Segmental Phonology Outline
Segmental Phonology Outline
Segmental Phonology Outline
Segmental phonology
Recall:
• phonetics: the physical manifestation of language in sound waves; how these sounds are
articulated and perceived
• phonology: the mental representation of sounds as part of a symbolic cognitive system;
how abstract sound categories are manipulated in the processing of language
So phonetics deals with the physiological and acoustic parts of the path between speaker and
listener, while phonology resides in the brain.
Phonemes
• consonants: p, t, k, b, d, g, f, θ, s, h, v, ð, z, m, n, ŋ, l, r, w, j, č, ȷˇ, š, ž
"distinctive" = can be used to make contrasts between different words. For the stops, using
minimal pairs (words that differ in exactly one sound):
For the vowels (for each individual pair of vowels we could come up with a minimal pair):
bead booed bid bade bowed bed bud bad bod bide Boyd
heed who'd hid hood head hawed had hide how'd
jean June jin Jane Jen John join lead
lewd lid laid load lead lad lod loud Lloyd
Allophones.
There are often differences in the way a phoneme is pronounced in a specific context. The
variant pronunciations are called allophones ("other sounds").
1
When it's important to make this difference:
• we'll use [square brackets] to indicate sounds from a phonetic point of view, i.e. focusing
on their physical properties and the details of actual pronunciation;
• and we'll use /slashes/ to indicate sounds from a phonological point of view, i.e. as part of
an abstract representation independent of potential differences in the way the sound in
pronounced in specific contexts.
• I.e., [ ] = allophone, / / = phoneme.
A classic example of sound alternation in English relates to the [s] found at the beginning of a
syllable before a voiceless stop.
spin is basically pin with [s] added, but the /p/ in each case is pronounced differently.
• pin contains an aspirated version of /p/, with a puff of air after the stop is released; [ph]
• spin contains a plain /p/, without a puff of air after the stop; this is written just [p]
The same is true for pairs like pit~spit, pot~spot, pair~spare, etc.
But the same generalization holds not just for /p/ but for the other voiceless stops, /t/ and /k/.
Compare these word pairs:
So more accurately, there's a single general statement that covers all these cases, stated in terms
of natural classes.
How do we know that aspirated and unaspirated voiceless stops are not different phonemes?
Same way Lois Lane knew Clark Kent was the Superman: they never appear at the same place
in the same time. = complementary distribution.
2
• complementary distribution: never at the same place in the same time, predictable
Expanding: voiceless stops are aspirated when they occur syllable-initially and are followed by a
stressed vowel (rápid, raph ídity); & word-initially regardless of stress (photháto).
At the beginning of a word, a preceding /s/ prevents the stop from being syllable- or word-initial.
Different stresses cause alternations: underlying sound /t/ is pronounced as [t] etc. or [th] etc.
This process is completely unconscious for most speakers, and often quite hard to unlearn.
English speakers who learn a language like French or Spanish impose aspiration according to
this rule; but that's wrong for these languages, and sounds foreign.
Similarly, a French or Spanish speaker learning English will typically fail to produce aspiration
in the right places; this is part of what it means to have a foreign accent.
• it's learned unconsciously by children imitating (quite accurately!) the details of the
language around them
• it's systematic, applying to all words with voiceless stops, not just some random selection
• it's defined in terms of a natural class (here "voiceless stops") rather than some arbitrary
set of three consonants
The study of phonology is largely the investigation of alternations like this -- what changes
occur, what sounds undergo them, and in what contexts.
Example: flapping
A prominent feature of American English affects /t/ and /d/, and is called flapping.
A flap is a quick motion with the tongue against the alveolar ridge.
All these English words have flaps where "t" or "d" is written in the spelling (relevant dialects).
butter caddy
3
pretty buddy
little water
The proper phonetic symbol for a flap is ɾ - it's an "r" missing the top left serif.
For most speakers, in the right context a phonological /t/ will end up sounding phonetically just
like a phonological /d/, since both become a flap [ɾ] (voiced), as these homophones show:
latter ladder
matter madder
mettle meddle
betting bedding
outty (belly button) Audi (car)
Of course, /t/ and /d/ don't always end up as flaps: minimal pairs illustrate.
hit hid
tin din
tear dare
melting melding
attain A Dane
1. The /t/ or /d/ has to be between vowels (this includes a syllabic [r] or [l])
o so not in hit, melting
2. The following vowel has to be unstressed.
o so not in tin, attain
If you compare the list of homophones (with flapping) vs. minimal pairs (without flapping),
you'll see that only the homophones satisfy both these conditions.
The same basic word (or word root, or morpheme) can change. This includes adding a vowel:
4
spot [t] spotty [ɾ]
mad [d] madder, maddest [ɾ]
bird [d] birdy [ɾ]
So aspiration and flapping are not learned in individual words (such as flapped latter), but are
part of what we know about the language.
There are other allophones of the phoneme /t/: [t] [ʔ] [ɾ] [th]
When these sounds occur in other languages, they may be distinct phonemes:
Also: sounds that are distinct phonemes in English, might be allophones in another language
Phonological processes
5
For instance, the different pronunciations of the plural suffix "s" (phonetics lectures) involve:
• Insertion of vowel after sibilants: churches, judges, kisses, guises
• Assimilation in voicing after all other sounds: picks, pigs
Assimilation
e.g. alveolar stops assimilate in place of articulation to the following consonant
I can see, I can bake, I can go
Dissimilation
e.g. Middle English purpre --> Modern English purple
Insertion
e.g. a voiceless stop between a nasal and a voiceless fricative: hamster, dance;
also /j/ insertion after initial alveolar consonants and before high vowels: few, puny, ?news
Deletion
e.g. /h/ may be deleted from unstressed syllables: He handed her his hat;
also /r/ is deleted after a vowel in some dialects: /ka/, /yad/
Exchange = Metathesis
e.g. Old English thridda --> Modern English third; acsian vs ascian (Old English)
Strengthening = Fortition
e.g. aspiration rule above
Weakening = Lenition
e.g. flapping rule above
Fronting
e.g. velar to alveolar nasal ("g"-dropping): running
Other
e.g. prenasal [I] lowering in the South, parts of Midwest, nad parts of the West of the US
Multiple processes
• Raising diphthong /aI/: followed by a voiceless consonant, it is "raised" so the first part is
more like the first vowel of mother than that of father.
Ø, b, d, v, z,ð, m, n, l, r, ȷˇ, ž
tie, jibe, hide, live, rise, tithe, time, line, tile, tire, oblige,
p, t, k, f, s ...
hype, white, bike, life, ice ...
• This distinction between [aI] and raised [əI] is there even when the voicing distinction is
eliminated by flapping. So if a speaker has raising in write, this is maintained in writer, while
rider will have [aI] just like ride.
So: while flapping eliminates the distinction in consonants, the words still do not rhyme.
6
Most speakers think the difference lies in the consonants. In the pronunciation, it’s in the vowels.
• The reason why speakers "hear" the difference in the consonants is because
o on an abstract level in their minds, the words are represented as /raI ter/ and
/raI der/, with the difference localized in the consonant.
o raising of /aI/ in the former, & flapping in both are subsequent & unconscious.
How does raising keep working when the conditioning factor on raising - voicing on the
following consonant - has been obliterated?
Raising the /aI/ in writer on analogy to write, where the conditioning factor is still intact?? NO
Speakers apply phonological rules to these representations in some order, so that the output
of one rule can be the input to another:
• writer
• rider