Treatment of Apraxia
Treatment of Apraxia
Treatment of Apraxia
1.
Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute and Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia;
2
Albuquerque VAMC and University of New Mexico;
3.
Human Motor Control Section, NINDS, NIH, Bethesda, MD
4.
Department of V.A. Affairs and the Baltimore Geriatric Research Education and
Clinical Center
5.
North Florida/South Georgia Veterans Health System and University of Florida,
Gainesville, FL
Laurel J. Buxbaum, Psy.D., Moss Rehabilitation Research Institute, 1200 West Tabor
1
Treatment of Limb Apraxia: Moving Forward to Improved Action
Abstract
frequently associated with stroke and degenerative diseases such as Alzheimer Disease.
Despite evidence that several types of limb apraxia significantly impact functional
Additionally, although the most disabling types of apraxia reflect damage to gesture
and/or object memory systems, existing treatments have not fully taken advantage of
principles of experience known to affect learning and neural plasticity. We review the
current state of the art in the rehabilitation of limb apraxia, indicate possible points of
contact with the learning literature, and generate suggestions for how translational
disabling disorder.
2
Introduction
providing them a processing advantage (i.e., in terms of accuracy and response time) as
compared with less-practiced movements. Although several types of apraxia have clear
impact upon functional abilities, and are common consequences of stroke, Alzheimer
about the definitions, distinctiveness, and mechanisms of various types of apraxia, and
indeed, whether any have critical functional significance. In addition, although the most
disabling types of apraxia reflect damage to systems involved in movement and gesture
representation (i.e., memory), the nascent apraxia treatment literature has not taken
advantage of principles of experience known to affect skill learning. The aim of this
article is to review the current state of the rehabilitation of limb apraxia, and based on the
learning and plasticity literature, generate suggestions for how translational principles
Definitions of apraxia
The term ‘apraxia’ was introduced by Steinthal 1. While this word is derived
from Greek and literally means without action, the term apraxia is used to describe a
series of papers written between 1900 and 1920 by Hugo Liepmann 2-4. Liepmann
described three forms of apraxia which, by virtue of his careful evaluations and
3
discussions, brought about a ‘paradigmatic shift’ in our understanding of motor control.
These three types were limb kinetic apraxia (also called melokinetic apraxia or
innervatory apraxia), ideomotor apraxia, and ideational apraxia. To this triad Rothi,
Heilman, Ochipa and colleagues 5-7 added another type, termed conceptual apraxia, and
DeRenzi as well as Heilman 8, 9 described a fifth type now called dissociation apraxia.
In this manuscript we will focus on ideomotor apraxia (hereafter, IMA), for two
recognized that IMA has important functional consequences, and the disorder is thus in
pantomime skilled tool-use movements with abnormal joint angles and limb trajectories,
and with uncoupling of the spatial and temporal aspects of movement 13. Spatiotemporal
15, 16
errors persist to a lesser degree with actual tool use . The deficit is not restricted to
17-19
meaningful movements, and has also been observed in meaningless postures and
20, 21
sequences . IMA is also associated with cognitive deficits in declarative knowledge
22 23
of the action appropriate to objects , impairments in mechanical problem-solving ,
21, 24-26 27, 28
deficits in motor planning , and difficulty learning new gestures . Testing for
objects, e.g., brush teeth) and intransitive (symbolic movements without objects, e.g.,
sign for crazy) movements, imitation of the examiner performing transitive, intransitive,
4
and novel meaningless movements, and gesture in response to seeing and holding actual
recognition (“representational” IMA) and IMA with intact recognition (“dynamic” IMA;
11, 29, 30
. In representational IMA, inability to discriminate correctly from incorrectly
performed meaningful object related hand movements correlates strongly with ability to
produce the same movements, suggesting that the same representations are likely to
underlie both 31. Additionally, representational (but not dynamic) IMA patients are
related movements 32. This in turn suggests that the damaged system underlying
The Functional Implications of Limb Apraxia: Does Limb Apraxia Matter in the
Real World?
Historically, most clinicians and researchers believed that limb apraxia had little or no
real world implications 4, 10, 33-35. This is emphasized by DeRenzi, who wrote, “…apraxia
artificial request.” Although not specified, it appears that this view was particular to IMA
and stemmed from the notion that apraxia was present when pantomimes to command
and imitation were tested but improved when the use of actual objects were examined.
It is now widely believed that IMA impairs real world functioning, but there are
still remarkably few studies demonstrating such a relationship. In addition, most studies
to date are fraught with problems. First, these studies usually do not rule out the
5
influence of all other factors, such as hemiparesis. They commonly compare the
performance of apraxic and nonapraxic patients with left hemisphere damage 36-41, but
relative to nonapraxics, apraxics are often more impaired in other domains, such as
language and sensory and motor skills. Therefore, it is difficult to know if limb apraxia is
the best predictor of functional skills. Second, apraxics typically have larger lesions than
patients without apraxia, and those lesions more frequently damage the left parietal and
frontal regions 42 that are also important for many other cognitive functions that could
again confound the findings. Regression approaches have been used in order to evaluate
the unique impact of various factors including limb apraxia on activities of daily living 41,
43-45
, in some cases after controlling statistically for factors such as lesion size, primary
motor deficits, and/or other cognitive deficits. However, these studies usually suffer
from statistical problems related to a small number of subjects relative to the number of
predictors examined.
the use of a wide variety of functional measures, including object use 46, 47, performance-
based measures of activities of daily living 36, 37, 39, 41, 43, 48, 49, and caregiver or patient
report of daily functioning 39, 44, 45, 50. These outcome measures vary in complexity from
51
isolated object use, such as brushing teeth , to simulated activities of daily living, such
as picking up a bean with a spoon 38, 39, 41, to instrumental activities of daily living, such
as eating a meal 36, dressing 49, 52, preparing food 43, 48, 53-55 or changing batteries in a
context of the specific study. In addition, there are significant problems with obtaining
6
reliable measures of these skills because the tasks are usually quite complex and the
dependent upon a great number of cognitive abilities, patients may be impaired for
Taken together, these problems in the literature suggest that future studies must 1)
examine the relationship of different types of limb apraxia to real world functioning
(activities of daily living and instrumental activities) of various kinds; and 2) utilize
sufficiently large groups of patients to provide sufficient power for analysis. It is also
reasonable to consider at least two different approaches for subject recruitment. The first
approach examines well- characterized patients with unilateral focal lesions, and the
second approach examines a broader range of patients with and without limb apraxia
without regard to lesion location. The latter approach may yield patients more broadly
Finally, some of the most innovative work in this area attempts to identify
cognitive mechanisms that are associated with ideomotor limb apraxia and potentially
with the resulting deficits in real world functioning (see 58 for a review). These cognitive
46
processes include mechanical problem solving , sequence planning and organization 21,
the ability to develop and/or retrieve optimal motor programs 13, knowledge of how to
manipulate an object 22, 25, 59, and knowledge of optimal hand position when real world
7
Treatment of Limb Apraxia
A recent review of the literature on treatment of limb apraxia yielded reports of ten
treatment approaches, many of which were single case studies. Methods reported were
Multiple Cues. The multiple cues treatment method was developed in 1991 by Maher,
Rothi & Greenwald 60 for a 55 year old male with chronic ideomotor apraxia and intact
cues, including tools, objects, visual models, and feedback. Errors were corrected using
systematically withdrawn. The individual participated in one hour sessions daily for two
weeks. The multiple cues method resulted in positive effects, with treated gestures
assessed.
Error Reduction. In an attempt to define the active components of the multiple cues
method, Ochipa and colleagues 61, 62 conducted a treatment study aimed at treating
specific error types. Two males, 44 and 66 years old, with chronic Broca’s aphasia and
Treatment duration and intensity varied, with the 44 year old receiving treatment four
times per week (n=44 sessions) and the 66 year old receiving treatment two times a day
twice a week (n=24 sessions). The goals of treatment consisted of reduction of external
configuration, movement, and internal configuration errors, depending upon the error
training the individual to correctly orient his hand to objects, while reduction of internal
8
configuration errors involved positioning of hand and fingers to accommodate a tool.
Movement errors were reduced through verbal descriptions to guide joint movement
while gesturing. Only one error type was addressed at a time and feedback was only
provided about the error type being trained. Error reduction treatment resulted in a
significant and lasting improvement on treated gestures for both individuals. However,
noted to continue at 2 week post treatment follow-up, but later follow-ups were not
performed.
Six Stage Task Hierarchy. The task hierarchy method was developed by Code & Gaunt
63
who studied in an individual with severe chronic aphasia, ideomotor apraxia and
ideational apraxia. This six stage task hierarchical treatment for limb apraxia was a
modification of an Eight-Step Continuum used for treatment of apraxia of speech 64. The
Code and Gaunt method involves requiring the patient to produce target words and signs
in various combinations and in concert with the therapist in response to a therapist model
weekly for 8 months. The six stage task hierarchy method resulted in acquisition of
trained signs and a non-significant trend toward improvement in untrained signs during
treatment. Maintenance of effects was not formally tested, but the authors provide
anecdotal reports of the patient’s continued use of signs in group treatment sessions.
Conductive Education. The conductive education method was developed by Pilgrim &
Humphreys 65for a patient with head injury and chronic unimanual apraxia of the non-
9
of goal-directed tasks. The treatment began with physical manipulation plus
verbalization of task elements (e.g., “reach the beaker, clasp the beaker, carry to my lips,
drink, stop”) and those cues were systematically withdrawn as performance improved.
There were daily 15 minute sessions for 3 weeks. The conductive education method
assessed.
technique for individuals with ADL (Activities of Daily Living) impairment secondary to
apraxia. This method was first described in the literature in a study of 33 individuals
with apraxia secondary to left hemisphere stroke 66. The patients were trained on three
ADLs, and the specific method of treatment was chosen based on each individual’s
utilizing 5ADLs was studied in another group of 56 individuals with left hemisphere
stroke and subsequent apraxia. Both strategy training approaches focused on the use of
strategies (ie, use of pictures) to maximize independence. The duration and intensity of
treatments varied among individuals in both studies. Strategy training resulted in positive
outcomes across all domains measured (effect sizes .37 for the ADL tasks and .47 for the
Barthel ADL index; both significantly greater than for patients receiving usual
occupational therapy treatment), but the improvements were not lasting 67, 68. In the final
study in this series, there was an additional finding of interest; namely, maintenance of
10
The transitive/intransitive gesture training method was investigated by Smania and
colleagues 69 in 22 individuals at least two months post onset of a left hemisphere stroke
with subsequent ideomotor limb apraxia. Treatment focused on the training of transitive
and intransitive gestures. Transitive gesture training consisted of three phases in which
the individual was (1) shown use of common tools, (2) shown a static picture of a portion
of the transitive gesture and asked to produce the pantomime, and (3) shown a picture of
common tool and asked to produce the associated gesture. The intransitive gesture
training also consisted of three phases in which the individual was (1) shown two
pictures, one illustrating a context and the other showing related symbolic gesture, and
asked to reproduce the gesture (2) shown the context picture alone, and asked to
reproduce the gesture (3) shown a picture of a different but related contextual situation
and asked to reproduce the gesture. Fifty-minute treatment sessions were administered
three times per week for approximately 10 weeks, with the number of total treatment
sessions ranging from 30-35. A control group was administered aphasia treatment only
for a similar intensity and duration. Results indicated there was a difference between the
two groups post-treatment, with the gesture training method resulting in improved
so-called rehabilitative treatment. It was noted that the treatment was “devised to treat a
wide range of gestures and to reduce several types of praxic errors…” and that it “used
11
different contextual cues in order to teach patients how to produce the same gesture under
different contextual situations” (p. 2052). Thus, although details were not provided, the
treatment appears substantially similar to the one previously reported by this group 69.
Forty-one post-acute left hemisphere stroke patients with limb apraxia (either ideational
or IMA – not defined) were assigned randomly to treatment or no-treatment groups. The
over the course of 10 weeks. Although the groups were equivalent in ADL performance,
apraxia scores, and ADL questionnaire scores prior to treatment, they differed
significantly on these measures after treatment, both immediately and after a 2 week
delay.
training method was developed by Goldenberg & Hagmann 51 for 15 individuals with
IMA (impairment on gesture imitation and gesture to sight of objects) who were on
average 6.1 weeks post onset of a left hemisphere stroke with subsequent aphasia and
severe limb apraxia. The errorless completion method utilized physical manipulation
during ADLs, simultaneous demonstration of ADL by the examiner and imitation by the
patient, and copy by the patient after demonstration during performance of three ADLs.
and critical features of action but did not incorporate direct practice of actions with actual
objects. These two methods were combined and treatment was applied to one ADL at a
time daily for 20-40 minutes for 2-5 weeks. Combined errorless completion/exploration
training resulted in positive effects that were lasting for individuals who remained active
12
Hagmann 37 comparing these two methods in 6 individuals with left hemisphere stroke
and subsequent chronic aphasia and limb apraxia. Each treatment type was applied on a
different pair of ADLs. The exploration training method had no effect. The errorless
completion method yielded a positive and lasting effect. When different objects were
used to test ADL, however, the rate of errors increased, and were comparable to
_________
_________
literature to date. Several trends are worth noting. First, apraxia type is frequently poorly
the integrity of gesture representations (which in turn, may have important implications
for rehabilitation strategies), recognition testing is usually not performed. Second, while
some studies provide data on treatment effects and generalization to untreated items, they
are more sparse with regards to treatment effect upon degree or nature of limb apraxia,
maintenance of treatment effect, and impact of treatment upon ADLs. Third, the duration
and intensity of treatment differs within and across studies making it difficult to
determine the active components of the treatment. Fourth, the length of time between
termination of treatment and follow-up differs across studies, which renders it difficult to
compare the lasting effects of treatment upon limb apraxia or ADLs. Finally, methods
such as the nature of the feedback or correction are commonly underspecified in these
13
Despite these issues, the data consistently suggest that intervention yields a treatment
of treatment effects, and impact upon nature/degree of limb apraxia as well as upon ADL
facility. Thus, it appears that the evidence based on these 10 Phase I studies suggests that
limb apraxia is amenable to treatment. However, according to Robey 71, the purpose of
Phase I research is to develop hypotheses, protocols, and methods, establish safety and
activity, determine the best outcome measures, identify responders vs. nonresponders,
determine optimal intensity and duration, and determine why the treatment is producing
an effect 71. Little of this information is found in these 10 reports and thus, we must
continue to promote systematic inquiry until the objectives of Phase I research are
the ultimate goal of rehabilitation is the use of acquired skill in the individual’s natural
bias); 5) Substrate mediated: development of a critical mass of skill needed to further the
14
therapeutic process; necessary for intrinsic/extrinsic mechanisms to operate; 6)
restoration of social context and change in perception regarding roles to promote activity
in the environment.
treatments that generalized included cross function and extrinsic mechanisms, but some
treatments that did not generalize included these mechanisms as well. Similarly, some
treatments that were mechanistic generalized while others did not. Of the 3 treatments
These equivocal results suggest that while limb apraxia may be amenable to treatment,
literature on motor learning and plasticity, with an eye toward applying this literature to
Some of the actions typically assessed in motor learning studies differ in complexity
and/or meaningfulness from the skilled actions that comprise praxis. A number of motor
learning studies, however, have used complex, learned actions that are arguably akin to
15
what we commonly term “praxis movements”. Other motor learning studies have
examined complex spatiomotor transformations that may have relevance to spatial coding
literature for points of possible convergence with the study of learning in apraxia.
Neuroanatomical Considerations.
have mapped the degree and extent of excitability of individual muscles on the scalp
surface. Body parts that are used more have a larger representation, and this
representation shrinks if the body part is not used (e.g., 74 ). Based on neuroimaging
the task and the stage of motor learning; in nearly all cases, however, there is activation
and declines when the movement is learned. This certainly indicates a role in learning,
and in particular suggests that the cerebellum may critical for developing the movement
representation but not storing it. The frontal and parietal lobes are also clearly involved
in motor learning, but the precise structures involved in early versus later stages of
learning are unclear. For example, a frontal to parietal shift in activation has been
observed as a sequence task is learned 76, a prefrontal to premotor, posterior parietal, and
cerebellar shift in activation has been observed in force adaptation learning 77. On the
other hand, several studies using motor sequence tasks and at least one using a rotational
learning task have demonstrated that parietal activation is associated with early stages of
16
learning, with greater cerebellar and/or premotor involvement in later stages 78-81. At this
juncture, we may conclude that the parietal regions so frequently lesioned in apraxic
There is evidence that perilesional plasticity may play a role in recovery of function
after stroke. It has been shown, for example, that after finger tracking movements,
paretic stroke patients improved in finger pointing accuracy and grasp and release
capabilities 82. These functional gains were accompanied by increased fMRI activations
in sensorimotor areas of the lesioned hemisphere, and diminished activations in the intact
praxis function 84. On the other hand, non-dominant hemisphere plasticity changes have
been demonstrated to be maladaptive in recovery from aphasia 85, and may plausibly be
learning) and that which is cognitively accessible. Ideally, the study of learning in
apraxia could tap into this large body of evidence to support the framing of hypotheses
and predictions. However, one critical concern is that it is not clear whether to align
praxis learning with explicit or implicit knowledge, or both. The types of complex skills
that fall under the rubric of “praxis” are not typically verbalized, yet they can be made
explicit under certain circumstances. It is perhaps most reasonable to begin with the
17
hyptothesis that praxis learning is more similar to implicit procedural learning than to
A typical exploration of skill learning entails the use of serial reaction time tasks
(SSRT). Participants are usually faster to perform sequences of key presses that are
repeated throughout and experiment, even though they are unaware of the repetition. This
is an example of implicit learning. With additional practice, the sequence can frequently
be specified; in this case, the learned information has become declarative as well as
procedural. Performance gets even better at this stage, but the subject's strategy can
implicit and explicit motor sequence learning using a SRTT and Positron Emission
Tomography. During the implicit learning phase, when the subjects were not aware of
the sequence, improvement of the reaction time was associated with increased activity in
correlation with correct recall of the sequence, was associated with increased activity in
the posterior parietal, precuneus and premotor cortices bilaterally, supplementary motor
area predominantly in the left anterior part, left thalamus, and right dorsolateral prefrontal
cortex. In a study by Grafton et al. 87, there was activation of the contralateral primary
motor cortex, supplementary motor area and putamen in an implicit learning task, and
18
In summary of the studies of motor learning in healthy subjects, it appears that
multiple structures in the brain are involved, and that differential involvement arises at
different stages. The primary motor cortex and cerebellum (and sometimes parietal
cortex) are active early and at least the former appears to play a role in implicit learning.
Premotor and parietal cortical areas are active later and appear to play a role in explicit
learning, perhaps in part by storage of the sequence. This concept is supported by the
observation that the premotor and parietal areas increase their activation in proportion to
the length of a sequence performed from memory 88. The relation to regions that when
Several basic principles of motor learning have been explored in other aspects of
motor control rehabilitation, but have received relatively little attention in the study of
IMA.
developing internal models that represent the kinematics (geometry and speed) and
dynamics (forces) of a motor task. Forward models calculate the movements resulting
from a given pattern of force (dynamics) or the limb positions resulting from a given
pattern of joint rotation (kinematics). Inverse models compute the muscle forces or
movements needed to reach a visual goal or goal posture 89. The learning (that is,
with planning, learning, and motor prediction, and another specialized for motor
19
execution and control (see 91). One influential account, for example, distinguishes
semantic representations necessary for motor learning and planning from pragmatic
representations subserving the control and execution of action 92. The planning mode has
environmental conditions.
Recent investigations provide indirect evidence that patients with IMA may be
impaired in learning and/or accessing internal models of movement. Motor imagery has
been proposed by several investigators to serve as a proxy for motor planning in the
absence of execution 93-97. Sirigu et al. 98 and Buxbaum, Johnson-Frey, & Bartlett-
Williams 25 demonstrated that participants with left parietal lesions and IMA were
impaired in motor imagery. In contrast, these patients perform well on tasks more reliant
upon on-line control, such as reaching and grasping with visual feedback 13, 26. The
nature and extent of putative deficiencies in generating and accessing internal models are
being explored in several of the authors’ laboratories using visuomotor and force-field
adaptation paradigms borrowed from the motor control literature. Such studies are an
Practice Schedules. It is clear that practice benefits motor learning, but optimal types
and schedules of training remain unclear, and may vary across tasks. In most motor
tasks, practice that is distributed over (rather than massed in) time appears to result in
optimized learning and retention 99. In learning new sensorimotor transformations, rest
breaks between sessions is of benefit, and may allow for the consolidation of newly
20
acquired internal models 100. It is also frequently beneficial to train a variety of similar
movements to encourage so-called contextual interference. Shea & Kohl 101, for example,
found in a force-learning task that filling the inter-test-trial interval with related motor
tasks significantly improved retention. Ollis et al. 102 demonstrated that learning a variety
of knot-tying movements enhances learning, even for novices practicing complex knots.
There is some suggestion, however, that the benefit of contextual interference may be
Parkinson’s disease did not enhance learning, suggesting that successful learning
strategies in healthy controls may not generalize to brain-damaged patients 104. It is also
of interest to note that training of items that share many features with other items is
disruptive and not beneficial in the lexical-semantic domain 105, 106. As object-related
praxis movements are complex skills with close ties to semantic knowledge 22, it remains
The Role of Feedback and Error Correction. Feedback and knowledge of results
frequently facilitate motor skill acquisition. Recent investigations have probed the types
of feedback that may be most optimal, and here, as in other areas of motor learning, the
answer is unclear. For example, varying the movement component about which feedback
is provided may benefit simple skill learning, but disrupt more complex motor skill
learning 107.
21
rewarding successful performance. In contrast, in the domain of simple movements, such
to correct error (e.g., 108). The role of error in these different types of learning remains
poorly understood; moreover, it is not clear whether and where praxis movements may
Hemiparetic stroke patients without IMA are able to adapt to forces applied
perpendicular to the moving hemiparetic arm 109 as well as to spring-like forces that act
against movement 110 when they receive feedback about error. This suggests that
hemiparetic patients can use error to adjust internal models of movement to achieve an
intended goal 109, 111. It has also been suggested that perception of gross errors may
anosognosia, or unawareness of deficit. They may recognize that they are unable to
move correctly, but fail to recognize the extent of deficit, or may attribute it to
augmented feedback about error. Fortunately, a number of virtual reality paradigms under
Paradigms using robot-assisted devices (e.g., 115, 116) can launch correct actions
based on electromyographic activity that is associated with the intention to act. Thus,
preparatory activity is linked to a correct response, and errors are prevented. This would
seem to be an extremely useful feature. However, given that IMA patients may fail at the
level of planning and intention, it is not obvious that robot-assisted therapies will be
22
helpful in the rehabilitation of IMA, unless the correct performance of an act can feed
There are several different subtypes of apraxia, resulting in some cases from
date reveals that the field is in the early stages of efforts to develop effective treatments,
and that most studies have relied upon individual case experimental designs. Additional
aphasia, variable criteria for diagnosing apraxia, vague description of treatments applied,
unequal application of treatment even within a given study, and absence of information
about treatment generalization. Most central to the aims of this review, principles from
the existing motor learning literature have not yet informed the development of treatment
studies.
The motor learning literature identifies several principles that may benefit the
the target task appears to improve learning and retention. Creating contextual
interference by interleaving the target task with other similar tasks may aid 117or disrupt
systematically vary one treatment feature at a time (e.g., massed versus distributed
23
practice schedule, similarity or distinctiveness of items, presence or absence of feedback,
while systematically holding the others constant. This is clearly preferable from the
perspective of clarifying the features of the training that are critical. On the other hand,
there is unfortunately very little to suggest how these motor learning principles are best
the treatment on all of the motor learning features that may plausibly be beneficial, and if
an effect is obtained, follow up with studies designed to disentangle the critical versus
which features were applied incorrectly, and this in turn would necessitate a return to the
“loading” the treatment with principles derived from the motor learning literature. One
might predict, for example, that deficits in naturalistic action may be most successfully
targeted naturalistic tasks, interleaved with other similar tasks. Principles of shaping
might be predicted to be beneficial, such that easy tasks are used early in training and
harder tasks later in training such that performance is successful. On the other hand,
The apraxia literature also provides some hints about other factors that may
impact rehabilitation. A recent learning study from the lab of one of the authors 118
assessed the role of the “affordances” of unfamiliar objects – in this case, the degree to
24
which the unfamiliar objects signal the actions associated with them by virtue of their
shape – in learning new object-related gestures. Patients with IMA, but not age- and
at learning new gestures when the gestures were highly afforded by their associated
objects. This “affordance benefit” could clearly be exploited in the design of future
number of other critical ways. Clearly, these early tasks should have few steps. Arrays
should be simple, with few visual elements, and no distractor (task-irrelevant) objects.
Spatial consistency of object placement from trial to trial is also critical 119. These task
and object features may all be titrated gradually such that tasks higher up in the shaping
Treatments must be applied identically across all treated subjects. Treated and
untreated patients must either be matched across a large number of putatively important
variables, including lesion size, severity of cognitive and language deficits, apraxia type
(and subtype) and severity, and motor impairment, or sample sizes must be large and
patients randomly assigned to treated and untreated groups. Efficacy of treatment should
performance of ADLs, and/or functional independence that are different from the trained
tasks.
25
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
and the University of Florida Dept. of Occupational Therapy, Gainesville, Florida. Work
on the manuscript was supported in part by NIH R01-NS036387 to the first author.
26
REFERENCES
1900;8:102-132,182-197.
6. Ochipa C, Rothi LJG, Heilman KM: Ideational apraxia: A deficit in tool selection
Brain 1992;115:1061-1072.
9. Heilman KM, Gonzalez Rothi LJ: Apraxia, In: Heilman KM, valenstein E, (eds).
1993:141-150.
11. Heilman KM, Rothi LJ, Valenstein E: Two forms of ideomotor apraxia.
Neurology 1982;32:342-346.
27
12. Goldenberg G: Imitating getures and manipulating a mannikin-the representation
13. Haaland KY, Harrington DL, Knight RT: Spatial deficits in ideomotor limb
apraxia in Alzheimer's disease and left hemisphere stroke: Limb transitive and
Neurology 1999;12(161-166).
18. Weiss PH, Dohle C, Binkofski F, Schnitzler A, Freund HJ, Hefter H: Motor
28
20. DeRenzi E, Faglioni P, Lodesani M, Vecchi A: Performance of left brain-
21. Harrington DL, Haaland KY: Motor sequencing with left hemisphere damage:
22. Buxbaum LJ, Saffran EM: Knowledge of object manipulation and object
24. Clark M, Merians AS, Kothari A, et al.: Spatial planning deficits in limb apraxia.
Brain 1994;117:1093-1106.
2005;43(6):917-929.
26. Jax SA, Buxbaum LJ, Moll AD: Deficits in movement planning and intrinsic
2076.
27. Gonzalez Rothi LJ, Heilman KM: Ideomotor apraxia: Gestural learning and
memory, In: Roy EA, (ed). Neuropsychological studies in apraxia and related
29
28. Faglioni P, Basso A, Botti C, Aglioti S, Saetti C: Gestural learning in apraxia, In:
Erlbaum; 1990:837-856.
30. Buxbaum LJ, Giovannetti T, Libon D: The role of the dynamic body schema in
2000;44:166-191.
31. Buxbaum LJ, Kyle KM, Menon R: On beyond mirror neurons: internal
32. Buxbaum LJ, Kyle K, Grossman M, Coslett HB: Left inferior parietal
294, 585-644.
34. Kimura D: Translations from Liepmann's Essays on Apraxia. 506 ed. London,
1986;24(1):129-134.
36. Foundas AL, Macauley BL, Raymer AM, Maher LM, Heilman KM, Roth LJG:
30
37. Goldenberg G, Daumuller M, Hagmann S: Assessment and therapy of complex
2001;11(2):147-169.
38. Sunderland A, Bowers MP, Sluman SM, Wilcock DJ, Ardron ME: Impaired
dexterity of the ipsilateral hand after stroke and the relationship to cognitive
39. Sunderland A, Sluman SM: Ideomotor apraxia, visuomotor control and the
40. van Heugten CM, Dekker J, Deelman BG, Stehmann-Saris JC, Kinebanian A:
Rehabilitation of stroke patients with apraxia: the role of additional cognitive and
41. Wetter S, Poole JL, Haaland KY: Functional implications of ipsilesional motor
deficits after unilateral stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil Apr 2005;86(4):776-781.
42. Haaland KY, Harrington DL, Knight RT: Neural representations of skilled
Feb 11 2003;60(3):487-490.
1988;10(4):363-379.
31
46. Goldenberg G, Hagmann S: Tool Use and Mechanical problem solving in apraxia.
Neuropsychologia 1998;36(7):581-589.
Neuropsychologia 2005;43:682-692.
48. van Heugten CM, Dekker J, Deelman BG, van Dijk AJ, Stehmann-Saris FC,
2000;10(4):401-414.
49. Walker CM, Sunderland A, Sharma J, Walker MF: The impact of cognitive
2000;31(2):430-433.
52. Walker MF, Lincoln NB: Factors influencing dressing performance after stroke. J
53. Buxbaum LJ, Schwartz MF, Montgomery M: Ideational apraxia and naturalistic
54. Schwartz MF, Montgomery, M.W., Buxbaum, L.J., Lee, S.S., Carew, T.G.,
Coslett, H.B., Ferraro, M., Fitzpatrick-DeSalme, E., Hart, T., & Mayer, N.:
32
Naturalistic action impairment in closed head injury. Neuropsychology 1998;in
press.
57. Mayer NH, Reed ES, Schwartz MF, Montgomery MW, Palmer C: Buttering a hot
cup of hot coffee: An approach to the study of errors of action after brain
1990:259-283.
press.
59. Buxbaum LJ, Veramonti T, Schwartz MF: Function and manipulation tool
knowledge in apraxia: Knowing 'What for' but not 'how'. Neurocase 2000;6:83-
97.
61. Maher LM, Ochipa C: Management and treatment of limb apraxia, In: Gonzalez-
Rothi LJ, Heilman KM, (eds). Apraxia: The Neuropsychology of Action. United
33
62. Ochipa C, Maher L, Rothi L: Treatment of ideomotor apraxia. Journal of the
63. Code C, Gaunt C: Treating severe speech and limb apraxia in a case of aphasia.
64. Deal JL, Florance CL: Modification of the eight-step continuum for treatment of
66. van Heugten CM, Dekker J, Deelman BG, van Dijk AJ, Stehmann-Saris JC,
68. Geusgens C, van Heugten C, Donkervoort M, van den Ende E, Jolles J, van den
2000;81:379-388.
34
70. Smania N, Aglioti SM, Girardi F, et al.: Rehabilitation of limb apraxia improves
2052.
71. Robey RR, Schultz MC: A model for conducting clinical-outcome research: An
1998;12:787-810.
72. Nadeau SE, Rothi LJG, Rosenbek JC: Language rehabilitation from a neural
73. Hallett M: Motor Learning, In: Freund HJ, Jeannerod M, Hallett M, Leiguarda R,
1045.
75. Karni A, Meyer G, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, Ungerleider LG: Functional
MRI evidence for adult motor cortex plasticity during motor skill learning. Nature
Sep 14 1995;377(6545):155-158.
Science 1997;277:821-825.
35
78. Bapi RS, Miyapuram KP, Graydon FX, Doya K: fMRI investigation of cortical
79. Graydon FX, Friston KJ, Thomas CG, Brooks VB, Menon RS: Learning-related
81. Toni I, Krams M, Turner R, Passingham RE: The time course of changes during
1998;8(1):50-61.
82. Carey DP: Multisensory integration: attending to seen and felt hands. Curr Biol
Nov 30 2000;10(23):R863-865.
83. Fridman EA, Hanakawa T, Chung M, Hummel F, Leiguarda RC, Cohen LG:
Reorganization of the human ipsilesional premotor cortex after stroke. Brain Apr
2004;127(Pt 4):747-758.
84. Rapcsak SZ, Ochipa C, Beeson PM, Rubens AB: Praxis and the right hemisphere.
36
86. Honda M, Deiber MP, Ibanez V, Pascual-Leone A, Zhuang P, Hallett M:
88. Catalan MJ, Honda M, Weeks RA, Cohen LG, Hallett M: The functional
89. Wise SP, Shadmehr R: Motor control, In: Ramachandran VS, (ed). Encyclopedia
90. Krakauer JW, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C: Independent learning of internal models for
1031.
91. Keele SW: Motor control., In: Brooks WB, (ed). Handbook in Physiology,
Section 1: The Nervous System, Vol Vol.2. Baltimore, Williams & Williams;
1981:1391-1414.
93. Blakemore SJ, Sirigu A: Action prediction in the cerebellum and in the parietal
94. Glover S: Separate visual representations in the planning and control of action.
37
96. Johnson SHR, M., Grafton, S.T., Hinrichs, H., Gazzaniga, M.S. & Heinze, H-J.:
97. Prinz W: Motor images are action plans (commentary). Behavioral and Brain
Sciences 1994;17(2):218.
98. Sirigu A, Duhamel J-R, Cohen L, Pillon B, Dubois B, Agid Y: The mental
1996;273(1564-1568).
101. Shea CH, Kohl RM: Composition of practice: influence on the retention of motor
102. Ollis S, Button C, Fairweather M: The influence of professional expertise and task
complexity upon the potency of the contextual interference effect. Acta Psychol
38
106. Plaut DC, McClelland JL, Seidenberg MS, Patterson K: Understanding normal
107. Wulf G, Horger M, Shea CH: Benefits of Blocked Over Serial Feedback on
109. Patton JL, Stoykov ME, Kovic M, Mussa-Ivaldi FA: Evaluation of robotic
training forces that either enhance or reduce error in chronic hemiparetic stroke
110. Dancause N, Ptito A, Levin MF: Error correction strategies for motor behavior
Neuropsychologia 2002;40(8):1313-1323.
113. Heilman KM, Gonzalez-Rothi LJ: Apraxia, In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, (eds).
114. Holden MK: Virtual environments for motor rehabilitation: review. Cyberpsychol
training in chronic stroke. Conf Proc IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc 2004;4:2722-2725.
39
116. Masiero S, Celia A, Rosati G, Armani M: Robotic-assisted rehabilitation of the
upper limb after acute stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil Feb 2007;88(2):142-149.
117. Hanlon RE: Motor learning following unilateral stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil
1996;77:811-815.
In Press.
119. Giovannetti T, Schwartz MF, Buxbaum LJ: The effect of divided attention on
40
Table 1: Summary of Apraxia Treatment Studies
Apraxia Trained Duration Intensity Treatment Generalization Maintenance Apraxia ADL
Type(s) Items Effect Impact Impact
Multiple Cues (n=1) IMA gestures 2 weeks One hour daily Y Y Y-treated items NA NA
only (2 weeks)
Error Type Reduction (n=2) IMA gestures Varied; 6- Varied; once daily 4 Y N Y- treated error N NA
11 weeks days/week- twice daily 2 types only (2
days/week weeks)
Six Stage Task Hierarchy (n=1) IMA + IA gestures 8 months 45 minutes; once weekly Y N NA N NA
Exploration Training (n=6) IMA ADL 2 weeks 6 sessions, one hour each N N N (3 months) NA NA
Legend: IMA = ideomotor apraxia, IA = ideational apraxia, Y = yes, N = no, NA = not assessed/no information provided
* “Inability to carry out purposeful activities”
41