second language learni ng as well as
11, o r 12) often ca tch up with th ose who begin earl ier. I n Clare Burs rall's
the rate oflearning at different ages brin gs us to a ques tio n that is pro bab ly
(19 75) lan dmark study, stu dents who had made progress in early -starr pro
ofgrea test interest to most read ers of this book: What can we co nclude abour
gtammes, sometimes found themselves placed in secondary school classes
the role of age whe n learning takes place p rimarily in an ed ucational setting?
with students who had had no previous ins tructio n. Teachers who had both
the mo re advanced early -start stu dents and th e students who had had fewer
Age and second language instruction total ho urs of instruction te nded to teach to a lower co mmo n de nominato r,
Many peopl e w ho have never heard of the critical p eriod h ypothesis believe and d ifferences between th e rwo groups of stude nts essentially disappeared .
th at , in school p rog ramm es for second or fo reign lan gu age teach in g, 'yo unger Th is situation is not at all u ncommon . Further mo re, in ma ny educatio nal
is bett er '. H owever. bo th expe rience an d research show th at sta rring early is settings, srarti ng ins truc tio n ear lier may no t actual ly entai l many m o re total
no gua rantee ofsuccess and tha t olde r learners can attain high levels of profi ho urs of instructi on . For exam ple, in Quebec, resp ond ing to p ressure from
ciency in their seco nd lan gu age. In considering the best age at w hich to begin parents, the age at whic h instruction in Eng lish as a second language began
98 Individ ual differences in second language /earning
Individual differences in second language learn ing 99
was lowered in recent years fro m abour age 9 or 10 to age 6, but m e to tal Age is only one of m e characte ristics tha t det ermine m e way in which an
n umber of hours of instruction was not increased. Ramer, me nwnbe r of indi vidual approach es second language learning and the event ual success of
minutes of instruction per week was spread over more years (Lightbown, mat lear ning. The opportunities for learning (both inside and outside the
20 12). Thus, after years of classes, learners who have had an early start may classroom) , me motivation to learn, and individual differences in aptirude
feel frusrrared by the lack of progress, an d thei r motivation to continue may for language learning are also important factors m at affect both rate oflearn
be d im inished . C learly th e age at which insrruction begins is not th e on ly ing and eventual success in learning. It is important to remind ourselves mat
varia ble that determi nes success in the second language classroom. some older learners do achieve me highest level of success and mat many
For many years, it was difficult to compare early-start an d later-star t learners more are able co use their languages in a vari ety of personal, social, an d work
because of all the varia tions in their educational contexts. Since the 1990 s, place activities.
many more studies have allowed us to investigate this question more effec
tively. Some large-scale research pro jects have been particularly useful in
ACTIVITY Reflect on individual differences and
separa tin g the effect of age and other faccors in school-based foreign lan
language learning success
guage learn ing. For example, in Spain, me Barcelona Age Faeror (BAP )
project st udied the effects of changing the age of beginning co teach English Look back at the notes you made inTable 3. 1 about your language learning
[Q Catalan/Spanish bilingual student s. experience and that of your colleagues and friends.
When the starting age for teaching English was lowered , Carmen Munoz and Which cases confirm your expectations about th e variables that are
her colleagues took advant age of me opportuni ty [Q compare me learning associated w ith success--or the lack of it- i n second language learning?
outcomes for stu den ts who had started learning at different ages. They were 2 Which ones seem to challenge those ex pectations?
able to loo k ar students' progress after 100 , 4 16 , and 726 hours of ins truc 3 To what extent do you think t hat the contexts in which the learning took
tion . Th ose who had begun to learn later (aged 11, 14, or 18+) performed place are responsible for the outcomes?
berrer o n nearly every meas ure than those who had begun earlier (aged 8).
This was particularly true of m easures based on metalinguistic awareness or 4 To w hat extent do you t hink t he differe nces in o utco me are due to
analytic ability. O n listening comprehension, younger sta rters showed some differences inherent in t he individuals?
advantages . Munoz suggests mat th is may be based on yo unger learners' use S Do you think t hat the individuals might have experienced different
of a m ore implicit approach co lear ning while older learners' advantages may outcomes in different situations?
reflect their ability to use more explicit approaches, based on their greater
cog nitive maturity. She points out mat, in foreign language instruction,
where tim e is usually lim ited , 'younger lear ners may not have enough time
and exposure [Q benefit fully from me alleged advan tages ofi m plicit learning'
Summary
(M unoz 2006: 33 ). . In this chapter, we ha ve learned mat me results of research on indiv id ual
differences are not always easy to interpret. Thi s is partly due [Q m e dif
One of me advan tages of me BAF project is mat the researchers were able ficulty of defining an d measuring individual characteristics and to m e fact
to follow the same learners' language develop ment over several years. Th is m at the characteristics are no t independent ofon e another. In addition, rela
enabled th em to examine whether the early lear ners would eventually surpass tionshi ps between individual characteris tics an d learning en viro nments are
the older learn ers as has been observed in the 'natu ral' setting. This did not complex, and different learners will react differently to the same learning
happen-although me you nger learners caught up, me older lear ners main conditions . Indeed , me same learner will react differently co th e same co nd i
tained their advantage over time.
tions at different times. Researchers are beginning [Q explore the nature of
Decisions about when to starr second language ins truction in schools should these complex interact ions , but it remains difficult to predicr how a par ticu
be based on realistic goals an d on realistic estimates of how long it takes [Q lar individ ual's characteristics will in fluen ce his or her success as a language
achieve the m . O ne or two hours a week will not produce advanced second learner. Nonetheless, in a classro om , me goal of the sensitive teacher is co
language speakers, no ma tter how yo ung the y were when they began . Older create a learn ing environment w ith a wid e variety of instructio nal activities
learn ers m ay be able [Q make berrer use of the lim ited time the y have for so that learners with different abilities an d learni ng p refere nces can be suc
second language instr uction. cessful in learning a second language .
100 Indiuidua! differences in second l.mguuge learning Individual differences in second language learning 101
EXPLAINING SECOND
LANGUAGE LEARNING
Preview
A general theory o f seco nd language acq uisition needs to account for lan
guage acq uisitio n by learners with a variety of characteristics in a variety of
co ntexts . In th is chap ter we examine some of th e theories that have been
offered ro account for second language developmental progress and final
learning outcomes. We will loo k at how the behaviourisr and in natist expla
nations for first language acquisition that we saw in Chapter 1 have been
extended to account for second language acquisi tion. We will also look at
some theories fro m cogniti ve psychology that have increasingly in formed
second language research in recent years. These theories emphasize th e way
th e mind perceives, retains, organizes, an d retrieves information. Finally, we
will loo k at sociocultural theory, a perspective that places second language
acquisition in a larger social context.
aud iolingual teaching ma te rials and in teache r trainin g. C lassroom act ivities Gram ma r offers the best pe rspec tive from which to understa nd second lan
em p hasized mimicry and me mo rization, and students learned dialogues and guage acquisition. O th ers. fo r exam ple Robert BIey-Vroman (1 990) and
sente nce pat tern s by heart . Beca use language develop ment was viewed as the Jacquelyn Schachter ( 1990) have sugges ted th at, although UG may be an
formation of hab its, it was assumed that a person lear ni ng a seco nd lan guage ap prop riate framework for understanding first language acqu isitio n, it does
would start off with the hab its formed in the first language and that these not o ffer a good explan atio n for th e acqu isitio n of a second language, espe
habits would interfere with th e new ones needed for the second language. cially by learners who have passed the critical peri od. In their view, this mean s
Thus, behaviourism was often linked to the contrastive analysis hypothesis. that second lan guage acquisit ion has to be explained by some other theory,
However, as we saw in Chapter 2, researc hers found that many of the errors perh aps o ne of the more gen eral psych ological theories described below.
learners make are no t predictable on the basis of thei r first lan guage, nor do
Vivian Cook (2003) and others point o ut that there is still 'th e log ical
they always make the errors that would be predicted by a simple comparison
problem' of second language acquisition. That is, we need an explanation
of their first an d second languages. This discovery led to the reject io n ofbotli.
for the fact that learners eventually know more about the language than they
the contrastive analysis hypoth esis and behaviourism , lead ing to a period
co uld reasonably have learned if the y had to depend entirely o n the input
during which both the role of the first language and the role of practice
they are exp osed to . The implication is that knowledge ofUG must be avail
in learning a second language received lim ited attention in both research
and pedagogy. ab le to seco nd lan guage lear ners as well as to first language lear ners. Some of
the the ori sts wh o hold this view claim that the nature and availa bili ty of UG
In ChaprerZ , we saw ample evidence that second language lea rners draw o n ' are the same in first and seco nd language acquisit ion . Others argue that UG
whar th ey already know-including pr eviou sly learned lang uages: H owever, may be present and availab le to seco nd lan gua ge learn ers, b ut th at its exac t
we also saw th at they are so me times relu ctant to transfer certain first lan guage na ture has been altere d by th e acq uisitio n of other lan guages.
pattern s, even w hen the translatio n equ ivalent wo uld be co rrect. And we saw
Research ers wo rking wirhi n the UG framework also d iffer in th eir hypo rh
tha t first language influ ence may become more app arent as mo re is learned
eses about how formal instr uctio n or the availabi lity of feedback will affect
abo ut the seco nd lan guage, leading learn ers to see simi larities that th ey had
learn ers' knowledge of the seco nd language. Bonnie Schwartz (1993) , for
not perceived at an earlier stage. All thi s suggests tha r th e influence of th e
exam ple, concludes that instruction and feedback ch ange on ly su perficial
learner's first language may not sim ply be a matter of habits, but a more
aspects of language performance and do not affect the underlying syste m
subtle and complex process of iden tifying points of similarity, weighi ng the
atic knowledge of the new lan guage. She argues that language acquisition
evidence in suPPOrt of some particular feature, and even reflecting (tho ugh
is based on the availab ility of natural language in the learner's environment.
not necessarily consciously) about w hether a cert ain feature seem s to ' belong'
in the target lan guage. Interaction with speakers of that language is sufficie nt to trigger the acquisi
tion of the underlying structure of the language. Lydia White ( 199 1) and
By the 1970s, many resear chers were convinced that behaviourism and the o th ers agree tha r acquisition of many grammatical features of the new lan
COntrastive analysis hypothesis were inadequate explanations fo r second lan guage takes place naturally when learners are engaged in meaningful use o f
guage acquisition . As we shal l see, however, as research o n second language the language. H owever, they also suggest that, because the nature of UG is
acquisition has evo lved, the explanatio ns offered by behaviourism and the altered by the acquisiti on of the first language, second language learn ers ma y
contrastive analysis hypothesis have been revisited and understood in terms som eti mes need explicit information abo ut what is not grammatical in the
of new learning theo ries. seco nd lan guage. Otherwise, they may assu me that some structures of th e
first lang uage have equivalents in th e seco nd language when, in fact, the y
The innatist perspective do no r. In C ha p ter 2 , we saw a good example of th is in Whi te's study of the
p lacement of Engl ish adverbs in sentences prod uced by Frenc h speakers.
As we saw in Chapter 1, th e rejection of behaviourism as an explanation In Chap ter 6 (Stu d ies 18 and 33), we will see some research o n th e effect of
for first lang uage acquisitio n was partly triggered by C ho msky's critiq ue of instruction and feedback on suc h lan guage feat ures .
it. Chomsky arg ued tha t innate knowledge of the principles of Universal
Grammar per mits all ch ild ren to acq uire the lang uage of thei r enviro nme nt Resear che rs who study seco nd langu age acquisition from a UG pers pective
du ring a critical per iod of th eir develop ment. While C homsky di d no t make are usu ally interested in the lan guage co m pet en ce of advanced learners
specific claims abou t the implications of his theory for second language their kn ow ledge of com plex gra mmar-rathe r th an in the simple lan gu age
learning, Lydia Wh ite (2003) and other lingu ists have arg ued th at Universal of beg inning learn ers. They are interested in whe ther the competence that
underlies the use of the second lan guage resem bles th e co mpetence underlying
106 Expla ining second language learning
Explaining second language learning 107
the lan guage perfor mance o f native speakers. Thus, their investigations often
invo lve grammaticality judgement or Other methods to probe what learners
know about the language rath er than observations of nat ural language use.
Perhaps the best known model ofsecond language acquisition influen ced by
Chomsky's th eo ry of first language acq uisition is Stephen Krashen's (1982)
Monitor Model, first described in the early 197 0s, at a time when there was
growi ng dissatisfaction with language teaching methods based o n behavio ur
ism, Krashe n desc ribed his model in terms of five hypotheses.
In the acquisition/learning hy po thesis, Krashen suggests that we 'acq uire' lan
guage as we are exposed ro sam ples of language that we understand in much
the sam e way that children pic k up the ir first lan guage-with no conscious
attention ro lang uage form. We 'learn' on the other han d through co nscious
attention to form an d rule learning. In Krashen's view, far mor e lan gu age is
acquired than learned. The Moniror Model been challe nge d by o ther researchers and theoris ts,
some ofwho m have argued that it is not possi ble to test Krashen's hypo theses
N ext, according ro the monitor hypothesis, second language users draw on
in empi rical research (Mclaughlin 1987) or th at he has drawn the wrong
what they have acquired when they engage in spontaneous communica
con clusions from resear ch (White 1987). Nevertheless, h is ideas about
tion . Th ey may use rules and patterns that have been learned as an editor
seco nd language develop ment were in flue neial d uring a period when second
or 'monitor' , allowing them ro make mino r changes and polish what the
lan gu age teach ing was in transition from structu re-based approaches that
acquired system has prod uced . Such m o niroring takes place only whe n the
emphasized lear ning rules or memorizing dialogues, ro approaches that
speake r/w riter has plen ty o f time, is concerned abo ut producing cor rect lan
emphasized using lan guage w ith a focus on meaning. Since then, as we will see
guage, and has learned the relevant ru les.
in Chapter 6, co m m unicative language teaching, including immersion, con
Th e natural order hypo thesis was based on the find ing th at, as in first language tent-based, an d task-based lan guage teaching, has been widely implemented .
acquisition, second lan guage acquisition unfolds in p redictable seq uences, as Krashen's hypotheses, especially th e comprehe nsib le input hypothesis, have
we saw in Chapter 2. The language rules that are easiest ro state (and thus ro been a source ofideas for research in second language acquisition. Classroom
learn ) are no t necessarily the first ro be acquired. research has confirmed that students can make agreatdeal ofprogress through
expo sure to co mprehensible input without direct instruction. Studies have
Th e comprehensi ble input hypothesis is that acquisition occ ur s when one
also shown, however, that students may reach a point from which they fail to
is exposed ro language that is co mprehensible and contains i + 1. Th e "i' rep
mak e further progress on some featu res of the second language u nless they
resents the level oflanguage already acquired, and the ' + I ' is a metaphor for
also have access ro gui d ed instruction. Some insigh ts from lear n ing theo ries
language (words, grammatical form s, aspects of pron un ciation) that is JUSt a
step beyond th at level. developed in cog nitive psychology help ro exp lain why th is may be so.
Krashen's affictive jilter hypothesis is proposed to account for the fact that
some peo ple who are exposed to large quantities of co mprehensible in p ut do ACT IVITY Examine t he M o ni t o r M od el
no t necessarily acquire lang uage success fully. Th e 'affective filter' is a meta A number of write rs have questio ne d the validity of Kra shen's Monitor Model,
phorical barrier that prevents learners fro m acquiring lan guage even when partly o n t he basis that it is difficult t o test the five hypotheses in e mp irical
app ropriate inp u t is available. Affict refers ro feelings of anxiety or negative stu dies. Ne ve rthe less, Kras he n's views have re mained influen t ial in seco nd
attitudes tha t, as we saw in C ha pter 3, may be associated with poor learn language tea ching.
ing out comes. A learner who is ten se, anxi o us, or bore d m ay jilter out input,
m aking it unavailable for acquisitio n. I C an yo u th ink of so me reasons wh y t his might be so !
108 Explaining second fangullge learning Explaining second language learning 109
2 Which of the hypotheses do you find intuitively convincing! frees up cognitive process ing resou rces to notice othe r aspec ts of the langu age
3 Wh ich ones leave you sceptical! Why!
that, in turn, gradually become automatic.
For proficient speakers, choosin g words, pronouncing them, and string
The cognitive perspective ing them rogether with the appropriate grammatical markers is essent ially
auto matic. Fu rthermore, much ofwhat the se speakers say is drawn from pre
Since jhe 1990s, research and th eories from cognmve psychology have dic tab le patterns o flan guage that are at least partly formulaic, That is, fluent
become increasingly central to our understanding ofsecond language devel speakers do not create new sentences by choosingone word at a tim e but rath er
opmen t. Some of these the ories use the computer as a metaphor for the m ind, by using strings o f words that typically occur together, This use of patterns
co m par ing lan guage acqu isitio n to th e capacities of co m pute rs for sto ring, applies not onl y to idiomatic expressio ns, but also to much co nve rsatio nal
integrating, and retrieving information. Some draw on neurobiology, seeking language and written language in a specific genre (Ellis, Simpson-Vlach, and
to relat e o bserved behaviour as dire ctl y as possible to brain acti vity. Ma ynard 200 8) .
As in first lan gua ge acq uis itio n, cogn itive and de velopmental psychologists Ano the r aspect ofauro rnaticity in lan gu age process ing is th e retr ieval ofword
argue that there is no need to hypothesize that humans have a language-spe meanings. When proficient listeners hear a familiar word, even for a sp lit
cific module in th e brain or that acquisition and learning are d istinct mental seco nd , they can no t help but understand it. Such auto matic responses do
pro cesses. In their view, general theo ries of learning can account for the not use up the kind of resources needed for pr ocessing new informati on.
grad ual development of co m plex syntax an d for learners' inabili ty to sponta Thus, p roficient langu age users can give their full attention ' to th e overall
neo usly use everything they know abou t a language at a given tim e. As not ed mea n ing ofa text or conversatio n. whereas less pro ficient learne rs use mo re
above, some ling uists have also concluded that, wh ile the in narist perspective of thei r attentio n on processing th e meaning of indi vid ual words and the
provides a plausib le exp lanation for first language acquisition, something relationsh ips between them. Th e lack of automatic access to meaning helps
else is requ ired for second language acqu isitio n, since it so often falls short to expla in why second language reade rs need more time to understa nd a text,
of full success. Fro m the cog nirive psych ology persp ective, however, first and even if th ey event ually do fully co m p rehe nd it. Th e information p rocessin g
seco nd language acq uisitio n are seen as dr awing o n the sam e processes o f model suggests that there is a limit to the amount o Hocused mental activity
perceptio n, memo ry, catego rizatio n, and generalization. The difference lies we ca n engage in at one time.
in th e circ umstan ces oflearn ing as well as in what th e learners already know
abo ut language and ho w that pri or knowledge shapes their perception of the Information pro cessing approach es to seco nd lan gu age acq uisitio n have
new language. been explo red by many researchers. Drawing on J. R. Anderson's (199 5)
wor k, Robert D eKe yser (1 99 8, 200 1, 2007) and oth ers have investi gated
seco nd language acquis ition as 'skill learning'. They suggest that most learn
InfOrmation processing in g, including language learning, starts with declarative knowledge, th at is,
Cogn itive psychologists working in an information-processing model o f kn owledge that we are aware of havin g, for example, a grammar rul e. Th e
human learning an d performance see seco nd lan gu age acquisition as th e hypothesis is that, through pr acti ce, decl arati ve knowledge ma y become pro
build ing up of knowledge that can eventually be called on automatically cedural knowledge, or th e ability to use th e kn owledg e. Wi th continued
for speaki ng and understanding. Robert DeKeyser (1998 ), Richard Schmidt p ractice, the procedural kn owledge can become automatized and the learn er
CWO1) and others have suggested that learners must pay attention at first to ma y for get havin g learned it first as declarative knowledge.
any aspect of the language that they are trying to learn or produce. 'Pay atten Acco rding to this perspective, once ski lls become automatized , thinking
tion' in this con text is accepted to mean 'using cognitive resources to process abou t the declarative knowledge while trying to perform the ski ll acrually
information' but there is a limi t to how much information a lear ner can pay disrupts the smooth performance of it. Think, for example, of trying to drive
att ent ion to. Thus, lear ners at the earliest stages will ten d to use most of their a car o r skate whi le intentionally thinking abou t and preparing every move.
resources to understand th e main words in a message. In that situation, they With enough pra ctice, procedural knowledge eclip ses the declarative knowl
may not notice the grammatical morphemes attached to some of the wo rds, edge, which, in time, may be forgotten. Fo r this reason , fluent speakers may
especially those tha t do not su bstant ially affect meaning. Gradually, through no t even realize that the y once po ssessed the declarative knowledge that set
expe rience and pr act ice, info rmatio n th at was new becomes easier to process, the process in motion .
and learners become able to access it q u ickly and even automatically. This
Explaining second language learning 11 1
110 Explain ing second Languagelearning
Sometimes changes in language beh avio ur do no t seem [Q be exp lainabl e learners enco unter specific linguistic features in the in put and the frequency
in terms of a gradual build-up of fluency through practice. These changes with which lan gu age features occur together. According to th is view, learn
have been descri bed in terms of restructuring (Mclaughlin 1990). They ers develop a stronger an d stronger nerwork of associations or connections
seem [Q be based on some qualitative change in the learner's knowledge . between these features as well as between lan guage features an d the contexts
Restructu ring may account for what appear [Q be bursts of p rogress, when in wh ich th ey occ ur. Eventually, th e presence of one situational o r lingu istic
lear ne rs suddenly seem to 'pu t it all together', even though they have no t had featu re will activate the otherfs) in the learner's mind. For exam ple, learners
any new instr uctio n or appare n tly relevan t exp osure to the language. It may migh t get su bject-verb agreeme nt co rrect, not because th ey know a rule but
also explain apparent backsliding, whe n a systematic aspect of a learner's lan because they have heard examples such as 'I say' an d 'he says' so ofte n that
guage incorpo rat es too much o r incorpor ates th e wro ng things. For example, each subj ect pronoun activates th e co rrect verb form .
as we saw in Chapter 2, when a learner finally masters the use of the regular Con nections may be strong because the lang uage featu res have occurred
-ed ending [Q show past tense, irregular verbs th at had previo usly been used together freq uen tly or th ey may be relatively weaker beca use th ere have been
correctly may be affected. Th us, after months o f saying 'I saw a film ', th e fewer opportunities to experi ence them to gether. Some of the evidence fo r
lear ner may say 'I seed' or even 'I sawed'. Such overgene ralizatio n erro rs are usage- based views co mes from th e o bservation mentio ned above that much
not based on practice of those specific items bu t rather on th eir integrati on of the language we use in o rdinary co nversa tion or in particular genres is
int o a gene ral pattern. predictab le, and to a considerable exte nt based on formulaic un its o r ch unks.
Another concept from psycho logy offers insig ht into how learners store and As sugges ted by N ick Ellis (200 3 , 2005) and ot hers , language is at least partly
retrieve langu age. Acco rdi ng [Q tr an sfer-approp riat e processing (TAP ), learned in uni ts larger than sing le words, and sentences or phrases are no t
information is best retrieved in situations that are similar to those in which it usually put toge ther one wo rd at a tim e. As no ted in Chapter 1, usage- based
was acquired (Lightbown 2008b) . Th is is because when we learn something research has shown that a learn ing mec hanism , simulated by a computer
our me mories also reco rd aspects of th e co ntext in which it was learned and program, can not on ly 'learn' from in put but can also generalize, even maki ng
even the cognitive p rocesses invo lved in th e way we lear ned it, for exam ple, overgeneralization errors.
by read in g or heari ng it. To da te, mo st of the research on transfer-app rop ria te
processing has been done in laborato ry experiments, for exam ple, co mparing The competitio n mod el
the learn ing ofword lists under different co ndi tions. However, the hypothesis Elizabe th Bates and Brian MacWhin ney (198 1) described the 'co m pet ition
seems [Q offer a pla usible way of explaining a widely observed ph eno m enon m odel' as an explanation for both first and second language acquisition that
in seco nd language lear ning: knowledge tha t is acquired mai nly in rul e learn takes into account no t on ly lan guage form but also lan guage me an ing an d
ing or drill activities may be easier [Q access on tests th at resemble the lear n ing
language use. Through expos ure to th ou san ds of examples oflanguage asso
activities than in communicative situ ations. On the Other hand, if learne rs'
ciated wi th particular mean ings, speakers of a particular language co me to
attention is drawn co grammatical forms during communicative activities understand how [Q use the 'cues' that signal specifi c functions . For example,
in wh ich their cognitive resources are occupied with a focus on mean ing,
the relationship between words in a sentence may be signalled by word
the retrieval of those forms on a grammar test may be more d ifficult. In order, grammatical markers, and the an im acy of the nouns in th e sentence.
C hapter 6, a classroo m investigatio n of L2 lear ning influen ced by transfer Mos t lan guages make use of multiple cues, but they di ffer in th e p rimacy of
appropriate pro cessing is described in Stu dy 40 . each. This becomes clear in a situa tion where the mean ing of a sentence is
no t immediately obvio us. What hel ps yo u figu re out the mean ing? English
Usage-based lea rning uses word order as the most common indicator of the relationships between
As seen in the discussion of first lang uage acquisition in C hapter 1, cognitive sentenc e componen ts. Most English sentences have the order Su bject-Verb
psycho logists, unlike innatists, see no need to hyp othesize the existence of Object (5VO) . That is, the typical English sentence ment io ns th e su bject
a neurological mo dule ded icated exclusively to lan gu age acqu isition. They first , the n the verb , then the object.
argue tha t what is innate is simply the ab ility to learn , rather than any specific Two - and th ree-year old English-speaking ch ildre n can usual ly use cues of
lingu isti c p rincip les. Some usage- based theories also attri bute less impor ani macy and their kn owledge of th e way th ings work in the wo rld to int er
tance to th e kind of de clarative kn owledg e th at ch aracter izes skill learn ing pret odd sent ences. Th us, if th ey hear a string of words such as 'Box push
and trad itio nal str ucture- based app roaches to second lan gua ge ins tru ctio n. boy' , the y will act it ou t by making a boy do ll push a tiny box , focusing o n
As Nic k Ellis (20 02) explains, th e em phasis is on th e freque ncy wit h whic h
112 Explaining second Lmgu,tge learning Explaining second language learning 113
the fact that th e 'boy' is the natural agent ofaction in this situation. H owe ver, 6 Accord ing to the competition model. how might these sentences be
the SVO patrern is so stro ng in English that, by the time the y are four years interpreted by speakers of a language with a more flexible word order than
old, ch ildre n heari ng th is sent ence will ignore the fact that boxes do n't nor English? Wh at wou ld those speakers focus on?
mally move on their ow n , and carefully demo nstrate how th e box pus hes th e
boy. For English speakers, wo rd order patterns are str onger than animacy
cues at thi s pai n e. At this age, ch ild ren may attri bute t he SVO relation ship The cog rnuve perspe ct ive e m phasizes the role of general h um an ab ili
to sentences in th e passive voice. That is, 'Th e box was pushed by the boy' ties ro process and learn info rma tio n-c--i nc luding language-on th e basis
may be interpreted as 'Th e box pushed th e boy.' Only later do they learn to of experience. In recent years , the term 'co gni tive lingui stic s' has eme rged
pay attention to the grammatical markers th at distinguish the active voice an d highlights the view that language is but one of th e complex knowledge
sentence from th e passive word order. syste ms that humans acquire. Peter Rob inson and N ick Ellis (20 08) suggest
tha t cognitive linguistics draws from an d builds on a number o f different
In contrast , Spanish and Italian have more flexib le word order, and speak app roaches that have in common th e hypothesis that language is learned
ers of these languages rely more on grammatical markers (fo r example, th e th rou gh our perceptual an d cog n itive experiences and th at like all other
agreement of subj ect and ver b, the case marking of pronouns) o r o n th e aspects oflear ning, lan guage learning involves th e discovery, categorization,
an irnacv of nouns to understand how sen rene e elem ents are related. When and determination of patterns through the use oflang uage.
English speakers are learn ing these languages, th ey may have difficulry sup
pressing their tendency to rely on word order as the basis for interpret at ion .
For examp le, an English speaki ng learn er of Italian may find it co nfusing to
La nguage and the brain
hear sent ences suc h as II giocattolo gUtlrda if bambino (the toy-is loo king Anot her area of work wit hi n but no t lim ited to the cog ni tive perspective
at-the boy). An Italian speaker, accustomed to more flexib le wo rd order. is concerned wi th language lear ning and the b rain. Some of the questions
focuses on the ani macy of t he rwo nouns and concludes tha t th e most rea investigated include whether first and second languages are acquired and
sonab le interpreta tion is th at the boy is looking at the toy. Acco rd ing to th e represented in th e same areas of the brain and whe ther th e b rain processes
co m petition model, seco nd language acq uisitio n requires that learners learn seco nd lang uage input d ifferently fro m first language in pu t. For a long
th e relar ive impo rtanc e o f the different cues app ropriate in the langu age th ey ti me the assumption was that lan guage fun cti ons were located in th e left
are learning (M acWhin ney 199 7). hemisphere of the brain. Nonetheless, recent brain imaging stu d ies show
activa tio n in different locati ons in both hemispheres of th e brain during lan
guage processing. This is rru e for bo th first and second languages. However,
ACTIVITY Look at how different cues lead to sentence differences ha ve been observed . depending on th e learners' age and level of
interpretation p roficiency. For example, when learners w ho acquire a seco nd lan gua ge later
Consider the following sente nces: in life are given a granlmatical task ro co m p lete, th ey show ac tivatio n in th e
I The boy eats the apple.
same neural areas that are activated for L I processing but also .ac tivatio n in
2 The apple eats the boy.
o the r areas of the brain. This is not the case with younger learners who show
3 The dog sees the ball.
acti vation o nly in the areas for LI processing (Berett a 2011 ). Other stu d ies
4 The ball chases the dog.
h ave measur ed the electrical activity in br ain waves ro exp lore differen ces in
S The ball is chased by the dog.
the processing oflanguage in p ut. Some of this research has shown that as an
L2 learn er's pro ficiency increases, the brain act ivity looks more like that of
I Do they all follow the patterns of English grammar? first language processing. There is also evidence that semantic pro cesses are
2 How can you tellwhich noun refers to the agent (the one who performs the first to loo k more like L 1 processin g patterns followed by syntactic p ro
the actio n)? cesses as proficiency in th e L2 in creases (H ah ne 200 1).
3 In each se ntence, what cue tells yo u which noun is the agent? While it is fascinating to th ink about con nectio ns between seco nd langu age
learnin g and the brain. it is important ro keep in mind that th is is a yo u ng
4 Is there more than o ne cue?
di scip line. Furthermore, the limited research that has been conducted has
5 How are sentences 4 and S above different from each other? pro d uced mixed find ings. Th erefore any impl icati ons o f lan gu age and br ain
research for second language teaching are premature.
Explaining second language learning 115
114 Explaining second language learning
development. Related to this is Merrill Swain's (1985) comprehenSible
Second language applications: Interacting; noticing; output hypothesis. She argued chat when learners must produce language
processing, andpractising . mat their interlocutor can understand, they are most likely to see me limits of
their second lan guage abil ity and the need to find better ways to express their
A number of hypotheses, theories, and models for exp laining second lan
meaning. The demands of pro ducing comprehensible Output, she hy poth
guage acquisition have been inspired by me cognitive perspective.
esized, 'push' learners ahead in their development.
The interaction hypothesis
The noticing hypothesis
Evelyn Hatch (1978), Michael Long (1983, 1996 ) ,Teresa Pica (1994), Susan
Richard Schmidt (1 990, 2001) proposed the noticing hypothesis, sug
G ass (1997), and many othe rs have argued mar co nversa tional interaction
gesring that nothing is learned unless it has been 'noticed' . N otici ng does
is an essential, if nor sufficient, condition for second lan gu age acquisition.
nor itself result in acquisition, but it is me essential starring point. From
These researchers have studied me ways in whic h speakers modify th eir
this perspective, co mprehensible inp ut does no t lead to growth in language
speech and their interaction parrern s in order to help learners participate
knowledge unless the learner becomes aware ofa particula r lan guage feature.
in a conversation or u nd erstan d meaning in a new language. Long (1983)
agreed with Krashen chat comprehensible inp ut is necessary for language Schmidt's original proposal of me noticing hypothesis carne from his own
acquisition. However, he focused On me question of how in put could be experience as a learner of Portuguese. After mo n ths of taki ng classes, living
.made comprehensible. He argued mar modified interaction is the ne~s in Brazil, and keeping a diary, he began to realize that certain features of
sary mechanism for making language comp rehensible. That is, what learners lan guage that had been present in the enviro n ment for the whole tim e began
nee d is opportunities to interact with ocher speakers, working rogerher to to enter his own second language system only when he had noticed memo
reach mutual comprehension through negotiation for meaning. Through This was because they were brought to his attention in class or some other
these interactions, interlocutors figure our what they need to do ro kee p the experience made them salient. Drawing on psychological lear ning theories,
conversation go ing and make the input comprehensible to me less profi Schmidt hypothesized char second language learners could no r begin to
cient speaker. According to Long, m ere are no cases of beginner-level learners acquire a language feature until they had become aware of it in the in put.
acquiring a second language fro m native-speaker talk mat has not been mod Susan Gass (19 88) also described a learning process that begi ns when learn
ified in some way. ers noti ce something in me second lan guage that is different fro m what they
expected or that fills a gap in their knowledge of m e lan guage.
Modified interaction does not always involve linguistic simplification. It may
also include elaboration, slower speech rare, gestu re, or me provision ofaddi The q uestio n of whether lear ners must be aware that they are 'no ticing'
tional contextual cues . Some examples of conversational modificario ns are: something in the input is me object of considerable debate . Accord ing to
info rmation processing rheories, anything that uses up our mental 'process
learner has understood (for example, 'Th e bus leaves at 6:30. Do you
contribute to learning. From a usage-based perspective, the likelihood of
understand?') .
acquisition is best predicted by the freq uency wit h which something is avail
clarify someth ing mar has not been understood (for example, 'Could you
repeat please?'). These requests from me learner lead to fu rth er modifica These q uestions about rhe im portance of awareness and attention con
tions by me native speaker. tinue to be the object of research. Several researchers have found ways to
3 Self-repetition or paraphrase-rhe more proficient speaker repeats his or track learners' attention as they engage in second lan guage interactio n. For
her sentence eit her partially or in its entirety (fo r example, 'She gOt lost example, Alison Mackey, Susan Gass, and Kim McDonough (2000) had
o n her way home from school. She was walking home fro m school. She learne rs watch and listen co them selves in Videotaped interactio ns and asked
got lost. '). questions leading them to explore what they were thinking as they partici
Long (1996) revised the interaction hypothesis, placi ng more emphasis on chat learn ers had to solve wh ile rh inking alo ud , th us provid ing some insight
cognitive facto rs such as 'noticing and corrective feedback during inter in to wha t they not iced about lan guage as they worked. Merrill Swai n an d
act ion. When communication is d ifficul t, int erlocu to rs m ust 'nego tiate Sharon Lapkin (199 8) recorded learners in pair work an d kept trac k of me
language teatures the y mentioned. These research designs cannor tell us if even though they progressed at dirferent rates . Th e researchers also found
learne rs no ticed rhings cheyd id nor m entio n . However. rhey do make it pos thar some lan guage feat ures did nor seem to be affected by rhese co nstralnts
sib le to ide n tify so me th in gs char learners we re aware o f and to look at how an d could be learned and used by lea rn ers wh o were ar d ifferenr develo p men
th is awarenes s is related [Q measures of the ir language knowledge. The ext ent tal stages. These were referred to as variational features .
[Q whic h lea rn ers' no ricing o f lan guage featu res affects their seco nd language
Pienemann (1999, 2003) d evelo ped processabiliry th eory on rhe basis of
development wi ll come up ;1gain in our discussion of research on seco nd
research with learners of different languages in a variety of settings, both
language acquisition in the classroom in C hapters 5 and 6 .
instructional and info rmal . One irn po rran r aspect of h is theory is th e inte
gra tio n of de velo p me ntal seque nces with first lan guage infl ue nce . He argues
Input processing
that his theo ry explains why lea rn ers do no t simply transfer features from
In his research wi rh American unive rsity students learn in g forei gn lan gu ages, their first lang uage ar ear ly stages ofacquisirion. Insread, they have to develop
Bill Van Parre n (2004) o bserved many cases of students m isinterpreting sen a certain level of processing capacity in the seco nd language before they can
reaces. Fo r example, as predi cred by the co m pe tition m odel discussed earl ier use their knowledge of the features rhar already exist in their first language.
in th is chapter. when English speakers heard sentences in Span ish, chey used We saw examples of this in the acquisition of negatives and questions in
wo rd o rde r to interpret the relarionships amo ng the no uns in the sentence. Chap rer 2.
Th us. they interp reted ' La sigu« el seizor' as 'She (subjecr pronoun) follows
the man'. The co rrec t inrerpretarion is 'Her (objec t p ro nou n) follows rhe The role of p ractice
man' (subject of the sentence). In orher words . the co rrect English trans latio n
O ne co m po nenr o f language learn ing that has seen a renewal of interest
wo uld be 'Th e man follows her'. In order to u nd erstand that . stude n ts need
with in th e cog nirive persp ecti ve is pract ice. As we saw in di scu ssion s of the
ro learn that in Spanish , a pronoun o bjec t o ften precedes the verb and that,
be ha vio u risr perspecti ve, a n approach to learning that is based o n drill and
rather than rely o n th e word o rder alone, it is essential to pay attention to
m ar separates pr actice from meaningful lan guage use d oes not usu ally lead
wh ether the form of the pronoun indicates a subject or an objec t.
ro co m m u nicative competence. This does nor mean , however, that practice
Van Parten argued that the problem arose in part from the fact that learn is nor an essenrial co mponenr oflanguage learning. Robert DeKeyser ( 1998)
e rs have lim ired processing capacity an d ca n no r pay at te ntion [ 0 form and asserts that some classroom interpretations ofbehavio urism missed rhe po int
meaning ar the same ti me . Nor surprisingly. they ren d to g ive prio rity [Q rhar practice is o nly effec tive if one practises t he behaviour th a t one wishes to
meaning, ove rlooking some fearures of the lan guage form. Whe n the conrext learn. As we w ill see in Chap ter G, the dr ills that characterized a udiolingual
in w h ich rhey hear a sentence hel ps rhem make se nse o f ir, thar is a goo d ins truction often failed to make rhe co nnec rion between th e lan guage par
strategy for u nd ersran di ng th e gen e ral idea. bur ir may interfere w ith learners' terns being d rilled and the meaning(s) associared wirh them .
progress in acqu irin g th e language. In C hap rer 6 we wi ll see ho w VanPatten
Researchers are now loo king m o re closely at how pr actice co nverts declarative
developed ins tructio na l proced u res that require learners to focus o n the spe
knowledge to pro cedural knowledge and th e n to automat ic pe rfo rmance .
cific language fearures in order [Q interpret the meani ng , th us pushing them
No re that from the cognirive perspective, the practice needed for language
ro acquire those features.
develo pmenr is no t mechanical, and ir is nor limired to the production of
language. Listening and reading are also affected by opportunities for prac
Pro cessability theory
rice. Lo ur des O rtega (2007) has pro posed three principles fo r practice in
jurgen Meisel. H arald C lahsen, and Manfred Pienemann (198 1) studied rhe the foreign lan gu age classr oom rh ar she sees as compati ble w irh rhe researc h
acq uis itio n of G erm an by a group ofad ult mi gram wo rke rs who had littl e o r carried o ur from wh ar she calls rhe 'cog nltive-In te ractio n ist' pers pec tive:
no seco nd lan gu age instruction . Th ey analysed large sam ples o f th eir speech
and described th e details of developmenral seque nces in rhe ir pr oducti on of 1 Practice sho uld be inre racr ive.
sim p le and co m plex sen te nces . Th ey co ncl ude d rhat the sequen ce ofdevelop 2 Practice sho uld be m ean in gful.
me n t for fearu res of synra.x and morpho lo gy was affected by how easy th ese 3 The re sho uld be a focu s o n tas k-esse n rial fo rms.
were to process. Ease o f processing was fo un d [Q dep end to a lar ge extent o n Elizabe rh G atbonto n and No r man Segalowirz ( 1988. 2005) have devel
the positio n of th ose features in a senre nce . Features rhar typically occ ur red oped an app roach to lan gu age teac hi ng ca lled ACCESS (Auro rnatizario n
ar the beginning o r end of a senrence were easier ro process (and learn ) rhan in Com mu n ica tive Co n rexrs of Esse nrial Speech Seg ments) . Ir d raws on rhe
those in che m iddle. All learners acq uired th e fea tu res in th e sa me seq ue nce . cog nit ive persp ective and is based o n classr oo m activities w hic h, by rhei r
118 Explaining second language learn ing Explain ing second language tearning 119
nature, require learners to use meaningful units of language repet itively is attached to the conversations th ems elves, wi th learning occurring through
in contexts where there are genuine exchanges of meaning. The goal is to the social interacti on . Soc ioc ul tu ral th eo ry holds that peopl e gain co n trol of
provide opportunities for using these units wi th sufficient frequency that and reorganize their cogni tive processes during mediation as knowledge is
they will become automatic. Segalowitz (20 10) has emphasized the impor internalized during soc ial activity.
tance of increasing the amount of language that can be used automatically,
th us freeing more cognitive resources for learning new thi ngs. Paul Nation Second language applications: Learning by talking
(2007) has suggeste d tha t automaticity, which he, like Segalowitz, refers to as
'fluency ' may be the most neglected aspec t of language teachin g in contexts Extending Vygotskyan th eo ry to second language acquisition, Jim Lan to If
whe re instruction focuses primarily on meaning. (2000), Richard Do nato (1994) , and others are interested in showing how
second lan guage learners acq uire lan guage when they collaborate and inter
act with other speakers. Traditionally, the ZPD has been understood to
The sociocultural perspective involve an expert and a no vice . H owever, recent work has broadened the
As we saw in Chapter 1, Vygorsky's theory assumes that cognitive develop term to include novice -novice or learner-learner interactions. An example
ment, including language development, arises as a result ofsocial interactions. of this is in Communication task B in Chapter 5 (p. 137 ). In th at excerp t ,
Unlike the psychological theories that view th inki ng and speaking as related the learners are struggli ng wit h French reflexive verbs as they try to co nstruct
but independent processes, sociocultural th eory views speaking and th ink a storyline fro m pic tu res. The example is fro m the work o f Merrill Swain
ing as tightly interwoven. Speaking (and writing) mediates thinking, which and Sharon Lapkin (200 2), wh o have investi gat ed sociocultural explanations
means tha t people can gain control over their mental processes as a conse for second language learning in C anad ian Fren ch immersion p rogrammes.
quence of int ernalizi ng what others say to them and what they say to others. Th eir work has its origins in Swain's comprehen sible o utput hypothesis and
This internalizing is tho ught to occur when an individual interacts with an the notion that when learners have to produce language, they must pay mo re
interlocutor within his or her zone of proximal development (ZPD)-that attention to how meaning is expre ssed through language than they ordi
is, in a situation in which the learner can perform at a higher level because of narily do for the comprehension of language. Swain (1985) first pro posed
the support (scaffolding) offered by an interlocutor. the comprehensible output hypothesis based on the observation th at French
immersion studen ts we re considerably weaker in their spo ken and writte n
In some ways, this approach may appear to restate some of th e hypotheses production than in their reading and liste ning comprehensio n. She advo
encountered elsewhere in thi s chapter. In fact , people sometimes wonder cated more op portunities for learners to engage in verbal production (i.e,
whether the ZPD is the same as Krashen's i + 1. William Dunn and James output ) in French immersion classroo m s. Since then, she and her colleagues
Lamolf (1998) addressed this question in a review art icle, argui ng tha t it have carri ed Out extensive research to investigate the effects of o utput on
is not possible to compare the rwo concepts because they depend on very
second lan guage learning.
with an interlocutor. In Kras hen's i + 1, the input co mes fro m outside the theory, but more recent work has been motivated by sociocultural theo ry.
learner and the emphasis is on the comprehensibiliry of input that includes Using the term collaborative dialogue, Swain and Lapkin and their col
lan guage structures that are JUSt beyond th e learner's current developmental leagues have carried out a series of studies to determine how second language
level. The emphasis in ZPD is on development an d how learners co-con learners co-co nstru ct linguistic knowledge while engaging in production
srruct knowledge based on their interaction with their interlocutor or in tasks (i.e. speaking and writing) that sim ultaneo usly draw their attention to
private speech. form and meaning. As shown in Communication task B in Chapter 5, learn
ers were testing hypotheses about the correct forms to use, discussing them
Vygotskyan theory has also been compared to the interaction h ypothesis together and deciding wha t forms were bes t to exp ress their mea ning . Swain
because of the in rerlocutor's role in helping learners understand and be under (2000) considers collabo rative dialogues such as these as the co nt ext where
stood . These rwo perspect ives d iffer primarily in the emphasis they place on 'language use and lan guage learning can co-occur. It is language use mediat
the internal cogn itive processes. In the interact ion hypothesis, the emphasis ing language learning. It is cogn itive act ivity and it is social activity' (p. 97).
is on the ind ivid ual cognitive processes in the mind of the learn er. Interacti on
facilitates those cognit ive processes by givin g learners access to the in put the y Therefore th e di fference berween the socio cultu ral perspective and that of
need to activa te internal processes. In Vygotskyan theory, greater importance o ther researchers who also view interaction as important in seco nd lan guage
Recent co gn itive perspe ctives ha ve o ften in volved com p u te r sim ula tio ns or are learn ed? Ifso. ho w have your ex periences as a teacher or learner
o f the linguistic knowledge that learn ers eventually have. invo lves awa ren ess of what is be ing learned-is somewhat con troversial.
That is, it has bee n argued t hat it is also possible to learn incidentally,
Inr era crionists emphasize the role of negotiation for meani ng in conversa withou t any awareness or even an inte ntion to learn. However, second
tional intera ctions . This perspective and the soc iocu ltu ral perspective provide language lea rners certai nly do have 'aha ' moments when they sudde nly
insights into the ways in which learners can gain access to new k nowledge unde rsta nd so mething abo ut how t he target language works. Do you have
abo ut (he language when the y have suPPOrt from an interlocu to r. Some any exam ples of noticingfro m your own language learning ex periences. or
linguists challe nge the inrcracrionist position, argu ing that much of what from th os e of your st ude nt s?
learners need to know is not availab le in the in p ut, a nd so they puc gr eater
3 From the perspective of the interaction hypothesis. mo dified interaction
Bot h lingu ists and psychologists draw som e of the ir evidence from ne uro distinguished from modi fied (o r simplified) input . Can you th ink of so me
logica l research. At present, most of the research o n language representation exa mples of each?What are some of th e feat ures of mod ified interaction
in the brain an d spec ific neurological activity during lan guage processing is t hat you think are especially helpful to learn ers ?Are th e re some featu res
inconclusive. However, advances in tec hnology are rapid ly increasin g oppor that may not support learning ?Wh at are th e co ntexts in wh ich sec o nd
tunities to observe brain ac tivity mo re d irecdy. Suc h researc h will eventually language learn ers are most likely to benefit from modified interaction? Do
co nt ribut e to reinterpretations of researc h that previously could examine you thin k that simplified input is (also) important?
5
Dornyei, Z. 2009. The Psychology ofSecond LanguageAcquisition. Oxford:
Oxford University Press. OBSERVING LEARNING
This overview of the theories that have been proposed to explain second AND TEACHING IN THE
lan guage acq uisition is both comprehensive and easy to read. Dornyei pro
vides detailed trea tment of the theories th at are discussed in this chapter, SECOND LANGUAGE
focusing pa rticularly on those arising from the research in cognitive
psychology. In addition, the book introduces the work in neurobiology CLASSROOM
that provides a new level of explanation for language acquisition and use.
Swain, M., P. Kinnear, and L. Steinman. 2010. Sociocultural Theoryand
Second Language Education: An Introdu ction through Narratives. Bristol:
Multilingual Matters .
In this book the authors cover the key concepts of sociocultural th eo ry Preview
(for exam p le, mediation, zone of proximal development. private speech,
collaborarive dialogue) through the use of narratives. The narratives come In this chapter we explore different ways in which researchers have observed
from the voices of language learn ers and teachers from different educa and descri bed what goes on in second language classroo ms: Befo re we do th is,
tional COntexts. The book is of particular interest to readers motivated to let us take a moment to reflect on the differences between classroom settings
understand how sociocultural theory relates to the teaching and learning for language learning and other settings where people learn a new language
ofsecond languages. without instruction.
VanPatten, B. and]. Williams (eds.). 2007. Theories in Second Language As we saw in th e activity in Chapter 2, learning a second language in a non
Acquisition: An Introduction. Mahwah, N]: Lawrence Erlbaum instructional setting is differen r from learning in the classroom. Many believe
Associates. that learning 'on the street' is more effective . This belief may be based on the
fact th at mos t successful learners have had experience using the language
VanParten and Williams set out a list ofobservations that have arisen from
outside the classroom. What is special about th is 'natural' language learn ing?
research studies in second language acquisitio n. Then, well-known authors
Can we create the same environment in the classroom? Should we? Or are
discuss how th e theo retical framework in which they have done their own
there essential contributions that only instruction and no t na tu ral exposure
research would explain these observations. For example, there are chapters
can provide?
on Universal Grammar. sociocultural theory, skill acquisition th eo ry, pro
cessabiliry, and input processing. The chapters are brief (about 20 pages,
including discussion questions and readi ngs) and written in a style that is Natural and instructional settings
accessible to those with limit ed background in resear ch and theory. The
Natural acquisition contexts should be understood as those in which the
final chapter, by Lourdes Ortega, provides a concise overview ofthe d iffer
lear ner is exposed to the language at work or in social interactio n or, if the
ent theories and identifies some ongoing ch allenges for explaining second
learner is a ch ild , in a school situation where mos t of the other chil dren are
language acquisition.
native speakers of the targe t language and where the instruction is directed
toward native speakers rather than toward learners of the language. In such a
classroom, much of a child's learning take places in interaction with peers as
well as through instruction from the teacher.
In structure-based instructional environments , the language is taugh t to a
group of second or foreign language learners. The focus is on the lan guage
itself, rather than on the messages carried by th e language. The teacher's
goal is to see to it that students learn the vocabulary and grammatical rules
o f the target language. Some students in structure-based classes may have
124 Observing leaming and teaching in the second Lmguage classroom Observing learning and teaching in the second language classroom 125
oppo rruru nes to co nti n ue learning th e target language o utside th e class ACTIVITY C o m pare learning contexts
room; for others, the classroom is me on ly contact with that language. In The cha rt inTable 5.1 is similar to the one in Table 2.1 in Chapter 2.ln that
some cases, the learners' goal may be to pass an examination rather man to chart, we compared the profi les of first and second language learn e r s. In this
use the language for daily communicative interaction beyond the classroom. one, we compare natural and instructional contexts for second language
Co m m un icative. content-based, and task-based instructional environmen ts learning.Think about the characteristics of the four contexts represented by
also in volve learners wh ose goal is learning the language itself, but the sty le of each column, For each context, decide whether the characteristics on the left
instruction places the emphasis on interaction. co nversatio n, and language are present or absent. Mark a plus (+) in the table if the characteristic is typical
use, rather man on learning about the language. The topics mat are discussed of that context. Mark a minus (-) if it is something you usually do not find
in co m m un icative and task-based instructional environments are o fte n of in that context.Write '?' if you are not sure . Note that the 'Co mmunicative
general interest to me learner, for example, how to obtain a driver's license. instruction' column has been subdivided into teacher-student and student
In content-based language teaching (C BLT), the foc~ of a lesson is usually student interaction .What happens when learners talk to each other? Is that
on the subject matter, such as history or marhemarics, which students are different from what happens in teacher-student interaction?
learning through me medium of the secon d language. In thes e classes, the
Characteristics Natural Structure- Communicative
focus may occasionally be on the language itself, but the emphasis is on using
acquisition based instruction
the language rathe r than talking about it. The language mat teachers use for instruction
teach ing is not selected so lely for the purpose of teaching a specific feature
of the langu age, bu t also to make sure learn ers have the lan guage they need Teacher- Student
to interact in a variety of contexts. Students' success in these courses is often
student student
meas ured in terms of thei r ab ility to 'get th ings do ne' in the seco nd lan guage, Learning one
rather than on their accuracy in using certain grammatical features. thing at a t ime
Frequent
In naturalacquisition settings feedback on
When people learn languages at work. in social interactions, or on the play errors
gro un d, their experiences are often quite differ ent from those of learners Ample time for
in classrooms. C o m p lete Table 5.1 on the nex t page. As you look at m e learni ng
pattern of + and - signs you have placed in th e cha rt, you will probably find
High ratio of
it matches the descr iption s below.
native speakers
o Language is not presented step by st ep . Th e learner is exposed to a wid e to learners
variety ofvocabulary and str uctu res. Variety of
• Learners' errors are rarely corrected. If th eir interlocutors can under language and
stand what they are saying, they do not remark on the correctness of the discourse types
learners' speech. They would probably feel it was rude to do so.
Pressure to
• The learner is surrounded by the language for many hours each day. speak
Sometimes the language is addressed to the learner; sometimes it is
simply overheard. Access to
Q The learner usually encounters a number of different people who use modified input
the target language proficiently. Photoccpiable © Oxford Univers icy Press
• Learners observe or participate in many different types of language
events: briefgreetings, co m mercial transactions, exchanges of informa Tab le 5. I Contexts (or language learning
tion, arguments, instruction at schoo l and in workplace interactions.
126 Ob serving lea rning and teaching in the second language classroom Observ ing learning and teaching in the second language classroom 127
• Older children and adulrs may also enco unre r rhe wrirren language in
the use of video and web -based materials .
• Learners must often use their limited second language ability to respond
f!1e;h- i~":!:1~f'< ~~'e-oJI _
to questions or to get information. In these situations, the emphasis is The a...x..1.'t1iJ ver-b, ""n.:c.."
on getting meaning across clearly, and more proficient speakers tend to "Dr-.~ t:4pPetVS after -t~e
be tolerant of errors that do no t In terfere with meaning. subjeL-t.., ..,...,st ~ove 'tP k~t:e
• Modified in put is available in many o ne-to-one conversations. In sit u i'l c:na.-L po~t"on .. .
ations where many na tive speakers are involved in th e conversation,
however, lear ners may have d ifficulty getting access to language the y
can understand.
~~>'4--3
<, r\~
~J~
In structure-based instructional settings
The events and act ivities that are typical of structure-based instruction differ
~~
,~?-
from th ose encounrered in na tural acq uisi tion set tings. In grammar tran sla
rio n approaches, there is considerable use of readi ng and writing, as learn ers
translate text s from one language to an o th er, and grammar rul es are taugh t
explicitly. In audio lingual ap proaches there is lit tle use o f translatio n, and
learners are expected to learn mainly through repetition and habit formation, Language classrooms are not all alike. The conditions for lear ning diffe r
although they may be asked to figu re out the grammar rules for the senrences in terms of the physical environment, the age and mo tivation of the Stu
they have memorized . dents, the amount o f time available for learning, and many oth er variables.
Classroo ms also differ in terms of the prin ciples that guide teachers in thei r
• Linguistic items are presented and practised in isola tion, one item at
language teaching methods and techniques. D esigners of commun icative
a time, in a sequence fro m what teachers or textbook wri ters bel ieve is
language teac hing programmes have sought to replace so me of th e charac
'sim ple' to that which is 'co mplex' .
teristics of st ructure-based instruction with those more typical of natural
• Errors are frequently corrected. Accuracy tends to be given priority over
acquisition con texts.
meaningful interaction.
• Learning is often limited to a few hours a week.
• In situatio ns of foreign lan guage learning th e teac her is often the only In communicative instructional settings
native or proficient speak er the student comes in contact with. In com m un icative and co ntent-based instructio n, the emphasis is on the
• Students experience a lim ited range of language discourse types . Th e communication of meaning, both between teacher and students and among
most typical of these is the Initiation/Response/Evaluation (IRE) the sru dents themselves in group- or pair- work. Grammatical forms are
exchange where the reacher asks a question, a student answers, an d the foc used on only in order to clarify meaning. The assumption is that, in focus
teacher evaluates the response. The written language studen ts encoun ing on mean in g, learners will acquire the language in a way that is similar to
ter is selec ted primarily to provide practice with specific grammatical natural acquisition.
feat ures rather than for irs content,
• Students often feel pressure to speak or write the second language and • Input is sim plified and made comprehensible by the use of contextual
to do so correctly from the very beginning.
cues, props, and gestu res, rather than th ro ugh structural grading.
• Teachers may use the lear ners' native language to give instructions or Students provide each o ther with simplified an d sometimes erroneous
for classroom management. When they use the target language, th ey input.
tend to modify their language in order to ensure comprehension and • There is a limited amount of error co rrec tion on the parr of the teacher,
compliance . and meaning is emphasized over form . Srudents tend no t to over tly
correct each other's errors w hen they are engaged in co m munica
tive practice. Because the focus is on meaning, however, req uests for
128 Observing teaming and teaching in the second ftmguage classroom
Obser ving teaming and teachi ng in the second lmgut.lge classroom 129
clarifica tio n ma y se rve as implicit feedback. N ego tia ting for meaning loo k a t classroom ethnography, an ap proac h that requires th e o bserver to
may help students see the need co say something in a different way. describe what happens in the classroom , looking for patterns and relation
• Learners usually have only limited time for learning. In a typical teacher ships. but trying not to limit the observation to any predetermined cat egories
fronted classroom with 25-30 students, individual students get very or expectations.
little o p po rtu nity CO produce language in a GO-minute class, and when
th ey do, it is usually in the form of a sho rt response co a teacher's ques
tion. When stud ents work in pairs or groups, they have opportunities Observation schemes
co produce and respond CO a greater amount and variety of language. Many different observation schemes ha ve been developed for use in seco nd
Sometimes, however, subject-matter courses taught through the second language classrooms. They differ in several respects, includ ing the number
language can add time fo r language learning. A good example of this of categories they contain, whether they focu s o n qualitative or quanti
is in immersion programmes where mos t or all the subject matter is tative descriptions, and whethe r they are used throughout a lesson or on
taught to a group of students who are all second language learners. selected samples of classroom interaction . The schemes also differ in rela
• As in structure-based instruction, it is usually only the teacher who is tion to whether they are used by o bservers in ' real time' while they are in
a proficient speaker. Learners have considerable exposure to the inter the classroom, o r used later outside the classroom to analyse audio or video
language of other learners, particularly in student-student interaction. recordings or transcripts of such recordings.
This naturally contains errors that would not be heard in an environ
m ent where the interlocu to rs are nati ve speake rs, but it provide s m an y O ne example of a scheme developed speci fically fo r seco nd language class
mo re opportu nities for stu de nts to use th e target la ng uage tha n is th e roo ms is th e Com m u nicative O rien ta tio n o f Language Teac hi ng (C O LT)
case in most teach er-fro nt ed activities. Observat ion Sc he me descri bed by Nina Spada an d M aria Fro hlich ( 199 5l.
• A variety o f discou rse ty pes m ay be int rod uced t hrough stories , pee r CO LT is di vided into two parts. Parr A d esc rib es teaching practices in terms
a nd gro up-work. the use of 'au the ntic' m aterials such as newspapers and of co nte nt. focu s. and o rganization of ac tivity types . 'X'he n using Parr A ,
television broadcasts . Text m aterials m ay include both those modified th e o bserve r ca n record, for exa m ple, whethe r the pedagogical ac tivities
for seco nd language learners and those intended for native speakers. In a re teach er- or learner-centred, whether the focus is o n language fo rm o r
the latter case, teachers use instructional st rategies to help learners get meaning, and whether there at e o ppo rt un ities for studen ts co choose the
th e meaning, even if they do not know all the words and structures. In topics for discussion. Parr B describes specific aspects of the language pro
student-stu dent interaction, learners may practise a range of sociolin duced by teachers an d stud en ts, for example, how much (o r how little)
gu istic and functional features oflanguage through role-play. language students produce, whether their language production is restricted
• There is little pressure to perform at high levels of accuracy, and there is in any way, the kinds of questions teachers ask. and wh ether and how teach
o ften a greater emphas is on co m preh ensio n than on production , espe ers respond to learners' errors.
cially in the early stages oflearning. The COLT scheme and others like it have been used primarily in classroom
• Modified input: is a defining feature of this approach to instruction. The research that is intended to look at how differences in teaching practices are
teacher makes every effort to speak to students in a level of language related co differences in second language learning. Observation schemes have
they can understand. Ifstudents speak the same first language, they may also been used in the training of new teachers and in the professional devel
have little difficulty in understanding each other. If they come from dif opment of experienced ones.
ferent language backgrounds, they may modify their language as they
Below is an activity in which you are asked co use a set of pre-determined
seek to communicate successfully.
catego ries similar to those used in the COLT scheme co characterize the
General descriptions of classroom instruction such as those above cannot nature of interaction between teachers and students and between students
capture the individual characteristics of particular classrooms . For this and students.
reason , researchers have developed a number of ways to study classroom
learning and teaching. We will discuss two approaches to classroom resea rch Classroom comparisons: Teacher-student interactions
in this chapter. We will look first at observation schemes, in wh ich research Excerpts from four transcripts of second language classroom interaction are
ers anticipate the occurrence of particular events and behaviours and make given in th is and the following section. The first twO present teacher-student
note o f them within pre-planned frameworks or checklists. Then we will interaction. The transcripts come from classrooms that differ in their approach
Observing learn ing and teachi ng in the second language classroom O bserving learn ing and teaching in the second language classroom 131
130
co secon d lan guage teaching; one of them represents structure-based instruc Unit 4 . Don't take your bo oks yet, don't take yo ur books. In 1,2 , 3
tion ; the other, a communicative approach. Stru cture-based approaches we worked in what ten se? What tense did we work on? OK?
emphasize language form throu gh either meralinguistic instruction (for s Past.
example, grammar translation) or pa ttern practice (for example, audiolingual) . T In the past-What auxiliary in the past?
s Did.
Wi th each tran script , th ere is a char t where you can indicate whether certain T D id (writes on board ' 1- 2- 3 Past'). Unit 4, Unit 4, we're goi ng co
th ings are happening in the int eractio n, fro m the point of view of the teac her work in the prese nt, present p rogressive, p resent conti nuous- O K?
and that of the stu dents. Before yo u begin readi ng the tran scrip ts, study th e
You do n't kn ow what it is?
followi ng interpretatio ns of th e categorie s used in the charts:
s Yes
1 Errors:Are there errors in the lan guage of either the teacher or the students? T Yes? What is it?
2 Feedback on errors: When stu dents make errors, do they receive feedback? s Little bit.
From who m? T A lit tle bit.
3 Gen uine q uestio ns: Do teachers and students ask q uestio ns to which they s
don't know the answer in advance? T Eh?
4 D isplay questions: Do teachers ask questions that they kn ow the answers s Uh, presen t continuo us
to so that learners can disp lay the ir knowledge of the lan guage (or lack T Present co nti nuous? What's that ?
of it)? s e-n-g
5 Negotiation fo r meaning: Do the teachers and students work to u nder T i-n-g
stan d what th e other speakers are saying? What efforts are made by the s Yes.
reach er? By the students? T What does that mean, present contin uo us?You do n't know? OK,
6 Meralinguis tic com m en ts: D o th e teachers an d students talk abo u t lan fine. What are you doing, Paul?
guage, in addition to using it to transmit infor mation? s Rien [nothing].
T N othing?
In the following excerpts, T rep resents the teacher; 5 rep resents a student.
s Rien-nothing.
(The first rwo examples come fro m unp ub lished data co llected by P. M .
T You're not doing anything? You're doing something!
Lighrbown, N . Spada, and B. Barkman.)
s Not doing anything.
T You're doing something!
Classro o m A: A structure-based appro ach
T H e is closing the do or (w rites ' H e's closing the door ' on the board). T Every day? Ah! Does n't your b ro-(ina ud ible) h is bicycle? Could his
What are yo u doing, Mario? brother lend his bicycle? Uh, yo ur bro ther doesn't have a bicycle ?
Classroom B: A communicative approach T Ah, well. Talk to your mom and dad about it. Maybe negotiate a new
(Student s in thi s class are IO-year-old French speakers. In this activity, they
are telling their teacher and their classmates what 'b ugs' th em. They have
s (inaudi ble)
T He has a new bicycle. But his brother needs a new one too.
written 'what bugs them' on a card or paper that they hold whil e speaking.)
s Yes!
Errors
T Martin, who has a new bicycle? You o r your brother?
s (inaudible)
Metalinguistic comments
s March 23.
5 Yeah !
s Stings me. T Fourteen. Well, why don't yo u tell your br other th at when he takes
T Do you get stu ng often? Does that happen oft en ? Th e bee stinging your bike you will take his bike? And he may have more scratches
many times? than he figures for. OK?
s Yeah.
T Often? (Teacher turns to students who aren't paying attention) OK. Characteristics of input and interaction
Sand ra and Benoit, you may begin working on a resear ch project, C om pare the rwo charts yo u hav e complet ed so far. What kinds of seco nd
hey? (Teacher turns her attention back to ' \'\'hat bugs me ') language input and opportunities for interaction are available to learners
s It bugs me (inaudible) and my sister pUt on my clothes. in each of the en vironments th at thes e tr ans cripts exemplify? HQw are the y
T Ab! She borrows your clothes? When yo u're older, you may
d ifferent?
app reciate it becau se yo u can switch clothes, ma yb e. (Turn s to chec k
ano ther student's wr itten work) Melan ie, this is yo urs, I will check Classroom A
OK. It 's good. I Errors: Very few on the part of the teacher, However her speech does have
s It bugs me when I'm sick and my brother doesn't help me-my-my som e peculiar characteristics typical of this type of teaching, for example,
brother, 'cause he-me-. the questions in statement form-often asked with dramatic rising into
T 0 K. You know-when (inaud ible) sick, you're sick at home in bed nation (for example, 'Yo u don't know what it is?'). Students don't make coo
and you say, oh , to your brother or your sister: 'W o uld you please get many errors because they say very little and what they say is usually limited
me a drink of water?' -'Ah! D rop dead! ' you know, 'Go play in the by the lesson.
traffic!' You know, it's nor very nice. Martin! 2 Feedback on errors: Yes, w henever students do make errors , the teacher
s It bug me to have- reacts.
T It bugs me. It bugzz me. 3 Genuine questions: Yes, a few, but the y are almost always related to class
s It bugs me when my brother takes my bicycle. Every day. room management. No questions from the students.
134 Observing team ing and teaching in the second language classroom Observing team ing and teaching in the second language classroom 135
4 Display questions: Yes , almost all of the teacher's questions are o f this type . are do ing. Rather th e teacher is highlighti ng th eir ability co say what they
Interestingly, however, the students sometimes interpret display ques are do ing, using th e correct verb form. There is a primary focus on correct
tions as gen uine questions (T : What are you doing, Paul? S: Nothing.). gram mar, displ ay questions, and error co rrection in the transcrip t from
The teacher wants stu den ts co produce a sentence-any sentence-in C lassroom A. In th e transcript from Classroom B, the focus is on mean ing,
the 'prese nt continuous ' but the student worries th at he's about co get in conversational interaction, and gen uine questio ns, although there are som e
tro uble and asserts that he is do ing 'nothing'. This is a good exam ple of brief references co grammatical accuracy when th e teacher feels it is necessary.
how the teache r's p ragmatic intent can be misinterpreted by the student,
and o f how stro ngly, even in this setting, students seek co find genuine Classroom compa risons: Student-student interactions
meaning in language.
S Nego tiation for meaning: Very little, learners have no need co paraphrase Th is sectio n presents so me student-st udent interactions. The tran script s are
based on the interactions between seco nd lan guage learners engaged in dif
or request clarifications, and no opportunity co determine th e direction
of th e d iscour se; the teacher is focused on ly on th e formal aspects o f the ferent communicative tasks.
learners' language. All the effort goes into getting students co p roduce a As in th e p revious section, the re is a ch art with each transcrip t where you can
sentence with the present continuous form of the verb. indicate whether certain things are happening in the interaction.
6 Meralinguisric com me nts: Yes, this is how the teacher begins the lesson
and lets the stu dents kn0:W what really matters! Communication task A: Picture description
The foll ow ing tran script is of twO girls aged 11- 12 years, both ESL learn
Classroom B ers in the ir first year of learning English in Australia. The first learner (S 1)
1 Errors: Yes, students make errors. And even the teacher says so me odd is from Hong Kong; the seco nd (S2) is from Somalia. They are engaged in
thi ngs sometimes. H er speech also contains in complete sentences, sim pli a task wh ere S 1 is describing a picture for S2 co d raw. They are sitting at a
fied ways of speaking, and an in formal speech style . table, separated by a sm all bar rier, so that they can see eac h other's faces and
2 Feedback on errors: Yes, sometimes the teacher repeats wha t the student hands (when they gestu re), bue no t each other's pic ture. The picture S 1 is
has said with the correct form (fo r example , 'he bugzz me '--emphasizing drawing is a black o u tline containing stic k figures-a boy flying a ki te an d
the third person sin gular ending). However, this correction is no t consist a girl holding his hand. Th e stick figu res are standing on some grass near
ent or intrusive as the focus is pri marily on leering students exp ress th eir a tree. Sq uar e brackets indicate non-target pronunciation. (This tran script
meanings. comes from unpublished data collected by Alison Mackey, Rho nda Oliver,
3 Genuine questions: Yes, alm ost all of the teach er's q uesti ons are foc used on and Jennifer Leeman.)
getting information from the students. Th e students are no t asking ques
tions in this exchange. However, they do sometimes intervene co change Student I Student 2
the direction of the conversation. Errors
4 Dis play questions: N o , because there is a focu s on meaning rather than on
Feedback on errors
accuracy in grammatical form .
S N ego tiatio n for meaning: Yes, from the teacher's side, especially in th e Ge nuine questions
long exchange abou t who has a bicycle! Display questions
6 Meralinguisrlc comments: No. Even though the te-acher clearly hopes co
get stu d en ts co use the third person ending, she does no t say so in these Ne go tiat ion for meaning
words. Meta linguist ic co mments
You no do ubt no ticed how strikingly different these two transcripts are ,
Phococopiable © Oxford University Press
even though the activities in both are teacher-centred. In th e tra nscript fro m
Classroom A, th e focus is on for m (i.e. grammar) an d in C lassroo m B, it is on S I And o-on th e right, there is a [tree]. It's a- a, the ki- , the kite is up .
meaning. In C lassroom A, th e only purpose of the interaction is to practise (Po ints up in the air) This is the kite. (Po in ts up again ) Thi s is the
the present co ntinuo us. Although the teacher uses real classroom events and kite . (Points yet again ) An d the [tree] is u p there.
some humour co accomplish thi s, there is no real interest in what st udents sz Three bird?
136 Obst!Tving learning and teachmg in the second language classroom Observing learning and teaching in rhe second language classroom lr
51 Huh ?
52 Is a three bird ?
Dara N ina
51 Huh?
Errors
5 2 Up , up-up me kite?
Feedback on errors
s 1 Yeah , m e kite is u-, the kite is up and me [tree ] is down. (Po inrs
S 1 Yes.
NElle ssse [She ssse]
5 1 It's-what?
so uvien r, no . [She remembers . . . ahh , she remembers She
5 2 It's lo ng an d [Iittle]?
remem bers . . . She remembers, no]
o C ho rale. [C hoir]
N Tout it co up ell« souu ient q u'elle . .. do it aller it la cho rale [All of a
C o mmunica t ion task B: Jigsaw
sudden she rem em bers that she has ro go to choir]
The follow ing transcript is of rwo students in a Gra de 7 F re ~ch im mersio n o [very softly] elle se so uvie nt ... non . [She remembers . . . no]
classroom. They are engage d in a jigsaw act ivity based o n a series of eigh t o Alo rs, elle [So, she]
pictures telling the Story o f a yo u ng girl bein g awakene d by her alar m clock N non, wait. Tour a coup elle .. . se souvienr? [no , wait .. . all of a
early in the morning. One stude n r has p ictures 1, 3,5 . and 7, and the oth er sudden she . . . rem embers?]
srude nr has pictures 2, 4, 6 , and 8 . Th ey take rums relling m e Story portrayed D Je pen se pas qu e c'esr se souvienr. [I don't th ink it's rem ember]
by me picrures and men th ey display all the pi ctures in sequence and write N oh, souvienr . , . so uvienr. [oh rem em ber . . . rem em ber]
the story th ey have JUSt rold . Telling me srory requires th e use ora number of o Elie souuient qu 'elle a Ie cho rale. [She rem emb ers th at she has choir]
reflexive verbs in Fren ch . In m e third person , th e form se is placed between N Qu'elle doit se prep arer. [ma t she has to get read y]
the subject and the finite verb. Thus, elle se ieve (she gets up ) and ellesesouuient D O ui. [Yes]
(she rem embers) are correct. Th e learn ers are called Oara (D) and Nina (N ). N pour le cho rale i . . no n, to u t it co up elie souuient qu'il ya un e pr atique
In the transc rip t incorrect uses of the reflexive verbs are in itali cs; o the r errors de cho rale. [for ch o ir . . . no . all of a sudden she rem embers that
are not marked . (Th e data are from Swain and Lapkin 2002.) th ere's a cho ir p ractice]
138 O bserving lea rning and teaching in the second language classroom Observing learning and teaching in the second language classroom 139
Characteristics ofinp u t and interaction 5 Negotiation for meaning : At this po int in the interactio n, th e stu dents
Compare the two ch ar ts yo u have completed . As before, wha t kinds ofsecond have agreed o n the content of the story. Thus, there is mo re negotiation of
language input and oppo rt uni ties for interacti on are available co learners in form, that is, more discussion of whe ther the y are using the co rrect forms
each of the en vironments that these tra nscripts exemplify? How are they d if to say what th ey've agreed th ey wa nt to say.
ferent from eac h other and the teacher-student interactions you looked at 6 Meralinguisric co mments: Although the y ar e not using words such as
pr evio usly? 'verb' or 'pro noun', the Stude nts are talki n g about language as th ey focus
on try ing to find the right form .
Com m unication task A
1 Errors: There are m any errors in the speech of both learners. This includes These two transcripts of Student- stud ent intera ction are very d ifferent from
grammatical and pro n un ciatio n errors. These erro rs are p resent in several eac h other. In the first commun ica tion task, the children are focused exclu
b reakdowns in th e learners' conversatio n. sivelyon meaning and on try ing co understand each othe r in order to complete
2 Feedback on errors: There is no error correction in terms of form as t he th e in fo rm atio n -gap activity. They are constantly using co m prehension and
learners struggle co understand each other's meaning. The difficulty they clarification requests as they negotiate fo r meaning in this task. In the second
are hav ing in communication may serve as a kind of implicit feedback. student-student transcript, however, the learners are focused on both form
That is, the fact that the in rerlocuto r does not understand may signal tha t and meani ng. While recons tru cting the sto ry, they make several explicit
there is something wrong w ith what they ha ve said . sta tem ents abou t whether th ey are using th e co rrect form of th e reflexive verb
3 G enu ine questions: Yes, the re are m any genuine questi ons . Naturally, S2 se souvenir an d co ntinually question the grammatical accuracy of their use of
asks m ost of these questio ns beca use she nee ds co get the informatio n fro m rh is for m as [hey conrinue co disc uss the co n te nt of the sto ry,
S 1 in or der to draw the p icture. S 1 also asks some gen ui ne questions and
In the activities in the preceding pages, we have described an d compared
the se are almost always to ask for clarification.
teacher- studen t and student-student interaction in terms ofsix observation
4 Display questions: No, there are no display questi ons because they
catego ries. Some observation sche mes use many more categories, cover ing a
are engaged in a real communication-gap exchange. S2 cannot see th e
broad range of inst ructional practices and procedures. O thers foc us o n one
picture that S 1 is describ ing. Therefore all the questio ns asked are gen uine
specific feature of classroom instruction and imeractio n. In .the followi ng
questions.
sectio ns, we review classroom resea rch in which one particular feature of
5 Negotiation for meanin g: Yes, indeed! Both lear ners are trying hard to
instr uctio n has been examined. Five studies examine co rrective feedback,
unders tan d each other, even th ough the y often fail to do so . This involves
fo ur investigate teach ers' use of questions and o ne describes th e amount an d
m any co m prehensio n questions and clarifica tion req uests , as we ll as reperl
d istr ibu tion of time for L2 learning.
tions of each other's utterances, often with emphasis , trying co unde rstan d
what the o ther learner has JUSt said.
6 M eral inguisric co m m en ts: No ne. Correctivefeedback in the classroom
Study 1: Recasts in content-based classrooms
Communication task B Ro y Lyster and Leila Ranta' (1997) de veloped an observation schem e which
1 Errors: Bo th lear ners make several grammatical errors, most no tably th e
describes different types of corrective feedback teachers give on errors and
repea ted failu re to produce the reflexive form of th e verb se souvenir.
also examines student uptake-an indication that the srudent has not iced
2 Feedback on errors: There is no act ual error co rrecti on provided. Neither
the feedback. . This scheme was devel oped in French immersion classroo ms
learner is really sure whar the correct form is. Instead, there is rneralinguis
w here seco nd language srud en rs learn the target language via subject-matter
tic reflection and discussion as they try to figure out whether the y are using ins tru ct ion (i.e. content-based language teach in g). It has also been used co
the correct form of the verb se souvenir.
desc ribe feedback in other types of seco n d language instruction.
3 Genuine questions: The questio ns tha t are asked are genuine. The content
is language form, but the students are genuinely sharing information They develo ped th eir sch eme by observing th e different types of co rrective
about how to complete the task. feedback provided during interaction in four French im m ersio n classrooms
4 D isp lay q uestio ns: There are no d isp lay q uestions. The students are actively wi th 9-II -year-old stud ents. They began their observations by usin g a co m
collaborating to reconstruct the Sto ry and are aski ng genuine q uestions of bina tio n o f some catego ries fro m Pan B of the C O LT scheme an d other
each other. categori es from models that had examined feedback in both first and seco nd
140 Obsn"vmg learning and teaching in the second language classroom Obseruing learning and tea.·hing in sbe second language classroom 14 1
language learning. They adjusted so me of the categories [0 fit their data, th e nature o f the erro r but attempt [0 elici t the information from the stu de nt
and they also develo ped additional categories. This resul ted in th e id en ri (fo r example, 'Is it femi nine? ') .
ficat ion of six corrective feedback types, defined below. The defini tions are
s We look at th e pe op le yesterday.
tak en fro m Lyster and Ranta (1997 : pp . 46-8) . The examples come fro m
T 'W'hat's the ending we put o n verbs when we talk about the past?
1O-ll -year -o ld students in ESL classes that we have observed.
s e-d
Explicit correction refers [0 th e explicit provision of the correct form . As
Elicitation refers ro at leas t three techniques that teachers use to directly e1icit
the teacher provides the correct form, he or she clearly ind icates that
th e correct form from the students . First, teachers elici t completion of their
what th e student had said was incorrect (fo r example, ' O h, you mean . .. ',
own utterance (fo r exam ple, 'It's a .. .') . Second, teachers use questions to
' Yo u sh o uld say .. . ') .
elicit correct forms (fo r example, ... ' H ow do we say x in English? ') . Third,
s The dog run fasdy. teachers occasionally ask st uden ts to reformulate their utterance. '
T ' Fasrly' doesn't exist. ' Fas t' does not take -Iy. That's why I picked
'q u ickly'. s My father cleans the plate.
T Excuse me, h e cleans the - -?
Recasts involve the teacher's reformulation of all or part of a student's utter s Plates?
ance, minus the error. Recasts are generally implicit in that they are not
Repetition refers to the teacher 's repetition of the student's erroneous utter
introduced by ' Yo u mean' , 'Use thi s word', or 'You sho uld say.'
ance. I n mos t cases, teach ers ad just th eir into na tion so as to hi ghl ight th e
SI 'W'hy yo u do n't like Marc?
erro r.
T \XThy don 't yo u like Marc?
includes phrases such as 'Pardo n me ' It may also include a repetition of T We is? But it 's two people, righ t? You see yo ur mistake? You see th e
th e error as in ' \xrha t do you mean by ?' erro r? 'W'hen it 's plural it's 'we are '.
conclude that 'no t all content teaching is necessarily good lan guage teaching' to notice the co rrective function of recasts. In the more meaning-oriented
(p. 68). The challenges of content-based language teaching will be discussed Frenc h immersion classes, howe ver, recasts were less likely to signal to the
further in Chapter 6. learner that the teacher was responding to a language err or. Thus is likely that
learners assumed tha t the teac hers' recast was simply a con firmation of what
Since Lyster and Ranta reported their findings, many more observation
they had said.
most frequentl y occurring type of feedback and that they appear to go unno the counterbalance hypothesis . Acco rdi ng to this hypo th esis, feedback is
ticed by learn ers. However, others report that learners do notice recasts in the mo re likely to be noticed ifl ear ners are oriented in a direction that is op pos ite
classroom. Below, [WO studies are described in which learners were observed to what th ey have become accustomed to in their instructional environment.
to no tice and to respond to recasts provided by their teachers. One example of th is would be that lear ners who receive L2 instruction that
is foc used on m ean ing/ coment need feedback tha t directs their attention to
In a study with adult foreign language learn ers of Japanese, Amy Ohra
(2000) exam ined the oral language that learn ers addressed co themselves Study 4: Correctivefeedback in context
during classroo m activ ities. She was ab le ro obtain this private speec h by Rho nda O liver an d Aliso n Mackey (200 3) carried out a descriptive study
attaching microphones to individual students during classroom interaction of an Australian pri mary ESL classroom with 6-12-year-olds. They inves
that focused on grammar and rneralinguisric instr uction. In this co ntext, tigated whether teachers' provision and lear ners' use o f corrective feedback
Ohta d iscovered that learners noticed recasts when the y were provided by differed depending on varying contexts for interactio n in a lesson . The y iden
the instructor. Furthermore, learners were more likely to reac t to a recast rified fo u r contexts in which teachers and learners interacted:
with private speech when it was directed ro an o ther learner or to the whole 1 content exchanges: the teac he r imparted knowledge or asked questions
class rather th an when the recast was di rected to the ir own errors. On the about the conten t of the cu rriculum
basis of these find ings, she concluded that recasts do get noticed in classroom 2 man agement exchanges: the teacher talked about the organization of the
interaction even if they do not lead to uptake from the student who originally lesso n an d appropriate classroom behaviour
produced the error.
3 communication exchanges: the emphasis was on studen ts using Englis h in
Study 3: Recasts in different instructional settings meaningful ways, and
Roy Lyster and Hirohide Mori (2006) compared learners' immedi 4 exp licit lang uage -focused exchanges: the em phasis was on grammar and
ate responses to corrective feedback in French and Japanese im m ersion the use of rneralinguistic term inology.
classrooms. They found that the teachers in both contexts used correc
Oliver and Mackey found that learners produced significantly more errors in
rive feedb ack in similar ways. However, the effects of recast s on learners'
the com m unicatio n exchanges. Thus opportunities for feedback were grea t
uptake were differe n t. In the Japanese immersion classes, learners freque ntly
est in this co ntext, The researchers found that feedback was provided in all
repaired their utterances after receiving recasts whereas learners in the French
instructional contexts but that it was most frequent in th e explicit language
immersion classes rarely did. Instead, the greatest proportion of repa ir came
focused exchanges, followed by conten t, comm un icatio n, and manageme m .
after prompts, th at is, the feedback types that indicated to students that a
When they exam ined how learners reacted to the corrective feedback, they
correction was needed an d tha t encouraged them to self-correct .
foun d th at learners mod ified their omput most often within exp licit language
In [heir efforts to understand these differences Lyster and Mori carried om focuse d exchanges, only some of the time in content and communication
a detailed analysis of th e instructional characteristics in these classes. Using exchanges, and never in management exchanges. Interestingly, the types of
the COLT observation scheme described above to capture differences in the corrective feedback also varied across co ntexts: recasts were used at a consist
pedagogical practices, th ey discovered that there was an analytic orientation ently high rate in managemem, com municatio n, and con ten r exchanges,
in the Japanese classrooms leading the teachers and learners to focus their bur less so in explicit language-focused exchanges; explicit corrective feed
atte ntion on language form and accu racy. Th e orient atio n in the French back was rarely pr ovided d uring co nte nt, management, and comm un icatio n
immersion classes was mo re experienti al with a greater focus on content exchanges, and freq uen tly d u ring expli cit language-focused contexts.
and communication o f messages. Lyster and M ori argued tha t because the
Japanese learners' attentio n was regularly drawn to form, they were primed
Observing IMming and teaching in the second language classroom 14"5
144 Observing learning and reaching in the second language classroom
O liver and M ackey's st udy em phasizes how differ ences in the instruc feedback every time they made an article error while they retold the story,
tional context affect teachers' feedback and learners' response to it. It is also Learners wh o wro te the sto ry received the ir narr ati ves with co rrectio ns for
important to keep in min d that different corrective feedback types can be article errors twO days later. To determ ine whethe r learners improved thei r
ab ility to use ar ticles co rrectly, their knowledge of articles was tested before
int erp reted differen tly dependin g on how they are del ivered. Recasts are a
case in poim. In a study of co rrective feedback in four different instruc the instructional treatment, immediatel y after th e treatment, and again 3-4
tio nal co nt exts. Younghee Sheen (2006) observed many co n trasting type s of wee ks later. The tests incl ud ed a spee de d di ctati on , a wri tte n narr ati ve, and an
erro r-co rrectio n task . All groups, except fo r the one th at received o ral recasts,
recasts , including recasts that were declarative versus interrogative. reduced
versus non-reduced, single words or short ph rases versus long phrases or significan tly outperformed the control gro up o n all im med iate and dela yed
clauses. Recasts can also differ according to whether they are delivered with post-testS. Sheen lnterprers these findings as evidence that the medium (o ral
or without stress or emphasis. In a study of adult learners of English , Shawn versus written) in w h ich co rrective feedback is provided is less important
Loewen and Jenefer Philp (2006) foun d that recasts containing 'prosod ic ' than the expli citness of the co rrective feedback (recasts versus meralinguistic
stress were thirteen times more likely to result in successful uptake' (p, 547), feedback).
that is, uptake in whi ch the student produced the co rrected form . However,
these interactions were not associated with improved performance on a sub Questions in the classroom
seq uen t test. Teac hers' qu estioning behavio ur has been the focu s of a good deal of research
Othe r facto rs that may affect learners' reacti ons to different types offeedback in second lan gu age classrooms. Q uest io ns are fund am ental in engaging st u
include age and learn in g goal s. For exa mp le, adults may be more likely to dents in interaction and in exp lor ing how much they understand. Two types
int erpret recasts as feedbac k on langu age form. particularly if a high level of or questions that have been extensively examined are referred ro as 'disp lay'
acc uracy in th e: seco nd o r fore ign lang uage is o ne of th eir goa ls. questio ns (to wh ich the teac her already knows the answe r) and 'ge nui ne'
or ' referent ial' questions (to whic h the teacher may not know the answer).
In this chap ter we have exam ined the role of cor rective feedbac k prim ar ily
The role they play in classroom interactio n has been exam ined in a num ber
in terms of learners' oral producti on. Most of th is research has been co n
of stu dies.
cerne d with the effects of correc tive feed back in relation to learners' linguistic
growth an d mo re spec ifically, learn ers' grammatical de velopment. There is Study 6: Teachers' questions in ESL classrooms
also an extensive body of research that has investigated the role of corrective Michael Long and Charl en e Saro (1983) examined the forms an d fun ctio ns
feed bac k on learners' written production. This research has been primarily o f questions asked by teachers in ESL classrooms and com pared th em with
co ncern ed with whether corrective feedback can help learners improve their ques tio ning behaviours observed o utside the classro om berween nati ve and
writing pe rfo rmance. Less attention has been given to whether and h ow non-native speake rs. Th ey were part icul arly interest ed in differ en ces between
written corrective feedback co nt ribu tes to learners' linguistic development. the quantity of 'd isp lay' and ' info rmatio n' ('gen u ine' or 'referent ial') ques
O ne recent study which has done th at is Younghee Sheen's research described tio ns. Audio. reco rdings made of the int eractions between reachers and
in Study 5 below. stu dent s in six adult ESL classes revealed th at teach ers asked more d ispla y
qu estions th an informatio n quest ions. In the native speaker/non-nati ve
Study 5: Oral and written correctivefeedback speaker con versati ons o utside rhe classroo m, referen tial questions were mor e
Younghee Sheen (20 10) compared the effects of two typ es oforal and written
frequent than di spla y questions . The researchers co ncluded that teacher
co rrective feed back on ad ult ESL learners' accurate use of articles. The oral
learner interaction is a 'grea tly distorted version o f its equivalent in the real
co rrective feedback consisted of recasts or metal inguistic information; par
wo rld' (p. 28 4), and they argued that the in reractional struc tu re ofclassroom
allel written corrective type s were d irect co rrection or direct rneralinguis ric
co nversatio n sho uld be ch anged .
feedback. Five gro ups participated in the study, of which one was a control
gro up. The other groups each received one of the fo llowing: 1) oral recasts, Even tho ugh language teaching methods have chan ged since th e Lon g an d
2) oral m eralinguisric feedback, 3) written dire ct co rrectio n. and 4) written Sa ro study, o the r classroom stud ies on teachers' questioning behaviour have
d irect meralingu isric feedback. also reported disproportionately higher numbers o f display questions. In the
All groups participated in two 30-minute activities in which they were asked
use fewer d isplay questions becau se they are th ough t to lead to short, sim ple
[Q read a Sto ry and then retell it either in the written or the oral mode. Learners
responses th at require linle cognitive effort on the pa rt of the learner. Instead,
in the oral corrective feed back group received either recasts or metalinguistic
146 Observing learn ing and teaching in the second language classroom Observing learning an d teaching in the second language classroom 147
they have been encouraged ro ask more referential (or genuine) questions McCormick and D on at o suggest th at q uestions should be examine d wi thi n
since th e latter are thought to require more cognitive processing and to gen th e fram ewo rk of scaffo lded interactio n and with reference to the teacher's
erate more complex answers. goals in a particular lesson o r interaction .
More recently, however, a re-evaluation of display questions has taken place.
This is based on the observation that there are different ways in which dis play
Study 8: Open and closed questions
Ano ther distinction similar to th e one between disp lay and gen uine ques
questions can be asked in classrooms, One is for the ~eac her ro ask a series o f
tions is that between open and closed q uestions. Clo sed q uestio ns typ ically
q uestions in a drill-like format such as 'D o you have a bro ther?' , ' D oes he have
a brother?', 'Do you have a sister?' , 'Does she have a sister?' In th is context, have only one possible answer an d they usually lead to simple one-word
responses, making them quick and easy to respond to. Open questions have
display questions do no t have a meaningful or communicative purpose. In
other COntexts , howe ver, display questions can serve important pedagogic mo re than on e possible answer and invite elaboration, typically leading to
longer and more complex answers, including, for example, exp lanation and
and interaction functions . The st udy below describes teachers' use of display
q uestio ns in a more positive light. reason ing. In content and language-integrated learning (CLIL) classes in
Austria, Christiane D alto n-Puffer (2006) observed and audio recorded the
Study 7: Scaffolding and display and referential questions types ofquestions asked by English teachers, as well as the responses stud ents
In a case study of one teacher's adult ESL class, -Daw n M cCormick and gave to the m . Stude nts produced a greater q uantity and q uality of outpu t
Richard Donato (2000) explored how th e teacher's questions were lin ked to after open questions. In addition, open questions that asked lear ners not JUSt
her instructional goals. Working within sociocultural theory, the researchers fo r facts but also for reasons o r exp lan ations led to th e most complex lin
chose the concept o f scaffolding to in vestigate teacher questions as 'med ia guistic outcomes. D alton-Puffer co ncluded that asking more complex open
tional tools within the dialogue between the teacher and students' (p. 184) . questions would benefit learners in these CLIL classrooms but that thi s level
As we saw in Chap ter 1 and Chapter 4 , scaffolding refers to a process in of question/response int eraction requires a h igh level of com petence in the
which, for example, a more knowledgeable (or expert) speaker helps a less foreign language on the parr of the teacher.
kn owledgeable (or novice) learner by providing an interactional framework
that the learner can build on . Study 9: wait time and teachers' questioningpractices
Another aspect ofteachers' questioning behaviour is 'wait rim e'- the amount
McCormick an d Donaro identified six functions ofscaffolding (for example, of rime the teacher pauses after having asked a q uestion to give the student
drawing the novice's attention to the task , and simp lifying or limiting the time to resp ond. Jo anna White and Patsy Lighrbown (1984) did a quan
task demands). Th e researchers examined another function-the teacher's tita tive an alysis o f wai t tim e in au d iolingual ESL classes. They fo und that
use of questions du ring scaffolded interactions-and how it contributed to teachers typicall y gave students no more than a second o r two before th ey
class participation and lear ner comprehen sion. In the example belo w, th ey d irected the question to another student or answered th e q uestion them
arg ue th at th e teacher's use of the disp lay question 'Who usually lives in selves. The y also tend ed to repeat or paraphrase the qu estion several times
palaces?' serves an important pedagogic fu nction because it draws the lear n rather than silen tly wai t for the stu dent to form ulate a response. Al tho ugh
ers' attention to the word 'palace' through the display question and facilitates such rapid q uestion/answer patterns are typical of audiolingual classes, the y
the learners' comprehension of the word. also occur in communicative instruction. Finding a balance between plac ing
T Palace?
roo m uch pressure on students to respond q uickly and crea ting awkward
51 Like castle?
silenc es seems to be a real chal lenge.
5 2 Special place, very good.
Research has sho wn that when teachers are tra ined to give thei r students more
5 3 Very nice.
time to respond to questions, not only d o students produce more responses
T Castle, special place , very nice . Who usuall y lives in pala ces?
but their responses are also longer and more complex. N ot surprisingly, th is
ss Kings.
effect has been observed to be stronger with open/ referential ques tions com
T Kings, and queens, princes and princesses.
pared with closed/display qu esti ons (Lo ng er al. 1985 ).
ss Yeah
In classroo ms with stu dents at diffe rent age levels and in different kinds of
T Big, beautiful ho use, yeah, really big.
inst ruction, find ing the righ t bala nce can lead to stu dents provi di ng fuller
answers, expanding their ideas , and more successfully processing the material
to be learn ed .
148 Obst!rving learning and tCtlchmg in the second !..znguage classroom Observing learning and teaching in the second language classroom 1-*9
Study 10: Time for learning languages in school outco mes. We now tu rn to a d ifferent a pproach ro describing and interp ret
Earlier in thi s cha p ter we calked about the differences betwe en learning a ing instructional sett ings for seco nd language learning.
seco nd language in the natural setting compared with the classroom (see
Table 5.1). One of the major differences is the amount of time available for
learning. In the natural serring there is ample time to learn w hereas in th e
Ethnography
typical classroo m setting, learners have limited time. One o f th e ways to Eth nograp hy is a way ofobserv ing teac hi ng and learni ng in seco nd or foreign
pro vide more time for learning a second/ fo reign language isvia co nt ent- based language classrooms without a set of predetermined categories. Instead, the
instruc tion . H owever, thi s is nor always feasible or desirable (Ligh rbow n observer takes ext ensive notes of th e acti vities, practices, and interactions and
2 0 12). Other alt ernatives include increasing the total instructional time o r looks for the patterns that emerge. This approach to classroom observation
distributing time more intensively over the school year. is sim ilar to the way in which an anthropologist tak es field notes in stu dy
We know that it takes a great deal of time to learn a seco nd language, but
research, the obs erver can either be a participant in th e classroom activities,
littl e resear ch has been done ro investigate how the distribution of instruc
for example, as a teach er's aid , o r as a non-particip ant, some o ne who sits
C anad ian resear chers wh o have examined different amounts and distribu
rions o f rim e in English and Fren ch as a seco nd language programmes. In one Ethnographic approach es to understanding teaching and learning involve
stu dy in Quebec, learners receiving intensive ESL instruction for five hours qualitative Stu d ies that are mu ch broader in sco pe th an th e stu d ies usin g
eve ry day fo r five monrhs ofo ne schoo l year (in Grade 5 or 6) we re co mpared ob serva tio n sche mes descr ibed above. Th at is. et hnog rap hies in seco nd or
to learners at the end of secondarv schoo l who h ad received the same total foreign language classroo ms do not focus so lely on learni ng or on teac hi ng
amou nt of instruct ion sp read ove r 7-8 years of schooli ng. O n a nu mber o f bu t also on soci al, cultu ral, and po lit ical realities and their imp act on learn ers'
meas ures, the st ude nts who received th e intensive inst ruc tio n pe rfo rmed as cog nitive, linguist ic. an d social develop men t. Fo r example, Ma rtha Crago's
well as o r better rhan rhose whose instruction was deli vered in wha t has been (1992) language sociali zation resea rch with Inui t ch ild ren led her ro argue
called a 'd rip feed ' ap proac h (Ligh rbow n and Spada 1994). that if children come from a culture in whi ch silence is a resp ectful and effec
tive way to learn from an adult, th eir second language instructor needs to
In subseq ue nt research , co m pariso ns were made between groups o f Grade 5
know this so that th e ch ild ren's behaviour is not misinterpreted as refusal to
and 6 stude nts who participated in int ensi ve English language instruction
participate o r inability to co m p rehend.
during a single schoo l year, but with the rime distributed differently: some
stude n ts received five hours o f English a da y for five months; others received Here are summaries of three ethnographies carr ied our in seco nd and foreign
th e same total number of hours, doing two and a half hours o f Engli sh eac h language classro oms: one in th e Sourh Pacific, one in C anad a, and on e in
day for 10 months. Th e researchers found that both groups benefited from Europe.
th e overall increase in hours o f instruction with some additional advantages
for learners receivin g the more int ensive instruction (Co llins et al. 1999 ; Study 11: Language in the home and school
Collins and White 2 0 1 1). The advantages were evident not only in su perio r Karen \'V'atson-Gegeo ( 1992) ca rried Out a longitudinal study over several
language abilities but also in attitudes toward the language and satisfaction years with nine fam ilies in the Solomon Islands. She explored language-use
with language learning experiences. Similar findings have been reported practices in th e home and in the schoo!. Observations in the homes revealed
for different models of intensive and core French programmes (N erren and en vironments that were rich and sti m ulating for both linguistic and cogn itive
Ge rmain 2004; Lapkin, Hart, and Harley 1998). de veloprnent. Nevertheless, a large number of the children failed in schoo!.
A detailed analysis uncovered many differences in language use and values
The classroom observation stu d ies we have described in this chapter focus between the home and school setting. There was no use of the children's first
o n specific features of classroom interaction. In these studies, the feature of language in sch oo!. Their first language was replaced with a restricted and
int erest was determined in advance of the observation on the basis of some o ften incorrect version of English . Although th ese language issues were con
hypothesis about what aspects of classroom instruction and interaction are trib uting factors to the children's failure, a broader analysis of the social and
important for learning, thar is, whether a particular type of corrective feed cultu ral context revealed other, more influential factors at p lay. Evidently,
bac k led ro more learner repair, whether a particular type of qu estion led parr of the children's lan guage socialization experie nce at home included
to more learner OUtpUt, and how the distribution of time affected learning parents negatively portraying their own experiences at school, expressing
150 Observing !earning and teaching in the second Language classroom Observing learning and teaching in the second Language classroom 15 1
fear s abour th eir children's ability to succeed and raisin g fundamental q ues developed th eir flue ncy, accuracy, an d co mprehe nsion skills in me p rocess.
tions about the value of school in th eir lives. The researcher concludes m at On me basis of th ese findin gs, Duff co ncluded mat sociopo litical transfo r
these factors were cen tral in contributing to the chi ldren's lack of continued mation affects classro om practi ce and ultimately second language learning.
cogn itive and linguistic development in school.
invo lve large numbers of learners in an effo rt to avoid th e possibility th at lead students to actually speak the language (Brooks 196 0: Lado 1964).
the unusual behaviour of one or two individuals might lead to a mis leading In C ha p ter 4, we saw that the audiolingual approach was based on beh av
conclusion about learn ers in general. iou rism and contrastive anal ysis. The examples below reflect au d iolingual
teaching. It is evide nt that, even though the emphasis is on the or al language,
Qualitative research , incl udi ng ethnogra ph ies and case sru dies , ofte n involves
students rarely use the language spo nt aneously. Teachers avoid letting begin
small n umb ers, perhaps one class or on ly one or rwo lear ners in that class.
ning lear ne rs speak freely o ut ofconcerns that this wo uld allow them to mak e
The emphasis is not o n wha t is most general but rather on a th o rough under
errors . The errors, it is said, co uld become habits. So it is bett er to prevent
stan d ing of what is particul ar about what is happen in g in this classroom. As
th ese bad habits before they happen and 'Get it right from th e beginn ing.'
pointed o u t by Ann Burns (20 10) and others, while quantitative and qualita
tive research are important in assessing theoretical proposals, action research Example 1
carried our by teachers in th eir own classrooms is also essential to answer spe (A group of 15-year -o ld students invo lved in an exercise based on the sim ple
cific local questions. In th is chapter we focus mainly on experimental studies . p resent of Eng lish verbs.)
Th ese are studies that were designed to test hyp otheses abo ur how teaching
affects second lan guage learn ing. Readers are enco uraged to follow up with S1 And uh, in the afternoon, uh, I come home and uh, uh , I uh,
further reading but also to exp lore related q uestions through research activi washing my dog.
ties wit hin thei r own teaching and learning en viro nments. T I wash.
SI My dog.
T Every day you wash yo ur dog?
1 Get it rightfrom the beginning SI No . [ben]
'Get it right from the begin ning' is probably the proposal that character S2 II n'a pas de chien! (= H e doesn't have a dog!)
izes more second and foreign language instr uctio n than any o ther kind. s INon, mais on peut le d ire! (= No , but we can say we dol)
Although communicative lang uage teaching has come to do minate in some (Unpublished data fro m P. M. Lightbown and B. Barkman)
environments, the srrucrure-based approaches discussed in Chap ter 5, espe
cially grammar translation, rem ain Widespread. C learly, in this case, the student's real experience with his do g (or even th e
fact that he did or did not have a dog) was irrelevant. What mattered was the
Th e gra mmar translat ion approach has its origi n in the teac hing of classi correct use of the simple present verb!
cal languages (fo r example, Greek an d Latin) . Students were presented with
vocabulary lists, often acco m panied by translat ion eq uivale n ts, and grammar &le 2
rules . The original purpose of th is approach was to help stu dents read lit (A grou p o f 12-year-old learners of English as a forei gn language.)
era ture rather than to develop fluency in the spo ken lan guage. It was also
T Repeat after me . Is there an y butter in th e refrigerator?
tho ught that thi s approach provided students with good m ental exercise to
CLAS S Is there an y butter in the refrigerator ?
help develop their in tellect ual and academic abilities .
T There's very little, Mom.
In a typical grammar translation activity, student s read a text toge ther line CLASS There's very little, Mom.
by line and are asked to translate it from the targe t language in to their native T Are th ere any to matoes in the refrigerator?
lan guage . Stu dent s may answer comprehension questio ns based on the CLAS S Are there any tomatoes in the refrige rator?
passage, often in their first lan gu age. The teacher draws attention to a specific T There are very few, Mom.
grammar rule that is illustrated by the text (fo r example, a cer tain verb form). CLASS There are very few, Mom.
Following th is, the students are given an exercise in which they are asked
(Unpublished data fro m P. M . Lightbown and B. Barkman)
to practise th e grammar rule, for exam ple, by fillin g in the blanks with th e
appropriate verb form in a series of decontextualized sentences th at mayo r Pure repetition: The students have no reaso n to get involved or to th ink
may no t be related to the text they have read and translated . abo ut what they are saying. Indeed, some students who have no idea what
the sentences mean will successfully repea t them anyway, while th eir minds
Audiolingual instruction arose in part as a reactio n to the grammar transla
wander off to other things.
tio n app roac h. The argument was that, un like grammar translat io n teachin g,
in wh ich stu dents learn ed abo ut the lan guage , aud iol in gual teach ing would
156 Second fanguilge It!aming in the classroom Second languagt! It!armng in the classroom 15
of seco nd lan guage learning. Th ese oppo nents o f th e 'G et it right fro m the
beginning' proposal argue that it is better to encourage learners co de velop
' fluency' before 'accuracy'.
97~/J ~/I/(\\)
Some researchers and educators have reacted to the version of communica
tive language teaching that advocates an exclusive focus on meaning. They
~OJ
argue that allowing learners too much 'freedo m' without correction and
./
."'E!a>I:'S explicit instruction will lead co early fossilization of errors. Once again we
o ON Tlfe J>e'Sl;. II. (, I( hear the call for making sure that learners 'G et it right from the beginning'.
\)/~ ~
stages of second language learning will, in the long run, lead co better results
than those achieved when the emphasis is on meaning in the early stages. To
test that hypothesis, it would be necessary to compare groups that are similar
(~~?-
in all respects except for the rype of instruction they receive. However, it is
not easy for researchers co find proper comparison groups. This is because
there are many parts of the world where one finds predominantly structure
based approaches co language teaching, and in the se settings there arc no (o r
very few) classrooms wh ere the teaching places an emphasis o n.meani ng. O n
th e o ther hand , with th e widespread ado ptio n of communicati ve lan gua ge
teac hi ng in o the r parts of th e wo rld , it is difficult CO find classrooms th at
are exclusively struc tu re-based . No ne theless, so m e findi ngs from second lan
Research findings guage classro om research do permit us co assess the effects of instructi on th at
Many adulr learners, especially those with good metalinguistic knowledge is strongly oriented co the 'G et it right from the beginning' approach . These
of their own language, express a preference for structure-based approaches. include descriptive studies of the interlanguage development of second lan
Learners whose previous language learning experience was in grammar guage learners in audiolingual programmes (Study 14), and comparisons of
translation classes may also prefer such instruction. As we saw in Chapter 3, the development ofsecond language proficiency between groups ofstudents
learners' beliefs about the kind of instruction that is best can influence their receiving different combinations of form- and meaning-based instruction
satisfaction and success. The grammar translation approach is useful for the (Stu dy 15).
study of grammar and vocabulary and can be valuable for understanding
important cultural texts. The audiolingual approach with its emphasis on Study 14: Audiolingualpattern drill
speaki ng and listening was used successfully with highly motivated adult In the late 1970s , Patsy Lightbown ( 19 83a / b) car ried OUt a series of lon
learners in intensive training programmes for government personnel in the gitudinal and cross -sectional investigations into the effecr of audiolingual
United States . However, there is little classroom research ro suPPOrt such instruction on interlanguage development. The investigations focused on
approaches for students in ordinary school programmes that must serve the French-speaking learners aged 11-16 in Quebec, Canada. Students in these
needs of students who bring different levels of motivation and aptitude co programmes typically participated in the rypes of rote repetitio n and pattern
the classroom. In fact, it was the frequent failure of traditional grammar practice drill we saw in Examples 1 and 2.
translation and audiolingual methods to produce fluency and accuracy in The learners' acquisition of certain English grammatical morphemes (for
second language learners that led co the development of more communica example, plural -r and the progressive-ing) was compared with the acquisition
tive approaches co teaching in the first place. of grammatical morphemes observed in the interlanguage of uninstructed
Supporters of communicative language teaching have argued that language second language learners (see Chapter 2 , p. 46). The results showed differ
is not learned by the gradual accumulation of one grammatical feature after ences between the developmental sequences we saw there and the relative
another. They suggest that errors are a natural and valuable part of the lan accuracy with which these classroom learners produced them. These findings
guage learn ing process. Furthermore, they believe that the motivation of suggested that the rype of instruction students had experienced-isolated
learners is often stifled by an ins istence on correctness in the earliest stages
158 Second language learn ing in the classroom Second language learn ing in the classroom 159
parrern practice drills-resulred in a developmental sequence that appeared interviewing a na tive speak er of French, reporting facts about oneself or one's
co be different from that oflearners in more natural learning env ironm en ts. recent activities, and describing ongoing acti vitie s in French.
For a time after their instruction had focused on it, learners reliably pro At the end of me period of instruction, there were no significant differences
d uced a particular grammatical morpheme in irs obligacory contexts. For between groups on the linguistic competence measures. However, th e com
example, after weeks ofdrilling on present progressive, students usually sup m unicative group scored significantly higher than me other twO gro ups on
plied both th e auxiliary be and the -ing ending (for example, 'He's playing the four communicative tests developed for the study. Savig no n in terp reted
ball'). However, they also produced one or more of me morphemes in places these results as support for the argument mar second language programmes
where they did not belong CHe's wan t a cookie') . The same forms were pro that focus only on accuracy and form do not give students sufficient oppor
duced with considerably less accuracy in ob ligatory contexts when they were tunity to develop communication abilities in a second language. Even more
no lo nger being practised in class and when th e third person singular simple important in the context of me 'G et it right from the beginning' approach
present -s was being drilled instead. At this point, many students appeared was the evidence that opportunities for freer communication did not cause
co revert co what looked like a developmentally earlier stage, using no tense learners to do less well on measures of linguistic accuracy.
marking at all (for example, 'He play ball') . These findings provided evi
dence mat an almost exclusive focus on accuracy and practice of particular Interpreting the research
grammatical forms does not mean th at learners will be able to use the forms The studies reviewed above provide evidence to support me intuitions of
correctly outside the classroom drill setting, nor that they will continue to teach ers and learners that instruction based on the ' G er it right from' me
use them correctly once other forms are introduced. Not surprisingly, this begin n in g' proposal has important limitations. Learners receiving audiolin
instruction, mat depended o n repetition and drill of decontexrual ized sen gual or grammar-translation instruction are often un able to com m unicate
tences, did not seem co favour the development of comprehension, fluency, their messages and intentions effectively in a second lang uage. Experience
or communicative abilities either, has also shown mat pr imarily or exclusively structure-based approaches to
teaching do not guarantee that learners develop high levels of accuracy and
Study 15: Grammarplus communicativepractice linguistic knowledge. In fact, it is often very d ifficult to determine what stu
In one of the earliest experimental studies of communicative language teach dents know about me target language. The classroom emphasis on accuracy
ing, Sandra Savignon (1972) studied me linguistic and communicative skills often leads learners to feel inhibited and reluctant CO take chances in using
of 48 college students enrolled in French language courses at an American their knowledge for communication. The results from these studies provide
university. The students were divided into three groups: a 'communicative' evidence that lea rners benefit from opportunities for communicative practice
group, a 'cu ltu re' group, and a control group. NI groups received about fo ur in contexts where the emphasis is on understanding and expressing meaning.
hours per week of audiolingual instruction where me focus was on me prac
tice an d manipulation of grammatical forms. In addition, each group had a It is important to em p hasize that in the Savignon study, all students contin
special. hour of different activities. The 'com m unica tive' group had one hour ued to receive their regular, grammar-focused instruction. They differed only
per week devo ted to communicative tasks in an effort to encourage practice in terms of me ptesence or absence ofan additional communicative practice
in using French in meaningful, creative, and spontaneous ways. The 'culture' component. In other words, this study offers sup po rt for me argument that
group had an hour devoted to activities, conducted in English, designed to meaning-based instruction is advantageous, not that for m-based instruction
'foster an awareness of me Fre nch language and culture through films, music, is no t. The contributions of communicative practice and grammar-focused
and art' . The control group had an hour in the language laboratory doing instruction will be discussed in more detail in relation to the 'G et it right in
grammar and pronunciation drills similar co those they did in their regular the end' proposal.
class periods.
Tests . to measure learners' linguistic and communicative abili ties were 2 Just listen . . . and read
administered before and after instruction. The rests of linguistic competence 'JUSt listen . . . and read ' is based on the hypothesis that language acq uisition
included a variety of grammar rests, teachers' eval uations of speaking skills , takes place when learners are exposed co comprehensible input through lis
and course grades. The tests of communicative competence included meas tening and/or reading. As noted in Chapter 4, the individual whose name is
ures of fluency and of th e ability to understand and transmit information in m ost closely associated with th is proposal is Stephen Krashen (1985, 1989).
a variety of tasks, which included: discussion with a native speaker of French, This is a co ntroversial proposal because it suggests th at second language
160 Second language lea rning in the classroom Second language learning in the classroom 161
learners do nor need to produce lan guage in order to learn it. except perhaps in English at all. Teachers did not 'teach' but provided organizarional and
to get other people to provide inp ut by speaking to th em . According to this technical sup po rt . Thus. lear ners received nat ive-speaker input from tapes
view, it is eno ugh to hear (o r read) and underst and th e target language. and books but they had virtu ally no interactio n in English with the teach er or
o th er learn ers. The y guessed at meaning by usin g the pictures o r by recogniz
Read Example 3 to get a feel for how thi s the o ry o f classroom seco nd lan gu age
ing cognate wo rds char are similar in French and English. Occasionally they
learning can be implemented in a classroo m . Th is descri p tion shows that
could refer to translation equivalents of a few wo rds, taped inside a book's
one way to obtain comprehensible input is to provide learners with listening
~kro~~ .
an d reading co m prehe nsion activities w ith no (or very few) op po rt un ities to
speak or int eract with th e teache r or other learne rs in th e classroom. Patsy Light bown and her colleag ues inves tigated the second language devel
opment of hundreds of chil dren in this comprehension-based programme
Example 3 an d compare d th eir lear ning with th at o f stu de nts in me regular ESL p ro
It is time for English class at a pri m ary school in a French-speaki ng co m gramme, which was mai nly an audiolingual ap proach. All m e stu de nts in
munity in New Brunsw ick. Canada . Th e classroom looks like a m in iatu re both programmeS had classes tha t last ed 30 minutes per day. After rwo years,
language lab, with about thirty small desks, on each of which there is a cas learners in the comprehensio n-based programme knew as much English
sette player and a set oflarge ear ph ones . Aro und the roo m, shelves and racks as (an d in some cases mo re than) learners in the regular pr ogramm e. Th is
display scores of books. Each book is packaged with an audiocassette that was true not only for comprehension but also for speaking, even though the
contains a reco rd ing of its content . The materi als are no t str ictly graded, bu t learners in rhe experimental programme had never practised spoken Englis h
some sets of books are very simple. and othe r sets are grouped so that they in their classes (Ligh rbown er al. 2002; Trofimovich et al. 2009).
are gradually more challenging. There are pre-school child ren's books with
a picture and a word or rwo on each page; illustrated stories with a few sen The students' Engli sh language abilities were reassessed three years later, when
tences per page; picture dictionaries; ESL textbooks for child ren; illustrated th ey were in Grade 8. Some students had co nt in ued in the comprehension
science books about animals, weather, vehicles, etc . Students (aged 8-10) o nly programme throughout that time. On comprehension measures and on
enter the classroom , select the material they want. and take it to their indi some measures of oral production, they continued to perform as well as StU
vidual works pace. They inse rt the cassette , put on th eir ear pho nes. an d open dents in the regular programme. On other meas ures, some gro ups ofstu de nts
their books . They hear and read English for the next 3 0 mi n utes. For so me of in the regular programme had made greate r p rogress, especial ly in wri ting .
the time th e teac he r walks aro und th e classroom, checking th at the machines Those students were in classes where the regular programme included not
are runn ing smoothly, bu t she d oes not interact with th e stu de nts co ncern on ly aud iolingual ins tructio n but also othe r speaking and writi ng co mpo
ing wha t they are do ing. Some of the stude nts are liste n ing wi th closed eyes; nents. teacher feed bac k, and classroom inte ractio n .
others read actively, mouthing the wo rds silen tly as they follow each line with
Study 17: Readingfor words
a finge r. The classroom is alm ost sile nt except for th e so und of tap es being Find ing reading material for p rim ary school students learning a seco nd lan
inserted and rem oved o r chairs scraping as students go to the shelves to select
guage is challenging. Find ing reading material for adults in early stages of
new tapes and boo ks.
second language acquisition is chal leng ing too, but graded readers specially
des igned for ad ult ESL learn ers are increasingly availab le. Th ese sim plified
Research findings literary classics, biogr aphies, romances, and thrillers offer int eresting and
Research relevant to th e 'J USt listen . .. an d read' pro posal includes stu d ies of age-a ppro p riate content, whi le the vocab ulary and writi ng sryle rem ain
co mp rehensio n- based teach ing and extens ive reading (Day et al. 20 1 1). We sim ple. Marlise Ho rst (200 5) used sim plified read ers in a study of voca bulary
will also look at some comprehension-based instruction in wh ich th e input development among ad ult immigrants who were enrolled in an ESL pro
is ma nip ulated in ways that are intended ro increase the likelih ood that stu gram me in a co m m unity cent re in M ontreal, C anada. Th e 2 1 particip ants
dents will pay atte nti on to lan gu age for m as well as m eaning. rep resen ted several langu age backgrounds and proficien cy levels. In ad di tion
to th e activ ities of th eir regular ESL class, students chose simplified read ers
Study 16: Comprehension-based instruction for children tha t were made availab le in a class libr ary. Over a six-week period . students
Exam ple 3 was a description of a real pro gramme implem ented in experi
roo k books home and read [he m o n th eir ow n.
me nt al classes in a Frenc h-speaki ng region in Canada . From th e beginn ing
of the ir ESL instructi on at age eigh t, stu dents only listen ed and read during H orst developed individualized voca bulary measures so mat lear ning co uld
thei r daily 30- m in ute ESL period . Th ere was no oral practi ce o r interaction be assessed in terms o f the books eac h student actually read . She found that
162 Second language Learning in the classroom Second language Learning in the classroom 163
th ere was vocab ulary growth att rib utable ro reading, even over this short th ey conti nued ro accep t senten ces that are gram ma tical in Frenc h bu t no t in
period, and that th e more students read, the more words th ey learned. She English ('The ch ildren leave quickly schoo l') . The stu dents' inab ility to recog
concluded that substantial vocabulary growth through reading is possible, nize th at adverbs in this position are ungram m atical in English suggests that
but that students must read a great deal (mo re than JUSt o ne or rwo books th e input flood could help them add something new to their interlan guage,
per semester) to realize th ose benefits. As we saw in Chap ter 2, when we bu t d id nor lead them to get rid o f an error based on their first language. As
interact in ordinary conversations, we tend to use mainly the 1,000 or 2 ,000 noted in Chapter 2, Lydia White (l99 1) argued tha t although exposure ro
mo st freq uent words. Th us, reading is a particularly valuable so urce of new language in put provides lear ners with positive evidence (in for matio n abo ut
vocabulary. St udent s who have reached an intermediate level of proficiency what is grammatical in the second language) , it fails to give them negative
may have few opportunities to learn new words in everyday conversation. It evidence (info rm atio n about what is not grammatical). Positive evidence is
is in reading a variety of texts that students are most likely to encounter new not enough to permit learners to notice the absence in the target language
vocabulary. The benefit of simplified readers is that students encoun ter a of elements that are present in their interlanguage (and their first language).
reasonable number of new words. This inc reases the likelihood that they ean Thus, more explicit information abo ut what is not grammatical in the second
figure out th e meaning of new words (or perhaps be motivated to look th em language may be necessary for lear ners' continued develop ment. This is d is
up). If the new words occur often enough, students may remember them cussed in more detail in the sect ion ' G et it right in the end' .
whe n they encounter th em in a new context.
Study 19: Enhanced input
Other research that explores the 'Just listen '. . . and read ' proposal includes M ichael Sharwood Smith (1993) coined the term ' in put enhancem ent' to
stu dies in wh ich efforts have been made to draw seco nd langu age learn ers' refer to a variety of th ings that m ight draw learners' attention to featu res in
attention ro language forms in th e in put, for exam ple, by providing high the second language, thus increasing the chan ces th at they would be lea rn ed .
frequency exposure to specific language features through an in p ut flood, In a study invol ving en hanced input, Joan na White (1998) exam ined th e
highlighting the fearures through enhanced input, and/or providing pro acquisition of possessive determ iners (specifically 'his' and ' her'; see Chapter
cessing instruction. All of these are described in more detail below but the 2, p. 53) by French-speaking learners aged 11-12 in inte nsive ESL classes.
emphasis in all cases is on getting learners ro notice language forms in the Stu d en ts received approximately 10 hours ofexposure to hundreds of posses
input, not on getting them to practi se producing the forms . The next th ree sive det erminers through a package of readi ng materials and comprehension
studies are examples of th is researc h. activities provided over a two-week period. The major difference between
this study and Trahey and White's in put flood is that typographical enhance
Study 18: Inputflood
ment was added. That is, every time a possessive determiner appeared in the
Martha Trahey and Lydia White (1993) ca rried out a srudy with young
texts, it was in bold type , underlined, italicized, or written in capital lett ers.
Frenc h-speaking learners (aged 10-12) in intensive ESL classes in Q uebec
The hypothesis was that th is would lead the learn ers to notice the possessive
such as those described in Chapter 5, Study 10. Th e goal of the research was
determiners as th ey read the texts.
to determine whether high -frequen cy exposure to a particular form in the
instructional input would lead to better knowledge and use of that form White compared the performance of learners who had read th e typographi
by the students. The linguistic form investigated was adverb place me nt in cally enhanced passages wit h tha t o flearners who read the sam e texts wi thou t
English (see Chap ter 2, p. 58) . For approximately 10 hours over a two-week enhancement. She found that both groups im proved in their kn owled ge
period, learners read a series ofshort texts in whi ch they were expose d to liter and use of these forms and that there was little d ifference between them. In
ally hundreds of instances ofadverbs in English sentences-so ma ny that the interpreting these find ings, White questions whether the en hancement was
investigators referred to th is study as an ' in put flood'. There was no teaching sufficien tly explicit to draw the learners' attention to possessive determin
of adverb placement, nor was an y error correction provided. Instead, stu ers. That is, even tho ugh the forms were h ighligh red by th e use of bold type ,
dents simply read the passages and completed a variety of comprehension capital letters, etc., students d id no t learn how to choose the possessive deter
activities based on them. miner to match the gen der of the possessor. In subsequent research , White
found that learn ers made more progress when they were given a sim ple rule
Alt ho ugh learners benefited from th is exposure to sente nces with adverbs
and then wo rked together to find the correct possessive deter miners (Spada,
in all the correct posi tions, their learning was incomplete. Th ey im p roved
Ligh rbown, and White 2005 ).
in their acceptance of sentences with word order th at is grammatical in
En glish bur nor in French ('Th e children quickly leave school'). However,
164 Second ltnguage learning in the classroom Second ltnguage learn ing in th« classroom 165
o n the co m p rehe nsio n tasks than learners in the p roduct ion gro up. they also
performed as well on production tasks.
Interp retin g the research
Researc h on comprehension-based approaches to second lan guage acquisi
/ tio n shows th at learners can make co nsiderable progress if they have sustained
exposure to language they understand. The evide nce also suggestS, ho wever,
tha t co m prehensio n- based activities may best be seen as an excellent way
to beg in learning and as a supplement to other kinds of learning for more
ad van ced learners.
Comp rehension ofmeani ngful lan guage is the fou ndatio n oflanguage acqui
sitio n. Active listening and readi ng for meaning are valuable components
of classroom tea chers' pedagogical pra ctices. Nevertheless, considerable
research and experience challenge the hypo th esis that comprehensible input
Enhancing the input is enough. Van f'atren's research showed that forc ing students to rely on spe
cific ling uis tic features in order to interpret meaning increased the chances
Study 20: Processing instruction tha t the y would be able to use th ese features in th eir own seco nd lan gua ge
Bill Van f'arren (2 004) and his colleag ues have investigated the effects of pro p roduc tio n.
cessing instruct io n, ano the r app roac h to co m prehensio n- based learning. In Anot her response to co mp rehension-based approaches is Merrill Swain"
processing instruction, learne rs are p Ut in situa tio ns where they ca n nor co m
(198 5) com prehensib le OUtput hypothesis. She arg ues tha t it is whe n Stu
p rehe nd a sen ren ee by depending so lely o n co n text . pri or kn owled ge, or other dents have to p rod uce lang uage tha t th ey begin to see the lim itati on s of th ei r
clues. Rather they m ust focu s on the language itself. In one ofthe first studies, interlanguage (see C hap ter 4) . H owever, as we will see in the disc ussion of
ad ult learn ers ofSpanish as a foreign language received instruction on diffe r th e 'Let's talk' proposal , iflearners are in situation s where thei r teachers an d
ent lingu istic forms , for example, object prono uns (VanPanen and Cadierno classmates unders tand them without difficulty, they may need additional
1993) . As noted in Chapter 4, VanPatten found that English-speaking lear n help in overcoming those limitations.
ers ofSpa nis h tend ed to trea t the objec t pronouns, which p recede the verb in
Spanish, as if they were subject pronouns. Thus, a sentence such as La sigue
1 senor (literally 'h er (object) follows the man (su bject)') was interpre ted as 3 Let's talk
'She follows th e man'. Advoca tes of the ' Let's talk' proposal em phasize th e im po rtance of access to
Two groups were co mpared in th e study, o ne receiving processing instruc both co m prehensible in put an d co nve rsat ional inte ractio ns with teac hers
tion, the other following a mo re traditional approach. The processing and othe r students. They arg ue th at when learners are given the opportunity
to engage in interaction. they are compelled to negotiate for meaning, that is,
instruction gro up received exp licit exp lanatio ns abou t obj ect p ronouns and
to expres s and clarify the ir int enti o ns, tho ugh ts, opinions, erc., in a way that
did some activities that drew the ir attentio n to the importan ce of noticin g
that o bject p ron ou ns could occur before th e verb. Then, th ro ugh a varie ty of permits th em to arrive at m utual understan d ing. Th is is especi ally tru e w hen
foc used liste ning an d reading exercises, learners had to pay atte n tion to h ow the learners are wo rki ng together to accom plis h a pa rtic ular goal, for exam ple
the target forms were used in o rde r to understand th e mean in g. For example, in task-based lan guage teachi ng (T BLT). According to the interactio n
they heard or read La sigue el senor and had to choose whic h p icrure-a man hypothesis, negotiati o n leads learners to acqu ire the lan gu age for ms-the
follow ing a wo ma n or a wo m an following a m an-eorresponded to the sen words and the grammatical str uc tu res-that carry th e mea ning they are
tence. A seco nd gro up of lear ners also received explicit info rmation about attend ing to . This is th e theo retical view u nderlyin g the teacher- stu dent
the targe t forms but instead of focus ing on comp rehens ion pr acti ce through behav io ur in the transcrip t from Class room B and from the student-student
processing instruct ion , th ey engage d in p roduct ion practice, do in g exercises interactio n in Co mmunicatio n task A in Chapte r 5.
to practise the forms being taught. After the instruction. learners who had
rece ived the comprehension-based processing instructio n no t o nly did better
166 Second language learn ing in the classroom Second language learning in the classroom 167
Negotiation for meaning is accomplished through a variety of modifications th ese studies have been car ried o ut in labo rat o ry settin gs and are m oti vated
that naturally arise in interaction, such as requests for clarification or confir by Mi chael Long's ( 1996) updated versio n of the interaction hypothesis
mation, repetition wit h a questioning intonation, etc. (see Chapter 4). Compared with the original version Stating th at conver
Look for negotiation for meani ng in the examples below and compare this sational interaction promotes second language develo pment (Long 1983) ,
with the ex~p les given for the 'Get it right from the beginning' proposal. the updated vers ion integrates learne r capacities that co ntribute [Q second
language learning (for example, attention) and features of interactio n that
Example 4 are most likely to facili tate lear ni ng. C o rrective feed back has been identified
(A group of 12 -year-old ESL students are discussi ng a questionnaire about as one feature that is believed to playa crucial role in helping learners [Q make
petS with their teacher.) connections between form and meaning. In fact, as we will see later in th is
chap ter, research relevant [Q the updated interaction hypo thesis is more in
5 And what is ~feed ' ?
speakers of English whose first language was Spanish, and six native English
T H ow are you doing this morning?
speakers. The non-native speakers were interm ed iate or advanced learn ers
s i I'm mad!
of English .
52 Why?
Each individ ual lear ner participated in separate discussions with a speaker
T Oh boy. Yeah, why?
advanced-level speaker, and ano ther with a native speaker of English. Long
5I My father.
168 Second language learn ing in tbe classroom Second language learning in the classroom [69
Study 22: Learner language andproficie1lCY level a su bseq ue nt task. Sto rch fo und tha t learn ers who participated in the col
George Yule and Doris Macdonald ( [ 990) investigated whether the role laborative and expert-novice pair s maintained more of their L2 knowledge
that different proficiency-level learners play in a two-way communication over time. Learners who participated in the dominant-dominant and domi
task led to differences in their interactive behaviour. They set up a task that nant-passive pairs maintained the least. Storch interp rets this as support for
required rwo learners to communicate information about th e location ofdif Vygo tsky's theory of cogni tive development (see Chapter 4) and the claim
ferent buildi ngs o n a ma p and th e ro ute to get th ere. One learner, referr ed to that when pai r work fu nctio ns collabo rat ively and learn ers are in an expert
as the 'sender' , had a map with a delivery route on it , and this speaker's job novice relationship, they can suc cessfully engage in the co-construction
was to describe the deli very route to the 'receiver ' so that he or she could draw o f knowledge.
the delivery route on a sim ilar map. The task was made mo re challenging by
the fact tha t there were minor differences between the two maps. Study 24: Interaction and second language development
Alison Mackey (1999) asked adult learners of ESL to engage in different
To determine whether there would be any difference in the interactions co m m unicative tasks, for example, story completion and picture sequen cing
acco rding to th e relati ve proficiency of the 40 adult participants, differ with nat ive speake rs of En glish . The tasks were designed to provid e co n
ent type s of learners we re paired together. O ne group had high-proficiency texts for learners to produce questions. Prior to participating in the tasks, the
learners in th e 'sender' role and low-proficiency learners in the 'receiver' role; learners were assessed in terms of the stage the y had reached in learning que s
the other gro up had low-proficiency 'senders' paired with high-profici en cy tions, as described in Chapter 2. Th ey were then divided into five groups.
.receivers' . The learners in G roup 1, referr ed to as' ' Inreracto rs.' ca rried o ut the tasks
\X'hen low-proficiency learn ers were senders, interactions were co nsiderably with native speakers, w ho modified their language as they sough t to clarify
longer and more varied than when high -p roficiency learners were the sen de rs. meani ng fo r the learne rs. Learne rs in Group 2 received th e same modified
The explanatio n for th is was tha t high -proficiency senders tended to act as input as learn ers in Gro up [ but they were not as advanced in th eir acquisi
if the lower-level receiver had little contributio n to make in the co mpletio n tio n ofq ues tio n forms and th us were referred to as the ' Lnter act o r Unreadies .'
of the task. As a result , th e lower -level receivers were almost forced to playa Learn ers in Group 3 , th e 'O bservers.' were asked to listen to the learn er and
passive role and said very lirtle. When lower-level learners were the senders the native speaker as they carried o u t th e task but they did not interact in an y
however, much more negotiation for meaning and a greater variety of inrerac way. Learners in Group 4, the 'Scripteds' carried out: the sam e tasks with the
do ns between the two speakers took place. Based on these findings , Yule and native sp eakers but th e nati ve speakers used lan guage th at had been sim p li
Macdonald suggest that teachers should sometimes place more advanced Stu fied and scripted in such a detailed manner that communication breakdowns
dents in less dominant roles in paired activities with lower-level learners. d id not occ u r and thus no negotiation for meaning took pla ce. There was also
Study 23: The dynamics ofpair toork co mp leted all the rests.
In a longitudinal study with adult ESL learners in an Australian university, Both Inreract ors an d In reracror Un readies demonstrated more sustained
N eo rny Storch (2 002) observed the patterns of int eraction between 10 pair s pr ogress in their quesrion formati on devel opment th an learn ers wh o did not
of students co m p letin g different rasks over one semester. Sh e identified four engage in interaction (i.e. Observers, Scripteds, and Control). Th e Observers
distinct parrerns of interaction: 'co llaborative' interactio n consisted of two and the Scripteds were sim ilar to th e C ontrol group learners, wh o changed
learners fully engaged with each other's ideas; 'domi na nt- d o m inan t' inter very li trle . Mackey also notes that the significant increase in development for
action was characterized by an unwillingness on the part of either learner the Inreractors was maintained on the delayed post-tests administered one
to engage and/or agree with the other's contributions; 'do m in ant- p assive' month after the treatment tasks. These results are interpreted as sup porr for
consisted of one lear ner who was authoritarian and another who was willing the hypothesis that negotiated int eraction leads to L2 development.
sup po rted the other in carrying our the task. To investigate whether the four In a study relevant to the updated version of the interaction hypothesis, Kim
type s of interaction led to differences in learning outcomes, Storch iden McDonough (2 004) investigated the use of pair- and small-group activities
tified learn ing events that occurred during the interactions (fo r example, in English as a foreign language classes in Thailand. Students engaged in
learning that the definite article is used with the names of some countries) . interactional activities in which they discussed environmental problems in
Then she looked at whether that lan gu age knowledge was maintained in thei r country. The topic was chosen as one that would generate contexts fo r
170 Second la nguage Learning in the classroom Second language Learning in th e classroom 171
the use of conditional clauses such as 'If people didn't leave water running (for example, recasts ) in pair-work situations is beneficial. A recent review of
while brushing their teeth, they would save an estimated 5-10 gallons each this research confirms that the positive effects for recasts are strongesr in the
time' (p, 213) . Learners were audio-recorded as they discussed the environ laboratory setting (M ackey and Goo 2007) . This may be because recasts are
mental problems. .
more salient in pair work, par ticularly if only one form is recast consistently
The recorded conversations were examined to see the extent to which stu (Nicholas, Lighrbown, and Spada 200 1).
dents used interactional features that are believed to facilitate second language In McDonough's classroom study, recasts (and other forms of corrective
learning, for example, negative feedback (i.e. clarification requests, explicit feedback) were more likely to have been no ticed beca use the Thai learners
co rrection, and recasts) an d mo dified output (i.e. a learner's more accurate/ were accustomed ro traditional grammar instruction and a focus on accuracy.
complex reformulation ofhis or her previous utterance). Learners were tested This is not always the case, however. As we learned in Chapter 5, when the
on their ability to produce conditional clauses in a pre -test, an immediate instructional focus is on expressing meaning through subject-matter instruc
post-test, and a delayed post-test. tion, the teachers' recasts may not be perceived by the learners as an attempt
McDonough found that lear ners who had used more negative feedback and to correct their language form but rather as JUSt another way of saying the
modified Output in their interactions sign ificantly improved in the accu same thing. Later in this chapter we will look at classroom studies related to
racy of their conditional clauses. Those who made less use of these feat ures the 'G et it right in the end' posi tion that have investigated the effects of more
did nor. McDonough also explored opinions about the usefulness of pair explicit corrective fc;edback on second language learn ing. .
work and small-group activities, asking whether such act ivities contributed
to learning. She found that the students did not perceive pair- and group 4 Get two for one
activities as useful for learning English. This was true both for students who
In content-based language teaching, learners acq uire a second or foreign
seemed to have made effective use of the interaction for learning and those
language as they study subject matter taught in that language. It is imple
who had nor. The fact that learners were sceptical of the benefits of group
mented in a great variety of instructional settings, for example, immersion
and pair-work activities suggestS a need to take account of learners' beliefs
programmes and the content and language-integrated learning (C LIL) pro
about learning (see Chapter 3) and to share with them our reasons for using
these activi ties. grammes in Europe described in Study 8 in Chapter 5. Other educational
programmes such as the European Schools extend this further by offering
instruction in two or more languages in addition ro students' home language.
Interpreting the research
The expectation of this approach is that students can get 'two for one', learn
Research based on the interaction hypo th esis has investigated facrors that ing the subject ma tter content and the language at the sam e time.
contribute to the q uality and quantity of interactions between second
language learners. It has provided some useful information for teach ing. In immersion and CLIL programmes, studen ts choose (or their parents
Certainly, the studies by Long and Porter, Yule and Macdonald, and Storch choose for them) ro receive content-based instruction in a second language.
contribute to a better understanding ofhow to organize group and pair work In many educational situations, however, no other option is available. For
more effectively in the classroom. The Mackey and McDonough studies are example, in some countries, the only language ofschoo ling is the language of
two examples of research that have measured second language development a previous colonial power. In others, educational ma terials are not available
in relation to different aspects of conversational interaction. Mackey's study in all local languages, so one language is chosen as the language ofeducation.
used one-on-one pair-work activities between trained native speakers and In count ries of immigration, students often have access to schooling o nly
non-native speakers focusing on a single grammatical feature in a laboratory through the majority language. Other students may have access to bilingual
context. Thus it is difficult to relate the findings to the kind of interactions education programmes that allow some use ofa language they already know,
that take place in classrooms. The McDonough study helps to fill this gap but the tran sitio n ro the majority language is usually made with in a year
because it is a classroom study that also demonstrates the benefits of inrerac o r two.
tion on second language learning over time.
Research findings
Recently, a num ber of labo rato ry studies have also examined the effects of
In many contexts fo r content-based instruc tion, it is simply assumed that
different interactio nal featu res on speci fic aspects o f second language lear n
students will develop bo th th eir academic skills an d second language ability.
ing over time. Several studies have shown that implicit corrective feedback
Second langu,tge learning in the classroom 173
172 Second Lmguage learning in the classroom
In recent years. researchers ha ve so ugh t to exam ine this assum p tio n mo re they take it back to Eng land . . . '). Roy Lyster ( 1994) foun d that th e use of
critically. the seco nd perso n prono un uous to pol itely address an individual was used so
rarely in classes that even afte r years of immersion instr uction , stu dents did
Study 26: French immersion programmes in Canada nor use it appropriately. Elaine Tarone and Merrill Swain (1995) no ted th at
Research in Canadian French immersion programmes is often cited in learners with only classroom exposure to the language did not have access to
suPPOrt of th e 'Ge t two for one' prop osal. M ost immersion pr ogrammes are th e s pe ech styles that wo uld be ryp ical of int eraction am ong nati ve speake rs
offered in primary and seco ndary schools, but some universities also offer of the same age. Increasingly, it was suggested that subject matter instruction
co nt ent -based instr uctio n th at expands opportunities for students to use needed to be complem ented by instruction that focused o n language form,
their second language in cognitively challenging an d informative courses. includ ing pragmatic features o f th e language. In so me experimen tal studies,
What have the studies shown? learners did benefit from fo rm-focused instruction on particular language
In terms of popular ity and longevity, French im mersion has been a great featu res (see th e ' G et it right in th e end' proposal).
success. Thousands of English-speaking Canadian families have chosen thi s
Study 2 7: Late immersion under stress in Hong Kong
option since its first implementation in the 1960 s (Lambert and Tucker In the 1960s the educational system in Hong Kong moved from one in
1972), both in areas where French is spoken in the wider community an d in whi ch students studied eith er exclusively in English or in Canto nese to one
those where French is rarely heard outside the classroom. Numerous stu d ies in which rhe majority of students stud ied in Cantonese in primary scho o l
have show n that Fren ch immersion stu den ts develop fluen cy, high levels of . (G rades 1-6) an d in Eng lish at seco ndary school (G rades 7- 13). These
listening com p rehe ns ion, and co nfide nce in usin g th eir seco nd langu age. late Eng lish im mersion p rogram mes were pop ular wit h C hinese parents
Th ey also ma intain a level o f success in the ir academic sub jects that is com who wa nte d their ch ildren to succeed p ro fession ally and academically in
pa rable to th at of th eir peer s whose ed uca tio n has been in English (Genesee the inte rnational co mmunity. They were also seen as be ing consis tent with
1987). Over th e years, how ever, ed ucato rs and research ers bega n to exp ress the Ho ng Kong govern ment 's goa l of maintai ni ng a hig h level of Chinese
co nce rn about students' failu re to achieve h igh levels of performan ce in so me
English bilingualism.
aspec ts o f French grammar, even after several years a Hull-day expos ure to the
second language in these programmes. In review ing some o f the resear ch o n teaching and learn in g beh aviours in
lat e English immersion classes in H ong Kong seco ndary sch ool s, Keith
So me researchers argue that the difficulty French immersion learners experi Johnson (1997) raised co nce rns abo ut the ability o f the edu catio nal system
en ce in their L2 production shows that comprehensible input is not enough to meet the demands for such programmes. He noted that students lacked
(H arley and Swa in 1984). They claim that the learners engage in roo little the English proficien cy needed to follow the secon da ry level cu rriculum suc
language production becau se the classes are largel y teacher-cen tr ed. Students cessfully. He also observed teach ers' difficul ties in effecti vely deli vering th e
are observed to speak relatively little and rarely req u ired to give extende d co n tent beca use of limitations in their own En glish profi ciency. He argued
answers. This permits them to operate successfully with the ir incom plet e th at several pedagogic behaviours consrlbured to th e inability o f learners to
knowledge of th e language becau se they are rarely pushed to be more preci se make adeq uate linguistic pro gress in thes e En glish immersion programmes.
or more accurate . \~en stu de nts do speak, com m un icatio n is usually sat One of them was teach er talk that con sisted o f English, Chinese, and 'Mix'
interlanguages are infl uenced by the same first langu age, the same learn ing
env iro nm ent, and the same limi ted contact wit h the target language ours ide Observational classroom studies revealed that Chinese and MLx pred omi
the classroom. Teachers also tend to understand students ' in rerlanguage, so nat ed in th e speech of teachers and that students interacted with the teacher
there is rarely a need to negoriare for m eani ng. Such successful comm unica and with each other in English only in min imal ways . Many students came
tion makes it diffic ult for an individ ual learner to work our how his or her use to the first year of secondary school withour any lite racy skills in Englis h.
of the language d iffers from rhe target language. To compensate for th is, teachers employed a variety of st rategies to help
stu dent s co m p rehend texts . They red uced the vocabulary load . sim plified
An other expla nation for students' lack of p rogress on ce rtain lan guage fea the grammar, encouraged the use of bilingual dictionaries , and provided
tures is their rarity in French im mersion instructio n. Fo r example, Merrill stu d en ts wit h su pplementary no res and charts in Chinese to assist their com
Swain ( 1988) observed that even history lesso ns. where past ten se verbs prehension. Johnson observed tha t, while 'the texts are no t translated, they
might be expected to occ ur, were often delive red in the ' histo rical present' are essentially pr e-tau gh t so that by the time students come to read the texts
(fa t example, 'Th e ships go down to the C aribbean ; th ey pick up sugar and
174 Second kmguage learn ing in the classroom Second language learn ing in the classroom 17 5
for themselves the more able students at least are sufficien tly familiar with be neficial for their d evelop ment of En glish lan guage skills, and Spani sh
the content to be able ro deal with them' ( p.I 77) . Although these strategies speaking students made especially rapid p rog ress in readi ng. O f particul ar
helped students un derstand the content, they may not have helped them importance was the fact that studen ts also continued ro develop their Spanish
learn ro use th e syntactic and discourse structures in the second language to language skills in ways that were not available ro students whose instruction
establish form-meaning relationshi ps. Therefor e it is no t surprising that th e was either in English only or in transitional bilingual classes where there was
st andards of reading in English at age 15 were repo rted to 'be significan tly very lit tle suPPOrt fo r their home language.
lower than those for C hinese. At the sam e time, howeve r, the ed uca tio nal In other d ual immersio n programmes, the number of students an d the
o ut comes for Hong Kon g student s in content su bjects contin ued to be high, amount o f time are distri bu ted differe n tly, often with more time devoted to
com parable to , and in so me areas superior to , achievements in other devel the teaching and lear ning of English, but sometimes with an early emphasis
oped countries. In addition, th e levels of Chinese Ll reading proficiency on the m inority language th at resembles the Canadian French immersion
remained high.
programmes, that is, where the English-speaking students receive nearly all
In spi te of p ro fession al development efforts ro help teachers achieve the d ual th eir early instruction in their second language. Th is ap proach is preferred
goals of language and content instruction , Phil ip H oare an d Stella Kong in settings where it is not possible ro have a substantial number of students
(2008) find that many teachers in th e H ong Kong immersion p rogrammes from the minoriry language. Indeed, a number of different models of d ual
conti nue to have difficulty implementing immersion pedagogy. They attrib imm ersio n have been developed, but they all are based on the p rinciple that
ute this in part ro the pressure teachers feel in a sociery.where performance the co n tin ued develop ment ofa ch ild 's home language is a strong foundation
on examinations is paramount. To ensure that their students do well on the on which ro bu ild second language abil it ies (H oward er al. 2007).
content exams, teachers o ften feel that they must teach in Chinese or in a In recent years, several research reviews ha ve exam ined th e evidence fo r
simplified English that does not give st udents access ro the lang uage th at is di fferent app roaches to educating English language lear ners in the United
appropriate fo r high-level academic work.
States (August and Shanahan 2008; Genesee er al. 2006). The researc h con
Study 2 8: Dual immersion firms better outcomes, in bo th En glish language learning and subject ma tter
In recent years, legislation has lim ited the availability of bilingual education knowledge, for minority lan guage stu d en ts in programmes that support the
for most minoriry language st ude nts in th e Un ited States. In most states, students' home lan guages than fo r those in English only o r 'early exit' bilin
English language learners' education must take place entirely in English , or gual program mes where they receive only to ken opportunities ro co ntinue
with on ly minimal suPPOrt for learning through their first lan guage. As we learning through their home language. Kathryn Lindholm-Leary (200 1)
saw in Chapter I, the result ofthis ap proach is often su bt ractive bilingualism. also found benefits for the majori ty language studen ts in dual immersio n
programmes, where they share the challenges and achievemen ts of second
Children grad ually lose their first lan guage or fail ro develop it for academic
purposes. In addition, they often fall behind in their academic work because language learn ing with mi noriry lan guage students. She suggests that this
they do not yet have the English language skills needed for dealing with th e app roach 'has th e potential to eradicate the negative status of bilingualism
grade-level subject matter. in the U S' (p.1).
Some jurisdictions allow 'dual immersio n' as an exception to the Strict Study 29: Inuit children in content-basedprogrammes
enforcement ofins truction thro ugh English on ly. In dual immersion, minor In an aboriginal com m un iry in Q uebec, Canada, N ina Spada and Patsy
iry language studen ts learn English in classro oms whe re English-speaking Lightbown (2002) observed the teaching and learning of school su bjects and
children also learn the minoriry language students' home lan guage. Patsy language with Inui t ch ildren. The children had been educate d in their first
Lighrbown (2007) observed classroom interaction and learning outcomes language, Inukritut, from kindergarten ro Grade 2 (aged 5-7). Then, except
in a school where an equ al number of native English- and native Spanish for occasional lessons in Inuit culture, their education was in one of Canada's
speaki ng students shared the classrooms. Starting in kindergarten, half th eir official langu ages, Fren ch or Eng lish . Nearly all students had so me d ifficulry
instruction was delivered in English by an English native speaker and half in coping with sub ject matter instruction in thei r second language.
Spanish by a na tive speaker of that lang uage. Teachers coo rdinated closely In a case study o f one Frenc h secondary-level class, the y observed instr uc
to ensure tha t the su bject rna rre r inst ructio n in th e two languages was co m tional activi ties, analysed instr uctional materials, and assessed stu dent s'
plement ar y rath er than red und anr. Stude nts' performance on a variery of ab iliry ro un derstand and to pro d uce wri tten French. In the observation
meas ures administered through Grade 3 showed that the programme was data from a social studies lesso n, it was evident that the teacher had to work
very hard co help st ude nt s understand a text o n beluga whales. H e did this delay in co mi ng co g rips with sc hooling can have lasti ng effects, as we saw in
in many ways-by paraphrasing, repeating, sim plifying, checking for com th e discussion or su btractive bilingualism in C hapter I . Majority language
prehension, gestures, etc . Despite his efforts it was clea r that most students students in immersion programmes-in Canada and in Hong Kong-seem
to do well in learning subject matter, an d it is also no teworthy that they
understood very little of the te-xt. In their French language classes, these same
st uden ts also lacked the terminology they needed co talk about grammatical recei ve a substantial amount of subject matter instruction through their
gende r in relati on to adje cti ve agreement. first language over the full co urse of their acade m ic careers. Similarly. dual
immersion programmes allow students from each language group to con
\'V'hen the students' performance on a wide range of measures was examined tinue development of the home language and to co ntin ue learning subject
to assess their knowledge of French (fo r example, vocabulary recognition , matter content in that language. However. although students in content
reading comprehension, writing) . it was evident that the students did not based language instruction are able to communicate with so me fluen cy in th e
have the French language skills they needed to cope with the demands of seco nd language. they often fall short of the high levels of linguiscic accuracy
typical secondary-level instruction. Furthermore. even though many of the that their years of schooling in the language might predict.
stude nts were able to speak French informally outside class, their oral abili
ties we re limited when th ey had to di scu ss more co m plex academic subject In recent years, propo ne nts of co nren t-based in srructi on have stressed the
matter. As we saw in Chapter 1. teachers are sometimes misled by students' need co recall that content-based language teaching is still language teaching.
ab ility to use th e language in informal settings, concluding that their aca For exam ple. jana Echevarria. Mary Ellen Vogt, and Deborah Short (2004)
demic difficulties co uld not be due co lan gu age problem s. have done research and developed teacher education pro grammes th at sh ow
the effect iveness of lesso ns t har have bo t h corue n r o bjectives an d lan guage
The stu de nts' lackor age-a p prop riate acad em ic Frenc h is a serio us p roblem. objectives.
Solving it wi ll involve complex educat ional. social . an d cultu ral quest ions.
One pedagogical element that mi ght co nt rib u te to a so lu tion is a bett er
balance be tween lang uage and sub ject matter instr uc tio n. focu sin g o n th e
5 Teach what is teachable
language that the students need to suc cee d in schoo l. An other possibiliry is Manfred Pienemann ( 1988) and his co lleagues have tried to explain why it
that furthe r development of the learners' L 1 literacy would better prepare often seems that some things can be taught successfull y whereas other th ings .
them for seco nd lan guage and subject matter learning. There is another good eve n after extensive or intensive teaching. seem to remain unacquired. Their
reason to support students' development of Inuktirur. There are increasing research provides evidence that some linguistic str uct u res, for exam ple, basic
co ncern s that Inuktirut will be lost as future generations shift to English or word order in sentences (bo th sim ple and complex) develop along a predict
French as their preferred language. An educational system that encourages able developmental path . These are labelled developmental features. The
the de velopment of both firSt and seco nd languages may ensure the su rvival developmental stages of English questions that we saw in C hapter 2 are based
or this heritage language (Taylo r. Caron, and McAlpine 2000). on this research. According to Pienernann (1988) an y attempt to teach a
Stage 4 word-order pattern to learners at Stage 1 will not work because learn
Interpreting the research ers have to pass through Stage 2 and ger to Stage 3 before they are read y to
Cont ent-based language teaching has many advantages. In general . it acquire what is at Stage 4. As we saw in 'G et it right from the beginning',
in creases the amount of time for learners co be exposed to the new language. students may produce certain struc tures after they have been taught them
It creates a genuine need to communicate. motivating students to acquire in class, but cease to use them later because they are not fully integrated into
language in order to understand the content. For older students , there is the their interlanguage systems . The underlying cause of the stages has no t been
or
advantage content that is cog nitively challenging and interesting in a way fully explained, but processabiliry theory (see Chapter 4) suggests that they
may be based at least in part on learners' developing ability to notice and
that is often missing in foreign language instruction, especially where lessons
are designed around particular grammatical forms. remember elements in the st ream of speech they hear.
N everth eless. there are also some problems with content-based instruction. Resear chers supporting this view also cla im that certain other aspects of
Our research with Inuit children adds further evidence co Jim C ummins' lan guage -for example, individual vocabulary items-can be taught at any
( 19 84) claim that students need 5-7 years before their ability co use the time. Learners' acquisition of these variational features appears to depend on
la nguage for cognitively challenging academic material has reached an age facto rs su ch as m otivation . th e learners' sense o f identity. language aptitude ,
and th e quality of insrrucrion, including how learners' identi ties and culrures Example 8
are acknowledged in the classroom. (Interviewing each other abo ut house pr eferen ces)
In Example 6 below, we see a teacher trying to help students with the word 5 I Is yo ur favourite house is a split-level?
order of questio ns. The st udents seem to know what the teacher means, but 52 Yes.
th e level oflanguage the teacher is offering them is beyond their current stage T You're saying 'is' twO times dear. 'Is your favo urite house a spl it-le vel?'
of develop men t. Students are asking Stage 3 q uestio ns, whic h th e teach er 5I A sp lit-level.
recasts as Stage 5 questions . Th e students react by simply answering th e ques T O K.
tion or acce pting th e teacher's fo rm ulati on.
In Example 9 the student asks a Stage 3 question, and th e teacher provides a
Example 6 Stage 4 correction that the student imitates. The interaction suggestS that the
Students in an intensive ESL class (l 1-12-year-old French speakers) inter student is almost ready to begin producing Stage 4 q uestions. Note, however,
viewing a stude nt who had been in the same class in a previous year (see that the student does not imitate the possessive 's, som eth in g tha t French.
s I Mylene, where you pur your 'Kid of the Week' pos ter?
Example 9
52 In m y room.
with recasts than lear ners at less advanced stages of question dev elopment
Interpreting the research
('unr ead ies'). The results revealed mat me ' readies' in the interaction plus
The resul ts of these studies sugges t that targeting instructional or interac
recasts group improved mo re tha n me ' read ies' in th e interaction without
tio nal in pu t to lear ners wh en they are deve lopmen tally ready to progress
recasts group. However. the 'un read ies' who were exposed co recasts did not
further in the second language can be . beneficial. H oweve r, other factor s
sho w more rapid improvement than those who were not exposed co recasts.
such as rype of inpur andfirst language influence can interact with learners'
develo pment al readiness in co m plex ways. If we compare the rypes o f instrue
Study 32: Developmental stage andfirst language influence
rional/ in teracrio nal in pu t acro ss the three studies , Pienemann provided th e
N ina Spada and Patsy Lighcbown ( 1999) have also in vestigated the acqu i
most exp licit instruction to learners who were both ' ready' and 'u nready' .
sitio n of questions in relation to learners' developmental ' read iness'.
The results showed tha t learners who were 'ready' moved into the next stage
French-speaking st udents (aged 11-12) in int ensive ESL classes received
of develo pment, whereas learners who were not ' ready' did not. The results
high-frequency exposure to q uestion forms that were one or rwo stages
of th e Mackey and Philp study also offer some sup port for m e teachabiliry
beyo nd thei r develop mental level. Learners who were judged on oral pre
hypothesis but reveal th at developmental readiness is not the only predictor
tests (Q be at Stage 2 or 3 were given h igh freque ncy exposure co Stage 4 an d
of success. Th e fact th at m e 'readies' be nefited mo re from recasts tha n the
5 questio ns in th e inst ruc tio nal in put.
'un read ies' sugges ts th at the rype of instru ctio nal/ interactio nal in put is also
The materials that contained the more advanced question forms were
im poreant. The Spada and Ligh tbown study shows how me learn ers' first lan
designed (Q engage th e learn ers mainly in com prehension practice. There was
guage may interact with developmental read iness in Contributing to learni ng
no student producti on an d th us no co rrective feed back, nor was th ere any
outcomes. Furthermore, in th at stu dy there was no explicit instructio n on
exp licit instruction o n q uestion format ion. The researchers wanted co know
questions. Learners we re sim p ly exposed to a high freque ncy of correctly
whethe r Stage 3 learn ers (i.e, those co nside red co be developmentally ' ready ')
formed h igh er-stage questions in the in put. Thus, th ey received increased
would be nefi t mo re from the hig h-frequency exposure (Q Stage 4 and 5 q ues
'exp os ure' bu t no ' instr uction' , and, in the en d, they did not show as much
tio ns than the Stage 2 learn ers, who we re not yet devel opmentally ' ready'.
developme ntal change as learners wh o received foc used instruc tion .
At first glance, thi s research seems ro con tradict Pien ernan n's claim th at Advocates of this p ro posal also agree wirh advocates of the 'Teach wha t is
learners should be taught what is 'next'. However, it is also possible that teachable' view that so me rhings can no t be taught if rhe teach ing fails to
the de velopmental parhs of differenr linguist ic features are based on diffe r take the student's readiness (stage of development) in to account. This pro
en t sorts of processing ab ilities. For example. Carherine Doughty (1 99 1) posal differs from rhe 'Teach what is teachable' proposal, ho wever, in th at it
suggested that once learners have learned ro use relative clauses in one po si em phasizes rhe idea that some aspects of language must be taught and may
tio n (usually rhe subject posi tion), rhere is no constraint on their abili ty ro need ':0 be taught qu ite explicitly, There are a number of siruatio ns in wh ich
learn rhe ot hers. What aU rhe Studies of relative clause teaching and learning guidance- form-focused ins truction or corrective feedback-is expected to
have in common is that learners acquire th e relative clauses in an order very be especially important. For example, when lear ners in a class share the same
similar to rhe accessibili ty hierarchy. That is, whe ther or not they learn what first language, the y will make errors that are partly rhe result of transfer from
is taught, th ey make progress by lear n ing subject, rhen direct object, then thar shared Ianguage. Because rhe errors are not likely ro lead to any kind of
indirect object, and so on.
communication breakdown, it will be virt ually impossib le for learners to
Th e 'Teach what is teachable' position is of great porenrial interest ro syl discover the er rors on their own .
lab us p lan n ers as well as teachers. However, it must be emphasized that a Examples 10 , II , and 12 are taken from a classroom where a group o f 12-y ear
description of a learner's develop m en tal path is not in itsel f a tem plate for a old French speakers are .Iearning Engl ish . In Example 10, they are engaged
syllabus. Th ere are n umerous practical reaso ns for th is, not least rhe fact th at in an activity where the words in senrences are reo rdered ro form new sen
o nly a small n umber of language features hav e been described in term s of a tences. Th e following sentence has been placed o n the board : 'So metim es my
developmen tal seq uence. While Pienernann's wo rk o n processability rheory mother makes good cakes '.
(see Chapter 4) provides insights into the pri nciples that ma y make so m e
features more difficult than others, those pri nciples are not easily tran slated Example 10
into instructional sequences. T Anorher p lace ro put our adv erb ?
As Patsy Lighrbown ( 1998 ) has suggested, the 'Teach what is teachable' 5I After makes?
researc h is irnporranr primarily for helping teachers understand why Stu T After makes.
de nts don't always learn wha t they are taugh t- at least not immediately. 52 Before good?
The research also shows tha t instruction on language th at is 'too ad vanced' T My mother make s sometimes good cakes.
ma y still be helpful by providing learners with samples oflanguage that they 53 No .
will be able ro inco rporate into their inrerlanguage when the time is right. T No , we can't do th at . It sounds yucky.
H owever, many other factors need ro be taken into consideration in choosing 53 Yucky!
language feat ures ro focus on. We will return ro rhis point after we discuss rhe T Disgusting. Horrible. Right?
final proposal for language teaching, 'Get it right in rhe end .' 54 Horrible!
This is hardly a typ ical grammar lesson! "An d yet the students' at ten tion is
6 Get it right in the end being drawn to an error that virtually all of them make in English.
Proponents of rhe 'G et it righ t in rhe end' proposal recognize an important Proponents of 'Get it righ t in rhe end' argue that what learners focus on
role for form-focused instruction, bur rhey do no t assume that everyth ing can eventually lead to changes in the ir interlanguage systems, not JUSt ro
has to be ta ught. Like advocates of rhe 'Let's talk', 'Two for one', and rh e 'JUSt an appearance of change. However, rhe supporters o f this p roposal do not
listen ... and read ' positions, the y h ave co ncl ude d tha t ma ny lang uag e fea claim that focusing on particular language points will prevent learners from
rures-from pro n un ciation ro vocabulary and grammar-will be acquired maki ng errors or that they will begin using a fo rm as soon as it is taught.
narurally if learners have adequate exposure ro rhe language and a motivation Rather, they sugges t that th e focused instruction will allow learners to notice
to learn. Thus, while they view co m p rehensio n-based, conrenr-based , task the target fearu res in subsequent input and interaction.
based, or other types of essentially meaning -focused instructio n as crucial Form -focused instruction as it is understood in th is position does not always
for language learn ing, they hypothesize th at learners will do better if they involve rnecaiinguisric explana tions, nor are learners expected to be able to
also have access ro some form- focus ed ins tructio n. They argue that learners exp lain wh y so me thing is righ t or wrong. They claim simply that rhe learners
will benefit in terms ofb orh efficien cy of rhei r learni ng an d rhe level of p ro need to no tice how rhei r language use differs from that of a more proficient
ficien cy rhey will eventually reach. speaker. As we will see in the examples below, teachers who work in rhis
St?condLanguage learning in the classroom 185
184 Second Language learn ing In the classroom
approach loo k for the right moment to crea te increased awareness on the Research findings
A grea t deal of research has examined issues relat ed to this pro posal. This
parr of the learner-ideally, at a time whe n the learner is m otivat ed to say
somethi ng an d wan ts to say it as d earl y and correctly as possi ble. includes bo th descriptive and expe rime ntal studies .
In descrip tive studies invo lv ing almost 1,000 students in 33 classes, Patsy
51 Because it's he r shoes ,
Lighcbown and Nina Spada 0990 , 1994) observed that teachers rarel y
As we saw in Chapter 4, Fren ch-s peaking learn ers of English ha ve di fficu lty focused on language form. The emphasis of the teaching was o n act ivities
with 'his' and 'h er' beca use French possessives use the gram matical gt nder of that focu sed o n meaning rather than form, opportunities for spontaneous
t he objec t possessed rather th an th e na tural ge nde r o f the possessor in select interactio n, a nd the provision o f rich an d varie d co mp rehens ible input. In
ing the appropriate possessive form. The teacher is aware of th is and-b riefly, the se classes , learners developed good listening co m p rehe nsio n and co m m u
without interrupting the activity-helps the learners no tice the correct form . nicative co nfide nce in En glish . H owever. th ey co nt inued to have probl em s
with linguistic accuracy and complexity.
Example 12
lTh e stu dents are playing 'h ide and seek ' with a doll in a doll 's house, as king In expe riment al stu d ies w ith a smaller number o f classes, th e effect s of form
questions until they find ou t where 'G eo rge' is hiding. Although a model tor focu sed instructio n and corrective feedback were exam ined with respect to
correct q uestions has been written o n the board, th e game becomes quite adverb placeme nt and question formation . In the first study, Lydia \'V'hite
lively a nd stu de nts spo ntaneously ask q uestio ns that reflect the ir inrerlan selec ted adve rb placeme nt for in vestigatio n because of the diffe rences
guage srage .) between English and French that have already been discussed (see Chapter 2
and Study 17 in 'Just listen . . . and read') . The hypothes is was that learn ers
s I Is Geo rge is in the living room? wo uld pe rsist in usin g adverb placeme nt rules co nsiste nt with Frenc h (th eir
T You said 'is' two ti mes, dear. Listen to you-you said 'Is George is first lan guage) if they were no t exp licitly told how rules for adverb place
in?' Look o n the board. 'Is George in the ' and then yo u say th e nam e ment differ in English and French. Questio ns we re selec ted fo r th e second
of the ro o m. study beca use th ey h ave been extensively inves tigated in the literat ure and
51 Is G eor ge in th e living roo m?
(im m edi ately following ins tru ctio n and six weeks later). In me follow-up feed back . A n um ber of studies have exp lo red the quest ion of how this can
tests a year later, however, me gains made by m e learners who had rece ived best be accomplished .
me adverb instructio n had disappeared and their perfo rmance on this Struc
tu re was like mat of un in structed learn ers (White 199 1). Study 34: Focusing on gender in French immersion
Birgit Harley (1998) exam ined the effects of instruction with young children
In th e question study me instr uct ed gro up also mad e signiflcan dy greater
in French immersio n programmes. Six classes of Grade 2 children (7-8 years
gains than m e uninstructed gro up on th e written tasks im mediately follow
old) were given focused instruction on a language feature chat is kn own to be
in g instr uctio n. Fur thermore, they maintained their level of kn owled ge on
a persistent problem for French immersion studenrs-i-grammarical gen der.
later testi ng (six weeks and six months after instruction). Th e instruction also
For 20 minutes a day over a five-week period these children carried our many
contrib uted to improvement in or al performance that was sustained over
activities based on children's games (for example, 'I spy ') m at were modified to
time (White, Spada, Ligh rbown , and Ranta 199 1).
draw their attention to gender distinctions and which required them to choose
Th e difference in long- ter m efiecrs of the two studies m ay be d ue to a differ between femin ine and masculi ne ar ticles (une or un, la o r Ie) . Srudenrs were
ence in the availability of me target forms in me classroom input to which also taught how certain noun endings provide clues about gender (for example,
learners were exposed. Analysis of classroom language showed mat adverbs -ette in la bicyclette for feminine, and -eau in le bateau for masculine).
were extremely rare in classroo m spee ch, giving learne rs little opportuni ty
The students were pr e-tested on th eir knowledge of gram matical gender via
to mai ntain their: newly-acquired knowledg e th ro ugh continued expo sure
listening and speaking tests before th e ins truction began and the same rests
and use. In contrast, mere w.e re hu ndreds of op porrunlries to hear and use
were administered im m ediately after instr uction and then again five months
ques tio ns every day in m e classroo m . Once lear ners ha d been given some
later. Learners w ho received instruction were much better at recognizing an d
foc used instruction , it seems they were able to continue to advance in their
producing accurate gen der distinctions for familiar nouns than those who
knowledge and use of questions (Spada and Llghrbown 1993 ).
did no r receive instruction. However, the instruction did not enable learn ers
In several of the studies carried out in intensive ESL p rogramm es, there is to generalize their learning co new nouns . Harley's interpretation of th is is
evidence of me strong influence of me learner's first language on their second th at coo much new vocabulary was introduced in the lat er teaching acti vities
language dev elopment. In Stu dy 32 , we described the ten den cy of intensive and this meant chat teach ers spent more tim e teach ing the meaning ofwords
ESL learners to reject in versio n in qu estions when the su bject is a no un but to than the noun en dings and their relationship co gender. Therefore, 'th e in put
accept inversion wh en the subject is a pronoun, consistent with th eir first lan on noun endings was simply no t available in su fficient quantity and intensity
guage. The influence of the learners' first lan guage in thei r acquisitio n of the for th e majority o f students to establish me predictive relevan ce of the noun
possessive determ in ers ' his' and 'her' was obse rved wi th this group oflearners en dings in questio n' (p. 169).
(see Chapter 2 an d Study 18). This led to me ques tion of whether form
focu sed instructi on mat includes expli cit contrastive information about how Study 35: Focusing on sociolinguisticfonns in French immersion
the first and second language diffe r wo uld help in th eir developme nt ofques Roy Lyster (994) exam in ed me effects of form-focused instruction on th e
tion formation and possessive determ ine rs. In a study to expl ore th is, learners knowledge and use of sociolinguistic style variations in three classes of Grade
who received instruction on possessive de termi ners improved mo re in their 8 French immersion stu den ts (about 13 years old ). One of the mai n features
knowledge and use of this fea tu re than did learners who received instruction examined in his study was me d istinctio n between the use of second person
on question forms . This finding appeared to be related to differences between pronouns tu and V0 11S. In addressing an individual, tu is used co indicate infor
the fo rm- mean ing connections of these two features. That is, a misused pos mality and familiarity while uous is used as a marker of respectful po liteness , or
sessive determiner ('He's going ho me with her mother') is more likely to lead social distance between speakers. Prior co instruction, immediately after, and
to a co mmunication breakdown than an ill-forme d question (for example, again one month later, the learners were tested on their ability co produce and
'Where he 's going?') . Res ults like th ese po in t to the importance of cons ide r recognize these forms (in addition co ochers) in appropriate con texts.
ing how instruction ma y affect language features in different ways (Spad a, The instruction cook place for abo ut 12 hours over a five-week period .
Lighrbown, and White 2005 ; White 2008). During th is time, students in rhe experimental classes were given explicit
As we saw in th e disc ussion of the ' G et two for one' proposal, there is instruction and engaged in guided p ractice activities chat included role-plays
growin g evide nce that learners in COntent-based programmes such as French in a variety of form al an d informal co ntexts and corrective feedback fro m
immersio n ne ed m ore opp ortu niti es to focus on form an d receive corrective teachers and peers . Students in the two comparis on classes co ntin ued with
their regu lar instruction witho ut any focused teac hing or guided practice
188 Second language learning in the classroom Second language learning in the classroom 189
in using soci olin gui sticalJ y appropriate forms. On the immediate post -test , expe rime nt al group received the ins tr uction bu t no co rrec tive feed back, and
learners in the experimental classes performed significantly better than lear n th e co m p ar ison group received neither instruction nor corrective feedback.
ers in the comparison classes on both written and oral production tasks and All groups continued their regular French immersion p rogram me of con
the multiple-choice test, and these benefits were maintained when learners tent-based instruction chrougho ut the srudy and they were all tested before
were tested a month later. the instructional treatment, immediately after, and again three monchs later.
Study 36: Focusing on verbfonns in content-based science classrooms O n the post-tests all three experimental groups (i.e. chose who received
Catherine Doughry and Elizabeth Varela ( 1998) carried OUt a study with a instruction) were significanrly more accurate chan the comparison group
group ofESL learners in their science classes. One class ofmiddle-school stu in assigning grammatical gender. In addition, the instr uctio n + prompts
den ts (11-14 years old) from a variety offirst language backgrounds received gro up did sign ifican rly better than the instruction + recasts group on the
corrective feedback on past tense and conditional verb forms in English. written measures. However, there were no significant differences between
For several weeks, while studenrs were engaged in oral and written work the three experimental groups in terms of/earners' performance on the oral
related co a series of science reports, me teacher provided corrective feed tasks. Lyster interprets th is finding as a task effect. That is', because of the
back on th eir errors in past tense and conditional forms-both explicitly and time-co nsumi ng nature of oral tasks, o n ly a randomly selected su b-sam p le
im plicirly. Students' abiliry CO use chese forms was assessed before and after of students participated in chis part of the Study. These students met with
the experimental period and again rwo months later. Their performance was the researcher in three intensive on e-on-one sessions. In order to ensure the
co m pared co that of a gro up o f Stu dents who were in another scie nce class accuracy of the d ata , the researcher en couraged students co speak as clearl y as
doing [he sam e scie nce repo rts but w ho did not receive co rrective feedback possi ble because previou s research had shown th at learners so metimes used
on the verb forms . a 'hybrid ar ticle' tha t cou ld be inter pret ed as eit he r masculin e or fem inine.
Th is em phasis o n th e clear articulation of articles provided all learne rs with
Studen ts who received the co rrective feed back mad e mo re prog ress in using individ ualize d arte n tion o n the target featu re an d th us may be th e reaso n
past an d co ndi tio nal forms th an the co m par ison gro up both im me diat ely why all three gro ups perfo rmed simi larly on th e oral measure.
aft er the period of focused feedback and two monrhs later. Their progress
was assessed in terms o f both increased accuracy and the presence of inter Study 38: Focus on form through collaborative dialogue
language forms chat showed students were doing more than repeating forms Motivated by sociocultural theory and the idea chat language learning occurs
th ey had heard. in dialogue, Merrill Swain and Sharon Lapkin (2002) observed the language
development of two Grade 7 French immersion students as the y wrote a
Study 37: Recasts andprompts in French immersion classrooms Story collaboratively. Later, in a 'no ticing' activity; the students compared
In C hapter 5, we saw som e o f Roy Lyster's descriptive research on the dif wh at they had written with a reformulated version of the Sto ry. Th e students
ferent types of co rrective feedback provided by teachers in Canadian French also took part in a stimulated recal l of their noticing activity. Swain and
immersion programmes and learners' immediate responses (uptake) co mat Lapkin wanted to find out what students noticed about differences berween
feedback. More recently, Lyster (2 004) explo red the effecrs ofform-focused thei r original versio n and the reformulated one and whecher they made
instruction and feedback rype on second language learning in an experimen revisions to their original stories based on their collaborative talk about the
tal study with Grade 5 srudenrs in French immersion classes. There were reformulated version ,
three experimental groups and a comparison group. The experimenral groups
received approximately nine hours of explici t instruction over a five-week The talk that learners produced in all phases of the researc h was recorded,
period, d uring which their attention was drawn to grammatical gender and transcribed and coded for language-related episodes (LREs) , 'any part of the
the fact that word endings can give a clue co grammatical gender in French dialogue where learners talk about the lang uage they produced, and reflect
(see Study 34). Students in rwo of me experimental groups also received cor on their language use' (p. 292). An excerpt of the learners' collaborative talk
rective feedbac k in the form ofeither recasts or prompts when they produced from this study is presented in Chapter 5, Communication Task B. The LREs
erro rs in grammatical gender. These feedback types differ in that recasts were coded in terms of whether they focused on lexical, grammatical, or dis
provide learners wi th the correct model , whereas promprs signal the need course features . The researchers used the original story that the rwo learners
for a correcrion and requi re the student co produce the target form through created together as a pre -test and the stories chat each learner constructed as
clarification requests, elicitation. and metalinguisric clues (see Chapter 5 a post-tese. Both learners were much mo re accurate on the POSt-test version
for definitions and examples of these different types of feedback). The chird of the Story. The researchers conclude that the multiple opportunities for
190 Second language learning in the classroom
Second language learn ing in the classroom 191
learners to engage in collaborative talk on the language features in question Study 40: The timing ofform-focused instruction
led them to a greater understanding of their correct use.
Nina Spada and her research team carried out a study to exam ine whether
Study 39: Focus on form in task-based instruction there may be a better time in the instructional sequence to draw learners'
In a descri ptive study investigating the importance of th e teacher's role in attention to form (Spada et al. 2012). Two classes of intermediate-level
task- based instruction, Virginia Sarnuda (2001) explored ways of guiding adult ESL learners were provided with 12 hours of instruction that differed
ad ult E5L lear ners' attention to form-meaning relationships by focusing on in terms of whether attention to form was em bedd ed in communicative
expressions of possibility and probability (fo r example, 'migh t', 'could', 'it's activities or separated from communicative prac tice. They are referred to as
possible'). In a task design that took learners through a 'meaning to form to integrated and isolated form-focused instruction. The target feature was the
meaning progression', learners were first asked to work in groups to speculate passive construction and learners were tested on their knowledge of it before
on the identity ofan unknown person (for example, age, gender, occupation) instruction, immediately after instruction. and again three weeks later.
by looking at a set ofobje cts thought to have come from that person's pocket. A second question motivating the research was whether the rwo types of
In carrying out th is task, learners were observed to produce expressions of instruction might lead to different kinds of L2 knowledge. This question
p robability and possibility such as 'It's possible that he smokes' and 'maybe was informed by transfer-appropriate processing theory and the idea that
it's a girl' , but few instances of modal auxiliaries (for example , 'must', 'may') we are more likely to remember something we have learned if the cogni
were used . tive processes that are activated during the learning process are the same as
In the second phase of the task, the students were asked to come together as a those activated during retrieval (see Chapter 4): Thus , the researchers were
who le group to tell each o ther what they had decided. During this phase, the interested in whether learners who obtained their knowledge of the passive
teacher acted as a co-communicator and maintained the focus on meaning while participating in com m un icative interactio n (i.e. integrared FFI) were
but gtadually shifted to form by using the language that the lear ners had better at retrieving that knowledge on an oral communication task than the
produced on their ow n and ptoviding them with alternative ways ofexpress learners who received isolated FFI. Similarly, they wanted to explore whether
ing uncertainty. Initially, this was done implicidy. For example if a lear ner learners who obtained thei r knowledge of the passive structure in grammar
said something like 'We th ink uh 50 per cent he smokes', the teacher said activities that were separated fro m communicative practice (i.e. isolate d FFI)
'So you're not certain that he smokes?' After each group had presented, the were better at retr ieving their knowledge on a written grammar test than the
teacher provided a more explicit focus. She drew the learners' attention to learn ers who received integrated FFI.
other ways of expressing possibility an d probability by overtly talking about Learners in both the integrated and isolated FFI classes improved signifi
language form as shown in the excerp t below (p. 13 1). cantly on both language measures over time. The find ings also revealed some
ST Businessman support for TAP in that learners who received integrated FFI outper formed
T Businessman ninety? OK So you're 90 per cent certain he's a the isolated FFI learners on the oral communication task and the learn
businessman, right? Here's another way to say this. You think it's 90 ers who received isolated FFI outperformed the integrated learners on the
per cem certain. so you chink he must be a businessman. He must be written grammar resr.
a businessman (writes it on the board) . So this (points to 'must be' on The overall results of this study point to the complementarity of the two
board) is showing how certain how sure you are. Not 100 per cent, types of ins truction, likely due to the fact that they both provide a focus on
bu t almost 100 per cent. 90 per cent. form and meaning, albeit at different times.
In the final stage of the task, the students prepared and presented a posrer
based on their conclusions about the identity of the unknown person to ACTIVITY Match pedagogical activities with teaching
the whole class. During this time, the teacher responded to the content and proposals
not the form of (heir work. When the researcher examined the differences
Below are brief descriptions of 12 pedagogical activities. Match each activity
between expressions of probability and possibility that the students used in
with the teaching proposal it represents and explain how you reached that
the firSt stage of this task and compared it with the final stage , there was evi
conclusion. For example, an activity such as 'Fill in the blanks with the correct
dence of improvement in that many more instances of modal auxil iaries were
present in the learners' speech. form of the verb' represents the 'Get it right from the beginning' proposal
because such grammatical exercises are typical of the grammar translation
approach with its emphasis on rule learning and accuracy. Keep in mind
192 Second language Learn ing in th e classroom Second language Learning in the classroom 193
tha t in some cases. an activity is compatible with more than one teaching possessive determi ne rs than q uestio ns becau se the re is a stro nge r fo rm
prop o sal.For exa mple, if the se ntences in the 'Fill in the blanks' activity came meaning connection with possessive determiners man with questions.
from an earlier draft of a letter w ritten for a co mmunicative activity.it might
Other language featu res for which form-focused instruction may playa crucial
be co nsist ent with the 'Get it right in the end ' proposal because it int egra tes
role are those mat are influenced by the learn ers' first language, particularly
attention to language form in a meaning-based activity.
wh en there are misleading similarities between L1 and U. Th e difficulty may
I Role-playa conversation between a travel agent and a tourist.
be inc reased in second language classrooms w here learn ers share me same first
2 Memorize a dialogue abo ut buying airline tickets.
language and reinforce each other's first language-based errors.
3 Underline the past tense ver bs while rea ding a sto ry.
Finally: the rules associated with some language features are more complex
than others. For example, the article system in English is both complex and
4 Arrange illustrations in the co rrect sequence after listening to a st o r y.
abst ract and notoriously difficult to teach and learn. Thus, learners may be
S W ork with a partner to write a story based on a cartoon strip.
better off learning abo u t articles via exposure in the input. On the o th er
6 Rearrange a set of scrambled words to form correct questions.
hand a simple 'rule of thumb' suc h as 'put an -$ at the end of a noun to make
it plural' may be a better target fo r instruction. In a recent meta-analysis of
7 Deba te or discuss a topic that was featured in a newspape r ar ticle.
th e effecss of type of in struction on 'co m plex' and 'sim ple' language features,
S Watch an episode of Sesame Street.
however, Spada and Tom ita (20 10) report that explicit instruction promoted
9 Demonstrate and describe the steps in a science experiment.
learningJo r both types o flang uage features.
10 Interview a mystery guest and try to discover his or he r occupation.
Research o n integrated and iso lat ed FFI is a rern inde r that the tim in g o f
stu dy w ith adult ESL learners is a good exam ple of integ rated FFI, illust rat
different kinds ofL2 knowledge is intrigu ing and resonates with th e experi
The overall results of the studies described above provide support for me
that explicit rule -based grammar teaching without co m m un icative practice
There is also co m pellin g evidence that more explicit attention to form is par
tion. These differences in L2 kn owledge have been vario usly referred to as
results also show, however, that the effects of instruction are not always
Some theo rists and researchers claim rha t L2 instruction can lead to exp licit
knowledge only. Furthermore, they argue tha t the results from meta-analyses
a linguis tic feature in the regular classroom input after the experimental treat
show ing positive effects for L2 instructio n are due to the fact that the rests used
merit ends .
to assess learn e rs' progress in me majority of studies have measured expl icit
We have also seen that form-focused instruction may be more effective with
knowledge using, for example, disc rete-poim grammar tests. Fo rt unat ely,
some language features than with others. For example, the successful lea rn
recent research has included a grea ter variety o f language measures to rap
ing of the tul uous distinction in Lyster's (1994 ) study could be d ue to the
in to learn ers' intuitive L2 knowledge such as oral communication tasks and
tant vocabulary items and thus may have been less d ifficul t to learn th an
without having tim e to 'th ink abou t it'. Nonetheless, we won't have a clear
syntactic features that affect meaning in less obvious ways. In the intensive
answer to th e question ofwhat type ofknowledge resul ts from L2 instruction
ESL research, learn ers may have been more successful after instruction on
until valid and reliable tests of both im plicit and explicit knowledge are used
in a larger num be r of studies (Ellis er al. 20 09).
Assessing the proposals
Although there is still much work to do , it seem s evident that proposals rep
Similar issues have been raised about research on corrective feedb ack but the
resenting an almost exclusive focus on form or those representing an almost
central focus of th is wo rk has been on investigating whe the r certain types of
exclusive focus on meaning alone can not be recommended. Approaches that
cor rective feedb ack are mo re effective than oth ers. The results fro m Lyster's
provid e atten tion to form wi thi n communicative and con tent -based int erac
stu dy in French im me rsion pro grammes suggests that learners benefit more
tio n receive the most support from classroo m research.
from feedback th at pushes th em to self-correct (i.e. p ro m pts ) th an from feed
bac k th at p rovides the correct form (Le. recasts). Research in o ther conte xts, We kn ow that some exceptionally gifi:ed learne rs will succeed in second
however, has p roduced diffe rent results . Fo r example, the majo rity ofla bora lang uage learn ing regardless of the teaching meth od . In the schools of the
tory st udi es of co rrective feed back report benefits for recasts over o the r types world, grammar translation is no dou bt the most widely applied method
o f corrective feed back includi ng prompts (Mackey and Goo 20 07). Th ese an d mo st of us have met individuals whose advanced p roficiency in a fore ign
conflict ing find ings are likely related to differen ces in context-the labora language develo ped Out o f th eir experience in such classes. Simila rly, audio
to ry is a mote controlled environment than the classroo m , whe re there are lingual instruction has produced highly p roficient second language speakers.
competing dem ands on lear ners' attention. As a resul t, learners may notice However , we also know-from personal experience and research findings
certain types of feedback in th e one-on-one laborato ry int eractions more that th ese methods leave many learners frustrated an d unable to participate
than they do in the classroo m in communicative or COntent-based classes, in ordi nary con versations, even mer years o f classes. G rammar tra nslation
where the primary focus is on mean ing. and audiolingual approaches will continue to be used, but th e evide nce sug
gests that 'Get it righ t from the beginning' does no t correspo nd to th e way
As d iscussed in Chapter 5, the specifi c pedagog ical activity in which correc
the majority of successful second language learners have acq uired their pro
tive feed back is provided also plays an im po rtan t role in terms o f whe ther
ficiency. On the other hand, in throwing out co ntrast ive analysis , feedback
learn ers recognize it as corrective feedback. Th e tim ing o f corrective feed back
on error, and rnetalinguistic exp lanations and gu idance, the 'comm unicative
may also be impo rtan t in L2 learning. To date little research has explore d
revoluti on' may have gone toO far.
whether it is pre ferable, fo r example, to provide feed back during or afrer
com m un icative pr act ice. O ne study of this issue was carried OUt by James There is increasing evide nce that learners continue to have difficulty with
Hunter (20 12). He investigated the effectiveness of feed back that the teac her basic structures of the language in programmes that offer littl e or no fo rm
provided after stu dent s ha d par ticipated in st udent-led conversations. His focused inst ru ctio n. This calls into question extre m e versions of the 'Just
findings show that such an ap p roach can result in a h igher proportion of liste n . .. and read ' and ' Get two fo r o ne' pro posals. While there is good
repa ir than feedback provi ded in whole-class teacher-led activities. evidence that lear ners make considerable progress in both com prehen
sion and production in comprehensio n-based programmes, we do not find
Recently there have been a number of meta-analyses of studies investigat
suPPOrt for the hypothesis that lan gu age acquisition wi ll take care of itself if
ing the effectiven ess of L2 corrective feedb ack o n L2 oral prod uction. The
second language learners sim ply focus on meaning in comprehensi ble in put.
results are m ixed , wi th some reporting benefits for recasts over other types of
Comprehension-based ap proaches are most successfu l wh en they include
feed bac k (Li 2010) and others reporring advantages for p rompts over recasts
guided attention to language features as a co mpo nent of instructio n.
(Lyster and Saito 20 10). Until there is greater con sens us o n the co nt rib utions
of different types of corrective feedback on L2 [ear n ing, a prudent approach Th e 'Le t's talk' proposal raises sim ila r co ncerns. Opportunities for learners to
would be to provide learners with a var iety of d ifferent types of corrective engage in conversati onal interactions in group and paired activ ities can lead
feed back and to keep in mi nd the co unterbalanc e hypothesis presente d in to increased comm unicative competence an d the ability to manage conversa
Chapter 5, whic h suggests that more explicit corrective feedback may be tio ns in a second lan guage. However, the research also shows that learners m ay
effective in contexts where th e learners' attention is focused on m ean ing / make slow prog ress on acquiring more accurate and sophi sti cated language
content while imp licit feed back may be sufficient to at tract learners' atten if there is no focus on form. This is espe cially tru e in classes where students'
tion in contexts where the focus of ins truction is typically o n language fo rm . shared langua ge an d learning backgrounds allow the m to communicate
successfully in sp ite of their errors. Because 'Let's talk' em phasizes mean ing
an d attem pt s to simulate 'natu ral' comm un icati o n in co nversational inter
action, the students' focus is naturally on what they say, not how to say it.
Fu rt he rmore, when feed back on error takes th e form o f recasts , lear ners may
1
Second language learning in the classroom 19~
196 Second language learning in the classroom
in promo ting second lan gu age learn ing tha n programmes that are lim ited
interpret it as a co nt in uatio n o f the conversati on ramer than focus on form.
to a virtu ally exclusive emphasis on comprehension , Huen cy, or accuracy
Th us, programmes based on the 'Le t's talk' approach are incomplete on their
alone. Thus, we would argue mat second lang uage teachers can (and should)
own, and learners' gains in confidence and conversational skills may not be
provide guided , form-foc used instruction an d corrective feedback in certain
marched by their development of more accurate and complex language.
circu m stances. For example, teachers sh o uld not hesitate to correct persistent
Id s important to emphasize that the evidence to su ppo rt a role for for m erro rs th at learners seem not to notice wi tho ut focused arren tion. Teach ers
focused instruction and corrective feedback does not suggest a return to the sho uld also be especially aware of errors mat m e majority of learners in a
' G et it right from the beginning' approach. Research has shown that learn class are making when they share me same first language background . They
ers do benefit considerably from communicative interaction and instructi o n should not hesitate to provide co ntr astive information about how a partic
that is meaning-based. The results of research in French immersion , other ular structure in a learner's first language differs from me target language.
co nt ent- based language teac hing, and communicative ESL are strong Teachers might also try to become more aware oflanguage features that are
indicators that learners develop higher levels of fluency through pr imarily JUS t beginning to emerge in the seco nd language development of their Stu
meaning-based instruction than through rigidl y grammar-based instructi o n. dents and provide so me guided instruction in me use of th ese forms . It can
Th e problem is that certain aspectS oflinguistic knowledge and performance also be useful to encourage learners to take part in the process by creating
are not fully developed in such programmes. activities that draw their attention to th e forms they use in communicarive
Research investigating the 'Teach what is teachab le' proposal is no t yet at a activities, by developing contexts in which th ey can provide each other with
poi nt where it is possible ro say to teach ers: 'H ere is a list oflin gui stic features feedbac k, and by encou raging th em to ask quesrions about langu age.
and the o rde r in wh ich th ey will be acq ui red . You shou ld teach them in th is De cisio ns about wh en and how to provide form focus must take in to account
order'. Th e number of featu res that researche rs have in vestig at ed in experi d ifferences in learner characte ristics, of co urse . Q u ite d ifferent approaches
me ntal studies wi thi n th is framewo rk is far to o sm all. O n the othe r hand, would be appropriate for, say, trai ned lingu ists learn ing a fo urth o r fifth
the re has been no stro ng evid ence tha t teaching acco rd ing to th e develop language, yo ung ch ild ren beginnin g their schooling in a seco nd langu age
mental sequences is necessary or even desirable or that it will improve the enviro n ment , both yo un ger and olde r irnrnigrants w ho cannot read and
long-term results in lan guage learning. What is mos t valuable about this pro w rire th eir own language, and adolescents stu dy ing a foreign language for a
posal is that it serves to help teachers set realistic expectations about th e ways few hours a week at schoo l.
in which learners' interlanguage may change in response to instruction . The
im p lica tions of 'Teach what is teachable' may be seen primarily in me fact
th at genuine progress in second language development must be measured Summary
in ways that include, but are not limited to , increased accuracy in language M any teache rs are aware of the need to balance form focus and m eaning
production . focus , and they may feel that recommendations based on research simply
sho uld be built primarily on creating opportunities for students to exp ress
acco m plish their goal. It is not always easy to step back from familiar prac
essent ial for learners' continued growth and development. The challenge is
against the prevailing trends in their educational contexts. Many teachers still
learners. The learn ers' age , rnetaling uisric sophistication, prior ed ucational
introd uction of communicarive language teaching methods has sometimes
experiences , rnoriva tion , and goals, as well as the similarity of the target lan
resulted in a co m plete rejection of attention to form and error-correction in
decisions are made about the amount and type of form focus to offer.
based and meaning-based instruction. Rather, the challenge is co find the
Class room data from a n umber of studies offer support fo r the view that best balance between these two orientatio ns.
form-foc used instruc tion and cor rec t ive feedback provided within me
co nt ext o f comm uni ca tive and content-base d programmes are more effective
198 Second language Learning in the classroom
1
Second language Learning in the classroom 199
Classroom-based research on second language learning and teaching has Suggestions for further reading
given us parcial answers co many questions. Through continuing research
and experience, resear chers and teachers will fill in more details, always rec Ellis, R. 2012 . Language Teaching Research and Language Pedagogy. Malden ,
ognizin g m at no Single answe r wi ll be adequate for all lear n ing environmen ts. MA: Wiley-Blackwell.
Among the questions we will continue to ask are these: In thi s volume, Ellis focuses on research tha t is designed specifically co
inves tigate the role of teach ing in sec o nd language learning. He review s
• How can classroom instructio n provide the righ t balan ce of meaning
bo th the m ethods an d m e find ings from a vast n umber of studies . Ellis
based and form- focused instruction?
app roaches the task from his d ual perspectives as a resear cher and as edi tor
• Which features of lan guage w ill respond best co for m-focused instruc
of me journal Language Teaching Research, which publishes me wo rk of
tion, and which will be acquired without explicit focus if/earners have
scholars an d educators who are investigating language teaching around
adequate access co m e language?
the world. Throughout the book, links are made be tween me research and
• Which learners will respo nd we ll co rne ral in gui sric informatio n
its implications for language pedagogy.
an d which w ill require so m e other way of focusi ng attentio n on
language form? Hedge, T. 2000 . Teaching and Learning in the Language Classroom. Oxford :
• When is it best co draw learners' attention co fo rm-before, aft er, or Oxford Universiry Press.
during communicative practice? Th is is a useful reference book fo r the classroom teacher. It covers a wide
" How should corrective feedback on lan guage form be offered? ran ge of topics releva nt to the 'teach in g an d learn ing of second/foreign
e When sho uld lear ners be allowed [Q focus their attention on th e content lan guages. It is divided into fo ur sections: a framework fo r teaching and
of thei r utt eran ces? learning, teaching the language system, de veloping the language skills,
planning, and assessing learning. Eac h chapter moves from th eo retical to
Continued classroom-centred researc h, including me action research by p ractical co nsideratio ns an d there is extensive use of tasks , activi ties, an d
teachers in their ow n classro o ms , will provide further insights into th ese and teach ing materials to motivate reade rs co reflect o n th e ideas p resented in
o ther important issues in second langua ge teaching and lear ning. relation to their own practice.
Lyster, R. 2007. Learning and Teaching Language through Content:
Que sti ons for reflection A Counterbalanced Approach. Amsterd am: John Benjamins.
I Keeping in mind that individual lea rner differe nces play an important role in This bo ok provides a comprehensive description and analysis oflanguage
seco nd language learn ing, do you t hink a particu lar learn er pr ofile might be teaching and lear n ing in co ntent- based classrooms. With a pr imary bur
more compatible with on e of the teaching proposals than anothe r ? no t exclusive focus on research in French immersion programmes in
C anad a, Roy Lyster synthesizes decades of empirical work m at has sought
2 If you were go ing to experiment with a new app roac h to teac hing in your
solutions co the challenges of teaching language and content simultane
classroom. wh ich of th e six pr o posals des cribed in this chapt e r woul d yo u
ously. The au thor's experience as both teacher and researcher is evident in
choose?Why?
the useful connections m ad e be tween theory and practice.
3 This chapte r co ncludes with the sugges tion that 'Get it right in the e nd' is
the best ap pro ach. Is t his con sist ent wit h your own views ?Why/w hy not? Nation, I. S. P. and]. M a calist er. 20 10. Language Cu rriculu m Design.
N ew York: Routledge.
Paul Nation has proposed four 'strands' mat are seen as essential-and of
equal importance-in a language teaching programme. The four strands
are: m eaning-focused input, m ea n in g-focused o utput, lan guage-focused
learning, an d fluency development. Together they tep resent a balanced
app roa ch ro language teach ing mat is compatible with research o n class
room learning. O rigin ally base d on Nation's research in vocabulary
learning, me four strands may also be see n as the elements ofa lesson , a syl
labus, o r even a curriculu m. N atio n (20 07 ) introduces m e ideas, an d th is
book elaborates on the fu ll range of issues related [Q cur ric ul um desi gn.
POPULAR IDEAS ABOUT
LANGUAGE LEARNING
REVISITED '
Preview
In thi s chapte r, we ret urn to th e 18 statements tha t yo u respo nded to in the
Introduction and summarize some of the related researc h and theory that
we have discussed in thi s book, shari ng so me of our ow n views about these
popular opinions.
and memorized sentences. They also identify patterns in the language and spee ch . Thus, th ey may co rrect an incorrect wo rd choic e, an incorrect state
extend them to new co ntexts. ment of the facts, or a rude remark, but the y do no t often react ro errors th at
do not interfere with communication. What this tells us is that child ren
If we use a narr ow definit ion of imi ta tion (the im mediate repetition of all or
cannot depend on consistent corrective feedback in order to learn the basic
part ofan o th er speaker's utterance) we find that so me ch ild ren im ita te a grea t
structure (th e word order, the grammatical morphemes, the intonation pat
deal as they acquire their first language. Even these childre n, however, do not
terns) of th eir language . Fortunately, they ap pear to be ab le to acqui re th e
im itate everything th ey hear. Instead, they selectively imi tate certain wo rds
ad ult fo rm o f the lan guage with little or no explicit feedback.
or structures tha t they are in th e p rocess of lear ning. Fu rtherm ore, child ren
who do little overt imitation lear n language as quickly and as well as tho se The case fo r second language learners is more complex. On the one han d ,
who imi tate more. Thus, this type of imitation may be an individual learning both children and adults can acquire a great deal of lang uage without any
strategy but it is not a universal ch arac teris tic oflanguage learners. formal instr uction or feedback on error. On the other hand, the evidence
suggests that, wi th o ut corrective feedback an d gu idan ce, secon d lang uage
Some secon d language learne rs also find it useful ro imitate sam ples of th e new
learners m ay persist in using cer tain ungrammatical for ms for years.
language. Classroom researchers have observe d students who repeat what they
hear others say, and some advan ced lear ners who are determined to improve
their p ronu nciation find it helpful to spend time carefully listening to and imi
3 Highly intelligent people ar e good language learners
tating language in a language labo ratory or tu torial. However, for beg inni ng Th e kind of intelligence that is measured by IQ tests is often a good pred ictor
learners, the imitation and rote memorizatio n tha t characterizes audiolingual ofsuccess in classrooms where the emp hasis is o n learning abou t th e language
approaches to language teaching is not effective iflearners do not also use the (fo r example. grammar ru les). Peop le who do well o n IQ tests ma y do well
sentences and p hrases they are prac ticing in meaningful interaction. Learners o n other kinds of tests as well. However, in narurallanguage learning settings
need ro do more than recite bits ofaccurate language in d rills and dialogues. an d in classrooms where interactive language use is emphasized, research
has shown that lear ners with a wide variety of intelle ctual ab ilities can be
Nevertheless, recent findings from corpus linguistics have p rovided a new successful language learn ers. This is especially true if the emphasis is on oral
appreciation for form ulaic lan guage use. We know from the d iscussion of co mmunication skills rather than meralinguistlc knowledge .
usage-based theories d iscussed in C hap ter 4 that a great deal of na tural Ian
guage use is p redictable on the basis of the frequency with whic h wo rds o r Most important , it m ust be recalled that language learning involves a great
phrases occur rogerher, Learners create strong associations between lan guage ma ny different skills and ab ilities that are no t m easured by IQ tests. Stu dents
features that tend to occ ur together. Thus, lang uage is parrly learn ed in chunks sho uld not be excluded from opportunities to lear n another language on
larger than single words. However, this internalization of the input does no t the gro unds tha t the y do not have the academic ab ility to succeed . In man y
depend on the learner's imitation of all or part of another person's utt erance educational contexts, students from immigran t or mino rity groups have no
in a rote-repetition fashion. It is the co m bined exposure ro language fearures cho ice ab out learning a second lan guage. What is essential is to find ways to
in the inp ut and their use in meaningful exchanges th at leads to learning. engage the different abilities th at students bring to the learn ing enviro nment.
2 Parents usually correct young children w hen they m ake 4 The best predictor ofsuccess in secon d language acquisition
gram.matical errors is motivation
There is considerable variation in the extent to which parents correct thei r Everyone agrees th at learn ers who Want to learn ten d to d o better than those
children's speech . The variation is based partly on the ch ildren's age and who don't. But we must not interpret this too rigidly. Sometimes, even highly
partly on th e paren ts' social, ling uistic, and educational background. When moti vated learn ers encounter grea t challenges in language lear ning. We kn ow,
chil dre n are very young, pare nts rarely comment on grammatical errors, for example, tha t lear ners who begin lear ni ng a second language as ad ults
although they may correct lapses in poli teness or the choice of a word that rarely achieve th e fluency and accuracy that childre n do in first lan guage
doesn't make sense. As child ren reach school age, parents may co rrect the acq uisition . Th is sho uld not be taken as evidence that ad ult second lan guage
kinds of non-standard speech that they hope their children will OUtgrow, fo r learners are not motiva ted to learn . It may be a reflectio n o f chan ges that
exam ple, 'M e and Fred are go ing outside now'. come with age or of other ind iv id ual d ifferen ces such as language lear ning
ap tit ud e, how the instr uction inte racts with ind ivid ual learn ers' styles an d
Extensive observatio ns of parents and ch ildren show that, as a rule, pare nts pre ferences for learnin g, how m uch tim e the learner can devo te to learn ing
tend to focus on meaning rather than form when they correct chil dren's
204 Papular ideas ab out language learning revisited Papular ideas about language learning revisited 205
the new language, and what o ppo rt uni ties the learner has to use th e language first lan guage . o r th ey live in jurisdi cti ons whe re legislation has mandat ed a
outside the classroom. single language of education tor all children, regardless of their background.
Teachers have no influence over learners' intrinsic motivation for learning a For the se children, it is crucial [0 have sensitive educators wh o respect the
seco nd language. Students come [0 classrooms from different backgrounds children's difficulty, who encourage parents to maintain the home language,
a nd life experiences, all of which ha ve contributed [0 their motivation [0 an d w ho un derstan d that second language learning takes time and effo rt .
learn and their arrirudes toward the target language and thecomrnuniry with Fo r foreign lan guage instruction or for second language instruction where
which it is associated. The principal way that teachers can influence learners' th e level of proficiency that is targeted is not native-like performance by all
motivation is by making the classroom a supportive environment in which students, the situation is quite different. When the goal of the educational
stu de nts are stimulated, engaged in activities that are appropriate [0 their age , p ro gram m e is basic co m m un ica tive skill for all students, and where there is a
interests, and cultural backgrounds, and, most importantly, where students strong commitment [0 maintaining and developing the child's first language,
can experience success. This in turn can contribute [0 positive motivation . it can be more efficient to begin second language teaching later. Older chil
leading to still greater success. . dren (fo r example, 10-year-olds) are able [0 catch up quickly with those who
began earlier (fo r example, at 6-7 years old) in programmes offering only a
5 The earlier a second language is introduced in school few hours a week ofinstruction . This is especially true if the for eign language
programmes, the gre~ter the likelihood of success in learning course include s a period of more intensive exposure to the new language.
The decision ab out w he n to introduce seco nd o r fore ign la ng uag e in st ru cti on A.ll scho o l p rog rammes sho uld be base d o n realistic esti mates of how lon g
mus t dep e nd o n th e o b ject ives of th e lan gu age program me in th e parti cu lar it takes to lea rn a seco nd lan gu age. O ne o r two hou rs a week-even for
soc ial co ntex t of th e school, W he n the o bjective is nat ive-lik e pe rfor man ce seve n o r eigh t years-will not produce adv anced seco nd lan gua ge speakers.
in the second lan gu age, th en it may be des ira ble to begin expos ure ro the Th is 'drip-feed' approach ofte n leads to fru stratio n, as learn ers feel that th ey
language as early as possible. as long as lear ners have exte nsive exposure to have bee n studying 'for years' withou t maki ng m uc h progress. Sad ly, th ey are
a nd opportunities to use th e second lan guage in a var iety of contexts . The ofte n righc about thi s.
research evid en ce is fairly strong th at those who begin second language learn
ing at an early age a re most likely to eventually be indistinguishable from 6 Most of the mistakes that second language learners make are
na tive speakers . due to interference from their first language
However, even in cases where native-like proficiency is targeted, it is impor First, we should recognize that knowledge ofone or more languages can con
tant to recognize certain disadvantages of an early star t for seco nd language tribute positively to many aspects of second or to reign language learning. If
learning. \Vhen an earl y star t means that children have little opporruniry to the languages are relatively close co usins (for example, English and German,
contin ue to develop their first language, the resulting subtractive bilingual Span ish and French), there is much that learners already ' kn ow'- includ ing
ism may ha ve lasting negative consequences . -t he alphabet, cognate words. as well as some basic principles of syntax,
For ch ild ren from minori ry-Ianguage backgrounds, programmes promoting On the other hand , the transfer of panerns from the native language is one
the development of the first language both at home and at school may be of the major sources of errors in learner language. When errors are caused by
more important for long-term success in the second language than an early learners' perception of some partial similariry between the first and second
stare in the seco nd language itself. Research shows that a good foundation in languages, they may be difficult to overcome, especially when learners are
th e child's first language, including the development of literacy. is a sound frequently in contact with other learners who make the same errors .
base to build on. Children who can begin their schooling in a language they
Aspects of the second language that are different from the first language will
already know will have more self-confidence, will be able [0 learn more effec
not neces sarily be acquired later or with more difficulry than th ose aspects
tively in the early school years, and will not lose valuable time in a period of
that are sim ilar. Second language learning is not simply a process of putting
limbo during which the y struggle JUSt to understand what is happening in
seco nd- language words into first-language sentences. In fact, learners may
the classroom .
not always be able to take advantage of similarities unless they are pointed
For many children, there is no opportunity to have their early schooling in o u r to rh ern , Learners can be overl y discriminating, failing to take advantage
their first language . They are members of smal l minoriry groups where it is ofsim ilarities because they assume, sometimes incorrectly, that the languages
not practical for schools to offer them an educational p rogramme in thei r must be different.
206 Popula r ideas about langu ag( l(arning reuisite. Popular ideas about language learning revisited 207
However, me first language is nor me only influence on second language understand and to make themselves und erstood if th ey also have an under
learning. Learners fro m different backgrounds often make me same ki nds of standing ofso me of the pragmatic feature s ofthe new language. For exam ple,
errors, an d some of thes e errors are remarkably similar to those made by first it is useful for them to focu s their attention on suc h things as how speakers
lan guage lear ners. In such cases, second-language erro rs are evidence of me show respect. apologize, or make requests . Th e cul tural d ifferences in these
learners' efforrs to discover m e structu re of the targe t language itself rather types of interactions so metimes lead to comm unication breakdown or mis
man attem pts to tran sfer patterns from fheir firsr lan guage. understan d ings, even when the words and the sentence structu res are correct.
7 The best w ay to learn new vocabulary is through reading 10 Teachers should p resen t grammati cal rules one at a ti me, and
This statement is true but it does not tell me whole Story. Children expand learners sho uld practise each one before go ing on to another
their vocab ulary dramatically during their school years, and reading is me Second lan guage learn ing is not simply linear in irs development. Learners
major source o f th is growth . Second language learners can also increase their may use a par ticular form accu rately at Stage x (suggesting mat they have
vocab ulary knowledge th ro ugh reading, bur few seco nd language learners lear ned that fo rm), fail to produce the fo rm (o r make errors whe n they
will read th e amount of target langu age text m ar a ch ild reads in m e co urse of attempt it) at Stage y, and produce it accurately again at Srage z. The decline
more m an a d ecad e of schooling. in accuracy at stage y may show that learners are incorporating new informa
Researc h evidence suggesrs tha t second language learners benefit from oppor tio n about the language into their incerlanguage. We saw, for example, how
tunities to read material that is interesting and important to them. However, learn ers may ask co rrect fo rm ulaic questions such as 'Wh ar's that? ' or 'How
thos e who also receive guidance from instr uc tio n and develop goo d strate do you say proche in English? ' an d th en produce questions like 'What you're
gies for lear n ing and remembering words will be nefit more m an those who doing wit h that?' at a larer rime . Language development is no r just adding
simply focus on getting the m ain ideas fro m a text. What is per ha ps most one ru le afte r ano ther, Rather. ir involves processes of incegrating new lan
striki ng in me research is the evide nce m at in order to successfully guess me guage for ms an d patterns inco an existing inrerlanguage , readjusting and
m eani ngs of new words in a text , a reade r usuall y needs to know more than resrructuring u nt il all the pieces fir,
90 per cent of me words in mat text. Some structure-based approaches to teaching are based on the false assump
tio n that seco nd language development is an accumulation o f ru les. This can
8 It is essential for learners to be ab le to pronounce all the be seen in the organization of textbooks that introduce a parti cular language
individual sounds in the second language feature in the first unit and reinforce it in several subsequent units , and then
Research o n pronunciation has shown tha t second language speakers' ab ility move on th e nex t featu re, with only rare opportunities for lear ners to p ractise
to m ake themselves understood depends more on their ab ility to rep roduce the ones previously raught. This isolated presenta rion an d pr actice of on e
the phrasing and stress patterns-the 'melody' of the language-than on structure at a time does not provide learn ers with an opportunity to discover
their ability to ar ticulate each individual sound. Ano ther important em p ha how different language features compare and contrast in normal language
sis in current research is the undeniable fact that most languages o f m e wo rld use. It is also likely mat, without opporrunities to continue hearin g, seeing,
are spoken in many diffe rent varieties. Thus, it no longer seems appropriate and using them , the language features learned in the firsc unit will have been
to insist that learners be tau gh t only one language variety or that only native forgotten long befo re the lase.
speakers of a particular variety are the best teac hers. Rather, learners need to
learn to understand and pro du ce language varieties that will permit them to 11 Teachers should teach simple language structures before
engage in communicative inceraction with th e interlocutors they are most complex ones
likely to encounter. Research has shown mat no ma tter how language is prese nced to learn ers,
certain structures are acquired before others. Th is suggesrs that it is nei ther
9 Once learners know 1,000 words and th e basic struct ure ofa necessary nor desirable to restrict learners' exposure to structures ma t are per
second language, they can easily participate in conversations ceived in linguistic terms to be 'sim ple'- particularly when this invo lves the
with native speakers isolated presentation, ordering, and p ractice o f''simple' to 'complex' features .
It is true th at most conversational language involves only a relatively limited At the same tim e, there is no do u b t mar second lang uage learners benefit from
nu mber of wo rds and sentence typ es. H owever, learners will find it easier to th e effo rts of na tive speakers an d fluent bilinguals to modify the ir speech
Popula r ideas about language {earning revisited 209
208 Popular ideas about la ngu age learn ing revisited
[Q help them understand. Th e language used in modified interaction may 13 Teachers should use materials that expose students only to
contain a variety of lin gu isric structures, some 'sim p le' and some 'com plex'. language structures they have already been ta ught
However, it also includes a range ofadjustments that enable second language Such a p roced ure can provide comprehensible inp ut of course, bur-given a
learners to engage in interactions with native and more advanced speakers meaningful context -learners can comprehend the general meaning of oral
of the second language more easily-mo re repetition. slower rate o f delivery, or written texts that contain vocabulary and structures they ha ve not 'mas
paraphrasing, etc . tered'. Thus, restricting classroom second language materials to those th at
Teachers must also be aware, however, that some linguistic forms are so rare contain little or nothing that is new may have several negative consequences.
There will undoubtedly be a loss of motivation if students are nor sufficiently
in classroom language that learners have little opportunity [Q hea r, use, and
challe nged. Students also need to develop str ategies for dealing with 'real ' o r
learn them if th e teacher does no t make a point of providing them. These
'auth entic' material if they are eventually going to be prepared for lan guage
are not necessarily difficult o r complex forms . As we saw in Chapter 6 . some
use outside the classroom. They do this first with the teacher's guidance and
'sim ple' language forms turn our to be extremely rare in classroom language.
even in content-based instr uctio n. then independently. Restric ting students to ste p-by-ste p exposu re to the lan
guage extends their dependen cy.
12 Learners' errors should be corrected as soon as they are made When a particular language feature is introduced for the first time, or wh en
in order to prevent the formation of bad habits the teacher feels there is a need for correction of a persistent problem , it
Errors are a natural part o fl anguage learnin g. Thi s is true of the development is ap p ro p riare to use narrow-focu s mat erials th at isolate o ne element in a
of a chi ld's first langu age as well as of seco nd langua ge learn in g by ch ild ren co nt ext w he re othe r th in gs seem easy. But it wo uld be a disservice to stu
and ad ults . Errors reflect the parterns of learners' develo pi ng interlan gu age de nt s [Q use such mater ials exclu sively or even predo m inantl y. \'{'e shou ld
systems- showing ga ps in their knowledge, overgeneralizario n of a second reme m be r tha r learners who successfully acq uire a seco nd langu age outs ide
lang uag e rul e, o r an inap p ro p riate tra nsfer of a first lan gu age pattern to the classrooms certainly are exposed to a grea t variety of forms and structu res
second language. they have not mastered .
Teachers have a responsibility to help learn ers do their best , and this includes 14 When learners are allowed to interact freely (for example, in
th e provision o f explicit, form-focused instruction and feedback on error.
group or pair activities) , they copy each other's mistakes
When errors are persistent, especially wh en they are shared by almost all stu
dents in a class, it is important to bring the problem to their attention . This Th e language that learners hear and read serves as input to their language
does nor mean that learn ers should be expe cted to adop t the correct form developm ent . Th e cog nitive processes that allow th em to learn from input
are not 'sh ut down' wh en they are interacting with other learners. Thus, when
or pattern immediately or consistently. If the error reflects a developmental
stage, the instruction or feedback may be useful only when the learn er is ready learners interact with each other. they ma y provide some incorrect input.
Fur the rmo re, wh en learners come from the same first language background
for it. It rnay be nece ssary to repeat feedback on the same error many times .
and are at roughly the same level of pr oficiency, the y are likely to understand
Of cour se, excessive feedback on error can have a negative effect on motiva each other very well , eliminating the need for negotiation for meaning that
tion; teachers need to be sensi tive to their students' reactions to correction . might lead th em to replace their inrerlanguage pattern s with more target
The amount and type of co rrec tion that is offered will also vary according to like ones . Nevertheless, the benefits of pair and group work far outweigh the
the specific char acteristics of the students , as well as thei r relationship wi th disadvantages, especially if the tasks are properly designed.
the teacher and with each other. Children and adults wi th little ed ucation
in their first language will not benefi t greatly from sophisticated metalin If the activities are well designed and learn ers are appropriately matched,
guis tic explanations , but university students w ho are advanced learners of pair and group work provides far more practice in speaking and participating
the lan guage may find such explanations of great value. Immediate reaction in conversations than a teacher-centred class ever could. Somewhat surp ris
to erro rs in an oral communicatio n setting may embarrass some students
ingly, resear ch has s how n that learners do not produce any more errors in
and discourage them from speaking wh ile others welcome such correction their speech when talking to learn ers at similar levels of proficiency than th ey
do when speaking to learners at mo re advanced levels or [Q native speakers.
as exactly what is needed ro help them notice a persistent error at JUSt the
moment when it occu rs. The resea rch also shows, however, that learners at similar levels cannot o rd i
narily provide each other with information that would help to correct those
210 Popula r ideas about language lea rning revisited Popular ideas about language learning revisited 2 11
errors. No neth eless, tasks can be devised in such a way mar learners working recasts are perc eived as corrective feedback, even though learners may not
together can discover nor only how to express or interpret meaning bur also always know exactly which language features the feedback is focused on.
how to discover the correct panerns in me seco nd language. In order for this However, in co ntent-based instruction (fo r example, immersion classes) and
to happen, che casks m ust be carefully planned ro give lear ners access ro m e in communicative instruction with younger learners, recasts often appear to
new language they need. be misinterpreted. Learners seem ro hear them as confirmation of meaning
Group and pair work is a valuable addition co the variety o f activities that rather than as correction o fform. In these situations, recasts have been found
encourage and pro mote secon d lan guage development. Used in combina to be more effective if the teacher has a method ofsign alling to me stu dent
tion w ith individual work and reacher-centred activities, it plays an essential rone ofvoice, gesture, or facial exp ression-mat says to me student, 'I think
role in language teach ing and learning . I understand what you are saying, and I'm showing you how you can say
it better'.
15 Students learn what they are taught
Teachers know from experience ma r students don't lear n everything they are
17 Students can learn both language and academic content (for
taugh t! Forrunately, learners also learn a great deal that no one ever reaches example, science and history) simultaneously in classes where
them. They are able ro use the ir own internal learn ing abilities to discover many the subject matter is taught in their second language
of the parrerns and assoclations th at underlie the language they are learning. In The advantages of conren t-based instruction are num erous. Motivation is
this sense, studen ts learn much more man they are explicitly raughr . increased when the material that is used for language teachi ng has an inher
Some reaching methods rypically give learners the opportunity to learn only a' ent value to th e students: it creates a genuine, immediate need to learn the
restricted n umber ofwords and senrence rypes. Even when the language reach language . Content-based instruction is also often associa ted with the oppor
ing method provides much richer lan guage in pur, me fact that someth ing tunity to spend more time in contact with che language, without losing out
is taught or made available in the in put does no t mean learners will acquire on instruction in other subject matter. In addition, the range of vocabulary
it right away. For example, some aspects of me second language emerge and and language fearures mar students encounrer in learning academic subjects
evolve according ro developmental sequences , and learners may be more likely is more varied than that which is typically available in second and foreign
to lear n certain lan guage fearures when they are developmentally ready. Thus, lan guage classes.
arremprs ro teach aspeCts of language that are roo far away from the learner's Research has confirmed m at studen ts in content-based and im mersion
current stage of development can be frustrating. Other language features , for classes develop com p rehensio n skills, vocabulary, and general communica
example, vocabulary, can be taught ar any rime, as long as the learners are in ter tive competence in me new language. Teachers and researchers have also
ested in the opportunity ro learn and the teaching methods are app ropriate co found, however, that rhe ability to understand me content and to functio n in
the learn er's age, interests , needs , experiences, and learning styles. classroom interaction does nor ensure mat students w ill continue to improve
in certain aspects of their second language, especially in areas of accuracy
16 Teachers should respond to students' errors by correctly on lan guage fearu res mar do not usually interfere with meaning. Th us, for
rephrasing what they have said rather than by explicitly example, srudents can spen d years in Frenc h im mersion without achiev
pointing out the error ing accuracy in marking nouns for gender or verbs for tense. Experimenral
studies in which an element of form-focused instruction was added to the
Th is kind of feedback, referred to as ' recasts' has been fou nd to be by far the
content-base d instruction have shown mat, with guidance, students can
most common rype offeedback in second language classrooms. This has been
improve in these areas as well. Born students and teac hers need to keep in
shown to be true for learners ar different ages and in different instructional
mind mat co nrent-based instruction is also language teac hing.
senings- from au diolingual to communicative and co n ten r- based instruc
tion. A recast has -the advantage o f not imerrupdng the flow of in teraction .
It is seen as Indirect and police, a way ofgivin g students the information th ey 18 Classrooms are good places to learn about language but not
need without em ba rrassing them. for learning how to use language
Some structure-based approaches ro lan guage teach ing have ten de d to treat
Resear ch in classes with a general focus on grammar an d accurate language
lang uage as a set o f grammar rules or as separate bits ofinformarion m ar need
use shows that learners are responsive to th is kin d of feedback. Resear ch in
to be learn ed before learners can use m e language as a comm u nicative too l.
which learn ers interact individually with interlocuto rs has also shown tha t
Other approaches such as communicative language teac hing, co nt en t-based,
212 Popular ideas about language Learning reuisited
and task-based instruction start from the principle that we learn language
by using it to achieve a goal. for example, understanding a story, making GLOSSARY
a medical appointment, writing a science report, or joining the fun on the
playground. With this in min d . classroom activities are designed to prepare
students to continue learning outside the classroom. by giving them experi
ence in language UJeS that are like those they will encounter there.
As we saw in Chapter 6, some theorists argue that second language instruction
The glossary contains items that have a special or technical meaning in second
can only lead to knowledge about language (explicit knowledge) and question
language acquisition research and second language teaching. The definitions
whether instruction can lead to the ability to use the language spontaneously
are intended to reflect the terms as we use them in this book.
and fluently (implicit knowledge) in a wide range ofcommunicative COntexts.
Contemporary approaches to L2 instruction target the development of both accessibility 'hierarchy: A ranking of relative clauses developed by Keenan
types of L2 ability by ensuring that students use the language in meaningfi.i.l and Comrie (1977). Differem languages use relative clauses to modify
interaction inside the classroom and that they learn effective strategies for using nouns in different grammatical roles . According to the accessibility
the language outside the classroom when they have opportunities to do so. hierarchy. for example, most languages allow relative clauses for sentence
subjects, while fewer languages allow them for the object of comparison.
ACTIVITY Support your opinion accuracy order: The relative accuracy of grammatical form s in learner
lan gu age. For exam ple, learners are oft en mor e accurat e in usin g plur al -J
Choose two or three of the 'popular ideas ' that you find especially important. s.
than in usin g possessive So me researc he rs have inferred that an accuracy
For each of these. identify and discuss how one or more of the research orde r is eq uivalent to a developmental seq ue nce .
studies you have read about in this book has strengthened your agreement!
disagreement with the statement or has led you to change your views. action research: Research carried out by teach ers. o ften in their own
classroo ms or in collaboration with other teachers. The research goals and
questions are local and specific to their own teaching environment.
Conclusion active lisreningi A teaching technique in which students not only listen
but also show their comprehension by thei r responses.
Knowing more about second language acquisition research will not tell you
what to do in your classroom tomorrow morning. We hope, however, that additive bilingualism: Learning a second language without losing the first.
this book has provided you with information that encourages you to reflect American Sign Language (ASL): Th e gestural language used by many
on your experience in teaching. We hope, in addition, that this reflection will North Americans who are deaf or who interact with others who are deaf. It
co nt ribute to a better understanding ofyour responsibilities as a teacher and is a true language, with complex rules of structure and a rich vocabulary, all
your students' abilities and responsibilities as language learners . expressed through motions of the hands and body.
.As we have seen, language learning is affected by many factors. Among these audiolingual approach: An approach to second or foreign language
are the personal characteristics and experiences of the learner. the social and teaching that is based on the behaviourist theory of learning and on
cultural environment both inside and outside the classroom. the structure of structural linguistics, especially the contrastive analysis hypothesis. This
the native and target languages, opportunities for interaction wi th speakers instructional approach emphasizes the formation ofhabits through the
of the target language, and access to corrective feedback and form-focused repeti tion, practice, and memorization ofsentence patterns in isolation
instruction. It is clear that teachers do not have control over all these factors, from each other and from COntexts of meaningful use.
Nevertheless. a better understanding of them will permit teachers and learn
auditory discrimination: The ability to distinguish language sounds, for
ers to make the most of the time they spend together in the rwin processes of
teaching and learning a second language. example minimal pairs suc h as ship! sheep.
behaviourism: A psychological theory that all learning, whether verbal or
non-verbal, takes place through the establishment of habits. According w
this view, when learners imitate and repeat the language they hear in their
1
surrounding environment and are positively reinforced for doing so, habit in situations. The term has sometimes been interpreted as the abili ty to
formation (or learning) occurs. convey messages in spire of a lack of grammatical accuracy.
bilingual education:· Schooling in which students receive instruction in communicative language teaching (CLT): CLT is based on the premise
two (or more) languages, usually the ir home lan guage and a second that successful language learning involves not only a knowledge of the
language. structures and forms of a language, but also the functions and purposes tha t
a language serves in different communicative settings. This approach to
bilingualism: The ability to use more than one language. The word itself
teaching emphasizes the communication of meaning in interaction rather
does not specify the degree of proficiency in either language.
than the prac tice and manipulation of grammatical forms in isolatio n.
brain imaging: A variety of techniques that allow researchers to observe
competence: Linguist Noam Chomsky used this term to refer to
and track activity in the brain.
knowledge oflanguage. This is contrasted with performance, which is the
child-d.iceeted speech: The language that caretakers address to children. way a person actually uses language- whether for speaking, listening,
In some cases, this language is Simpler than char which is addressed to reading, or writing. Because we cannot observe competence directly, we
ad ults. In some cultures, it is also slower, higher pitched, more repetitive, have to infer its nature from performance.
and includes a large number of questions.
comprehensible input: A term introduced by Stephen Krashen to refer to
chunk: A unit oflanguage chat is often perceived or used as a single'unir. language that a learner can understand. It may be comprehensible in part
Chunks include formulaic expressions such as Thank you or What's that? but because ofgestures, contextual information, or prior knowledge/experience.
also bits of language tha t frequently occur together, for example, ice cream
comprehensible output hypothesis: The hypothesis that successful
cone or significant difference.
second language acquisition depends on learners producing language (oral
classroom observation scheme: A tool (often in the form of a grid) that or written). Swain (1985) proposed this hypothesis in response to Krashen's
consists ofa set of predetermined categories used to record and describe ( 1985) comprehensible input hypothesis.
teaching and learning behaviours.
comprehension-based instruction: A general term to describe a variety of
cognate: A word in o ne language that comes from the same origin as a second language programmes in which the focus of instr uction is on
word in another language and has the same meaning, for example, 'nation' comprehension rather than production.
in English and nation in French or uaca and uache (cow) in Spanish and
connectionism: A theory of knowledge (including language) as a complex
French. The term false cognate is used to refer to words that may come from
system of units that become interconnected in the mind as they are
the same origin but have evolved to have different meanings, for example,
encountered together. The more often units are heard or seen together, the
librairie (bookstore) in French does not have the same meaning as library in
more likely it is that the presence of one will lead to the activation of
English.
the other.
cognitive: Relating to how the human mind receives, processes, stores, and
content and language-integrated learning (CLIL): An approach to
retrieves information. The focus is on internal learn ing mechanisms that are
content-based language teach ing that has been developed primarily in
believed to be used for learning in general, not JUSt language learning alone.
secondary schools in Europe.
cognitive maturity: The ability to engage in problem-solving, deduction,
content-based language teaching (CBLT): Second language instruction
and complex memory tasks.
in which lessons are organized around subject matter rather than language
collaborative dialogue: A conversation between learners in which they points. For example, in immersion p rogrammes, students study science ,
wo rk together to solve a problem, for example, reconstructing a story they history, mathematics, etc. in their second language.
have heard. While the focus is on the task, learners may also focus on the
contrastive analysis hypothesis (CAH): The expectation that lear ners will
communicative competence: The ability to use language in a variety of those of the first language than th ose that are differe nt.
researcher is investigating. Performance of me control gro up is used to show developmental features: Th ose aspects o f a language which , acco rding ro
that the variable in question is the best (o r only) explanation for changes in Pienemann and his colleagues, develop in a particular sequence, regardless
the experimental group. Also so meti m es referred to as 'comparison group' . of input variation , learner motivation , or instructional intervention .
corpus (plural: co rpo ra): A principled collection of oral or w rit ten developmental sequence: The order in which cert ain fearures of a
language samples mat can usually be accessed and explored with computer lan guage (fo r example, negation) are acquired in language learning. Also
based tools. Some of me rno sr fam o us corpora contain millions of wo rds called developmental stages o r order o f acquisition.
from, for exam ple, newspapers. Samples of the language produced by display question: A question to which the asker already knows me an swe r.
learners have also been collected for second language acquisition research. Teachers often ask these questions (fo r example, What colou r is your shirt?)
corpus linguistics: An approach to the srudy oflanguage that is based on to get the learner to display his or her knowledge of the language.
the analysis of language corpora. See corpus . enhanced input: Input mat is altered in an effort to make some language
corrective feedback: An indication to a learner m at his or her use of th e featu res more salient to learners. It can be more or less explicit, ranging
tar get language is in correct. fro m explicit rneralinguistic co m m ents to ty pograph ical enh anc eme nt
(bold type or underlining) or exaggerated stress in speaking.
co r rela ti o n s A sta tistical procedure that compares the relative frequen cy o r
size of different variables in order to determine whether there is a ethnography: D escriptive research in which the ob server seeks to
relatio nsh ip between them . understan d a group or co m m u nity fro m wi thi n its own perspect ive. The
research requi res extensive per iods of observatio n as well as co nsu ltation
counterbalance hyp o thesis: Th e hypothesis that lea rn er s' attention wi ll
with group m em bers to validate the observer's desc riptions .
be drawn to classro om even ts tha t are d ifferent from those they are
accustomed to . exp e rimental study: Research des igned to test a hypo thes is abo ut the
im pact of one o r mo re spec ific var iables o n ano the r var iab le. A stric tly
critical period hypothesis (CPH) : The p ro posal th at th ere is a limited
expe rime nt al scudywo uld have 'experim ental' and 'co ntro l' gro ups th at
pe riod during w hic h language acq u isition can oc cur.
di ffer from each o the r only in th e pres ence or absence of th e variable(s) of
cross-linguistic influence: The effect on kn owledge of o ne language by inte rest. In ed ucatio nal research, it is often difficult to create all of me
the kn owl edge o f ano th er. This term is pr eferred over previous terms suc h as co ndi tio ns m at permit a study to be termed as a 'gen uine' expe rime nt al
interference to indicate mat knowledge of one language can be beneficial to study. In this book, me term is used in a non-technical sense to refer to
learning another. The term also reflects the fact mat me influence can go research in which an anempt has been made to investigate a sin gle varia ble
fro m a known language to th e o ne being learned bu t also from the new in an educational setting.
language to one already known .
field independent/field dependent: This distinction has been used to
cross-sectional study: A study in which parti cipan ts at di fferent ages and/or des cribe people who differ in their tenden cy to see th e for est o r th e trees.
stages of development are studied . This co n trasts wi th longirudinal studies, That is, so me people (calle d field independent) are very quick to pick o ur
the hidden figure s in a complicated drawing. Others (called field
declarative knowledge: Informati on th at we h ave and kn ow we hav e. An
dependent) are more inclined to see the whole drawing and have difficulty
exam p le would be a rule such as 'th e verb must agre e with me subject ro
form a correct sentence'. In some skill learning theories, it has been separating it in to parts.
hypothesized that all learning begins with declarative knowledge. This first language (Ll , mother tongue, native language): The language first
contrasts with procedural knowledge. learned. Many children learn more man one language from birth and may
descriptive study: Research mat does not involveany manipulation, be said to have more than one 'first' language.
cha nge, o r intervention in me phenomenon being stud ied . The researcher's foreigner talk: The modified or simplified language that some native
goal is ro observe and record what is happening. This co nt rasts with speak ers address to seco nd language learners. A special category of foreigner
widely used in the community (for example, lear ning French in China). immersion programme: An ed uca tio nal programme in which a second
This is sometimes contrasted with 'second language learning', where the language is taught via co nte nt- based instruction. That is, students stu dy
language being learned is used in the community (for example, learning subjects such as mathematics and social stu dies in their second language.
Italian in Florence). Typicall y, students in imm ersio n programmes shar e the same first lan guage
and teachers adjust their instructional language and materials to meet the
form-focused instruction: Instruction that draws attention to the forms
needs of second language learners.
and structures of the lan guage within the context of communicative
interaction. This may be done by giving metalinguistic information, simply information processing: A psychological theory based on the idea that
highligh ting the form in question, or by providing corrective feedback. learners' cog nitive resources are limited and they can't pay attention to
everything at the sam e time. But with repeated experience and practice,
formulaic: Expressions or phrases that are often perceived and learned as
things which at first required attention become auto matic, leaving more
unanalysed wholes . For example, a child or second language learner may
atte ntion available fo r focus on something else.
first hear 'What's that?' as a single uni t oflanguage rather than as three
units. innatist: In language acquisition research, this is the theoretical perspective
based on the hypothesis that human beings are born with mental structures
fossilization: This term is used to describe a persistent lack of change in
that are designed specifically for the acquisition oflanguage.
interlanguage patterns, even after extended exposure to or instruction in
the target language. input: The language that th e learne r is exposed to.(either written or
function words: Words that are used mainly as linking or supporting spoken) in the environment:
words for nouns, verbs, adj ectives, and adverbs. For example, prepositions input flood: A technique for pro viding a large number of examples of a
('to' , 'for', 'by') and articles ('a', 'the') are two types of function words. They particular language feature in the input to learners. It has been used in
have litt le or no meaning when they occur alone, bu t they have an research projects to explo re q uest ions related to whether comprehensible
important effect on the meanings of the words they accompany. input is sufficient for lan guage acquisition.
generalization: Extending a pattern learned in one context to another instrumental motivation: Motivation that is essentially prac tical, such as
one. See also overgeneralization. the need to learn the language in order to get a better job.
genuine question: A question to which the asker does not know the integrative motivation: Motivation for second language learning that is
answer in advance (for example, What didyou do fast weekend?). Also called based on a desire to know more about the cul ture and co mm unity of the
'referential' or 'informatio n' questions. Cont rasts with display question . target language group and even a desire to be more like members of that
grammar translation: An approach to second language teaching group.
char acterized 9Ythe explicit teaching of grammar rules and the use of intensive ESL: In thi s book, 'i n tensive' ESL is used to refer to an
translation exercises. instructional approach in Quebec where lO-12-year-old French-speaking
students learn English as a second language. Most Quebec Students in this
grammatical morphemes: Morphemes are the smallest units oflanguage
age group have onl y an hour or two ofESL instruction each week.
that carry meaning. A simple word is a morpheme (for example, 'book'),
'Intensive ESr.: classes provide much more time. Most of the classes
but when we talk about 'gram matical morphemes' we are usually referring
observed in intensive ESL research set aside one five-month block of time
to smaller units that are added to words to alter their meaning (for example,
in one school year and devot e full days to ESL instruction during th at
the -s in 'books' indicates plural) or function words (fo r example, the) which
period. The pedagogical approach observed in these classes is
are ordinarily attached to another word.
p redominan tly communicative language teaching. In contras t to
grammaticality judgement: A test or task in which participants are asked immersion programmes, intensive ESL classes do no t usually include
to make a decision about whether a sentence is grammatically correct or not. content-based langu age teaching.
hypothesis (plural: hypo theses): A sta teme nt of a possible fact tha t can be interaction hypothesis: The hypothesis that language acquisitio n is based
tested th rough research . Most empirical research starts from one or mo re both on learners' innate abilities an d on opportunities to engage in
hypotheses and involves the design of a stu dy that can either show support conversations, often those in which other speakers modify their speech and
their interaction parcerns {Q match the learners' com m u n ication morpheme: See gram ma tical morphem es.
requirements. The innate abilities are not seen as being specific to language native-like: The ability to comprehend and produce a second language at a
or language acquisition. level of performance that is indistinguishable from that of a native speaker.
interlanguage: A learner's developing second language knowledge. It may native speaker: A person who has learned a language from an early age and
have characteristics of the learner's first language, characteristics of the who is deemed ro be fully proficient in that lan guage. Na tive speake rs differ
seco nd language, and some characteristics that seem ro be very general and in terms ofvocabulary and stylistic aspects oflanguage use, but they tend ro
tend ro occur in all or most interlanguage systems. Inrerlanguages are agree on the basic grammar of the language. The notion 'native speaker'
systematic, but they are also dynamic. They change as learners receive more must always be undersrood within a specific geographic region or
input and revise their hypotheses about the second language. socioeco no mic group because there is wide variation among 'native
interlocutor: A participan t in a conversation. speakers' of most languages. .
language acquisition/language learning: In this book, these two terms are natural order: See developmental sequence.
most often used interchangeably. However, for some researchers, most negotiation of form: An interaction in which language learners work
notably Stephen Krashen acquisition represents 'unconscious' internalization toward the correct form in a context where meaning is undersrood. If a
oflanguagt knowledgt, which takes place when attention is focused on teacher is involved in the interaction, he or she seeks ro guide students {Q
meaning rather than language form, and IMming is described as a 'conscious' Rnd the right form inst ead o f pro viding it for them.
process th at occ u rs wh en th e learner's objective is {Q learn abou t the langu age
itself rathe r tha n ro un de rsta nd messages co nveyed thro ug h the language.' negotiati o n fo r meaning: Int eractio n betwee n spea kers who make
adj ustments {Q th eir speec h and use other tech niques ro repair a breakdown
langu ag e-related episodes (LREs): Parts of co nve rsatio nal interactions in in co mm unication. See also mo dified interaction.
wh ich lang uage learners talk abo ut the lan gu age forms th ey are usin g and
engage in self- and peer -correction . noticing hypothesis: Th e hypo thes is, pr op osed by Rich ard Schmi dt. th at
language learners learn only that which th ey have first 'no ticed' or become
longitudinal study: A study in which the sam e learners are studied over a aware of in the input.
period of time. This contrasts with a cross-sectional study.
obligatory contexts: Places in a senten ce where a particular grammatical
meaning-based instruction: See communicative language teach ing. form is required if the sentence is to be correct. For example, in the sentence
meta-analysis (plural: meta-analyses): A statistical procedure that allows ' Last week, my brother rent a car', the speaker has created an obligatory
researchers ro combine the findings from a large number of quantitative COntext for the past tense by the use of 'Last week', but has not used the
studies in order ro assess the overal l parcerns of findings on a similar ropic . required form of the verb in that context.
metalinguistic awareness: The ability {Q trea t language as an object, for order of acquisition: See developmental seq ue nce.
exam ple, being able to define a word, or {Q say what sounds make up that overgeneralization: This rype of error is th e result of trying to use a rule or
word . patt ern in a context where it doe s not belong, for exam ple. putting a regular
mitigation: In pragmatics, a phrase or tone of voice used {Q reduce or -ed ending on an irregular verb , as in 'buyed ' instead of ,bought' .
so ften the possible negative impact of what is said. pattern practice drill: A teaching technique in which learners are asked to
modified input: Adapted speech that adults use to address children and practise sentences chosen to represent particular linguistic forms. Typical of
nati ve speakers use ro address language learners so that they will be able ro the audiolingual approach.
understand. Examples of modified input include shorter, simpler sentences, performance: The way we use language in listening, speaking, reading,
and basic vocabulary. writing. Performance is usually contrasted with co m petence, which is the
modified interaction: Adapted conversation patterns that proficient knowledge that underlies our ability to use language. Performance is subject
speakers use in addressing language learners so that the learner will be able ro variations due to inattention or fatigue whereas competence, at [east for
{Q understand. Examples of interactional modifications include the mature native speaker, is more stable.
co mprehensio n checks, clarification requests, and self-repetitions.
222 Glossary Glossary 223
phonemic: Small differences in language sounds that can change meaning scaffolding: Th e language th at an interlocu to r uses co suppo rt the
within a particular language. For exam ple, the consonants p and b are communicative success of another speaker. It ma y includ e the provision o f
phonemic in English, but not in Arabic. missing vocabulary or the expansion o f the speak er's incomplete sentence.
pragmatics: Aspects oflanguage use that go beyond vocabulary and second language (L2): In this boo k, the term refers to any language other
grammar to includ e rules of how to use lan guage appropriately in different than th e first language learned. Th us, it may actually refer to th e third or
contexts and with different speake rs. It also includes an understanding of fourth language.
the implied as well as th e explici t meaning oflanguage.
segmental: The individual sounds of a language. Contrasted wi th
private speech: Th e language we use when we are talki ng to ou rselves, no t 'sup rasegrnen rals', which are patte rn s of intonation.
expecting anyone to hear or respond.
significant difference: Th..is is a tech nical term that refers co differences
procedural knowledge: Knowledge that underlies fluent or automatic between groups which, according co a variety ofstatisti cal tests, are unlikely
performance. It is contrasted with d eclarative kn owledge. to have happened by chance. Such diffe ren ces can be sm all or large. Their
processahility theory: M anfred Pienernann's theory oflearners' developing 'significan ce' is due co th e consistency of the di fferences as well as their size.
ability to process linguistic elements in different sent ence posi tions. simplification: Leaving out elements of a sentence, for example, using the
processing instruction: An approach to instruction in which learners are same form of a verb regardless of person, number, tense ('I go today. H e go
given explicit in formation abo ut the language feature co be learned and yesterd ay') .
th eir pr actice activities involve the co mprehension (no t production) of sociocultural theory: An explana tion for knowledge and learn ing th at is
sentences or texts that cannot be understood without a focus on th e based on the assumption th at all learning is first social then individ ual.
language itself. The app roach was developed by Bill VanPatten. Learning is viewed as a process that is socially mediated, th at is, it is
qualitative research: An approach tha t uses detailed descriptions of the depen dent on dialogue in face-co-face interaction . The claim is that during
phenomena being studied rather than counting or measuring the exact communication, learners join tly construct kn owledge which is intern alized
amount of some specific variable or variables. Q ualitative research requires by the individual.
extensive o bservatio n and ins ightful interpretation. standard variety: The variety o f a given lan gu age that is typically used in
quantitative research: An approach that requ ires precise counts or formal writing and formal public speaking (including broadcasting) . Th e
numeric measurements of variables. In a quantitative study, both the standard variety ofwidely spoken languages may be different in d ifferent
variable that is believed to affect learning and the learning itself are places. For example, American English, British English, Can ad ian English ,
measured or 'q uan tified' . Quantitative research requires careful selection of an d Indian English each has its own standard variety, as well as numerous
the measurem ents that will be used to repre sent the variables being studied. eth nic, regional, and socioeco nom ic varieties .
rate oflearning: The speed with which learners progress in th eir language structural grading: A technique for organizing or sequencing material in a
development. textbook or lessons . The basis for the organization is a gradual increase in
co mplexity of grammatical features.
recast: To repea t a learner's incorrect utte rance , makin g ch anges th at convert
it to a correct phrase or sentence. ' Recast' is also used as a noun, that is, a subtractive bilingualism: Partially or complete ly losing the first language
recast is the interlocutor's modified!corre cted form of the learner's utterance. as a second lan gu age is acquired .
register: A style or way of using language that is typ ical o f or approp riate suprasegmentals: The sounds of a language that involve the melody an d
for a particular setti ng. For example, speaki ng and writing usually require rhythm of the language (e.g, stress an d intonation), rath er than the
d ifferent registers; the register used in wri tin g a research report is differe nt pronunciation ofindividual sounds.
fro m tha t used in writing a letter co a friend . target language: The lan guage being learned, whether it is the first
restructuring: Cognitive activity that is seen as causing changes in the way language or a second (or th ird o r fourth) language.
information is organized in the brai n, even though no new inform atio n has task-based language teaching (TBLT): Instr uct ion in which classroom
bee n learned. activities are 'tasks' sim ilar to th ose lear ners mi gh t engage in outside the
GIoJSary 225
224 Glossary
second or foreign language classroom. Tasks may be complex, for example, facto rs co nt ribute to wrc including social, individ ual, situatio nal, and
creating a school newspaper. or more limited, for example, making a phone mo rivarionai.
call to reserve a train ticker. working memory (WNf) : The cognitive 'space' in which we actively
teacher talk: See modified input and foreigner talk. process new information or information that is currencly in focus. Also
called 'short-term memory' .
transfer: The influence ora learner's first language knowledge in the
second language. Also called 'interference' . The term 'cross-linguistic zone of proximal development (ZPD): The metaphorical 'place' in which
influence' is now preferred by many researchers. It better reflects the a learner is capable of a higher level of performance because there is suPPOrt
complex ways in which knowledge of the first language may affecr learners' from interaction with an interlocutor. In Vygotsky's theory, learning takes
knowledge and use of another. place through and during interaction in the learner's ZPD.
Th is list b rings togeth er all the wo rks cited in th e text as well as th ose that
have been suggested for further read ing.
Altenberg, E. P. 2005 . 'The judgment, perception , and production of
consonant clusters in a second language.' International Review 0/.Applied
Linguistics 43/ 1: 53-80.
Ammar,A. and P. M. Light bown. 2005 . 'Teach ing marked linguistic
strucru res-s-rnore about the acquisition of relative cla uses by Arab lear ners
of English' in A. Housen and M . Pierrard (eds.): Investigations in Instru cted
Second Language A cquisition. Amsterdam: Mouto n de Gruyte r. pp . 167- 98.
Anderson, J. R. 1995. Learning and Memory: An Integrated Approach.
New York: W iley.
August, D. and T. Shanahan (eds.) . 2008 . Developing Reading and Writing
in Second-Language Learn ers. N ew York: Rou tled ge.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 1999 . 'Exp [0 ri ng the i nte rlan guage 0 f interlanguage
pragmatics: A research agenda for acquisitional pragmatics.' Language
Learn ing49/4: 677- 7 13.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. 2000. TenseandAspect in Second Language Acquisition :
Form, Meaning, and Use. O xfo rd : Blackwell.
Bardovi-Harlig, K. and B. S. Hartford. 1993 . 'Learn ing the ru les of
academic talk: A longitudinal study of pragmatic development. Studies in
Second Language Acquisition 15/3 : 279-304.
Barlow, M. 2005. 'Computer-based analyses oflearner lang uage' in R. Ellis
and G . Barkhuizen (eds.). Analysing Learner Language. O xford : O xford
University Press. pp . 335-57.
Bates, E. A. an d B. MacWhinney. 1981. 'Second-language acquisition
from a fu nctio nalist per spective: Pragmatic, semantic, and perceptual
strategies ' in H. W initz (ed.): Native Language and Foreign Language
Acquisition. Annals o/ the New York Academy of Science. N ew York: N ew York
Academy o f Science, Vol. 379: 190- 2 14.
Bennet, G. R. 20 10. Using Corpora in the Language Learning
Classroom: Corpus Linguisticsfor Teachers. Ann Arbor, MI: U nive rsity
of Mi chigan Press.
228 Bibliography Bibliography 229
Beretta, A. 20 11. 'Th e language learning brain' in M . Long and Celce-Murcia, M., D. M., Brinton, and J. M. Goodwin. 1996 . Teaching
C. Doughty (eds.): The Handbook o/Language Teaching, Oxford: Wiley Pronun ciation: A Reference[or Teachers 0/English to Speakers 0/Other
Blackwell. pp . 65-80 ,
Languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Berko Gleason,]. 'The child's learning of English morphology.' m"rd 14,
Chomsky, N. 1959. ' Review of"Verbal Behavior" by B. F. Skinner.'
150-77.
Language35 /1: 26-58.
Berko Gleason,]. and N. Bernstein-Ratner (eds .). 2009. The Development Clement, R., S. C. Baker, and P. D. MacIntyre. 2003. ' W illingness to
ofLanguage 7 th edn. New York: Allyn and Bacon.
communicate in a second language: The effects of context, norms, and
vitality.' Journal o/Language and Social Psychology 22/2 : 190-209.
Bialystok, E. 2001. Bilingualism in Development: Language, Literacy, and
Dalton-Puffer, C . 2006. 'Q uestio ns in CLIL classrooms: Strategic Dornyei, Z. 2009 . The PsychologyofSecond Language Acquisition. Oxford :
questioning [Q encourage speaking' in A. Martinez-Flor and E. Uso (eds.): Oxford Un iversity Press.
New Perspectiveson Teaching the Language ProcessingSkills. Ams terdam:
D ornyei, Z. and P. Sk eh an . 2003. 'Individ ual differences in second
Mo uto n de Gruyrer, pp. 187-213.
lan guage learn ing' in C. ] . Doughty an d M . H. Long (eds.): The Handbook
Day, R,J. Bassett, B. Bowler, S. Parminter, N . Bullard, N. Prentice, ofSecond Language A cquisition. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 589 -630.
M. Furr, M. Mahmood, D. Stewart, an d T. Robb. 20 11. Bringi ng
Doughty, C. 199 1. 'Second language instructio n does mak e a difference:
Extensive Read ing into the Classroom. O xford : Oxford Un iversity Press.
Evide nce fro m an em pirical study ofS L relativizarion.' Studies in Second
DeKeyser, R M. 1998 . ' Beyo nd focus on form: Cognitive perspectives on Language Acquisition 13/4: 43 1-69.
learning and practicing second language grammar' in C. ]. Doughty and
D oughty, C. and E. Varela. 1998. 'Communicative focus on form' in
]. W illiams (eds.): Focus on Form in Classroom Second Language Acquisition.
C. Dough ry and] . Will iams (eds.): Focus on Form in Classroom SLA .
Camb ridge: Cam bridge U niversity Press. pp. 42-63.
Cambridge: C ambridge University Press. pp. 114-38.
DeKeyser, R. M . 2000. 'The robustness of critical period effects in
Duff, P. 1995. 'An ethnography of comm unica tion in immers ion
secon d lan guage acquisitio n.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 22/4 :
classrooms in Hungary.' TESOL Quarterly 29 /3: 505-37 .
493-5 33.
Dunn, W. E. and ]. P. Lantolf. 1998. 'Vygo rsky's Zone of Proximal
DeKeyser, R M. 2001 . 'Aurornariciry and au tomatization' in P. Rob inson
Developmen t and Krashen's i+1: Incom m ensur able const ructs; .
(ed.): Cogn ition and Second Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge
incom mens urable theo ries.' Language Learning 48/3 : 4 11-42.
University Press. pp. 12 5- 5 1.
Echevarria, J ., M. Vogt, and D. J . Short. 2004. Making Content
DeKeyse r, R M . 2007. 'Introducti on: Situ ating the concep t of prac tice' in
Comprehensiblefor English Language Learners: The SlOP Model. Bosron :
R. DeKeyser (ed.): Practice in a Second Language: Perspectives from Applied
Pearson .
Lingu isticsand Cogn iti ve Psychology. Camb ridge: Cambridge University
Press. pp. 1-18. Eckman, E, L. BeU, and D. Nelson. 1988. ' O n the generalizatio n
of relative clause instruction in the acquisi tion of English as a second
de Villiers, J . G. and P. A. de Villiers. 1973. 'A cross-sectio nal study of the
lan guage.' Applied Linguistics 9/ 1: 1-20.
acqu isition of grammatical mo rphemes.' Journal 0/Psycholinguistic Research
2/3: 267-78. Ellis, N . C. 200 1. 'Memory for langu age' in P. Robinson (ed.): Cognition
and Second La nguage Instruction. C am bridge: Camb ridge U niversity Press.
Derwing, T. M. and M.J. Munro. 20 09. 'Purring accent in its place:
pp.33-68.
Rethi nking o bstacles to communicat ion.' Language Teaching42/4: 476-90 .
EUis, N. C. 2002. ' Freq uency effects in language acq uisitio n: A review with
Derwing, T. M. and M . Rossit er. 2003. 'The effects of pronunciation
impli cations for theories of implicit and explicit lang uage acquisition.'
instruction on th e accu racy, fluency, and complexity ofL2 accented
Studies in Second Language Acquisition 24/2: 143-188.
speech .' Applied Language Learning 13/ 1: 1- 17.
Ellis, N . C . 2003. ' Co nstr uctio ns, chu n king, and connectionism: The
Donato, R 1994 . 'Collective scaffold ing in second language learn ing' in
emergence of second language structure' in C.]. Do ughty and M . H . Long
] . Lantolf and G . Appel (eds.): Vygotskian Approaches to Second Language
(eds.): The Handbook ofSecond Language Acquisition. O xford: Blackwell.
Research. Norwood, N] : Ablex. pp. 33-56.
p p. 63-103.
Dornyei , Z. 200 1a. Motivational Strategies in the Language Classroom.
Ellis , N. C. 2005 . 'At the int erface: Dynam ic interactions of explicit and
C ambridge: Cambridge U niversity Press.
im plicit language knowledge.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition
Dornyei, Z. 2001 b. Teaching and Researching Motivation. H arlow: 27/2: 305-52.
Longman. Ell is, N. C. 2009. ' O ptimizing the input : Freq uency and samp ling
Dornyei, Z. 2005. The Psychology ofrhe Language Learner: Indi vidual in usage-based and fo rm-focus ed learn ing' in C.] . Dou ghty and
Differences in Second LanguageAcquisition. Mahwah, N]: Lawrenc e M. H . Long (eds.) : The Handbook o/Language Teaching. Malden, MA:
Erlbaum and Associates. W iley-Blackwell. pp. 139-1 58 .
232 Bibliography Bibliography 23.3
Ellis, N. c., R Simpson-Vlach, and C. Maynard. 2008 . ' Form ulaic Gatbonton, E., P.Trofimovich, and M. Magid. 2005. ' Learn ers' ethnic
language in native and seco nd language speakers: Psycholinguisrics, corpus group affiliation and L2 pronunciation accuracy: A sociolinguistic
linguistics, and TESOL.' TESOL Quarterly 42/3 : 37 5-96 . investigation.' TESOL Qua rterly 39 /3 : 4 89- 5 11.
Ellis, R 2012. Langu age 'Teaching Research and Langllage Pedagogy. Malden, Genesee, E 1976 . 'Th e role of intelligence in second language learning.'
MA: Wiley-Blackwell. Language Learni ng 26/ 2: 267-80.
Ellis, R. and G. Barkhuizen, 200 5. Analysing Learner Language. Oxford: Genesee, F. 1987 . Learning through Two Languages. New York:
Oxford Uni versity Press. Newbury House.
Ellis, R, S. Loewen, C. Elder, R. Edam,]. Philp, and H . Reinders. 2009. Genesee, E , K. Lindholm-Leary, vv. M . Saunders, and D. Christian
Implicit and Exp licit Knowledge in Second Language Learning: Testingand (eds.). 2006. Educating English Language Learners:A Synthesis ofResearch
Teaching. Bristol, UK: Multilingual Matters. Evidence. Camb ridge: Cambridge University Press.
Erard, M. 201 2. Babel No More: The Searchfor the World's Most Goldschneider, J. M. and R M . DeKeyser. 200 1. 'Ex plaining th e narural
Extraordi nary Language Learn ers. New York: Free Press. order ofL2 morpheme acq uisition in English: A meta-analysis of multiple
determinants.' Language Learning 51/ 1: 1-50.
Erlam, R 2005. ' Language aptitude and its relationship to instructional
effectiveness in second language acquisition.' Language Teaching Research Greer, D . L. 2000. 'The eyes of hiro: A Japanese cultura l mo nito r of
9/2: 147-1 71. . behavior in th e com municative lan guage classroom. ' JALTJournal
22/ l: 183-95 .
Ganschow, L. and R. L. Sparks . 200 1. 'Learn ing difficulties and foreign
lang uage learning: A review of research and inst ruction .' Language Teaching G uillo tea ux , M. J. and Z. Dornyei. 2008 . ' Mo tivating language learn ers:
34 / 2:79- 98. A classroo m-o riented investi gati on of th e effects o r"mo tivatio nal st rategies
o n student motivation.' TESO L Quarterly 42 /1 : 55-77 .
Gardner, D. 2004 . 'Vocabulary input thro ugh extensive reading:
A comparison of words found in children's narrative and exposito ry Guiora, A., B. Beit-Hallahami, R Brannon, C. Dull, and T. Scovel.
reading materials .' Applied Lingu istics 25/1: 1-37. 1972 . 'Th e effects ofexperimentally induced changes in ego states on
p ro nunciation abiliry in a seco nd language: An exploratory stu dy.'
Gardner, H . 1993. Multiple Intelligences: The Theory in Practice. New York:
Comp rehensive Psychiatry 13/5: 421-8.
Basic Books.
Hahn, L. D . 2004 . 'Primary stress and inrelligibiliry: Research ro motivate
Gardner, R. C. and vv. E. Lambert. 1972. Attitudes and Motivation in
the teaching ofsuprasegmenrals .' TESOL Quarterly 38/2 : 20 1-23 .
Second Langllage Learning. Rowley, MA: Newbury House.
Hahne, A. 2.00 1. ' What's different in seco nd-language processing?
Gass, S. M. 1982. 'Fro m theory to practice' in M . Hin es and
Hatch, E. 1978. 'D isco urse analysis and second language acquisition' in Kasper, G. and K. R. Rose. 2002. ' Pragmat ic Development in a Second
E. Hatch (ed .): Second Language Acquisition: A Book ofReadings. Rowley, Language.' Language Learning 52/Supplement 1.
MA: Newbu ry. pp . 401-35. Keenan, E. and B. Comrie. 1977. 'Noun phrase accessibiliry and Un iversal
Heath, S. B. 1983. ways with WO rds'. Cambridge: Cambridge University Grammar.' Li ngu isti c Inquiry 8/1: 63-99.
Press. Kellennan, E. 1986. 'An eye for an eye: Crosslinguistic cons trai nts o n th e
Hedge, T. 2000. Teaching and Learning in the Language Classroom . O xford: develo pment of me L2 lexicon' in E. Kellerman and M. Sharwood Sm ith
O xford U niversiry Press. (eds.): Crosslinguistic Influence in Second Language Acquisition. New York:
Pergamon. pp. 35-48.
Hoare, P. and S. Kong. 2008. ' La te immersion in Hong Kong: Still stressed
Horwitz, E. K. , M. B. Horwitz, andJ. Cope. 1986 . 'Foreign language Krashen, S. 1982. Principles and Practice in Second Language Acquisition.
classroom anx iety,' Modern Language journal 7 0/ 2: 125-32 . Oxford: Pergamon.
Howard, E. R., J. Sugarman, D. Christian, K. Lindholm-Leary, and Krashen, S. 1985 . The Input Hypothesis: Issues and Implications. London:
D. Ro gers. 200 7. Guiding Principles for Dual Language Education 2nd edn. Longman.
Washington, DC: Cent er fo r Applied Linguistics. hcc p:llwww.cal.org/rwi/ Krashen, S. 1989 . 'We acquire vocabulary an d spelling by readi ng:
guid ingprinciples.h tm Additio nal evidence for the inpu t hypothesis.' Modern Language journal
Hu, M. and I. S. P. Nati o n. 2000. 'U nkn own vocabulary density and 7 3/4 : 440-64.
reading comprehension.' Reading in a Foreign Language 13/ 1: 403-30 . Lado, R. 1964. Language Teaching: A Scientific Approach. New York:
Hulstijn,J. and B. Laufer. 2001. 'So me empirical evidence for the McGraw-Hill.
involvement load hypothesis in vocabulary acq uisition.' Language Learning Lambert, w: E. 1987 . 'Th e effects of bilingual and bicu ltu ral experiences
5 1/3 : 539-58 . on children's attitudes and social perspectives' in P. Homel, M. Pali], an d
Hunter, J. 20 12. 'Small talk : developing fluency, accuracy, and com plexity D . Aaronson (eds.): Childhood Bilingualism: Aspects ofLinguistic, Cognitive,
in speaking.' ELTjournal 661 1: 30-41. and Social Development. Mahwah, NJ : Lawrence Erlbaum an d Associates .
pp .197-221.
Itard, J .-M .-G . 1962. The Wild Boy ofAveyron (L'enfant sauvage). New York:
Meredith. Lambert, w: E. and G. R. Tucker. 1972. Bilingual Education ofChildren:
The St. Lam bert Experiment. Rowley, MA: Newbury.
Jarvis, S. and A. Pavleoko. 2008. Crosslinguistic Influence in Language and
Cognition. New York: Ro utledge. Lantolf, J . P. (ed.), 2000. Sociocultural Theory and Second Language
Learning. Oxford: Oxford Universiry Press.
Johnson, J. and E. Newpo rt. 1989 . 'Critical period effects in second
language learning: The in fluence of maturational state on the acquisition of Lapkin, S., D. Hart, and B. Harley. 1998 . 'Case study of co m pact Core
Englis h as a Seco nd Language.' Cognitive Psychology 21 11: 60-99. French models: Attitudes and achievement' in S. Lapkin (ed .): French
Second Language Education in Canada: Empirical Studies . Toronto, Canada :
Johnson, R. K. 1997. 'Th e Hong Kong education system: Late immersion Universiry ofToromo Press. pp. 3-30 .
under stress' in R. K. Johnson an d M . Swain (eds.): Immersion Education:
International Perspectives. C amb rid ge: Cambridge Universiry Press. Leow, R. P. 199 7 . 'Attention, awareness, and foreign language beha vior.'
pp . 171 -89. Language Learning 47/ 3: 46 7-505 .
236 Bibliography Bibliograph} 237
Li, S. 20 10. 'Th e effecti veness of co rrect ive feedback in SL\: A meta Loewen, S . and]. Philp. 200 6. 'Re casts in th e ad ult Eng lish Ll class:
analysis.' Language Learning 60 /2 : 309-365. characteristics, explicitness, and effectiveness. ' Modern Language[o urna
90/4: 536-56.
Lieven , E. and M. Tomasello. 2008 . 'C hildr en's first lan gu age from a usage
perspective' in P. Ro binson an d N . C. Ellis (eds .): Handbook of Cogn iti ue Loewen, S., S. Li, F. Fei, A. Thompson, K. Nakatsukasa, S. Ahn, and
Linguisti cs and Second Language Acquisition. New York: Roucledge. X. Chen. 2009. 'Second language learners' beliefs about grammar
pp ..168-96. instructio n and error co rrectio n .' Modern Langu ageJo u m al 93/ 1: 91 - 104 .
Lightbown, P. M. 1983a. 'Acquiring English L2 in Quebec classrooms' Long, M . H . 1983. 'N ative speaker/non-native speaker conversation and
'in S. Felix and H. Wode (eds.): Language Development at the Crossroads. the negotiation of comprehensible input.' Applied L inguistics 4/2: 126-41.
T iibingen : Gunter Nar r, pp . 15 1- 7 5.
Long, M . 1996 . 'The role of the linguistic environment in second language
Ligh t bown, P. M. 1983b. 'Exploring relarionships between developmental acquisition' in W. Rit chie and T. Bhatia (eds .): Handbook ofSecond
and insrru ctio nal sequences in L2 acquisition' in H. W. Seliger and Language Acquisition. New York: Academic Press. pp . 413-68 .
M . H . Long (eds.) : Classroom-Orienud Research in Second Language
Long, M . H. and P. Porter. 1985 . 'Group work, interlanguage talk , and
Acquisition. Row ley, MA : Newbury House. pp. 21 7-45 .
seco nd language acquisition .' TESOL Quarterly 19/2: 20 7-28.
Ughtbown, P. M . 1998. 'The importance of riming in focus on form '
Long, M. and C. Sa to. 1983. 'C lassroo m foreigner talk discourse: Forms
in C. D oughcyandJ. \Xfilliams (eds.): Focus on Form in Classroom
an d fun cti on s of teachers' q uestions' in H. W. Seliger and' M . H . Lon g
Second La nguage Acquisition. Cam bridge: Ca m b ridge Un iversity Press,
(eds.) : Classroom -Oriented Research in Second Language Acquisition. Rowley,
pp. I77-%.
J'v1A: Newbury House. pp. 268-85 .
Ligh t bown, P. M. 2007. 'Fair tra de: Two-wa y bilin gual ed uca rio n.'
Long, ] ., C. Brock, G. Crookes, C. Deicke, L. Po tt er, and S. Zhang.
Estudios de Lingidstica Inglesa Aplicada 7: 9-34.
1985. 'The effect of reach ers' qu est ioning partern s and wai t tim e on pu pil
Ligbtbown, P. M . 2008a. 'Easy as pie? Children learning languages.'
parti cipation in public h igh school classes in H awaii for stu den ts of limited
Concord ia WO rking Papers in Applied L inguistics 1:1- 25.
English proficiency.' TESOL Quamrly 19/3 : 605-07.
hrrp :/ /doe. concordia. cal copal/ index.p hp/cop al/volumes.hrml
Lyst er, R. and L. Ranta. 1997. 'C orrect ive feedback and learner uprake:
Ligh t bown, P. M ., R. Halter,]. L. White, and M. H o rst. 2002.
Mulrilingual Maners.
Macintyre, P. 1995. ' How does anxiety affect second language learning? S. D oval-Suarez (eds .): The Dyn amics ofLanguage Use: Functional and
A reply to Sparks and Ganschow. ' Modern Language journal 791 1: 90-9. Dyn amic Perspectives. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.
Mackey, A. 1999. 'Input, interaction and second language development: http ://www.lognostics.co.ukltools/ pp . 269-83.
An empirical study ofquestion formation in ESL.' Studies in Second Meisel,]. M. 1987. 'Reference to past events and actions in the development
Language Acquisition 21 14: 557-87. of natural second language acquisition' in C. Pfaff (ed.): Firstand Second
Mackey, A. and]. Goo. 2007. ' Int eraction research in SLA: A meta Language A cquisition Processes. Cam bridge, IvfA: Newbury House.
analysis and research syn thesis .' in A. Mackey (ed.): Con versational Meisel, J. M., H . Clahsen, and M. Pienemann. 1981. 'O n determining
Interaction in Second Language Acquisition: A Series of Empirical Studies. develo pment al stages in natural second language acq uisitio n.' Studies in
Oxford: Oxford Universiry Press. pp. 40 7-452. Second Language Acquisition 3 /2 : 109- 3 5.
Mackey, A. and]. Philp. 1998. 'C onversatio nal interaction and second Mwioz, C. 2006. 'Th e effects of age on foreign language learni ng: The BAF
language development: Recasts, responses and red herrings.' Modern project' in C. M unoz (ed. ): Age and the Rate ofForeign Language Learning.
Language journal 82 /3: 338-56. Clevedon , UK: Multilingual Matters. pp. 1-40.
Mackey, A., S. M. Gass, and K. McDonough. 2000. 'How do learners Munro, M. J. and T. M. Derwing. 2011. 'Th e fou ndations of accent and
per ceive interactional feed back?' Studies in Second Language Acquisition intelligibiliry in pron unciation research.' ~anguage uaching44 /3: 3 16-27.
22/4: 47 1-497.
Nagy, W. E., p"A. Herman, and R.Anderson. 1985. 'Learning words from
MacWhinney, B. 1997. 'Seco nd lang uage acq uisition and the competition context.' Reading Research Quarterly 20/2: 233-5 3.
mode l' in A. M . B. de G root and J. F. Kroll (eds.): Tutorials in Bilingualism:
Nation, I. S. P. 2001. Learning Vocabulary in Another Language.
Psycbolinguistic Perspectives. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and
Associates. pp . 113-44. Cambridge: C am bridge Universiry Press.
Nation, I. S. P. 2007. 'The four strands.' Innovation in Language Learning
MacWhinney, B. 2000. The CHILDES Project: TOols for Analyzing
and uaching I l l : 1-12.
Talk (2 volu mes) . Mahwah , NJ : Lawrence Erlba um and Associates.
http:/ /childes.psy. cmu.edu/ Nation, 1. S. P. and]. Macalister. 2010. Language Curri culum Design.
New York: Routledge.
Masgoret, A. M. and R C. Gardner. 2003. 'Atti tudes, motivation, and
second language learning: A me ta-analysis ofstudies conducted by Gardner Netten,]. and C. Germain (eds.). 2004. 'Int ensive French in Ca nada .'
and associates.' Language Learning 53/1 : 123- 63 . Canadian Modern Language Review 60 /3 : pp . 249-435 .
McCormick, D. E. and R. Donato. 2000. 'Teacher questions as scaffolded Newpo rt, E. 1990. 'Maturational constraints on language learning.'
assistance in an ESL classroom' in J. K. Hall and L. Verplaetse (eds.): Second Cognitive Science 14/1 : 11-28.
and Foreign Language Learning through Classroom Interaction. Mahwah. NJ:
Nicholas, H., P. M. Lightbown, and N . Spada. 2001 . ' Recasts as feedback
Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates. pp. 183-201 .
to language learners.' Language Learning 51/4: 719-58.
McDonough, K. 2004. ' Lear ner-learner interaction during pair and small
No rris, ] . M. and L. O rtega. 2000. 'Effectiveness ofL2 instruction:
Edward Arnold .
Norton Peirce, B. 1995. 'Social identity, invest ment, and language
McLaughlin, B. 190/0. 'Restructuri ng.' Applied Linguistics I II 1: 113-28. learn ing. TESOL Quarterly 29 11: 9-31 .
Meara, P. M. 1980. 'Vocabulary acquisition: A neglected aspect oflanguage Norton, B. and K. Toohey. 200 1. 'C han ging perspe ctives on good
lear ning.' Language Teaching and Linguistics Abstracts 13: 221-46. language learners.' TESOL Quarterly 35/2: 307-22.
Meara, P. M. 2005. 'D esigning vocabulary rests for Englis h, Spanish Ohler, L. 1989. 'Exceptional secon d lan guage lear ners' in S. Gass,
and other languages' in C. Buder, M . dos Angeles Cornez-Gonzales, and C. Madden, D . Preston, and L. Selinker (eds.): Variation in Second
240 Bibliography Bibliography 24 1
Language A cquisition, Vol. II: Psycbolinguistic Issues. C levedo n: Multilingual Pienemann, M. 2003 . ' Lan guage processing capacity' in C. J. Doughty
Marrers . pp. 141-59. and M. H. Long (eds .): The Handbook ofSecond Language Acquisition.
Oxfo rd: Blackwell. pp . 679-714 .
O dlin, T. 2003. 'Cross-Iingulsric influence' in C. J. Doughry and
M. H. Long (eds.) : TheH andbook ofSecond Language Acquisition. Oxford: Pien em ann, M., M . Jo hnston , and G. Brindley. 1988. 'Constructing an
Blackwell. pp. 436-86. acquisition-based procedure for second language assessment.' Studies in
Second Language Acquisition 10/2: 21 7-4,.3 .
Ohta, A. 2000. ' Rethinking recasts: A learner-centered examination
of corrective feedback in the Japanese classroom' in J. K. Hall and Pimsleur, P. 1966. The Pimsleur Language Aptitude Battery. New York:
L. Verplaetse (eds.) : Second and Foreign Language Learning ,hrough Harcourt, Brace, Jovanovic. .
Classroom Interaction. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates. Piper, T. 2006. Language and Learning: The Home and School Years 4th edn,
Oliver, R. and A. Mackey. 2003 . ' In teractio nal context and feedback in Piske, T. 2007. ' Im plications ofJames E. Flege's research for the foreign
child ESL classrooms.' Modern LanguageJournal 87/4: 519-33. language classroom' in O . Bohn and M . Munro (eds.): Language Experience
Ortega , L. 2007. 'Meaningful L2 practice in foreign lang uage classrooms: in Second Language Speech Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjarnins.
in a Second Language: Perspectives from Applied Linguistics and -Cognitive P iske, T., I. R. A; MacKay, and J. E. Flege. 2001 . :Factors affectin g degr ee
Psychology. C ambridge: Cam bridge Uni.versity Press. pp . 180-207 . of foreign accent in an L2: A review.' [ournal ofPhonetics 29 /3 : 191 -215 .
Paradis, J., F. Genesee, and M. B. Crago. 201 1. D ual La nguage Ranta, L. 2002. 'The role of learn ers' language an alytic ab iliry in the
Development and Disorders:A Handbook on Bilingualism and Second co mm unicative classroom' in P Robinson (ed .): Individual D ifferencesand
Langu age Learn i ng 2nd edn. Baltim ore: Paul H . Brookes. Instructed Language Learning. Amsterdam: John Benjarnins. pp . 159-80.
Patkowski, M. 1980 . 'The sensitive period for the acqu isition of syntax in a Reid ,]. (ed.). 1995. Learning Styles in the ESUEFL Classroom. New York:
Pearson, B. Z. 2008. Raising a Bilingual Child: A Step-by-Step GuidefOr Richards,J. 1974. 'A non-contrastive approach to error analysis' in
Parents. New York: Living Language (Random H ouse). J. Richards (ed.) . Error analysis. Londo n: Longman. pp. 172-88.
Piaget, J. 1951. Play Dreams, and Imitation in Childhood. New York: Ringbom, H . 1986. ' C rosslingu istic influence and the foreign language
Norton. (Originally published as La fOrma tion du symbole chez len[ant. learning process' in E. Kellerman and M. Sharwood Smith (eds.):
Neucharel: Delchaux er Niestle.) Crosslinguistic Influence in Second Language Acquisition. New York:
Pica, T. 1983. 'M ethods of morpheme q uantification: Their effecr on Pergamo n Press. pp . 150-72. .
the interpretation ofsecond language data.' Studies in Second Language Rob in so n , P. (ed.). 2002. Individual Differences and Instructed Language
Acquisition 6/ 1: 69 - 78 .
Learning. Ams terdam: John Benjamins.
P ica, T. 1994. ' Research o n negotiation : What does it reveal abo ut second Robinson, P. and N. C. Ellis. 2008. 'Conclusio n: Cognitive linguis tics,
language acquis itio n? Conditions, processes, and outcomes' Language seco nd language acq uisition, and L2 instruction-issues fo r research'
Learning44/3: 493-527. in P Robinson and N. C. Ellis (eds.): H and book of Cognitive Lingu istics
P ien em ann, M. 1988. ' De term in ing the influ ence of instruction on L2 and Second Language Acquisition . New York: Routledge. pp. 489-545.
speec h processing.' AlIA Review 5/ 1: 40-72. Roy, D. 20 09. 'New horizon s in the stu dy of child language acquisitio n.'
Pi en em ann, M. 1999. Language Processing and Second Language INTERSPEECH-2009, Brighton , UK.
Development: Processability Theory. Amsterdam: Joh n Benjamins. Sachs, J., B. Bard, an d M. Johnson. 1981. 'Language learn ing with
restric ted inp ut: Case studies of two hearing children of deaf parent s.'
Samuda, V. 2001 . 'G uiding relationships between form and meaning Sharwood Smith, M. 1993 . ' Inp ut enhancement in instructed SLA:
during task performance: The role of the teacher' in M. Bygate, P. Skehan, Theoretical bases.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition 15/2: 165-79.
Schachter, J. 1974. 'An error in error analysis.' Language Learning in the ESL classroom.' Language Teaching Research 32 /2: 203-34.
24/2: 205-14.
Simpson, R. C, S. L. Briggs,]. Ovens, an d]. M. Swales. 1999 . The
Schachter,]. 1990. 'On rhe issue of completeness in second language Michigan CorpusofA cadem ic Spoken English (MICASE). Ann Arbor, MI:
acquisition.' Second Language Research 6/1: 93-124. The Regents of the University of Michigan. http://micase.e1icorpora.info.
Schieffelin, B. 1990 . The Give and lake o/Everyday Life: Language Sinclair,]. M. (ed.). 2004. How to Use Corpora in Language Teaching.
Socialization o/Kaluli Children . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.
Schleppegrell, M. 2004. The Language o/Schooling. Mahwah, N]: . Skehan, P. 1989 . Individual Differences in Second Language Learning.
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. London: Edward Arno ld.
Schmidt, R. 1990. 'The role of consciousness in second language learning.' Skinner, B. F. 1957 . Verbal Behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.
Applied Lingu istics 11/1: 17-46.
Slobin, D. I. 1973. 'C ognitive prerequisites for the development of
Schmidt, R. 2001. Attention' in P. Robinson (ed.): Cognition and Second grammar' in C. A. Ferguson and D. 1. Siobin (eds.): Studies of Child
Language Instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 3-32. Language Development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.
perception s conceming'rhe role of grammar teaching and cor rective Siobin, D. I. 1985-1997. The Crosslinguistic Study 0/Language . Vols 1-5 .
feedback: USA-Colombia.' Modern LanguageJournal 85/2 : 244-58 . Mahwah, N]: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.
Schumann,]. 1979 . 'Th e acquisition of Engli sh negation by speakers of Smith, N. and I. M. Tsimpli. 1995. The Mind ofa Savant: Language
Span ish: a review of the liter ature ' in R. W Andersen (ed.) : TheAcquisition Learning and Modularity. Oxford: Blackwell.
and Use ofSpanish and English as First and Second Languages. Washington,
Snow, C 1995. 'Issues in the study of input: Fine- tuning, universality,
DC:TESOL.
individual and developmental differences, and necessary causes' in
Schwartz, B. 1993. 'O n explicit and negative data effecting and affecting P. Fletcher and B. MacWhinney (eds.): The Handbook ofChild Language .
competence and linguistic behav ior.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 180-93.
15/2: 147-163.
Snow, C and M. Hoefnagel-Hohle, 1978. 'The critical period for
Segalowitt, N. 2010. Cognitive Bases o/Second Language Fluency. lan guage acquisition: Evidence from second language learning.'
Seidlhofer, B. 2011. Understanding English as a Lingua Franca. Oxford: Spada, N. 20 11. ' Beyond form-focused instruction: Reflections on past,
Oxford University Press. present and fut ure research.' Language Teaching 44/2: 225-36.
Selinker, L. 1972. 'Inr erlan guage.' International Review 0/Applied Spada, N. and M. Frohlich. 1995. The Communicative Orientation
Linguistics 10/2 : 209-31 . 0/Language Teaching Observation Scheme: Coding Conventions and
15/2: 205-24.
Spada, N. and P. M. Lightbown. 1999. ' Insrructio n , L1 influence and Swain, M. and S. Lapkin. 1998. ' Int eractio n and seco nd language
developmental readiness in second language acquisition.' Modern Language lear ning: Two ado lescent Fre nch immersion stude nts working toge ther.'
JournaL8311: 1-22. Modern Language[ournal 82/3: 33 8-56.
Spad a, N. and P. M . Light bown. 2002. 'L 1 and L2 in the education of Swain, M . an d S. Lapkin. 2002. 'Talking it through: Two French
Inuit children in northern Quebec: Abilities and perceptions.' Language immersion learners' respo nse to reformulation.' Internationaljournal of
and Education 16/3 : 212-40. Edu cational Research 3 7/3-4: 285-304.
Sp ada, N . and Y. Tomita. 2010. ' Interact io ns between type of instruction Swain, M., P. Kinnear, and L Ste inman. 2010 . Sociocultural Theory and
and type oflanguage feature: A me ta-analysis .' Language Learning Second Language Education: An Introduction through Narratives. Bristo l:
60/2: 1-46. Multilingual Maners .
Sp ada, N., P. M. Lightbown, and ]. L Whit e. 2005 . 'The importance Taro ne, E. and M. Sw ain. 1995 . 'A sociolinguistic perspective on
oHorm/mean ing mappings in explici t form -focussed instruction' in seco nd-language use in immersion classrooms.' Modern Language [ournal
A. Housen and M. Pierrard (eds .): Investigations in Instructed Second 79/2: 166-178.
Language Acquisition. Amsterdam: Mouton de Gruyter, pp. 199-234. Tarone, E. and B. Swierzbin. 2009 . Exploring Learner Language. Oxfo rd:
Spada, N . and M . Santos Lima. 2010. ' EFL and ESi. teacher and learner Oxfo rd University Press.
views about integrated and isolated form-focused instructi on.' Proceedings Taylor, D ., M. Caron, and L. McAlpine. 2000. 'Patterns o f perceived
of th e IX Inrern ati o nal Conference o f the Brazilian Assoc iatio n of C ana d ian lang uage abili ty and use in Arctic Q ue bec.' Bilingual Education and
Stud ies: Po rro Alegre, Brazil. BilinguaLism 3/4: 28 3-96.
Spad a , N., K. Barkaoui, C. Peters, M. So, and A. Valeo. 2009. Tomita, Y. 20 11. The Role of Form-Foc used Instruction : Learn er
.De veloping a questionnaire to m easure learners' preferen ces for isolated Investment in L2 C o mm un ication. Unpubl ished thesis . University of
and integrated form-focused instruction.' System, 37/ 1: 7 0- 8 1. Toronto. h ttps:!!rspace.library.utoronw.caJbitstream/1807/29892/ 1/
Spada, N., L. Jessop, W; Suzuki, Y. Tomita, and A. Vale o . 2009. The Tornira, Yasuyo_20 1106_PhD_thesis.pdf
Conrributions of Isolated an d Inregrated Fo rm-Focused Instruction Toohey, K. 2000. Learning EngLish at School: Identity. Social Relations and
on Different Aspects of L2 Knowledge and Use. Paper p resented at the Classroom Practice. C levedon: Multilingual Maners .
American Association for App lied Linguistics: Denver, CO.
Trahey, M . and L White. 1993. ' Positive evidence and preemption in
Spielmann, G. and M. J . Radnofsky. 2001 . ' Lear ning language under the second lang uage classroom.' Studies in Second Language Acquisition
tension: New directions from a qual itative study.' Modern Language journal 15/2: 18 1-204.
85/2 : 259-78 .
Tro6m ov ich, P. 2005. 'Spoken-wo rd process ing in native and second
Storc h, N. 2001 . ' Patterns of interaction in ESL pair work.' Language lang uages: An investigatio n o f auditory word priming.' Applied
Learning 52/1: 119-58. PsychoLinguistics 26/4: 479-504.
Swain, M. 1985. 'Comm unicative competence : Some roles o f Tr ofim ovich , P., P. M. Lightbown, R. H. Halter, an d H. Song. 2009.
comprehens ible input and comprehensib le o ut put in its develo pm ent ' 'Co m prehension- based practi ce: The develop m ent o f L2 pro n unciation
in S. Gass an d C. Madde n (eds.): Input in Second Language Acquisition. in a listen ing and read ing p rogram .' Studies in Second Language Acquisition
Rowley, MA: N ewb ury H o use. pp. 235 -53 . 31/4: 609-39.
Swain, M. 1988. 'M anipulating and complementing content teaching to VanPatten, B. 2004 . ' Inp ut pr ocessing in SLA' in B. Van Patte n (ed .):
maximize second language learning. ' TESL Cana da journal Gt s: 68-83. Processing Instruction: Theory, Research, and Commentary. Mahwah, N] :
Swain, M. 2000. 'The ou tput hypo th esis and beyond: Me diating Lawrence Erlba um and Associa tes. pp. 5-3 1.
acquisit io n th rou gh collabo rat ive d ialogue' in] . P. Lanro lf{ed. ): VanPatten , B. and T. Cadiemo. 1993. ' Explici t instructio n and input
Sociocultural Theory and Second Language Learning. Oxford: Oxfo rd processing .' Studies in Second Language A cquisition 15/2: 225-41 .
University Press. pp . 94-114.
246 Bibliography Bibliography 247
VanPatten, B. and]. Williams (eds.). 200 7 . Theories in Second Language White, L. 2003 . Second Language Acquisition and Universal Grammar.
Acquisition: An Introduction. Mahwah. NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum and Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Associates.
White, L., N . Spada, P. M. Lightbown, and L. Ranta. 1991. 'In put
Vygotsky, L. S. 1978 . Mind in Society. Cambridge, MA: Harvard enhancement and syntactic accu racy in L2 acquis ition.' Applied Li ngu istics
University Press. 12/4: 416-32.
Wajnryb, R 1992 . Classroom Observation Tasks: A Resource Bookfor Wode, H. 1978 . 'D evelopment al sequences in naturalistic L2 acquisition'
Language Teachers and Trainers. Ca m bridge: Camb ridge University Press. in E. M. H atch (ed.): Second Language Acquisition: A Book o/Readings.
Walker, R 20 10. Teaching the Pronunciation 0/English as a Lingua Franca. Rowley, MA: Newbury House. pp . 10 1-1 7.
O~ford: Oxford University Press. Wode, H. 1981. ' Language acqui sitional universals: A unified view of
Watson-Gegeo , K. A. 1992 . 'Thick explana tion in the ethnograph ic study language acqu isition' in H . Winirz (ed.): Native Language and Foreign
Langu age Acquisition. N ew York: Annals 0/the New YorkAcademy ofSciences
of child socialization: A longitudinal study of the p roblem of school ing
for Kwara'ae (Solomon Islands) child ren' in W A. Corsaro and P. J. M iller 379. pp. 218-34.
(eds .): Interpretive Approaches to Children's Socializa tion. Special issue ofNeu: Wong Fillmore, L. 1979 . 'Individ ual differences in second language
D irectionsfor Child Development 58: 5 1-66. acquisition' in C. J. Fillmo re, W -S. Y. Wang , and D . Kemp ler (eds.):
Individual Differences in Language Ability and Language Behavior.'
Werker,]. E, w: M . Weikum, and K. A. Yoshi da. 2006. 'Bilingual
speech processing in infants and adu lts' in P. McCardle and E. Hoff (eds.): New York: Acade mic P ress, pp. 203 -28. .
Childhood Bilingualism: Researchon Infancy through School Age. C levedon: Wong Fillmore, L. 2000. 'Loss of family languages: Should educarors be
Mu ltilingual Matters. pp.1 -18. concerned ?' Theory into Practice 39/4: 203 -10.
Wesche, M . B. 198 1. 'Language aptitude measure s in scream ing, matchin g Yule, G. and D . Macdonald. 1990. 'Resolving referential conflicts in
stu dents with metho ds, and diagnosis oflearni ng pro blems' in K. Diller L2 interaction: The effect of pr oficiency and interactive role.' Language
(ed. ): Individual Differences and Universals in Language Learning Aptitude. Learning 40/4: 539-56.
Rowley, MA: Newbury H ouse. pp. 119-39.
Zimmerman, C. B. 2009. WOrdKnowledge: A Voca bula ry Teacher's
White, J. 1998. 'Getting the learners' attention: A typographical input Ha ndbook. Oxford: Oxfo rd University Press.
enhancement stu dy' in C. Dou gh ty and J. Williams (eds.): Focus on Form in
Zobl, H. 1980. 'The formal and development al selectivity of Ll influence
Classroom SLA . Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp . 85- 113.
on L2 acquisition.' Language Learning 30 / 1: 43-5 7.
White,]. 2008. 'Speeding up acquisition of his/her. Explicit Ll/L2
Zobl, H. 1984. 'Th e wave mo del of linguistic change and interlanguage
contrasts help' in J . Philp , R. Oliver, and A. Mackey (eds.): Second Language
systems .' Studies in Second Language A cquisition 6/2: 160-85.
Acquisition and the Younger Learner: Child's Play? Amsterdam : John
Benjamins. pp. 193- 22 8.
White, J. and P. M. Lightbown. 1984 . 'Asking and answering in ESL
classes.' Canadian Modern Language Review 40/2: 228-44.
White, L. 1987. 'Againsr comprehensible input: the inp u t hypothesis an d
the development ofL2 competence.' Applied L ingu istics 8: 95-11 0.
White L. 1989. Universal Grammar and Second Language Acquisition.
Amsterdam: Joh n Benjamins.
White, L 1991. 'Adverb placeme nt in second language acquisition: Some
effects of positive and nega tive evidence in the classroom .' Second Language
Research 7/2: 133-61.
INDEX
Speech Seg me nts ) II7 aptitude, lan guage learning 78. 80-3
activi ties
arrirudes, lear n ers' 87-8 .
learn ing 35
auro ma ticiry 108-9
incerpretarion [[2
Barcelona Age Faeror proj ect 98
meaning [66
approach
learnlng 3-4
BleS (basic in te rpersonal communication
75-7
childhood bilingualism 30-3
130-1
'bo rrowed ' wo rds 63
affect 10 6
care take r talk se« child-directed speech;
age
characteristics of learn ers see lear ner
of learner 9:1.-6
(C H IL DES) :1.6
250 Index Index 25 1
children
com p rehe nsion-based instruction 159- [65. an d develo pme n tal stages 180
early start for lan guage learning 93. 96-9 ,
clauses 48-56
lan gu age
content-based language reachi ng (CBLD firSt language transfer causing 'errors' 42,
Engl ish as a ling ua fran ca (ELF ) 71
classroom s co nt ent an d langua ge-int egrated lea rnin g an d proc essing capab ility II6 •
217g
classroom instr uction and indi vid ual (CLIL) 147, 171, 215g
and pro nun ciatio n 69
env ironment. language 1(( also classroo ms;
d ifferences 92-6
focus o n usage [2+ [27-9. 2II- 12
cross-sect io nal stu d ies 8, 66 , 'l16g in put
classroom lear nin g 39
'get twO for one' 171-7
deaf ch ildren
amount of expos ure (tim e) im po rtant 38.
classroo m observation schemes [29-49 ,
and increasing th e tim e available for
and th e Cri tical Period Hypothesis 23
69, !l8, [48-9
2I4g
language study [48
and innatlsrn'zr
and th e behaviourist perspective 14-19
cognates 63, u 4g
cor p us linguistics 72, 202, 216g
developmental seq uences 217g
can be a sign of progress 4[ , 56, IIO
co llaboration
error co rrectio n in communicative
157- 8
eth nography [49-51. 217g
pai r work
and 'get it righ r in th e en d' 182-95
lan guage 57, [80--[
extroversion 84--6
and phonology 68
repetitio n 14!
q uestion formation [0-[2
acquis itio n o f, gene rally 5- 34
and pragmatics 67
self-correctio n vs other correctio n 194
of relative clauses 54-5
im po rtance of cont in ued dev elopment of
132- 3
classroom 139-45
and 'teach what is teach able' 177-82, [96
174, 175, 176. 20 4
Teaching (C O LD 129
wri tten corrective feedback 144 - 5
in first lan guage acq uisition 29-30
infl uence o n second language lear n ing
[58-9. 215g
counterbalance hypo th esis 1+3, 1I6 g disp lay questions [30, 145-9, 21n fluency
com pe ti tio n model Ill- 13
C rit ical Period H ypothesis (C PH ) 22-4. d rills
as aurornaticity II8
co m prehensible inp ut 106, 165-71
92- 6, 216g
audiolingu al pattern drills 157- 8
learn ing befo re accu racy in
and authenti c materials 209. 215g
cross-cul tu ral research . into first language
an d behaviourist approaches II7
com m unicative ap proaches 157
and pronunciation 70
94,95. 171
co nversational interaction 14, 165- 71
longitudinal studies 7-8. 9. 66. 89, 98, 149.
166.195
input processing n6
isolated form-focused instruction 191
in co nte nt-based language teaching
gram mar
struct ured input 81- 2
language acqui sition order see developmental
foreign er talk 39. 2l7g
imitation
teachabiliry hypothesis 177-82
learning styles 83-4
not always a predicto r of success 203-4
nego tiation of fo rm 139 , zz rg II!I! also processabiliry theory 1I6-17. 177, 182, 222g corpus Iingu isrics 72, 202, 216g as part of ph on o logy 68. 69 , 70 , 7t
form-focused ins tr uctio n p rocessing capac ity II6 cross-secrional stud ies 86. 216g in recasts 144
neurologi cal resear ch I13 processing instr uc tion 16 2, 164-5. 122g descriptive stud ies 143, 216g str uctural gradi ng 223g
no ticing hyp othesis II5, 177 . r83-4, 189, 2llg profi cien cy difficulry in findi ng co m parison groups srudenr-srude nc in teractio ns II!/! learn er
object pr onou ns 164 an d bil ingual ism 31- 2 157
learn er in teractions
o bligatory con texts 46-8, 158. az rg diffic ulties in resear ch ing 78-9 ethnography 149-51
su b mersi o n m ethods 32
obs ervatio n learner language an d proficiency level experime ntal stud ies r53-4
su btra ctive bili ngu alism 32-3. t74, 177 . 223g
ethno graphy r49-51 r67-8 lon gitu d inal st udies 86
sup rasegm enrals 68. 70 . 223g
ob servatio n schemes 129-48 pro m pts observa rio n schemes 129-49 large t lan guage 223g
op en q uesrio ns 147 clar ificatio n requests I14. 140, 188 q ualita tive researc h 86. 129. 149, 154. 222g task-based langu age teaching (TBLn 67. 165,
or der of acquisition ul g I I!I! developmen tal elicita tion 141. 188 q uan ti tative resear ch 86. 129, 153. 222g 190, u3g
seq uences enco uragi ng self-co rrection 188-9. 194 researcher paradox (researcher affecti ng teach ers
cvergene ralizar io n errors 221g p ron un cia tion 68-71
th e st udy ) 189 ro le in mo tivation 204
an d the behavio ur ist perspect ive 18- 19 psych o logical theories II!/! behaviourism:
researchi ng teache r- st ude nt int eractions teacher-lear ner in teractio ns 129-35. [45
in first language acq uis irio n 12 info rmario n proc essing mod el 129-35 teacher questio ning in th e classroom
and the info rma t io n processing model 110 pu be rty 94-5 sam pling issues 72 45-9
in pronunciation 68-9 q uest io ns for st udy ing learner characte ristics 77-8 teachers' mo tivati on al practices 88-9
in second language lear ning gene rally 44 child acquisirio n of ro-ta, 19 rest rucruring 1°9-10. 222g teach er talk 39. 224g
pair wo rk 168-9 , 170 . 209 display q uestions r30 routines (form ulai c lan guage) II!/! fo rm ulaic teach in g, classroo m I I!I! audio lin gual
paraphras ing 1I4 . 147 II!I! also recasts seco nd lan guage lear nin g 49-5r. 180 langu age app roach ; co mmun icative language
parent-child interacti on 26-7 teacher q uestioning in th e classroo m 145-9 scaffolding 25, u 8, 146-7, 223g teaching (CLn ; co ntent -based
pasl tense 55-{). 173 rate oflearnin g 96 , 97 . 222g seco nd lan gua ge 159, 223g lan gu age teach ing (CBLn ; focus on
patt ern p ractice drills 157-8. 22rg readi ness ro learn [8c>-1 school years , an d first lan guage acq uisitio n meaning; form -focused instruction;
patt erns in lan guage. lear nin g 18 reading 13- 14 grammar translati on me thods; task
pee r group, lear n ing fro m I19 II!I! also effect of literacy o n firsr language segrne n tals 68. 70. 123g based language teachin g (TB Ln
lear ner-lear ner im eracrions deve lopmen t 13 self-co rrec tion vs o ther cor rect ion 194 'teach w hat is teach able' 177-82. 210
perceptio n of so un ds 69 , 70 'just listen" . an d read' 159-65 self-re pe ti tio n U4 technology
perceptually-b ased lear ning sryles 83 and practice II7 sensitive pe riod (Critical Period H ypothesis) co m puter -base d tools fo r sam pling 72
perfo rm an ce 12, 45, 71. 79. 85. 94 , 96, 107 , p ro blems in lear ning to read 29-30 22-4,9 2- 6 in put for ch ild lang uage acq uisitio n needs
117, 186, u lg read ing as a sourc e of voca bulary growth seq uential bilingual s 30 to be h umans not electronic 6, 28
personal characterist ics of lear ners II!I! lear ner 63-.f, 206 sho rr-re rrn memory 8C>-1 'teleg raph ic' sen te nces, ba bies' 6-7
characteristics readi ng m aterial fo r learn ers 161-2 sign lan guage 23 timing
perso nal ity 84-6 recasts 2Ug Sign ifican t di fference [59, 189 , 223g amount of exposure (tim e) in new
pho nemi c d isti nctio ns 222g comparison o f feedback th at co rrects vs silence. use of in classroom I.p . 149 language impo rtant 38, 69, u S, 148- 9
infants' ab ility 6 p ro m p ts 194 sim pli fica tio n 44 . 114, 223g
seco nd language learners 69 in co nten t-base d classroo ms 139-45
256 Index