Emojis
Emojis
1
https://www.engadget.com/2014/04/30/you-may-be-
accidentally-sending-friends-a-hairy-heart-emoji/
example where Crying Face (😢) can mean sadness in one 3. With whom do you use the special emoji? Multiple
culture, in another might mean sleeping2. choice answer with ‘other’ free-text option
4. Which emoji is it? Short text response
Technology mediated personal communication
5. What does that emoji mean? Short text response
Much of the emoji literature has focused on cross-cultural
6. Why do you use this emoji? Short text response
and cross-language differences in meaning and
7. Why do you have an emoji shorthand for this thing?
understanding at national and continental scales. However, Short text response
technology mediated communication platforms are often
used to create very personal and private places for None of the questions were compulsory. If a participant
communication [14] meaning differences in usage can be answered “No” to the Question 2 then they were thanked for
observed in much smaller communities. Previous work has the time and the survey ended, they were not asked any
found that emoji can play and important role in the digital further questions.
creation and maintenance of personal relationships. Kelly
For Question 4, simple advice was given to participants on
and Watts explored how people “appropriate” emoji for
how to bring up the emoji keyboard on a Mac device
purposes other than expressing emotion, show that people
(ctrl+⌘+space). Entering emoji on Windows and Linux
use them to simply maintain connections with another
person, to add a playful element to communications, or to devices is more complicated and so participants using these
create a “shared uniqueness”, for example by telling stories platforms were advised to type a description of the emoji if
generated from randomly chosen emoji [9]. However, this they were unable to locate the emoji pictogram when
behaviour was reported from a small proportion of responding to the survey. Of the 72 valid responses, 17
participants and the usage was ephemeral; the meaning of participants reported the emoji in text description form.
certain emoji did not permanently change. Procedure
The survey was advertised on Twitter and Facebook. We
In this paper we consider the specific cultures that develop
attempted to use snowball sampling by requesting “retweets”
in small groups: partners, families and friends. In particular,
or “shares” from both participants and non-participants. In
we focus on how these groups actively repurpose emoji to
total the survey was retweeted 37 times on Twitter and
serve new functions, their motivations for these
shared 7 times on Facebook. The survey was also advertised
repurposings, and how the affordances of different emoji
on the research ‘subreddit’ of Reddit, the content aggregating
influence how they get repurposed. We define “repurposing”
website. Survey completion was not reimbursed in any way,
as giving an emoji a specific and constant meaning beyond
so there was no particular incentive for participants to
the initial “intention” of the emoji designer; this meaning
mindlessly work through the whole survey. Given this lack
would be inaccessible to an outside observer without
of incentive, the survey was kept purposefully succinct and
explanation. What is specific to this paper, is the
did not require a large amount of demographic data from the
consideration of personalised repurposing. The act of
participants. By keeping the survey short, we aimed to
repurposing has been reported in large groups (e.g., [1]), but
reduce the drop-off rate. The survey could comfortably be
here we consider emoji repurposing in smaller micro
completed in two minutes.
communities. We contribute a three dimensional analysis of
emoji repurposing, exploring why users need to use an emoji Participants
at all, why that particular emoji was chosen, and what The survey was completed by 134 participants. Fifty-seven
sentiment they intend to convey. participants (43%) reported that they did not use a
repurposed emoji so did not provide any further data. One
Method
participant was removed for providing fabricated data
A brief web-based survey was created using the Google
intended to be amusing. Four participants’ data were
Forms web app. The survey was piloted before dissemination
removed for reporting emoji use considered to be widely
and the questions unchanged. However, Question 4 had
understood and therefore neither repurposed nor personal
further instructions appended to help participants enter an
emoji if they were on a desktop computer. The survey (for instance, the ☺ emoji to represent happiness and the 😉
contained the following questions (with response types are to be suggestive). In total 72 responses were used in the data
in italics): analysis. Of these participants, three were between 0-17
years old, 28 were between 18-29, 35 were between 30-39
1. How old are you? Age range response and six were over 40.
2. Is there an emoji you use that has a special meaning for
Data Processing
just you and the recipient? (The meaning would be
The survey was open for 14 days to collect responses. After
unclear to a third party seeing that emoji) Yes/No
this, the data was cleaned to remove null responses and
response
responses that were not considered to fit the request of being
2
https://www.acuitytranslations.com/blog/item/becky-
kinnersley
personal or repurposed. Emoji were all assigned their Friend, Family Member, Family group chat, friend group
Unicode name (e.g., 🛀 would be person taking a bath) and chat, other. Partner was the most commonly reported
were categorised according to the common categories used recipient for the repurposed emoji, with 47% of participants
on Emojipedia, Apple’s iOS keyboard, and Google’s reporting they used a special emoji with a partner. Figure 2
Android keyboard (Smileys & People, Animals & Nature, shows the distribution of people with whom participants used
Food & Drink, Activity, Travel & Places, Objects, Symbols, their repurposed emoji.
Flags). The free text data from questions 4-7 were then Five participants used the “Other” category to report their
openly coded for common themes. A Content Analysis emoji recipient. Two reported a sexual partner, two reported
procedure was followed, with open codes being generated to using the emoji in a public space (e.g., Twitter) and one
cover the question of how that emoji was chosen (relating to reported using it for themselves. Out of all 72 responses, 85%
Question 6 of the survey), why an emoji is needed (relating of respondents reported using the emoji with just a single
to Question 7 of the survey) and what theme the emoji other person, whereas only 15% reported using it in a group
covered (relating to Question 5 of the survey). These codes situation.
were generated by one researcher, and validated
independently by a second researcher. Discrepancies were
discussed and a final code assigned. For each of the three
components (e.g., “Why an emoji is needed”), only one code
was assigned for each response. No component could have
more than one code.
This shows that repurposed emoji can be both the cause of a The use of repurposed emoji in this study also reflects the
new understanding within a microculture (e.g., accidentally way that “nicknames” are generated and used in spoken
making a typo and attributing it meaning post hoc) or the languages: names are shortened for efficiency (Ease in this
symbolic representation of an existing understanding (e.g., study), nicknames can also arise because they are ironically
choosing a symbol to represent an in joke that has already opposite to the person they describe (Irony) or can use, for
been established in face to face communication). example, an animal or other word entirely to refer to
someone (Image of Word, Historical).
Visual affordance of emoji
Some users reported using the visual representation of an In these ways, the findings in the current study do not detail
emoji and repurposing it to mean something visually similar. novel or new human behaviour, but instead highlight how
Wijeratne, Balasuriya, Sheth, and Doran [25] explain that such common behaviours can manifest in new forms of
similarity of emoji should be based upon their semantic technology mediated communication. In particular , we show
meaning, not their visual similarities. However, here we have how the affordances and constraints of the specific context
observed that people do indeed make use of that visual of emoji influence the expression of this common human
feature of emoji when ascribing meaning to it. behaviour.
Using emoji in this way is a phenomena that has been noted Limitations
on a larger scale in the study conducted by the Emojipedia There are a number of limitations in the current study, which
website, who discovered that the Peach emoji (🍑) is most we believe represents an initial exploration of the
commonly used to refer to buttocks, rather than the fruit [1]. repurposing of emoji.
This is due to its visual similarity. The wide spread
We would firstly like to address the demographic data Additionally, the redrawing of emoji must be carefully
collected. A future extension of this work could explore how considered. Just as users reacted badly to Apple redrawing
repurposing varies across culture. Just as the interpretation the Peach emoji, smaller communities of other users may be
and use of emoji changes depending on country of use, the affected by redrawing of any number of emoji currently in
act of repurposing may also vary. Understanding the device use because of how they look, and not what they represent.
upon which the emoji is most commonly used, and the
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
medium would also help in understanding whether
We would like to thank all the survey respondents for sharing
repurposing is limited to certain aspects of personal digital
the ways in which they repurpose emoji. We appreciate the
communication.
candidness of replies and have striven to ensure anonymity
This survey allowed users to enter free text to describe their of all participants. We would also like to thank the reviewers
responses. In doing so, we were able to generate a set of and committee members who reviewed this work for their
codes to explain and explore the use of repurposed emoji. valuable insights and helpful suggestions for improvements.
Future work could expand upon these codes by additionally REFERENCES
collecting interview data to further explore the origins and 1. Hamdan Azhar. 2016. How We Really Use The
usage of repurposed emoji. Peach. Emojipedia. Retrieved from
Future Work https://blog.emojipedia.org/how-we-really-use-the-
The current study represents an initial exploration of the peach/
phenomenon of personalised emoji repurposing. Building 2. Paul Baker. 2003. Polari-the lost language of gay
upon this work, we can see that future work might consider men. Routledge.
how repurposing varies across geographic location [3] or by
age or other demographic characteristics [5]. 3. Francesco Barbieri, German Kruszewski, Francesco
Ronzano, and Horacio Saggion. 2016. How
Further work can be conducted to investigate the wider Cosmopolitan Are Emojis?: Exploring Emojis
applicability of the codes generated in this paper. Is the list Usage and Meaning over Different Languages with
of codes generated comprehensive, for instance? Distributional Semantics. Proceedings of the 2016
CONCLUSION ACM on Multimedia Conference, 531–535.
In this paper we have highlighted how people using http://doi.org/10.1145/2964284.2967278
computer and mobile mediated communication have come to 4. Edwin Chan, Teddy Seyed, Wolfgang Stuerzlinger,
repurpose certain emoji. By using the ambiguity in meaning Xing-Dong Yang, and Frank Maurer. 2016. User
that emoji naturally have, users can create a shared personal Elicitation on Single-hand Microgestures.
meaning between themselves and another person, or small Proceedings of the 2016 CHI Conference on
group, creating a new cultural understanding of particular Human Factors in Computing Systems, 3403–3414.
emoji. This in turn can help people feel closer to one another. http://doi.org/10.1145/2858036.2858589
Previous work has tended to explore emoji understanding on 5. Zhenpeng Chen, Xuan Lu, Sheng Shen, Wei Ai,
a large cultural level, whereas this work looks at emoji Xuanzhe Liu, and Qiaozhu Mei. 2017. Through a
understanding at a micro scale. Gender Lens: An Empirical Study of Emoji Usage
Results of this paper will contribute to future work into emoji over Large-Scale Android Users. 1–20. Retrieved
sentiment analysis, as the work highlights that emoji do not from http://arxiv.org/abs/1705.05546
always correspond to their intended, nor culturally accepted 6. Henriette Cramer, Paloma de Juan, and Joel
meanings. At times emoji are chosen at random to mean a Tetreault. 2016. Sender-intended Functions of
specific concept, and equally some emoji are chosen Emojis in US Messaging. Proceedings of the 18th
purposefully because they convey the complete opposite of International Conference on Human-Computer
the intended sentiment. This implies caution is required Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services, 504–
when using machine learning techniques to understand the 509. http://doi.org/10.1145/2935334.2935370
meaning and use of emoji.
7. Maurizio Gotti. 1999. The language of thieves and
As with other papers in this area [13,21], we also conclude vagabonds: 17th and 18th century canting
that a universal rendering of emoji may be required to lexicography in England. Walter de Gruyter.
standardise how they appear across platforms. It is well 8. Jialun “Aaron” Jiang, Jed R. Brubaker, and Casey
understood that emoji facial expressions can be Fiesler. 2017. Understanding Diverse
misconstrued, for example, but this paper also highlights that Interpretations of Animated GIFs. CHI’17 Extended
discrepancies in rendering can also affect a whole range of Abstracts, 1726–1732.
emoji that people choose to use due to the way they are http://doi.org/10.1145/3027063.3053139
drawn. The current method of rendering emoji in a range of
different ways may be preventing users from using the 9. Ryan Kelly and Leon Watts. 2015. Characterising
implicit affordance of emoji. the Inventive Appropriation of Emoji as
Relationally Meaningful in Mediated Close Conference Companion Publication on Designing
Personal Relationships. Experiences of Technology Interactive Systems - DIS ’17 Companion, ACM
Appropriation: Unanticipated Users, Usage, Press, 7–12.
Circumstances, and Design. http://doi.org/10.1145/3064857.3079109
10. Brian Koerber. 2016. People are mad as hell after 19. Swiftkey. 2015. Swiftkey Emoji Report. Retrieved
Apple ruins the peach emoji. Mashable. Retrieved from
from http://mashable.com/2016/11/01/peach-emoji- http://www.scribd.com/doc/262594751/SwiftKey-
butt-no-more/#y0IZqe3q7iqd Emoji-Report
11. Xuan Lu, Wei Ai, Xuanzhe Liu, et al. 2016. 20. K Thorpe, R Greenwood, a Eivers, and M Rutter.
Learning from the ubiquitous language. 2001. Prevalence and developmental course of
Proceedings of the 2016 ACM International Joint “secret language”. International journal of
Conference on Pervasive and Ubiquitous language & communication disorders / Royal
Computing - UbiComp ’16, ACM Press, 770–780. College of Speech & Language Therapists 36, 1,
http://doi.org/10.1145/2971648.2971724 43–62. http://doi.org/10.1080/13682820120395
12. Hannah Miller, Daniel Kluver, Jacob Thebault- 21. Garreth W Tigwell and David R. Flatla. 2016. “ Oh
Spieker, Loren Terveen, and Brent Hecht. 2017. that’s what you meant !”: Reducing Emoji
Understanding Emoji Ambiguity in Context: The Misunderstanding. Proceedings of the 18th
Role of Text in Emoji-Related Miscommunication. International Conference on Human-Computer
Proceedings of the Eleventh International AAAI Interaction with Mobile Devices and Services
Conference on Web and Social Media, 152–161. Adjunct (MobileHCI ’16), 859–866.
Retrieved from http://doi.org/2957265.2961844
https://www.aaai.org/ocs/index.php/ICWSM/ICWS 22. Unicode Inc. 2017. Emoji and Pictographs.
M17/paper/view/15703 Retrieved from
13. Hannah Miller, Jacob Thebault-Spieker, Shuo http://www.unicode.org/faq/emoji_dingbats.html
Chang, Isaac Johnson, Loren Terveen, and Brent 23. Joseph B. Walther and Kyle P. D’Addario. 2001.
Hecht. 2016. “Blissfully happy” or “ready to fight”: The Impacts of Emoticons on Message
Varying Interpretations of Emoji. International Interpretation in Computer-Mediated
AAAI Conference on Web and Social Media, Communication. Social Science Computer Review
Icwsm, 259–268. 19, 3, 324–347.
http://doi.org/10.1089/cyber.2011.0179 http://doi.org/10.1177/089443930101900307
14. Midas Nouwens, Carla Griggio, and Wendy E 24. Sanjaya Wijeratne, Lakshika Balasuriya, Amit
Mackay. 2017. “WhatsApp is for family; Sheth, and Derek Doran. 2017. EmojiNet : An Open
Messenger is for friends”: Communication Places Service and API for Emoji Sense Discovery. the
in App Ecosystems. CHI ’17 - Proceedings of the 11Th International Aaai Conference on Web and
2017 CHI Conference on Human Factors in Social Media (Icwsm-17), May, 437–446. Retrieved
Computing Systems, 727–735. from
15. Oxford Dictionaries. 2015. Word of the Year 2015. https://www.aaai.org/ocs/index.php/ICWSM/ICWS
Retrieved from M17/paper/view/15551
https://en.oxforddictionaries.com/word-of-the- 25. Sanjaya Wijeratne, Lakshika Balasuriya, Amit
year/word-of-the-year-2015 Sheth, and Derek Doran. 2017. A Semantics-Based
16. Jaram Park, Vladimir Barash, Clay Fink, and Measure of Emoji Similarity.
Meeyoung Cha. 2013. Emoticon Style : Interpreting http://doi.org/10.1145/3106426.3106490
Differences in Emoticons Across Cultures.
Proceedings of the 7th International AAAI
Conference on Weblogs and Social Media
(ICWSM), 466–475.
17. Ann Senghas, Sotaro Kita, and Asli Özyürek. 2004.
Children Creating Core Properties of Language:
Evidence from an Emerging Sign Language in
Nicaragua. Science 305, 5691, 1779–1782.
http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1100199
18. Selina Sutton and Shaun Lawson. 2017. A
Provocation for Rethinking and Democratising
Emoji Design. Proceedings of the 2016 ACM