Progress in Motor Control - Theories and Translations
Progress in Motor Control - Theories and Translations
Progress in Motor Control - Theories and Translations
Jozsef Laczko
Mark L. Latash Editors
Progress
in Motor
Control
Theories and Translations
Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology
Volume 957
More information about this series at http://www.springer.com/series/5584
Jozsef Laczko Mark L. Latash
•
Editors
123
Editors
Jozsef Laczko Mark L. Latash
Department of Information Technology Department of Kinesiology
and Biorobotics, Faculty of Science Pennsylvania State University
University of Pécs University Park, PA
Pecs USA
Hungary
This volume continues the tradition of the series Progress in Motor Control started
in the previous millennium with the publication of the first volume subtitled
“Bernstein’s Traditions in Movement Studies” (Latash 1998). That volume con-
tained chapters written by speakers at the first conference with the same name held
in 1996 in State College, Pennsylvania. Over the past 20 years, the field of motor
control has grown substantially, which is reflected in the creation of the journal
“Motor Control” and the International Society for Motor Control (ISMC). The
conferences Progress in Motor Control have become biennial meetings of ISMC,
and so far seven volumes have been published forming a kind of encyclopedic
compendium of motor control that is updated every other year.
Most of the chapters in this volume were written by speakers who attended the
conference Progress in Motor Control X in Budapest, held during July 22–25,
2015. In addition, we invited several additional groups of authors who have per-
formed new, exciting studies over the past two years. The subtitle of this volume,
Theories and Translations, reflects the two actively developed directions of
research. One of them tries to develop theoretical approaches to biological move-
ment that would make motor control a subfield of natural science, physics of living
systems. The other applies recent advances in motor control to areas such as
robotics, movement disorders, brain–computer interface, and rehabilitation.
The volume consists of six parts that are focused on specific aspects of motor
control. Part I, “Theoretical Motor Control”, opens with a chapter by Andrea
d’Avella entitled “Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning”. The author
addresses a central issue of how the central nervous system (CNS) overcomes the
complexity of multi-joint and multi-muscle control and suggests that modular
architecture may simplify control by embedding features of both the dynamic
behavior of the musculoskeletal system and of the task into a small number of
modules. Recent studies, in which human subjects used myoelectric control to
move a mass in a virtual environment, have suggested that recombination of
modules may be more efficient than learning or adapting a skill by acquiring new
modules. According to the view of Jeroen Smeets and Eli Brenner in their chapter
“Synergies in Grasping”, both transport of the hand and formation of the grip
v
vi Preface
reflection of disordered control of the threshold of the tonic stretch reflex and offers
practical lessons for motor rehabilitation.
Part III of the book addresses neurophysiological mechanisms of motor control.
The chapter by Richard Carson and colleagues, “What Do TMS-Evoked Motor
Potentials Tell Us About Motor Learning?” reviews the role of transcranial mag-
netic stimulation (TMS) in studies regarding the effects of motor learning. The
authors emphasize the restricted explanatory scope of the TMS technique and
consider a specific example of cross education: the interlimb transfer of functional
capacity. Winfried Mayr and colleagues cover in their chapter, “Motor Control of
Human Spinal Cord Disconnected from the Brain and Under External Movement”,
the role of the spinal cord in motor control and coordination. They describe a model
of the human spinal cord with reduced and altered motor control and discuss how
knowledge about human motor control as well as neurophysiology teach us to
perform external modification of upper motor neurons by electrical stimulation and
external control of afferents to spinal cord. Anticipatory adjustments during object
manipulation are described in the chapter by Thomas Schneider and Joachim
Hermsdörfer entitled “Anticipation in Object Manipulation: Behavioral and Neural
Correlates”. The authors review studies using brain functional imaging and
examining the deficits of patients with localized brain damage to provide an insight
into the basic principles of anticipatory motor control and their underlying neural
substrates.
Part IV dedicated to problems of learning skilled behaviors opens with a chapter
“Brain Plasticity and the Concept of Metaplasticity in Skilled Musicians” by Eckart
Altenmüller and Shinichi Furuya, which explores the importance of brain plastic
adaptations for enhanced sensory, motor, and cognitive functions. In particular, the
authors focus on plastic changes in neuroplastic functions, so called metaplasticity,
in musicians. The potential role of this mechanism for prevention of developing
maladaptive changes in the nervous system, possibly leading to focal dystonia in
musicians, is discussed. The next chapter, “The Coordination Dynamics of
Observational Learning: Relative Motion Direction and Relative Phase as
Informational Content Linking Action-Perception to Action-Production” by John
Buchanan, emphasizes identifiable movement features that constrain and inform
action-perception and action-production processes. The author puts forth relative
phase as an informational variable that links perception to action. Across a series of
tasks, it is shown that the relative motion and relative phase between limbs and
joints are picked up through visual processes and support the observational learning
of motor skills. Elizabeth Torres reviews new technological advances and new
analytical methods in the study of movements and their changes in the clinical
setting in a chapter titled “Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use”. She
emphasizes the importance of variability in the emergence of movement patterns
and presents examples of solutions amenable to the habilitation and rehabilitation of
volition in patient populations.
Part V of the book covers the field of impaired motor control and rehabilitation.
Sainburg and colleagues in their chapter titled “Motor Lateralization Provides a
Foundation for Predicting and Treating Non-paretic Arm Motor Deficits in Stroke”
viii Preface
xi
xii Contents
xiii
xiv Editors and Contributors
Contributors
Andrea d’Avella
Abstract How the central nervous system (CNS) overcomes the complexity of
multi-joint and multi-muscle control and how it acquires or adapts motor skills are
fundamental and open questions in neuroscience. A modular architecture may
simplify control by embedding features of both the dynamic behavior of the
musculoskeletal system and of the task into a small number of modules and by
directly mapping task goals into module combination parameters. Several studies of
the electromyographic (EMG) activity recorded from many muscles during the
performance of different tasks have shown that motor commands are generated by
the combination of a small number of muscle synergies, coordinated recruitment of
groups of muscles with specific amplitude balances or activation waveforms, thus
supporting a modular organization of motor control. Modularity may also help
understanding motor learning. In a modular architecture, acquisition of a new motor
skill or adaptation of an existing skill after a perturbation may occur at the level of
modules or at the level of module combinations. As learning or adapting an existing
skill through recombination of modules is likely faster than learning or adapting a
skill by acquiring new modules, compatibility with the modules predicts learning
difficulty. A recent study in which human subjects used myoelectric control to
move a mass in a virtual environment has tested this prediction. By altering the
mapping between recorded muscle activity and simulated force applied on the mass,
as in a complex surgical rearrangement of the tendons, it has been possible to show
that it is easier to adapt to a perturbation that is compatible with the muscle syn-
ergies used to generate hand force than to a similar but incompatible perturbation.
This result provides direct support for a modular organization of motor control and
motor learning.
A. d’Avella (&)
Department of Biomedical and Dental Sciences and Morphofunctional Imaging,
University of Messina, Messina, Italy
e-mail: [email protected]
A. d’Avella
Laboratory of Neuromotor Physiology, Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy
Introduction
Any goal-directed limb movement, even one that appears simple and effortless such
as reaching for a cup on a table, requires complex control processes. Mechanically,
limbs are open kinematic chains with multiple segments and their dynamic behavior
is complex due to the interactions between the different segments (Hollerbach and
Flash 1982). A single segment rotates around a joint with an angular acceleration
proportional to the applied torque. However, the angular accelerations of two or
more segments in a chain depend on torques, angular velocities, and angular dis-
placements at all joints. Moreover, muscles are redundant, i.e., there are more
muscles in a limb than joints, and they generate force with complex dependences on
neural activation, muscle length, and shortening velocity. How the CNS transforms
sensory information about a goal into coordinated activations of many muscles
necessary to achieve the goal is a fundamental and open question in neuroscience.
In robotics, the torques necessary to generate a desired limb movement can be
computed from the equations of motion for the limb. However, the complexity of
the equations of motion increases dramatically with the number of joints. As it is
unlikely that the CNS explicitly represents the equation of motion and performs
analytical computations, the CNS may instead rely on an implicit and possibly
approximate knowledge of the dynamic behavior of the limb in response to muscle
activation. A long-standing hypothesis in motor control is that motor commands are
generated by a modular control architecture (Sherrington 1948; Bernstein 1967).
Thus, motor modules may store approximate yet sufficient knowledge to construct
adequate motor commands.
In the following sections, I will first address why and how modularity may
simplify motor control. I will then review experiments showing that, in many tasks
and conditions, motor commands are generated by the combination of a small
number of muscle synergies, coordinated recruitment of group of muscles with
specific amplitude balances or activation waveforms, suggesting that the CNS
organizes muscle synergies as motor control modules. Finally, I will discuss how
modularity may also help understanding motor learning. In a modular control
architecture, new motor skills may be acquired by learning new combinations of
existing modules or by learning new modules. As learning new modules is likely a
slower adaptive process than learning new combinations of existing modules,
modularity predicts learning difficulty. A recent study in which human subjects
used myoelectric control to move a mass in a virtual environment has shown that
the difficulty in adapting to a perturbation depends on the compatibility of the
perturbation with the muscle synergies used to generate hand force. This study
provides direct evidence for muscle synergies as motor control modules and
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 5
suggests that motor learning relies on two adaptive processes: a fast learning of
muscle synergy combinations and a slow learning of muscle synergy structure.
most general knowledge of the dynamic behavior of the limb. However, such high
degree of generality has a high cost in terms of acquisition. How would the CNS
acquire and represent complex functions of many variables? As a large recurrent
neural network may represent complex functions, the CNS might be able to learn
the most general sensorimotor transformations through practice. However, given
the large number of variables and the functional complexity involved, learning
sensorimotor transformations by trial-and-error might require an enormous number
of trials, which seems at odds with the existence of both sophisticated innate motor
behaviors and fast motor skill learning. At the opposite extreme in terms of gen-
eralization capabilities, a one-to-one mapping of a goal and an initial limb state into
a specific motor command, i.e., a specific set of muscle activation profiles, would be
easy to acquire and to represent. A specific motor program can be learned and used
to reach a target at a given position from a given initial arm configuration, However,
a different motor program would have to be stored and retrieved for each goal. As
goals in most tasks vary continuously, e.g., the position in space of the target to
reach, a mapping for each one of possibly infinity many goals would potentially
require infinite storage. The solution for implementing adequate sensorimotor
transformations that evolution may have endowed the CNS with or that the CNS
may discover through practice and learning (Giszter et al. 2010) is likely a com-
promise between a hard to learn but very general mapping and a very extensive set
of easy to learn one-to-one mappings. Motor modules may represent such a com-
promise. A long-standing hypothesis in motor control is that motor commands are
generated by a modular control architecture. While different types of modules have
been proposed, ranging from spinal reflexes (Sherrington 1948) to unit burst gen-
erators (Grillner 1981), from spinal force fields (Bizzi et al. 1991) to muscle syn-
ergies (Tresch et al. 1999; d’Avella et al. 2003; Ivanenko et al. 2004) and kinematic
synergies (Santello et al. 1998), motor modules may be characterized by their
ability to implement sensorimotor transformations with a small number of param-
eters. They may allow storing limited yet sufficient knowledge of the dynamic
behavior of the limb to construct adequate motor commands by mapping goals into
a small number of module combination parameters, thus simplifying control.
Considering again a reaching task, as the target position changes the muscle
patterns required to reach the target will also change. However, the muscle patterns
for reaching two targets close to each other will only have small differences and
most of their structure will be shared. Differences will increase with distance but
there will still be structure shared among muscle patterns for all targets. Thus, rather
than computing each muscle pattern on the fly or storing it as a separate mapping,
shared structure may be exploited to generate the muscle patterns for each possible
goal of a task as the combination of a small number of task-specific motor modules.
Once an appropriate set of motor modules are stored, the sensorimotor transfor-
mations will simply map goals into a small number of parameters that determine the
contribution of each module for the generation of the appropriate muscle pattern.
Thus, motor modules reduce the dimensionality of the output of the sensorimotor
transformations.
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 7
How a modular control architecture may reduce the dimensionality of the control
problem has been investigated with a simulation in which a planar kinematic chain
actuated by torque profiles was used as a model of an arm performing a reaching
task (Alessandro et al. 2013). Despite the nonlinearity of the equations of motion,
the torque profiles bringing the arm endpoint from a given starting position to any
target can be generated by a linear combination of a small set of basic actuation
profiles, i.e., motor modules. Instead of planning a desired trajectory and using
inverse dynamics to compute torque profiles, motor modules allow mapping
directly a target into motor commands by selecting a few combination coefficients.
In the simulation, interpolation of the kinematic trajectories generated by the
dynamic responses to each basic activation profile was used to determine the
combination coefficients but a neural network can easily learn a low-dimensional
mapping between target positions and combination coefficients by trial-and-error.
Importantly, the number and choice of motor modules affects performance. Good
task performance can be achieved with a small number of modules if the modules
are selected as torque profiles that achieve the task in a rich enough set of proto-
typical conditions, i.e., if they embed features of the system dynamics and of the
task.
What is the evidence that the CNS actually employs motor modules to simplify
control? In the last two decades, an increasing number of studies have investigated
whether muscle synergies, coordinated recruitment of groups of muscles with
specific amplitude balances or activation waveforms, are motor modules organized
by the CNS. Muscle synergies may store structure in the muscle activity patterns
shared across tasks and task conditions. Thus, if muscle synergies are motor
modules, one expects a small number of synergies to be able to reconstruct the
muscle patterns observed across a variety of conditions. Indeed, studies based on
the decomposition of the EMG activity recorded from multiple muscles in different
species during the performance of a variety of motor tasks have shown that muscle
patterns can be reconstructed by the combination of a small number of muscle
synergies, thus supporting a modular organization of motor control. In this section,
I will first present the different quantitative definition of muscle synergies that have
been introduced in the last two decades and I will then briefly review some of the
studies that have used EMG decomposition approaches to identify muscle syn-
ergies, focusing in particular on reaching movements and locomotion. A number of
review articles can be consulted for more details on recent studies on muscle
synergies (Bizzi et al. 2008; Tresch and Jarc 2009; Lacquaniti et al. 2012; d’Avella
and Lacquaniti 2013; Giszter 2015; Ting et al. 2015).
8 A. d’Avella
The idea that muscles are activated together, either as a desirable strategy to
simplify control or because of pathological loss of control due to neural damage,
has a long history and the term “muscle synergy” has been used in different con-
texts with different meanings. In the context of motor control, muscle synergies
have been defined as coordinated activations of groups of muscles allowing the
CNS to control movements through a smaller number of variables than those
required to control individual muscles (Bernstein 1967) or as a neural organization
that provides stability of an important performance variable by covaried adjust-
ments of elemental variables such as coordinated muscle activations (Latash 2012).
The existence of a muscle synergy as the activation of a group of muscles with a
fixed activation balance was first investigated by analyzing the activations of pairs
of muscles (Lee 1984; Macpherson 1991). Muscles recruited synergistically were
expected to have correlated activations. These investigations failed to fully support
the existence of muscle synergies, as pairwise correlations were often observed but
not as frequently as expected (Buchanan et al. 1986; Soechting and Lacquaniti
1989; Maier and Hepp-Reymond 1995). However, strong pairwise correlations
provide a clear signature of a muscle synergy defined as a fixed balance of muscle
activation, i.e., capturing spatial (across muscles) structure, only when a synergy is
recruited by itself. If the same muscles belong to more than one synergy, each with
a different fixed activation balance, and different synergies are combined flexibly
across task conditions, pairwise correlations may be weak (Bizzi et al. 2002). Thus,
the prediction of a model that allows for flexible combinations of fixed spatial
muscle synergies is the existence of a low-dimensional covariance structure among
all muscles rather than high pairwise correlations. To test quantitatively such pre-
diction, a model based on the linear combination of spatial muscle synergies was
introduced and a method to identify synergies from the EMG signals recorded from
many muscles was developed (Tresch et al. 1999). Thus, spatial muscle synergies
(also known as time-invariant or synchronous synergies) have been defined as basis
vectors in muscle activation space:
X
N
mðtÞ ¼ ci ðtÞ wi ; ð1Þ
i¼1
diagonalization of the data covariance matrix, but the basis vectors extracted with
this procedure do not satisfy the nonnegativity constraint and they are instead
constrained to be mutually orthogonal. An iterative optimization algorithm based on
gradient descent of the reconstruction error was then developed for muscle synergy
identification (Tresch et al. 1999). At the same time, a more efficient iterative
algorithm for nonnegative matrix factorization (NMF) was developed for image and
language decomposition (Lee and Seung 1999) and later became the standard
approach for EMG decomposition.
In addition to being defined in the spatial domain, i.e., as groups of muscles with
specific activation balances, muscle synergies have also been defined in the tem-
poral domain, as specific activation waveforms shared across groups of muscles.
Temporal muscle synergies (also known as temporal components or basic activa-
tion patterns) are defined by the same equation (Eq. 1) used to define spatial muscle
synergies but the time-varying coefficients [ci(t)] rather than by the muscle synergy
vectors or weights (wi) are taken as the modular elements. Thus, the two models can
only be distinguished when considering muscle patterns for multiple task condi-
tions. In the case of spatial synergies, one set of synergy vectors is combined by
different time-varying coefficients in different conditions while for temporal syn-
ergies one set of temporal components is multiplied by condition-dependent
weights (Russo et al. 2014). The same matrix factorization algorithms, such as
NMF, can be used to identify both types of modules, once the data matrix is
constructed by stacking data for individual conditions either along the spatial or
along the temporal dimension [see Fig. 2 in Russo et al. (2014)].
Muscle synergies may also capture more complex coordination patterns, such as
the recruitment of different muscles in a sequence, which may be described by
specific collections of muscle activation waveforms. Such spatiotemporal muscle
synergies (also known as time-varying muscle synergies) may be defined as
sequences of vectors in muscle activation space (d’Avella and Tresch 2002;
d’Avella et al. 2003):
X
N
mðtÞ ¼ ci wi ðt ti Þ; ð2Þ
i¼1
2007; Russo et al. 2014). If time-shifts are included, the decomposition of the data
into spatiotemporal synergies requires identifying onset parameters in addition to
scaling coefficients and synergies. An iterative decomposition algorithm was
developed specifically for this purpose (d’Avella and Tresch 2002; d’Avella et al.
2003). Finally, in a spatiotemporal synergy the activation waveform is the same for
all muscles it can be expressed as the product of a spatial times a temporal synergy,
i.e., as a space-by-time synergy (Delis et al. 2014).
Spatial, temporal, and spatiotemporal synergy models have been used to
decompose the EMG activity recorded from multiple muscles in different species
during the performance of a variety of motor tasks. The initial studies were per-
formed in frogs (Tresch et al. 1999; Saltiel et al. 2001; d’Avella et al. 2003; Hart
and Giszter 2004), cats (Ting and Macpherson 2005), and humans (Ivanenko et al.
2003). Those and a growing number of additional studies investigating spinal
reflexes, postural control, walking, running, pedaling, reaching, hand force gener-
ation, grasping, finger movements, and many other tasks have provided support to
the modular hypothesis showing that in most conditions the muscle patterns can be
reconstructed by the combination of a small number of muscle synergies. I will now
briefly present the results of two of these studies: one investigating reaching using
spatiotemporal synergies (d’Avella et al. 2006) and a second investigating the
development of locomotion using temporal synergies (Dominici et al. 2011).
A number of kinematic and kinetic features are preserved across reaching
movement conditions, such as the straightness of the hand path and the shape of the
tangential velocity profiles for different planar movements (Morasso 1981), the
specific hand path for movements at different speeds or with different loads
(Soechting and Lacquaniti 1981; Lacquaniti et al. 1982; Atkeson and Hollerbach
1985), the linear relation between torques at different joints (Soechting and
Lacquaniti 1981; Lacquaniti et al. 1986; Gottlieb et al. 1997). Such invariances
suggest that the CNS uses simple rules for controlling reaching. In contrast, the
EMG patterns for reaching show complex changes in the shape and timing of the
muscle activation waveforms as a function of movement direction and speed
(Flanders et al. 1994, 1996). The apparent discrepancy between regularities in the
kinematic and kinetic features and variability of the muscle patterns for reaching
may be resolved at the level of muscle synergy organization. The decomposition of
the EMG patterns for reaching in different directions and at different speeds shows
that the complex dependences of the muscle activation waveforms on movement
parameters result from the combination of a small number of spatiotemporal muscle
synergies according to simple rules (d’Avella et al. 2006, 2008). Combinations of
four or five spatiotemporal synergies could accurately reconstruct the phasic EMG
waveforms recorded from up to 19 shoulder and arm muscles during fast
point-to-point movements between a central location and eight peripheral targets in
both a frontal and a sagittal plane (d’Avella et al. 2006). Phasic waveforms,
responsible for accelerating and decelerating the arm, were computed by subtracting
the tonic components used for balancing gravitational forces and maintaining
postural stability (Flanders 1991). The extracted synergies involved specific subsets
of muscles, acting at multiple joints and with synchronous and asynchronous
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 11
waveforms. In many cases, individual muscles were recruited by more than one
synergy. The good reconstruction of the muscle patterns for movements in different
directions with five synergies [see Fig. 8 in d’Avella et al. (2006)] demonstrates the
accuracy of the spatiotemporal synergy model and the parsimony of the synergy
representation of the muscle patterns. The entire set of waveforms constituting a
muscle pattern for a specific movement could be generated by selecting five
amplitude coefficients and five onset times. Moreover, movement direction and
speed modulated the amplitude coefficients according to simple rules. The synergy
amplitude coefficients depended on the movement direction in most cases according
to a cosine function. When movement speed varied in addition to movement
direction, phasic spatiotemporal synergies maintained the same directional tuning
and were modulated in amplitude by speed (d’Avella et al. 2008). These simple
modulation rules support the notion of muscle synergies as a mechanism for
implementing a direct mapping of movement goals into motor commands.
Modular decomposition of EMG patterns may also provide insights on motor
development. Whether the muscle activation patterns for locomotion are innate, or
whether they are acquired during development is an important question. A study of
temporal synergies underlying locomotion (locomotor primitives) at different
developmental stages demonstrated that there are both innate and learned modules
(Dominici et al. 2011). Human newborns, supported and with their feet in contact
with a surface, generate stepping-like movements that generally disappears
at *2 months after birth. Decomposition of EMG activity during newborn step-
ping by NMF revealed two primitives that were roughly similar to two of the four
primitives observed in the adult, but more prolonged in duration. As in adults, one
primitive was related to body support during stance, while the other to limb flexion
during swing. In newborns, however, there were no specific primitive related to
either touch-down or lift-off. In toddlers (*1-year-old) at their first unsupported
steps, in addition to the same two primitives seen in the newborn, two new prim-
itives timed at touch-down and lift-off appeared and they were similar to the two
other primitives seen in the adult. In preschoolers (2–4 years), all four primitives
changed shape with increasing age, becoming narrower and closer to the adult
waveforms. Moreover, comparing the development of locomotor patterns in
humans with that in other vertebrates it appears that locomotion of several animal
species is built starting from common modular elements, perhaps related to
ancestral neural networks. However, with development, the motor patterns may be
adjusted to satisfy the specific biomechanical requirements of a given animal
species, such as those for erect bipedal locomotion in humans.
The studies briefly reviewed above and many other studies based on the
decomposition of multi-muscle EMG recordings indicate that muscle synergies
capture regularities in the spatial, temporal, and spatiotemporal organization of the
muscle patterns, supporting the notion that the CNS organizes muscle synergies to
simplify control. However, the observed regularities might also derive from
biomechanical constraints. The low dimensionality of the muscle patterns captured
by muscle synergies might simply reflect the limited number of ways in which a
task can be performed by the musculoskeletal system. For example, the
12 A. d’Avella
X
N
mðt; g; xÞ ¼ ci ðt; g; xÞ wi ð3Þ
i¼1
assume that the synergies wi do not depend on the goal and the initial state. Then,
for a set of tasks for which those synergies provide an adequate basis for the
generation of the motor commands, different task goals are achieved by selecting a
different combination of the existing synergies. Figure 1a represents schematically
such mapping in baseline condition, i.e., before attempting to learn a new task or to
adapt to a perturbation. A new motor skill is compatible with the existing synergies
if it can be acquired by learning a new mapping of goals and initial state onto the
existing synergies (Fig. 1b). Similarly, a perturbation to a task is compatible with
the existing synergies if it can be compensated by adapting the mapping between
goals and initial states onto the existing synergies (Fig. 1c). Throwing while
wearing prism glasses (Martin et al. 1996) provides an example of a perturbation
and a new motor skill likely compatible with existing synergies. Immediately after
wearing prism glasses, which deviate the light path laterally so that the direction of
gaze deviates from the target direction, subjects throw in the direction of gaze and
show large errors. With practice, subjects gradually adapt to the perturbation of the
mapping from visual targets to muscle patterns induced by the prisms and throw
accurately again. As there is no need to change the throwing movement and, thus,
to adapt the muscle synergies underlying the throwing skill, adaptation likely
occurs at the level of the mapping of visual targets onto synergy combinations.
Indeed, invariance of synergy structure has been observed during adaptation to a
visuomotor rotation in a force reaching task (Gentner et al. 2013). After removal of
the glasses, errors are in the opposite direction with respect to the initial error
(aftereffect), indicating that the original mapping has been altered, and additional
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 13
the storage of sensorimotor transformations may become unstable. New motor skills
may erase existing ones. In a noisy and highly redundant neural network such as the
CNS, a way to address such stability–plasticity dilemma may be to segregate synaptic
weights corresponding to different skills in different regions of the huge synaptic
weight space that, even when weights are hyperplastic, represent and maintain stable
sensorimotor transformations (Ajemian et al. 2013). By operating distinct adaptive
processes at the level of modules and module combinations, the CNS may achieve a
trade-off between stability and plasticity and facilitate the acquisition of segregated
synaptic representations of different skills. Acquisition of skills and adaptation to
perturbations compatible with the existing modules may require adjusting a smaller
number of synaptic weights than acquiring skills or adapting to perturbations
incompatible with the modules, which must represent in their synaptic weights all
necessary knowledge of the dynamic behavior of the musculoskeletal system to
implement adequate sensorimotor transformations. However, a larger synaptic
weight space has more capacity for storing different modules in segregated and
noninterfering (i.e., orthogonal) regions of the space. Thus, learning or adapting an
existing skill through recombination of modules is likely a faster adaptive process
than learning or adapting a skill by acquiring new modules.
Faster learning of module combinations than of module structure has been
demonstrated in a simulation of a reaching task with a musculoskeletal model
(Ruckert and d’Avella 2013). Spatiotemporal muscle synergies and synergy com-
bination coefficients generating shoulder and elbow joint torques for reaching six
different targets on a sagittal plane can be learned by reinforcement with hand
distance from the target and squared muscle activation as costs. Modeling the
muscle activation waveforms for each synergy as Gaussian pulses and allowing
each synergy to be scaled in amplitude and shifted in time, with four synergies and
11 muscles, there are 132 task-invariant synergy structure parameters and 48
task-specific synergy combination coefficients. It takes about 3000 simulated trials
to learn these parameters with a stochastic search method and achieve a good
reaching performance. However, once synergy structure is learned, adaptation to a
30° target rotation by relearning only the synergy combination coefficients takes
only less than 200 trials. Thus, a modular representation of the motor commands
makes learning of the representation feasible relying only on a reinforcement signal
and makes adaptation after a compatible perturbation very fast.
As learning or adapting a skill through recombination of modules is likely a
faster adaptive process than learning or adapting a skill by acquiring new modules,
modularity predicts that the difficulty in motor learning depends on the compati-
bility with the modules. Thus, comparing the difficulty for learning or adapting
skills which are either compatible or incompatible with the modules allows testing a
prediction of the modular hypothesis. Consider a perturbation of the way in which
the activation of the muscles acting on the arm generates force at the hand, such as a
surgical rearrangement of the tendons. Imagine a tendon transfer that alters the
amplitude and direction of the forces generated by the muscles involved in a
specific spatial muscle synergy. If the forces are altered such that they cancel each
other when the muscles are activated according the balance prescribed by the
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 15
synergy, recruiting that synergy would generate zero force [see Fig. 1 in Berger
et al. (2013)]. Thus, to compensate such incompatible perturbation it would be
necessary to generate muscle activation patterns that are not in the direction of the
synergy made ineffective by the tendon transfer. If synergies are only a description
of the regularities in the muscle patterns generated by a control architecture that has
direct access to individual muscles, it should be equally difficult to generate new
muscle patterns when they can be expressed as synergy combinations as when they
cannot. In contrast, if the CNS relies on a set of muscle synergies to generate
muscle patterns, if the new muscle patterns required to overcome the perturbation
cannot be generated by the synergies, the synergies must be adapted or new syn-
ergies must be learned. Thus, if muscle synergies are organized by the CNS,
adapting to an incompatible perturbation is expected to be more difficult than
adapting to a compatible perturbation. In contrast, if synergies are just a description
of regularities generated by a controller that has access to individual muscles,
adapting to an incompatible perturbation is expected to be equally difficult than
adapting to a compatible perturbation, provided that the required changes in the
activation of individual muscles are comparable in the two cases.
A recent study in which human subjects used myoelectric control to move a
mass in a virtual environment has tested the prediction that in a modular archi-
tecture adaptation to incompatible perturbations must be harder than adaptation to
compatible perturbation (Berger et al. 2013). Subjects were instructed to perform a
reaching task by displacing a cursor according to either the isometric force applied
on a transducer attached to a forearm, wrist, and hand splint (force control) or the
force estimated from the EMG activity recorded from many shoulder and arm
muscles (myoelectric or EMG control). The cursor and the reach targets were
spheres displayed by a flat monitor occluding the subject’s hand and appearing on a
virtual desktop matching the appearance and position the real desktop. Initially the
reaching task was performed using force control and, for each individual partici-
pant, the force and EMG data collected were used to estimate, using multiple linear
regression, a linear mapping between the recorded EMG activity of each muscle
and the force generated at the hand [EMG-to-force matrix, see Fig. 3 in Berger et al.
(2013)]. EMG data were also used to identify spatial synergies using NMF and to
estimate, through the EMG-to-force matrix, the force generated by each synergy.
Subjects then performed the rest of the experiment using EMG control. In baseline
condition, i.e., when the movement of the cursor depended on the force computed
with the unperturbed EMG-to-force matrix, subjects were able to perform the task
immediately after switching from force control, showing that the linear mapping
was sufficiently accurate. Because the forces were computed in real-time by “vir-
tual” muscles, it was possible to arbitrarily modify the EMG-to-force mapping and
to perform the type of tendon transfers discussed above, i.e., a “virtual surgery” on
the musculoskeletal system. This allowed to compare the adaption rate after a
surgery compatible with the muscle synergies identified in force control with the
adaptation rate after an incompatible surgery. Both compatible and incompatible
surgeries were generated by transforming the instantaneous muscle activity vector
through a multidimensional rotation in muscle space, which affected the amplitude
16 A. d’Avella
and direction of the force generated by each muscle in a complex way but by an
equal amount, on average across muscles, in the two cases. However, after a
compatible surgery, the forces generated by the synergies, also transformed in a
complex way, could still span the force space, i.e., could be combined to generate
any force. In contrast, after an incompatible surgery, the forces generated by the
synergies were aligned along a single direction and did not span the force space.
Forces in any other direction required generating muscle patterns that could not be
obtained by synergy combinations.
Task performance, quantified by the angular error of the cursor’s initial move-
ment direction with respect to the target direction and by the fraction of trials in
which the cursor did not reach and hold the target position in the available time [see
Fig. 6 in Berger et al. (2013)], dropped significantly in the first block after both
types of surgeries but it then improved faster after a compatible surgery than after
an incompatible surgery. The performance in the last block after the compatible
surgery was significantly better than after the incompatible surgery. Performance
improvements after incompatible surgeries, even if they occurred significantly more
slowly than after compatible surgeries, were associated with changes in the muscle
patterns that could not be captured by the original muscle synergies. There was a
significant reduction of the muscle pattern reconstruction quality [see Fig. 8 in
Berger et al. (2013)] at the end of the exposure to incompatible virtual surgery with
respect to compatible virtual surgery. In sum, differences in adaptation rates after
compatible and incompatible virtual surgeries support a neural organization of
muscle synergies and suggest that a slower synergy adaptation process is active
when the set of synergies usually employed for a task becomes ineffective.
In this chapter, I have suggested that the CNS relies on modularity to reduce the
complexity of motor control by embedding features of both the dynamic behavior
of the musculoskeletal system and of the task into a small number of modules and
by directly mapping task goals into module combination parameters. Evidence for
muscle synergies as modules has come from the decomposition of EMG patterns
recorded in many species and tasks. For example, the combinations of small
number of spatiotemporal synergies explain the organization of the muscle patterns
across reaching movement directions and speeds and synergies are modulated in
amplitude according to simple rules. Finally, in a modular architecture, acquisition
of a new motor skill or adaptation of an existing skill after a perturbation may occur
at the level of modules or at the level of module combinations. As learning or
adapting an existing skill through recombination of modules is likely faster than
learning or adapting a skill by acquiring new modules, compatibility with the
modules predicts learning difficulty. The observation of slower adaptation after
incompatible than after compatible virtual surgeries provides direct evidence for
modularity in motor control and motor learning.
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 17
References
Ajemian R, D’Ausilio A, Moorman H, Bizzi E (2013) A theory for how sensorimotor skills are
learned and retained in noisy and nonstationary neural circuits. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 110:
E5078–E5087. doi:10.1073/pnas.1320116110
Alessandro C, Carbajal JP, d’Avella A (2013) A computational analysis of motor synergies by
dynamic response decomposition. Front Comput Neurosci 7:191. doi:10.3389/fncom.2013.
00191
Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements.
J Neurosci 5:2318–2330
Berger DJ, Gentner R, Edmunds T, Pai DK, d’Avella A (2013) Differences in adaptation rates
after virtual surgeries provide direct evidence for modularity. J Neurosci 33:12384–12394.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0122-13.2013
Bernstein N (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movement. Pergamon, Oxford
Bizzi E, Cheung VC, d’Avella A, Saltiel P, Tresch M (2008) Combining modules for movement.
Brain Res Rev 57:125–133. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.08.004
Bizzi E, D’Avella A, Saltiel P, Tresch M (2002) Modular organization of spinal motor systems.
Neuroscientist 8:437–442
Bizzi E, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Giszter SF (1991) Computations underlying the execution of
movement: a biological perspective. Science 253:287–291
Buchanan TS, Almdale DPJ, Lewis JL, Rymer WZ (1986) Characteristics of synergic relations
during isometric contractions of human elbow muscles. J Neurophysiol 56:1225–1241
d’Avella A, Tresch MC (2002) Modularity in the motor system: decomposition of muscle patterns
as combinations of time-varying synergies. In: Dietterich TG, Becker S, Ghahramani Z
(eds) Advances in neural information processing systems, vol 14. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA,
pp 141–148
d’Avella A, Lacquaniti F (2013) Control of reaching movements by muscle synergy combinations.
Front Comput Neurosci 7:42. doi:10.3389/fncom.2013.00042
d’Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (2003) Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a
natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6:300–308
d’Avella A, Portone A, Fernandez L, Lacquaniti F (2006) Control of fast-reaching movements by
muscle synergy combinations. J Neurosci 26:7791–7810. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0830-06.
2006
d’Avella A, Fernandez L, Portone A, Lacquaniti F (2008) Modulation of phasic and tonic muscle
synergies with reaching direction and speed. J Neurophysiol 100:1433–1454. doi:10.1152/jn.
01377.2007
Delis I, Panzeri S, Pozzo T, Berret B (2014) A unifying model of concurrent spatial and temporal
modularity in muscle activity. J Neurophysiol 111:675–693. doi:10.1152/jn.00245.2013
Dominici N, Ivanenko YP, Cappellini G et al (2011) Locomotor primitives in newborn babies and
their development. Science 334:997–999. doi:10.1126/science.1210617
Flanders M (1991) Temporal patterns of muscle activation for arm movements in
three-dimensional space. J Neurosci 11:2680–2693
Flanders M, Pellegrini JJ, Soechting JF (1994) Spatial/temporal characteristics of a motor pattern
for reaching. J Neurophysiol 71:811–813
Flanders M, Pellegrini JJ, Geisler SD (1996) Basic features of phasic activation for reaching in
vertical planes. Exp Brain Res 110:67–79
Gentner R, Edmunds T, Pai DK, d’Avella A (2013) Robustness of muscle synergies during
visuomotor adaptation. Front Comput Neurosci 7:120. doi:10.3389/fncom.2013.00120
Giszter SF (2015) Motor primitives—new data and future questions. Curr Opin Neurobiol 33:
156–165. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2015.04.004
Giszter SF, Hart CB, Silfies SP (2010) Spinal cord modularity: evolution, development, and
optimization and the possible relevance to low back pain in man. Exp Brain Res 200:283–306.
doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2016-x
18 A. d’Avella
Gottlieb GL, Song Q, Almeida GL, Hong DA, Corcos D (1997) Directional control of planar
human arm movement. J Neurophysiol 78:2985–2998
Grillner S (1981) Control of locomotion in bipeds, tetrapods, and fish. In: Brooks VB
(ed) Handbook of physiology: section I. The nervous system. American Physiological Society,
Bethesda, MD, pp 1179–1236
Hart CB, Giszter SF (2004) Modular premotor drives and unit bursts as primitives for frog motor
behaviors. J Neurosci 24:5269–5282. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5626-03.2004
Hollerbach MJ, Flash T (1982) Dynamic interactions between limb segments during planar arm
movement. Biol Cybern 44:67–77
Ivanenko YP, Grasso R, Zago M et al (2003) Temporal components of the motor patterns
expressed by the human spinal cord reflect foot kinematics. J Neurophysiol 90:3555–3565
Ivanenko YP, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F (2004) Five basic muscle activation patterns account for
muscle activity during human locomotion. J Physiol 556:267–282
Klein Breteler MD, Simura KJ, Flanders M (2007) Timing of muscle activation in a hand
movement sequence. Cereb Cortex 17:803–815. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhk033
Kutch JJ, Valero-Cuevas FJ (2012) Challenges and new approaches to proving the existence of
muscle synergies of neural origin. PLoS Comput Biol 8:e1002434. doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.
1002434. PCOMPBIOL-D-11-01006 [pii]
Lacquaniti F, Ivanenko YP, Zago M (2012) Patterned control of human locomotion. J Physiol
590:2189–2199. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2011.215137
Lacquaniti F, Soechting JF, Terzuolo CA (1982) Some factors pertinent to the organization and
control of arm movements. Brain Res 252:394–397. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(82)90410-3
Lacquaniti F, Soechting JF, Terzuolo SA (1986) Path constraints on point-to-point arm movements
in three-dimensional space. Neuroscience 17:313–324
Latash ML (2012) Fundamentals of motor control. Academic Press
Lee WA (1984) Neuromotor synergies as a basis for coordinated intentional action. J Mot Behav
16:135–170
Lee DD, Seung HS (1999) Learning the parts of objects by non-negative matrix factorization.
Nature 401:788–791
Macpherson JM (1991) How flexible are muscle synergies? In: Humphrey DR, Freund H-J
(eds) Motor control: concepts and issues. Wiley, Chichester, pp 33–47
Maier MA, Hepp-Reymond MC (1995) EMG activation patterns during force production in
precision grip. II. Muscular synergies in the spatial and temporal domain. Exp Brain Res
103:123–136
Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT (1996) Throwing while
looking through prisms. II. Specificity and storage of multiple gaze-throw calibrations. Brain
119(Pt 4):1199–1211
Morasso P (1981) Spatial control of arm movements. Exp Brain Res 42:223–227
Ruckert E, d’Avella A (2013) Learned parametrized dynamic movement primitives with shared
synergies for controlling robotic and musculoskeletal systems. Front Comput Neurosci 7:138.
doi:10.3389/fncom.2013.00138
Russo M, D’Andola M, Portone A, Lacquaniti F, d’Avella A (2014) Dimensionality of joint
torques and muscle patterns for reaching. Front Comput Neurosci 8:24. doi:10.3389/fncom.
2014.00024
Saltiel P, Wyler-Duda K, D’Avella A, Tresch MC, Bizzi E (2001) Muscle synergies encoded
within the spinal cord: evidence from focal intraspinal NMDA iontophoresis in the frog.
J Neurophysiol 85:605–619
Santello M, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1998) Postural hand synergies for tool use. J Neurosci
18:10105–10115
Sherrington CS (1948) The integrative action of the nervous system. Univeristy Press, Cambridge
Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1981) Invariant characteristics of a pointing movement in man.
J Neurosci 1:710–720
Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1989) An assessment of the existence of muscle synergies during load
perturbations and intentional movements of the human arm. Exp Brain Res 74:535–548
Modularity for Motor Control and Motor Learning 19
Ting LH, Chiel HJ, Trumbower RD, Allen JL, McKay JL, Hackney ME, Kesar TM (2015)
Neuromechanical principles underlying movement modularity and their implications for
rehabilitation. Neuron 86:38–54. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.02.042
Ting LH, Macpherson JM (2005) A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a
postural task. J Neurophysiol 93:609–613. doi:10.1152/jn.00681.2004
Tresch MC, Jarc A (2009) The case for and against muscle synergies. Curr Opin Neurobiol
19:601–607. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2009.09.002
Tresch MC, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (1999) The construction of movement by the spinal cord. Nat
Neurosci 2:162–167
Synergies in Grasping
Keywords Reach-to-grasp Digits Finger-thumb asymmetry Visuomotor
Prehension Hand transport Grasp control
Introduction
Marc Jeannerod started the study of the grasping movement by postulating two
independent visuomotor channels through which visual input controls the move-
ment (Jeannerod 1981). The argumentation for this postulate was based on the
observation that grasping can be functionally split into a reach component (bringing
the hand to the location of the target) and a grasp component (opening the hand in
accordance with the size and shape of the target). A corresponding distinction is
present in the nature of visual information: information about intrinsic properties
like shape and color (“what”) is essentially different from extrinsic properties such
as distance and orientation (“where”). Both distinctions are also thought to be
present in the neural processing: the neuromuscular system for shaping the hand is
to a large extent independent of the system that is used to transport the hand to the
object (Brinkman and Kuypers 1973) and it has been argued that there are distinct
neural pathways for “what” and “where” (Trevarthen 1968; Ungerleider and Haxby
1994).
An underlying assumption in the above argumentation is that we can understand
human sensorimotor control better if we regard it as two (neuro-) anatomically
distinct pathways, each serving a distinct function. This assumption that distinct
structures are the building blocks of behavior is not specific to the reasoning of the
papers cited above, but also underlies some other influential schemes, such as the
two visual systems hypothesis (Goodale and Milner 1992; Milner and Goodale
2006, 2008). However, one could also bring order into distributed control systems
by the concept of synergies (Lee 1984; Soechting and Lacquaniti 1989; d’Avella
et al. 2003; Ting and Macpherson 2005; Latash et al. 2007; Tresch and Jarc 2009;
Overduin et al. 2015). The cited studies all define synergy in their own way. For the
present purpose, the most important commonality is that a single biomechanical
element (e.g., muscle or joint) can be part of several synergies. Importantly, the
concept of synergies implies that there is no need to have a strict separation between
anatomical aspects of a task. The components could therefore be selected such that
they could be combined with other components to perform quite different tasks.
This is potentially a more efficient strategy, because it allows any combination of
components of movements to be combined in different ways for different tasks.
A consequence, however, is that the components might not be optimized for a
single task.
We will concentrate on kinematic synergies: movements of joints that are
controlled as a single unit. In our earlier work (Smeets and Brenner 1999, 2001,
2008; Verheij et al. 2012), we proposed that the tips of the individual digits are
controlled in grasping. In terms of synergies, we thereby assume the existence of
two synergies: a thumb synergy and an index finger synergy. Given the fact that the
index finger and thumb are part of the same hand, the two synergies’ substrates
overlap: they both contain the shoulder, elbow, and wrist; they differ in the more
distal joints.
At the level of muscles, synergies have been defined as coherent time-varying
patterns of muscle activations (d’Avella et al. 2003; Tresch and Jarc 2009). It has
been shown for force control that the directional tuning of the synergies that are
determined on the basis of the forces exerted at the end effector closely resemble the
tuning of synergies that are based on an analysis of the EMG of more than ten
muscles that are involved (Ting and Macpherson 2005). Therefore, we limit our
analysis to the movements of the end effectors: the index finger and thumb. The
Synergies in Grasping 23
essence of the concept of synergies is that the same synergies are used in many
tasks to simplify control. In other words, specific characteristics of synergies should
be visible in more than a single task. If the thumb and finger synergies are indeed
the building blocks of grasping movements, we should be able to see the signature
of the same two synergies in other tasks like pointing (we use this term for pointing
to an object by moving to touch it with a single digit). In this chapter, we will
describe three ways to study the signature of synergies in kinematics. The first way
is to study idiosyncratic differences in the kinematics of the end effector: if syn-
ergies vary between individuals, the same differences should be visible in all tasks
that involve these synergies. The second way is to adapt the synergies: if a synergy
is adapted in pointing, this adaptation should transfer to grasping. The third way is
to observe fast responses: if a target changes during a goal-directed movements, the
fastest responses should be a direct manifestation of the synergies, neglecting
higher coordination.
Idiosyncratic Kinematics
If the movements of the digits are the synergies in grasping movements, the
characteristics of the movements of the digits during grasping movements should be
similar to those of movements of a single digit. The typical grasping movement
starts with both digits at a staring position, then moving the hand toward the object
while at the same time moving the digits apart to open the hand to be able to grasp
the object (Jeannerod 1981). At about 75 % of the movement time (at 95 % of the
distance; Cuijpers et al. 2004), the digits start closing to contact the object
(Jeannerod 1984). How can we compare the movements of the digits in this task
with a movement of a single digit with similar constrains?
We argued that we could do so by looking at the difference in shape of the
trajectory between finger and thumb. If a separate finger synergy and thumb syn-
ergy exist in grasping, the resulting trajectory formation is likely to be slightly
different for the two digits. Of course, finding a difference does not prove that they
are separate synergies. It might be that the trajectory of the thumb is less curved
than that of the index finger because the thumb is transported during grasping, and
the finger moves relative to the thumb, as has been proposed on the basis of such
findings (Haggard and Wing 1997; Mon-Williams and McIntosh 2000; Galea et al.
2001). If this were the case, one would not expect to find the same difference
between finger and thumb if one were to compare the trajectories of pointing
movements with these digits. On the other hand, if the difference between finger
and thumb are completely due to differences in the synergies, one would expect the
same differences to be present in other tasks involving the same synergies. To test
these predictions one could use a task that is expected to recruit the same synergies
as in the reach-to-grasp movement, but while independently performing different
movements at different times with the two digits.
24 J.B.J. Smeets and E. Brenner
Asymmetry (x2)
Synergies in Grasping 25
0 0 0
Subject Grasp Push Subject Grasp Push Subject Grasp Push
Fig. 2 Predictions for two hypotheses and the actual experimental values for the correlation
between asymmetries. The correlation is calculated between the two tasks within each subject, or
between subjects within the grasp or push task. For the predictions, we plotted “strong correlation”
as 0.9, and “weak correlation” as 0.1. For the experimental data (reanalysed from Smeets et al.
2010), the error bars show the standard error of the mean across subjects and pairs of subjects
26 J.B.J. Smeets and E. Brenner
differences between the digits. Most importantly for the present discussion is that
the consistency in the asymmetry across subjects is the same for both tasks. In terms
of the digit control hypothesis: apparently, despite the anatomical similarities
between subjects, the finger synergy and the thumb synergy differ in a consistent
way between subjects.
Pointing
A second line of research that supports the existence of separate synergies for the
thumb and index finger is that of visuomotor adaptation. We performed an
experiment in which subjects were pointing alternatingly to the left and right side of
a cube, using the thumb and the index finger, respectively (Schot et al. 2014). The
cube could be placed at one of three positions at the far end of a wooden board.
Subjects made their (curved) movements under the board, so the hand was invisible
during most of the movement. Only the digit that made contact with the cube
became visible when it was near the cube. We used the last position of the digit
before it became visible to measure adaptation.
After a baseline phase with an unperturbed (binocular) view of the target, we
gave the subjects a special pair of prism spectacles. These spectacles contained one
leftward and one rightward deviating prism, combined with shutters. The shutters
ensured that the subjects only looked through one of the prisms; which one
depended on the movement they were asked to make. They experienced a leftward
deviated view when pointing with their thumb and a rightward deviated view when
pointing with their index finger. As subjects saw the target cube displaced 5 cm to
the left of its actual position when moving the thumb, subjects initially moved their
thumb to a position 5 cm more to the left than without a prism. In a next trial (with
the index finger), subjects saw the cube 5 cm to the right, leading to an error in the
opposite direction. After 45 trials with each digit, we removed the prisms, and let
the subjects view binocularly again. Comparing the behavior in the post-adaptation
phase with the baseline is a clean measure of the effect of adaptation of the digits’
movements. In a second session, we reversed the pairing (Schot et al. 2014). What
do we expect to happen to the arm movements?
Visuomotor adaptation can be divided in a visual and a motor (proprioceptive)
component (Redding and Wallace 1988). As human vision combines the images of
the two eyes from early vision on, the visual part of the adaptation will be common
for the movements of both digits, and thus cancel each other. If one does not
assume that separate synergies for the control of the digits’ movements adapt
independently, but assumes that each of the joints adapts independently, one would
predict that adaptation will be distributed over the joints that are involved in the
movement (depending on how much each joint contributes to the movement). As
Synergies in Grasping 27
the shoulder, elbow, and wrist are responsible for most of the transport of the digits
in space, this would mean that most of the adaptation would occur in these joints.
The consequence would be that the effect of the two opposite perturbations would
cancel each other. We would therefore expect very little adaptation. Only infor-
mation about the orientation of the eyes and of some joints in the hand would adapt.
If the synergies underlying the finger and thumb movements adapt, one would
expect considerable adaptation of both synergies in opposite directions.
What we found is that both digits adapted to the visual displacement that was
associated with their own movement (Fig. 3b, c). The adaptation that was obtained
was about 60 % for both digits (asymptotes in Fig. 3). Each digit’s adaptation
resembled the conventional result for viewing through prisms (Martin et al. 1996)
or moving through force fields (Shadmehr and BrashersKrug 1997). Such adapta-
tion can be described by various models of (sensori-)motor learning (Smeets et al.
2006; Smith et al. 2006). The results are therefore consistent with the predictions
based on independent synergies for the index finger and thumb.
The adaptation was incomplete. This could be interpreted as evidence that the
adaptation of the two digits was not independent. However, it has been shown that
in conditions with terminal feedback continuous forgetting can lead to incomplete
adaptation (van der Kooij et al. 2015). If forgetting were the cause of the incomplete
adaptation in Fig. 3, a similar incomplete adaptation would be obtained for blocks
of trials with only movements of the thumb (or of the index finger), although the
adaptation would probably be less incomplete because the trials with the same digit
Target cube 10
Index finger
Lateral deviation (cm)
5
Starting cube Index finger
0
10 cm Thumb
-5
Starting
10 diopter
cube Thumb
-10
pre adaptation post pre adaptation post
Fig. 3 Simultaneous adaptation of index finger and thumb to opposite prism displacement; data
are reanalysed from (Schot et al. 2014). a Side and top views of the subject performing a finger
pointing movement. The hand remains invisible until just before the end of the movement. b,
c behavior for the two sessions with opposite pairing between the digit that was moving and the
viewing eye (and therefore the direction of the displacement). Points average response of the eight
subjects. Curves exponential fit to the data points (constrained to have a change in deviation equal
to the size of the prism-induced displacement at the time of addition/removal of the prism)
28 J.B.J. Smeets and E. Brenner
Deviation (cm)
adaptation
-2
-4
3 subjects
0 50 100
Trial
will be closer to each other in time (or number of movements). We tested this with a
small number of subjects (3), and found complete adaptation (Fig. 4). Apparently,
the present paradigm (involving real prisms and goal-directed movements that
contact the targets) does not lead to trial-to-trial forgetting. We do not know why,
but perhaps the presence of haptic feedback in the present experiments is important
(Cuijpers et al. 2008; Schenk 2012). Thus, the adaptation that we found on its own
does not provide conclusive evidence for adaptation within synergies related to the
digits.
Transfer to Grasping
In the previous section, we argued that we could adapt the synergies for the thumb
and index finger separately. The main claim in this chapter is that the same syn-
ergies that are used in pointing are used in the reach-to-grasp movement. If this
claim is correct, adaptation of the synergies during pointing should result in an
aftereffect in the reach-to-grasp movement. As the adaptation is in opposite
directions for the two digits, we predict that the aftereffect of adapting pointing
movements will be an increase or decrease of grip aperture (depending on the
direction of adaptation).
The best way to test a prediction for the transfer of an aftereffect from pointing to
grasping would be to make it quantitative. However, based on Fig. 3b, we realized
that such a prediction would be unrealistic. After removal of the prisms, the pre-
dicted deviation of each digit has changed by about 3 cm. For grasping, this would
imply that the grip aperture would be either increased or reduced by 6 cm,
depending on the pairing between prism and digit during the adaptation phase. As
our objects’ widths are less than 5 cm, the planned positions for the digits in the
Synergies in Grasping 29
12
11
Aperture (cm)
10
thumb-right
8 pre
thumb-right
thumb-left thumb-left
7
2.3 4.6
Size (cm)
Fig. 5 Transfer from pointing to grasping. Left panel the paths corresponding to 100 % transfer
for both pairings. The solid squares represent the target; the open squares and curves represent the
situation corresponding with a full aftereffect. These paths are impossible to produce. Right panel
the actual grip apertures when the digits were approaching the border of the board (for the two
target sizes). There is a clear transfer of the aftereffect
30 J.B.J. Smeets and E. Brenner
shows that grasping is not controlled fundamentally differently from moving the
individual digits during pointing. Assuming that synergies regulate movements of
the thumb and index finger separately, irrespective of the task, provides a parsi-
monious explanation for these findings. Assuming that grip aperture is regulated by
a separate synergy involved in grasping cannot account for the transfer.
Humans are known to be able to adjust their movements to changes in the position
of a target with a very short (*110 ms) latency (reviewed by Cluff et al. 2015;
Smeets et al. 2016). Such fast responses are very interesting, as they must be based
on the least amount of information processing possible: a fast link between visual
information and the fundamental elements of control. As these fast responses do not
take into account all information, the responses can sometimes be counterproduc-
tive. We showed this for a situation in which an obstacle is initially positioned to
the left of the line connecting the hand to the target. In unperturbed trials, subject
then veer slightly rightward. If this obstacle jumps to the right of the line, the
optimal response would be to veer slightly leftward. This is however not what
happens: subjects follow the target, and veer more to the right, and therefore hit the
obstacle (Aivar et al. 2008). The reason is probably that the movement was planned
to pass the obstacle on the right, and the first response to a change in position was a
direct response to the change in the obstacle’s position, without reconsidering one’s
options.
As fast responses link low-level elements of perception to those of motor con-
trol, they can be used to reveal the synergies that underlie grasping behavior. This
has been done in paradigms that involved changes in the position and/or size of an
object (Paulignan et al. 1991a, b; Smeets et al. 2002; van de Kamp and Zaal 2007;
Hesse and Franz 2009; van de Kamp et al. 2009). For instance, the fact that a
perturbation of the contact position for one digit sometimes has a small effect on the
trajectory of the other digit (van de Kamp and Zaal 2007) has been used to argue
that they cannot be controlled independently. However, the coupling between the
digits can also be mechanical. We therefore present data on responses to object
rotation.
There are relatively few studies on responses to object rotation when grasping
(Desmurget et al. 1996; Voudouris et al. 2013). The oldest experiment showed that
you can adjust the orientation of your hand during a whole-hand-grip grasping
movement to a change in object orientation with a short latency (Desmurget et al.
1996). In our experiment (Voudouris et al. 2013), we let subjects grasp lightweight
objects (a cube or a sphere) with a precision grip. The objects were magnetically
connected to a motor that could rotate them very quickly over 12° (clockwise or
counterclockwise) as soon as the subject initiated their grasping movement. For
grasping the cube, it is clear that subjects should respond to the perturbation,
otherwise their grip orientation will not coincide with the surfaces of the cube. For
Synergies in Grasping 31
grasping the ball, rotating the hand in response to the ball’s rotation does not
provide any advantage. What would we expect for various direct couplings between
visual information and controlled synergies?
According to the grasp control hypothesis, the presumed synergies are the
transport and grip: the transport synergy, that includes the orientation (Jeannerod
1981; Desmurget et al. 1996), is coupled to the extrinsic object properties position
and orientation, whereas the grip synergy is coupled to the intrinsic object property
size. As none of these properties changes when a ball rotates, this hypothesis
predicts no response (which is indeed optimal). According to the digit control
hypothesis, the presumed synergies are the digit’s movements. These movements
are directed to suitable contact positions on the surface of the object. So, if the
object rotates, the intended contact positions move, which will lead to fast
adjustment of the digits’ movements.
In line with the prediction of the digit control hypothesis, we always see a fast
response to object rotations (Fig. 6). The sign of the response depends on the
direction of the rotation: the digits follow the object’s surface. The response is
initially the same for the cube and the ball, but continues for more than 200 ms for
the cube (left panel), whereas it disappears within 50 ms for the ball (central panel).
To test whether the short duration of the response for the ball is due to a
reselection of grasping points, we performed an experiment in which the cube was
placed in an ambiguous orientation that allowed for two grasping orientations (right
panel of Fig. 6). We restrict our analysis to the subjects who changed their grip
orientation in response to the perturbation. We see an initial following response that
stops after about 50 ms and reverses its direction. This response is very similar to
the response to the rotation of the ball. Thus the fastest response consists of digits
following the local position on the surface, whereas only the later parts of the
Grip rotation
200 °/s
Switch group
0 100 200 0 100 200 0 100 200
time after rotation onset (ms)
Fig. 6 The velocity of rotation of the grip in response to target rotations in opposite directions.
Data replotted from Voudouris et al. (2013). The gray area indicates times at which the rotation
speed differed for the two directions. Three conditions are displayed: grasping a cube for which it
is clear by which surfaces it can best be grasped (left), grasping a ball (center), and grasping a cube
that is oriented so that it is not evident how best to grasp it (right). In the latter case, subjects
sometimes switch their choice of surfaces in response to the perturbation. In all conditions we see
an initial fast response in the direction of the rotation of the target (solid curves above dashed
ones). For the ball and the cube with the ambiguous orientation, subjects chose new contact points
within 50 ms of their initial response
32 J.B.J. Smeets and E. Brenner
response consider the object’s shape (for the ball) and orientation (for the cube).
The results of the fast responses thus suggest that the synergies are therefore related
to the positions on the surface of the object (i.e., digit control), rather than to the
object as a whole (grasp control).
We used three totally different paradigms to study whether the classical grip control
(with synergies transport and grip) or digit control (with synergies for the indi-
vidual digits) yield the most comprehensive description of the reach-to-grasp
movement. We found that the peculiarities of the digits’ movements varied con-
siderably between subjects, but were (within a subject) remarkably consistent across
grasping with two digits and pushing with a single digit. Second, we showed that by
using prisms, we can adapt the pointing movements of finger and thumb in opposite
directions, and that the aftereffect of these adaptations transfers to grasping. Third,
we show that when grasping a ball with a precision grip, the digits show a fast
following response to a (task-irrelevant) rotation of the ball. All these findings
suggest that the elements that are controlled in grasping are synergies for the
individual digits, rather than synergies for transport and grip.
References
Aivar MP, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2008) Avoiding moving obstacles. Exp Brain Res 190:
251–264
Brinkman J, Kuypers HGJM (1973) Cerebral control of contralateral and ipsilateral arm, hand and
finger movements in the split-brain rhesus monkey. Brain 96:653–674
Cluff T, Crevecoeur F, Scott SH (2015) A perspective on multisensory integration and rapid
perturbation responses. Vision Res 110:215–222
Cuijpers RH, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2008) Consistent haptic feedback is required but it is not
enough for natural reaching to virtual cylinders. Hum Mov Sci 27:857–872
Cuijpers RH, Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2004) On the relation between object shape and grasping
kinematics. J Neurophys 91:2598–2606
d’Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (2003) Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a
natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6:300–308
Desmurget M, Prablanc C, Arzi M, Rossetti Y, Paulignan Y, Urquizar C (1996) Integrated control
of hand transport and orientation during prehension movements. Exp Brain Res 110:265–278
Galea MP, Castiello U, Dalwood N (2001) Thumb invariance during prehension movement:
effects of object orientation. NeuroReport 12:2185–2187
Goodale MA, Milner AD (1992) Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends
Neurosci 15:20–25
Haggard P, Wing A (1997) On the hand transport component of prehensile movements. J Mot
Behav 29:282–287
Hesse C, Franz VH (2009) Corrective processes in grasping after perturbations of object size.
J Mot Behav 41:253–273
Synergies in Grasping 33
Jeannerod M (1981) Intersegmental coordination during reaching at natural visual objects. In:
Long J, Baddeley A (eds) Attention and performance ix. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 153–169
Jeannerod M (1984) The timing of natural prehension movements. J Mot Behav 16:235–254
Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2007) Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Mot Control
11:276–308
Lee WA (1984) Neuromotor synergies as a basis for coordinated intentional action. J Mot Behav
16:135–170
Martin TA, Keating JG, Goodkin HP, Bastian AJ, Thach WT (1996) Throwing while looking
through prisms. 1. Focal olivocerebellar lesions impair adaptation. Brain 119:1183–1198
Milner AD, Goodale MA (2006) The visual brain in action, 2nd edn. Oxford University Press,
Oxford
Milner AD, Goodale MA (2008) Two visual systems re-viewed. Neuropsychologia 46:774–785
Mon-Williams M, McIntosh RD (2000) A test between two hypotheses and a possible third way
for the control of prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:268–273
Overduin SA, d’Avella A, Roh J, Carmena JM, Bizzi E (2015) Representation of muscle synergies
in the primate brain. J Neurosci 35:12615–12624
Paulignan Y, Jeannerod M, MacKenzie C, Marteniuk R (1991a) Selective perturbation of visual
input during prehension movements. 2. The effects of changing object size. Exp Brain Res
87:407–420
Paulignan Y, MacKenzie C, Marteniuk R, Jeannerod M (1991b) Selective perturbation of visual
input during prehension movements. 1. The effects of changing object position. Exp Brain Res
83:502–512
Redding GM, Wallace B (1988) Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent
exposure: Organization of the eye-hand coordination loop. Percept Psychophys 44:59–68
Schenk T (2012) No dissociation between perception and action in patient df when haptic feedback
is withdrawn. J Neurosci 32:2013–2017
Schot WD, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2014) Simultaneous adaptation of the thumb and index finger
of the same hand to opposite prism displacements. J Neurophys 111:2554–2559
Shadmehr R, BrashersKrug T (1997) Functional stages in the formation of human long-term motor
memory. J Neurosci 17:409–419
Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (1999) A new view on grasping. Mot Control 3:237–271
Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2001) Independent movements of the digits in grasping. Exp Brain Res
139:92–100
Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2008) Grasping weber’s law. Curr Biol 18:R1089–R1090
Smeets JBJ, Brenner E, Biegstraaten M (2002) Independent control of the digits predicts an
apparent hierarchy of visuomotor channels in grasping. Behav Brain Res 136:427–432
Smeets JBJ, Martin J, Brenner E (2010) Similarities between digits’ movements in grasping,
touching and pushing. Exp Brain Res 203:339–346
Smeets JBJ, Oostwoud Wijdenes L, Brenner E (2016) Movement adjustments have short latencies
because there is no need to detect anything. Motor Control (in press)
Smeets JBJ, van den Dobbelsteen JJ, de Grave DDJ, van Beers RJ, Brenner E (2006) Sensory
integration does not lead to sensory calibration. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 103:18781–18786
Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R (2006) Interacting adaptive processes with different
timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4:1035–1043
Soechting JF, Lacquaniti F (1989) An assessment of the existence of muscle synergies during load
perturbations and intentional movements of the human arm. Exp Brain Res 74:535–548
Ting LH, Macpherson JM (2005) A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a
postural task. J Neurophys 93:609–613
Tresch MC, Jarc A (2009) The case for and against muscle synergies. Curr Opin Neurobiol
19:601–607
Trevarthen CB (1968) Two mechanisms of vision in primates. Psychologische Forschung 31:
299–348
Ungerleider LG, Haxby JV (1994) ‘What’ and ‘where’ in the human brain. Curr Opin Neurobiol
4:157–165
34 J.B.J. Smeets and E. Brenner
van de Kamp C, Bongers RM, Zaal FTJM (2009) Effects of changing object size during
prehension. J Mot Behav 41:427–435
van de Kamp C, Zaal FTJM (2007) Prehension is really reaching and grasping. Exp Brain Res
182:27–34
van der Kooij K, Brenner E, van Beers RJ, Smeets JBJ (2015) Visuomotor adaptation: how
forgetting keeps us conservative. PLoS ONE 10:e0117901
Verheij R, Brenner E, Smeets JBJ (2012) Grasping kinematics from the perspective of the
individual digits: a modelling study. PLoS ONE 7:e33150
Voudouris D, Smeets JBJ, Brenner E (2013) Ultra-fast selection of grasping points. J Neurophys
110:1484–1489
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled
Movements—What, Whether and How?
Katja Kornysheva
Keywords Motor timing Spatiotemporal control Sequence learning Modular
representation Cortico-subcortical loops
Introduction (“What”)
In the middle of the past century, the engineer and photographer Gjon Mili developed a
technique to capture trajectories of movements in space such as those produced by
musicians, athletes and painters using stroboscopic cameras. He was able to record
skilled movement sequences by attaching a light to the subjects’ effector of interest,
such as the hand holding the violin bow, and letting the movement unfold in darkness
with a long film exposure. The artist himself was only captured towards the end of the
sequence when illuminating the room (Fig. 1a). Recording these trajectories revealed
the skillful movement sequences humans are able to retrieve from memory and produce
K. Kornysheva (&)
Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, UK
e-mail: [email protected]
K. Kornysheva
Department of Neuroscience, Erasmus MC, Rotterdam, The Netherlands
K. Kornysheva
School of Psychology, Bangor University, Bangor, Wales, UK
with their body in space. What remained invisible to Mili’s lens is how the captured
trajectory unfolded in time. It is left to the observer’s imagination what velocity,
acceleration and deceleration patterns the trajectory follows, how these spatial patterns
emerged in time—its temporal features.
While traditionally the focus in motor neuroscience has been on the spatial
dimension of movement sequences, such as the ordering or evolution of movements
in space (Tanji and Shima 1994; Graybiel 1998; Hikosaka et al. 2002; Shenoy et al.
2012), the temporal dimension is equally crucial for the production of many skilled
actions. Producing muscle activations in a correct order, but with inaccurate timing
can have detrimental effects on performance in domains such as speech, complex
tool use and music—a verbal utterance would become incomprehensible to the
receiver, the tennis racket would miss the tennis ball and the violinist would
desynchronize from the orchestra’s pace.
At a purely descriptive level, skilled timing of a movement sequence in space entails
that the movement has a reproducible temporal structure relative to an external stimulus
or an internal motor state such as the occurrence of a movement onset. Here repro-
ducibility entails that there is a certain level of temporal accuracy—typically within tens
of milliseconds for most skilled motor sequences—relative to such a point of reference,
when reaching a particular extrinsically (e.g. in Cartesian space) or intrinsically (e.g. in
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 37
joint or muscle space) defined state of the body. Thus, when repeating a skilled spatial
sequence of movements such as the position of the hand controlling the bow, the
particular points in time (T2, T3) after movement onset time (T1), cluster at the same
extrinsic positions of the bow in two-dimensional space (x and y coordinates), respec-
tively. In other words, a certain spatial configuration is reached at about the same time,
with the degree of clustering reflecting the temporal precision of the movement. The
temporal pattern of a movement trajectory becomes particularly evident with increased
jerk, which reflects the strength of changes between acceleration and deceleration and
whether the movement sequence contains activation pauses such as in a finger pressing
task (Fig. 1b). Defining the motor points of interest is more straightforward for the latter
type of actions (Fig. 1b), as they involve discrete kinematic events. When measuring
motor timing, the timing of several kinematic and dynamic variables may be of interest
depending on the motor task requirements, such as the variability of the spatial trajectory
in time, the interval between an external stimulus and the maximum force, position,
velocity, of a movement, etc., as well as between movements produced using the same
or different effectors. Thus, in principle these variables may capture different aspects of
temporal dynamics of skilled motor sequences as diverse as typing out a Morse code
involving one effector and uttering a word or phrase which engages hundreds of mus-
cles, both of which have to be executed with precise timing.
How does the nervous system represent and integrate the temporal features of
such spatio-temporal sequences?
from the aftereffects and adaptation to velocity-dependent training. The authors con-
cluded that there is an automatic bias to learn state-dependent instead of time-dependent
representations during motor adaptation. Notably, the force field profile employed in
the time-dependent condition was designed to be similar to a velocity-dependent force
field, involving a bell-shaped perturbation with a maximum force in the middle of the
movement when subjects produced the highest velocity. The primacy of
state-dependent representations occurred when a perturbation environment was similar
to a viscous field (water like environment). It is thus feasible that time-dependent force
field profiles that are less correlated with movement velocity may override this bias.
However, in a follow-up study, Mussa-Ivaldi and colleagues (Karniel and
Mussa-Ivaldi 2003) demonstrate that a time-dependent force field that is uncorre-
lated to movement velocity still produces no motor adaptation. Here the
time-dependent force followed a sinusoidal amplitude at 3 Hz and was presented
continuously during the experiment. This important study suggests that the CNS is
unable to form a representation of a regular, temporally predictable force profile that
is uncoupled from state-dependent representation. However, the employed
time-dependent perturbation was not coupled to the onset of the movement as in the
previous experiment (Conditt and Mussa-Ivaldi 1999), or at least to an external cue
relevant to movement initiation. It can thus be hypothesized that this link may be a
constraint for the acquisition of a time-dependent movement adaptation.
Indeed, Medina and colleagues demonstrated that learning motor timing during
adaptation in smooth pursuit eye movements could be independent of
state-dependent encoding (Medina et al. 2005). In training trials, a target moved
horizontally for a fixed duration (500 ms) and deflected vertically from a horizontal
to vertical movement. Probe trials were used to assess adaptation by looking at eye
movement velocity into the vertical direction. Learning to time movements cor-
rectly was independent of the position of the eyes on the horizontal plane and of the
distance/velocity of the movements. Importantly the adaptation effects were
dependent on the predictive power of each variable. If both the time from target
motion onset and the distance travelled were equally predictive, the adapted eye
movements were a mixture of the two representations, whereas if only one variable
was predictive of the vertical perturbation, the adaptation reflected the learning of
time or distance only, respectively. This highlights the flexibility of motor adap-
tation with regard to the representation of time and space depending on which
variable leads to task success.
Diedrichsen and colleagues showed that time- and state-dependent representation of
spatio-temporal movements that involves the coordination of two effectors—the arm
and the thumb—depends on whether their activation overlaps in time (Diedrichsen
et al. 2007). Following a training phase in which the movements had to be timed
precisely, the subjects were asked to reduce the speed of the arm movement. The thumb
press was also timed and scaled in length proportionally to the arm movements,
suggesting that the thumb movement was made dependent on the state (velocity) of the
arm movement and not on absolute time since arm movement onset. Interestingly,
absolute timing was employed when the movements were separated in time during
training, that is when the thumb preceded the arm movement by 100–500 ms. This
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 39
suggests that training temporally overlapping movements produces a bias to encode the
movements of multiple effectors relative to their state, efficiently binding the effectors
together to achieve well-timed coordination. Indeed, it would be detrimental to actions
such as throwing a ball to a target to time arm and wrist movements based on inde-
pendent time estimates. Independent noise levels or drifts would quickly lead to a
decoupled motor state where the timing of muscle activations is disrupted, as in
cerebellar ataxia, and may lead to a state resembling movement decomposition (Bastian
et al. 1996; Timmann et al. 1999).
The impact of overlap between different motor activity states on their temporal
encoding echoes the findings on discrete (non-overlapping) versus continuous
(overlapping) timing tasks. Ivry and colleagues suggested a dichotomy of dedicated
versus emergent encoding of timing for discrete versus continuous movements,
respectively (Spencer et al. 2003; Ivry and Spencer 2004; Ivry and Schlerf 2008).
Temporal variability on continuous tasks characterized by smooth transitions
between different motor states (e.g. circle drawing) have been reported to be
uncorrelated with the temporal variability on discrete tasks characterized by
movement pauses in between boosts of motor activity (e.g. tapping) (Zelaznik et al.
2005). Moreover adjustment to timing perturbations is faster and more precise for
discrete as opposed to continuous movements (Elliott et al. 2009; Repp and
Steinman 2010; Studenka and Zelaznik 2011) and patient studies suggest that these
movements might rely on different neural substrates (Spencer et al. 2003; Spencer
and Ivry 2005). Yet, it is unlikely that movement kinematics alone determine
whether temporal encoding is dedicated versus emergent: As discussed above, even
continuous movements like smooth pursuit can be controlled using dedicated
timing mechanisms and independently of parameters such as movement velocity,
whenever the absolute timing predicts task success (Medina et al. 2005), or when a
periodic circle drawing tasks contains a salient auditory cue marking the completion
of a cycle (Zelaznik and Rosenbaum 2010; Braun Janzen et al. 2014).
When it comes to dissociating the spatial and temporal organization of sequential
motor skills, the focus has been on learning the organization of sequences of move-
ments rather than on learning the production of the constituent movements per se. Thus,
typically subjects are trained to sequence simple overlearned movements like finger
presses (Sakai et al. 2003; Ullen and Bengtsson 2003; O’Reilly et al. 2008; Kornysheva
et al. 2013; Kornysheva and Diedrichsen 2014). With training the production of
sequences becomes more accurate and is retrieved faster as evidenced by shorter
sequence duration or reaction times (RT) depending on the task employed. In addition,
a temporal grouping idiosyncratic to the subject or facilitated externally by the
sequence structure emerges, such that certain movements in the sequence become
closer in time than others creating so-called chunks. There is compelling evidence that
breaking up the sequence within chunks as opposed to between chunks when
reordering the sequence leads to losses in performance [for reviews see (Sakai et al.
2004)]. This suggests that a dedicated representation has been formed for each chunk of
movements in space which facilitates performance—similar to chunks in working
memory and cognitive control (Baddeley 2010). It has been hypothesized that this
temporal grouping is a sign of a skill becoming automatic and pairing the sequence
40 K. Kornysheva
with a different temporal structure would lead to losses in performance as this automatic
representation has not been formed (Hikosaka et al. 2002; Sakai et al. 2004).
Interestingly, there is evidence that while changing a chunking structure (ex-
ternally induced) can lead to performance losses, these are not as pronounced as
when performing a novel sequence (O’Reilly et al. 2008). This suggests some form
of independence for the spatial organization of sequences, on top of the integrated
spatio-temporal chunking structure. In contrast, many studies have shown that
retaining the timing while changing the spatial feature of movement sequences does
not provide any benefit as compared to a new sequence, which advocates that the
temporal structure of these sequences is invariably bound to their sequential
movements in space (Shin and Ivry 2002, 2003; O’Reilly et al. 2008).
This, however, has been challenged recently in a series of experiments
(Kornysheva et al. 2013; Kornysheva and Diedrichsen 2014). Here the experi-
mental test involved producing sequences following training of a single
spatio-temporal sequence of finger presses in a timed SRT task (Penhune and Steele
2012). These were either repeated in a block of several trials or new on every trial.
The results suggested that RT savings for a trained temporal feature paired with a
new sequence of finger presses (spatial feature) could only emerge once the new
spatial feature became more predictable through repetition (Fig. 2a, b). Note that
the advantage for the trained temporal features is relative to the control condition in
which the sequence was also repeated and the finger sequence became equally more
predictable with repetition. In contrast when the finger order was new on each trial
comparable to the random spatial sequence controls in the studies discussed above,
there was no advantage related to learning the timing of the sequence. It is unlikely
that this is an effect of whether these sequences were learned implicitly or had an
explicit component, as both the presence and the absence of temporal transfer were
found depending on the familiarity with the spatial feature.
More formally, drift diffusion modelling demonstrated that these results can be best
approximated using a multiplicative integration of independent spatial and temporal
sequence feature representations as follows such as Zn+1 = Zn + V + S + (S * T),
rather then an additive integration (Zn+1 = Zn + V + S + T), or a combined
spatio-temporal term without a separate temporal representation
(Zn+1 = Zn + V + S + C). Here Z is the selection layer corresponding to the five fin-
gers, V is the visual stimulus in the serial reaction time task (SRTT), S the spatial, T the
temporal and C the combined representation in which the temporal sequence feature is
linked to a specific spatial feature (weights and noise terms are omitted for abbreviation
purposes). Essentially, this means that while effects of the spatial feature representation
act independently (additive integration) the temporal representation can only be
expressed when S > 0, in other words there is some knowledge of the spatial repre-
sentation. The difference between an integrated spatio-temporal versus an independent
temporal representation which is multiplicatively combined with the spatial one is
critical, as only the latter allows for temporal transfer which we could reliably observe
across experiments (Fig. 2b, c).
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 41
Fig. 2 Evidence suggesting that spatial and temporal features of movement sequences are
represented independently. a Participants were trained on a specific spatio-temporal finger
sequence (green) and then tested on a novel sequence (black) or on sequences that retained either
the temporal (red) or spatial (blue) structure (Kornysheva et al. 2013; Kornysheva and Diedrichsen
2014). The numbers 1–5 in exemplary sequences correspond to the thumb, index, middle, ring and
little finger, respectively. b Reaction time advantages relative to a new sequence that are related to
a learned trained temporal feature can only be expressed when the spatial feature becomes more
predictable. Solid lines correspond to “trained”, “temporal” and “novel” conditions in which the
corresponding sequences are presented 10 times in a row, whereas the dashed lines correspond to
conditions where the trained temporal feature is paired with a new spatial feature on every trial
(dashed red) and compared to a sequence that changes both the temporal and the spatial feature on
every trial. Stars indicate significant differences across trials (Kornysheva et al. 2013). c Reaction
time results indicate independent transfer of spatial and temporal features to test conditions
(Kornysheva and Diedrichsen 2014). d Separate, but partly overlapping spatial (blue) and temporal
(red) representations of finger sequences can be revealed bilaterally in premotor cortex (PM and
SMA) using multi-voxel pattern analysis. The two features are integrated in contralateral M1 only
(green). In a series of behavioural and fMRI experiments employing (Kornysheva and Diedrichsen
2014) e The premotor nucleus HVC in zebra finches reflects changes in the temporal feature of a
bird song (red line), such as a prolonged syllable, but not changes in its pitch feature (blue line).
Both types of changes were acquired through aversive conditional auditory feedback (adapted
from Ali et al. 2013)
neural level. Moreover, subtracting out the main effects of independent spatial and
temporal features from the overall activity patterns isolated residual patterns, which,
if unique for each sequence, were taken as integrated neural representations.
The results revealed that fine-grained patterns in overlapping patches of the
lateral (dorsal and ventral) and medial (SMA) premotor cortex carried information
on the independent spatial as well as independent temporal patterns, whilst the only
region informative of an integrated spatio-temporal representation was the con-
tralateral primary motor cortex, the output stage of the neocortex (Fig. 2d). Thus, in
M1 each sequence may recruit a subpopulation of neurons that controls a particular
combination of spatiotemporal synergies (d’Avella et al. 2003). The latter cannot be
synergies of individual finger movements as each finger movement occurred in each
sequence, but particular spatio-temporal transitions within sequences. The same
principle, but now for spatial and temporal parameters would apply for the premotor
cortex—unique combinations of synergies capturing particular spatial
(timing-invariant) or temporal transitions.
The alternative is that the encoding observed in M1 is not sequential encoding
per se, but reflects the two spatial and temporal codes being combined nonlinearly.
Also while the force level for each finger matched well across sequences, it cannot
be completely excluded that tiny biases—thumb, index finger, etc., being more
active in one sequence than in another—may have contributed to the encoding to
some extent. Yet, this explanation is unlikely, since encoding in contralateral M1
correlated with sequence learning, but not with sequence classification accuracy
based on the force at each finger.
The presence of independent spatial and temporal codes, as well as integrated
representations suggests varied levels of abstraction from the actual motor response
implementation. To be transferable across different temporal profiles, the spatial
sequence in the premotor cortices has to lack specifics on the kinematics or
dynamics of each effector involved during sequence production, and may carry
more abstract information such as on sequential transitions between movements
(Tanji and Shima 1994). Conversely, the temporal feature representation is bound
to lack any information on the effectors and the dynamics such as force on each
finger to be transferable across different finger movement sequences.
Interestingly, a similar dissociation in the control of spatial (pitch) and temporal
sequences has been found in songbirds (Ali et al. 2013). Using aversive auditory
conditioning, the authors taught the animals to selectively modify temporal and
spectral features of their song, such as changing the length of a syllable, or its pitch
which requires a different configuration of muscle activations controlling the syrinx
(Fig. 2e). The basal ganglia analog was required for the modification of the spectral
properties (pitch), but not for changes in the temporal structure. By contrast, the
activity in HVC (an analog to the premotor cortex) reflected the temporal but not
spectral features of the song. This dissociation and therefore modularity of spatial
and temporal features in motor sequence control may thus be a universal property of
the CNS.
These findings resonate with the hypothesis by d’Avella and colleagues sug-
gesting that the control of movement may be modular during a variety of reaching
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 43
It has been hypothesized that a variety of neural structures are capable of encoding
the timing of movements, which corresponds to the widespread involvement of
these areas in explicit or implicit motor timing tasks—in particular the cerebellum,
the striatum and the lateral and medial premotor cortices (Lewis and Miall 2003;
Buhusi and Meck 2005; Ivry and Schlerf 2008; Buonomano and Laje 2010; Teki
et al. 2011; Laje and Buonomano 2013). This is surprising as these different parts of
the nervous system have diverse neural architectures, as well as physiological and
44 K. Kornysheva
stimulus, US). The latter is transmitted by the climbing fibre system, and leads to
the depression of those parallel fibre to PC synapses active just before the time of
the aversive stimulus, partly mediated by plasticity in interneurons inhibiting the PC
(Medina and Mauk 2000; Heiney et al. 2014). This eventually leads to decreased
PC simple spike cell firing during the interval between the two stimuli with the most
pronounced reduction timed just before the conditioned response (CR), the latter
being initiated via the disinhibition of the DCN (Jirenhed et al. 2007; Ten Brinke
et al. 2015). It has been repeatedly shown that the intact cerebellar cortex is nec-
essary for a precisely timed response, as the intact DCN alone produces a
short-latency response without any temporal features necessary for the task (Perrett
et al. 1993; Koekkoek et al. 2003). Importantly, this notion advocates a distributed
motor learning architecture across the cerebellum (Gao et al. 2012), and argues for a
special role of the cerebellar cortex in motor timing.
More recently it has been proposed that the temporal profile of the response can
be acquired locally in the PC (Johansson et al. 2014). Specifically, pairing a CS
consisting of a direct stimulation of the parallel fibres (circumventing the granular
cell layer) with a US consisting of direct climbing fibre stimulation led to a Purkinje
cell CR that was adaptively timed. The cell reached maximum suppression of
75 ms before the onset of the US across different CS-US intervals. Importantly,
even when blocking inhibition from inhibitory interneurons that are also innervated
by parallel fibres and could have had an effect on the PC response, the learned
timing was preserved. This led the authors to conclude that the encoding of the
precisely timed response is located in the PC at the molecular level. Specifically,
blocking mGluR7 receptor has been shown to disrupt timing in the direct stimu-
lation paradigm above (Johansson et al. 2015). While the exact mechanism of
molecular timing is still unknown, it has been hypothesized that the CS may initiate
a predictable biochemical cascade while the US onset induces interval-specific
changes to this cascade. This could take place in form of a selection of different
molecular components with particular properties with regard to the duration of ion
channel open states, so that the time course of the PC simple spike depression
matches the CS-US interval.
Regardless of whether the timing mechanism is distributed or localized, the parts
of the cerebellar cortex involved in classical conditioning project to a specific target
effector in the periphery and cannot be expected to be transferable across different
effectors, spatial configurations or motor states. For instance, the cerebellar cortical
projection to the anterior interpositus of the DCN nucleus involved in eyeblink
conditioning innervates periorbital muscles of the eye via the brain stem (Ten
Brinke et al. 2015). However, a more abstract representation of timing for
spatio-temporal movements is still conceivable in those regions of the cerebellum
that project to the premotor and prefrontal cortices via the dentate nucleus (Kelly
and Strick 2003), albeit only if they receive climbing fibre stimulation at the time of
the US during learning which has not been investigated systematically so far.
Another timing mechanism has been attributed to the basal ganglia, the striatal
beat frequency model (Matell et al. 2004; Buhusi and Meck 2005). Unlike the
cerebellar timing mechanisms described in this chapter, the latter is relevant for
46 K. Kornysheva
interval timing that involves intervals of seconds-to-minutes. While even the lower
range may appear too long to be relevant for motor timing many skilled movements
like verbal utterances, musical and dance sequences, as well as the typing Morse
code messages involve sequences of movements that unfold over the timescale of
several seconds to tens of seconds. The basal ganglia is organized in cortico-basal
ganglia-thalamo-cortical loops with the majority of the excitatory input coming
from the cortex and then sent out to direct and indirect pathways of the basal
ganglia which excite and inhibit the cortex, respectively, via the thalamus (Graybiel
1998). Here each medium spiny neuron in the striatum receives up to 30.000
separate axons from the cortex. Thus, it has been proposed that through learning the
medium spiny neurons in the striatum act as coincidence detectors of neural
oscillations that operate at different frequencies in the neocortex (Buhusi and Meck
2005). With trial onset the phase of the oscillations is reset (“start-gun”). During
learning a reward signal at the end of the interval to be trained is conveyed by
dopaminergic input from the substantia nigra pars compacta and the ventral
tegmental area. Experience-dependent changes in cortico-striatal transmission (both
LTP and LTD) lead to a ramp of striatal activity with a peak at the time of the
expected reward, i.e. at the end of the interval. Accordingly, following training
striatal neurons may be capable of detecting the unique coincidence of phases of the
neural oscillators that project to these neurons, respectively. Interestingly such
adaptively timed ramping activity has also been observed in the neocortex, such as
in a motor synchronization-continuation task involving isochronous intervals per-
formed at different speeds in the monkey supplementary motor area
(SMA) (Merchant et al. 2013) and an interval reproduction task in the parietal
cortex (Jazayeri and Shadlen 2015). Although there has been no direct experimental
evidence from studies involving sub-second intervals, it is likely that such ramps
reflect the striatal activity via the direct basal ganglia thalamic route to the neo-
cortex. Indeed, imaging, lesion and pharmacological studies have confirmed the
involvement of the striatum in interval timing (for a review cf. Buhusi and Meck
2005).
Finally, the neocortex could be regarded as most closely related to models
involving random recurrent networks (Thomson and Bannister 2003; Buonomano
and Laje 2010). Recent concurrent multiunit recordings from premotor and primary
motor cortices suggest that the trajectory of a movement is not represented in terms
of its features such as position, velocity, direction, force and timing as suggested
before, but rather as a compound of variables correlated leading to the performed
trajectory in space (Churchland et al. 2006; Shenoy et al. 2012; Kaufman et al.
2015). Here the timing is merely an emergent feature of the evolution of the
multiunit activity which controls the spatial movement trajectory. Accordingly, a
model of randomly connected networks can be trained to produce skilled sequential
movements and have perfectly reproducible temporal dynamics without any dedi-
cated encoding of the temporal dimension in the model (Laje and Buonomano
2013). Such a network of interconnected units can be trained to represent the
spatio-temporal evolution of a trajectory as complex as handwriting (in
two-dimensional space).
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 47
randomly recurrent units, the precise changes in multiunit space would be ignored,
such that a certain cascade of states would be mapped onto the same spatial state
(configuration of finger activations). The temporal evolution would then be speci-
fied at the stage when both are combined either by acting on integrated
spatio-temporal M1 representations (Kornysheva and Diedrichsen 2014) or
downstream in the case of direct cortico-spinal projections from the premotor
cortex.
How do these regions interact with each other to achieve precise motor timing of
skilled movements? Here only projections with a short latency (“online”) trans-
duction up to tens of milliseconds can be considered to exhibit control at time scales
relevant to online motor control.
For a long time it has been assumed that the basal ganglia and the cerebellum
operate in parallel to each other at the subcortical level, having separate thalamic
relays to the neocortex (Bostan et al. 2013). However, in rodents (Ichinohe et al.
2000) and more recently in primates (Hoshi et al. 2005; Bostan et al. 2010) disy-
naptic connections from the DCN to the striatum have been established. The relay is
located in the intralaminar nuclei of the thalamus which contain projections to the
striatum. Recently, it has been determined that the propagation speed between DCN
and the dorsolateral striatum can be as low as 10 ms (Chen et al. 2014). This
suggests a rapid transmission of cerebellar output to striatal. High frequency and
well-timed bursts in DCN neurons can modulate activity at the entry stage of the
basal ganglia, thereby coordinating cerebellar output with the basal ganglia com-
putations in real time. Interestingly when stimulation of the DCN was combined
with concurrent cortico-striatal input, the cortico-striatal activation was potentiated
(Chen et al. 2014). The cerebellar output signals which carry a high temporal
resolution profile of a signal are therefore impacting the neocortical input at the
level of the ramping activity of medium spiny neurons. At the same time the
subthalamic nucleus to which striatal neurons project via the indirect pathway
innervates the cerebellum via the pontine nuclei. The propagation speed of this
connection is currently unknown.
As with the basal ganglia, the premotor cortex forms reciprocal disynaptic
connections with the cerebellum (Kelly and Strick 2003). The DCN project to the
neocortex via the ventrolateral nucleus of the thalamus and affect not only supra-
granular layers, but also directly layer V in M1 as shown by optogenetic stimulation
of the cerebellar Purkinje cells (Proville et al. 2014). Importantly, the DCN inhi-
bition is followed by a rebound excitation following the offset of Purkinje cell
stimulation at around 60ms and in M1 40ms later. At the same time this study
revealed a short-latency transmission between M1/S1 and the lateral cerebellar
cortex, with onsets of Purkinje cell frequency modulation as early as 10ms after
neocortical stimulation. Finally, non-invasive research in humans has shown that
the latency of cerebellar inhibition of the cortex as measured by M1 triggered MEP
is highest at 5 ms delay (Ugawa et al. 1991), confirming a rapid transmission
between the cerebellum and the neocortex. In other words, it is likely that the (pre-)
motor cortical networks relevant for temporal encoding receive a precisely timed
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 49
(high resolution) signal from the cerebellum while the latter is modulated by
neocortical input, with these interactions unfolding almost instantaneously.
Why do we need parallel timers in our brain operating in parallel and what is
their specific contribution? A schematic model based on the current review is
presented in Fig. 3 (cf. caption for details). At the current stage, any answers to this
question will remain speculative. Most of the invasive electrophysiological
recordings that could provide direct evidence for this report only from one region at
a time. Yet, in an intact brain it is impossible to disentangle whether the activities
reported relay the input of interconnected regions, or whether this activity originates
and is causally involved in the production of well-timed movements. Even lesion
studies (temporal inactivation, TMS, patients, etc.) are of limited use, as they cause
Fig. 3 Temporal encoding for skilled spatiotemporal sequence production. a Modular represen-
tation of temporal (red dots, a longer and a shorter interval) and spatial (blue dots) sequence
features. The temporal representation modulates the signal originating from two different spatial
representations (black broken arrows) (Kornysheva et al. 2013). This allows two different
sequences S1 and S2 to utilize the same learned temporal structure flexibly (adapted from
Diedrichsen and Kornysheva 2015). b The premotor cortex, the cerebellar cortex and the striatum
utilize different computational mechanisms that can be harnessed to learn and control motor timing
—either independently of the movement in space as shown here or in an integrated spatio-temporal
fashion (see main text). These regions are interconnected with each other by short-latency circuits
via the thalamus and the pons, respectively. The following model of motor timing for skilled
movement sequences is proposed in the current review: The neocortex produces sustained
dynamic activity in a population of interconnected neurons which can be utilized for the duration
of a whole sequence of movements (Buonomano and Laje 2010). This multi-unit activity is read
out by the MSN in the striatum based on oscillation phase detection and chunked into a series of
ramps that mark the interval between movement onsets or between an external stimulus and a
motor response (Buhusi and Meck 2005). Crucially, the cortical and striatal activity is fed into the
cerebellum, providing a sequential context signal for each movement unfolding in the seconds time
range. This activity is transformed by the cerebellar cortex into a precise high temporal resolution
output on a sub-seconds scale in the deep cerebellar nuclei for each sequence component. Through
disynaptic projections, the latter modulates both the ramps in the striatum and the population
clocks in the neocortex to achieve a more precisely timed representation of the sequence.
Abbreviations: il—intralaminar; MSN—medium spiny neurons; S—sequence; T—time point; Tha
—Thalamus; va—ventroanterior; vl—ventrolateral
50 K. Kornysheva
reorganization in the network, that unless recorded, remains hidden and may impact
conclusions with regard to behaviour. Short-lived local inactivation though mus-
cimol, optogenetic stimulation (animal models) and transcranial magnetic stimu-
lation (humans) whilst recording from the site to which the region that is disrupted
projects are likely to provide more conclusive answers to this question. For
instance, to assess the individual contribution of cortical and subcortical sites to
learned timing, a pioneering study by Mauk and colleagues has been conducted to
decompose the contributions of the neocortex versus the cerebellar nuclei to trace
eye blink conditioning (Siegel and Mauk 2013). This task is known to rely not only
on the cerebellum (in contrast to delay eyeblink conditioning), but also on the
cortex and the hippocampus. Here it could be demonstrated that ramping activity
observed in prefrontal cells, as well as the well-timed conditioned motor response is
abolished when cerebellar output is inhibited, whereas the sustained activity during
the duration of the CS remained intact. In the future similar studies need to be
designed to directly probe the contribution of the premotor cortex, the striatum and
the cerebellar cortex to skilled motor timing.
References
Ali F, Otchy T, Pehlevan C et al (2013) The basal ganglia is necessary for learning spectral, but not
temporal, features of birdsong. Neuron 80:494–506. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2013.07.049
Averbeck BB, Chafee MV, Crowe DA, Georgopoulos AP (2002) Parallel processing of serial
movements in prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 99:13172–13177. doi:10.1073/pnas.
162485599
Baddeley A (2010) Working memory. Curr Biol 20:R136–R140. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2009.12.014
Bastian AJ, Martin TA, Keating JG, Thach WT (1996) Cerebellar ataxia: abnormal control of
interaction torques across multiple joints. J Neurophysiol 76:492–509
Bostan A, Dum R, Strick P (2010) The basal ganglia communicate with the cerebellum. Proc Natl
Acad Sci 107:8452–8456. doi:10.1073/pnas.1000496107
Bostan A, Dum R, Strick P (2013) Cerebellar networks with the cerebral cortex and basal ganglia.
Trends Cogn Sci 17:241–254. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2013.03.003
Braitenberg V (1967) Is the cerebellar cortex a biological clock in the millisecond range? Prog
Brain Res 25:334–346. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)60971-1
Braun Janzen T, Thompson WF, Ammirante P, Ranvaud R (2014) Timing skills and expertise:
discrete and continuous timed movements among musicians and athletes. Front Psychol
5:1482. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01482
Buhusi C, Meck W (2005) What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval
timing. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:755–765. doi:10.1038/nrn1764
Buonomano DV, Laje R (2010) Population clocks: motor timing with neural dynamics. Trends
Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 14:520–527. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.002
Chen C, Fremont R, Arteaga-Bracho E, Khodakhah K (2014) Short latency cerebellar modulation
of the basal ganglia. Nat Neurosci 17:1767–1775. doi:10.1038/nn.3868
Churchland MM, Santhanam G, Shenoy KV (2006) Preparatory activity in premotor and motor
cortex reflects the speed of the upcoming reach. J Neurophysiol 96:3130–3146. doi:10.1152/jn.
00307.2006
Conditt M, Mussa-Ivaldi F (1999) Central representation of time during motor learning. Proc Natl
Acad Sci USA 96:11625–11630
d’Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (2003) Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a
natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6:300–308. doi:10.1038/nn1010
d’Avella A (2017) Modularity for motor control and motor learning. Adv Exp Med Biol 957:3–19
Dean P, Porrill J, Ekerot C-F, Jorntell H (2009) The cerebellar microcircuit as an adaptive filter:
experimental and computational evidence. Nat Rev Neurosci 11:30–43. doi:10.1038/nrn2756
Dean P, Anderson S, Porrill J, Jorntell H (2013) An adaptive filter model of cerebellar zone C3 as a
basis for safe limb control? J Physiol 591:5459–5474. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2013.261545
Diedrichsen J, Kornysheva K (2015) Motor skill learning between selection and execution. Trends
Cogn Sci (Regul Ed) 19:227–233. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.003
Diedrichsen J, Criscimagna-Hemminger S, Shadmehr R (2007) Dissociating timing and
coordination as functions of the cerebellum. J Neurosci Official J Soc Neurosci 27:
6291–6301. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0061-07.2007
Elliott MT, Welchman AE, Wing AM (2009) Being discrete helps keep to the beat. Exp Brain Res
192:731–737. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1646-8
Evarts EV (1968) Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement.
J Neurophysiol 31:14–27
52 K. Kornysheva
Gao Z, Beugen B, De Zeeuw C (2012) Distributed synergistic plasticity and cerebellar learning.
Nat Rev Neurosci 13:619–635. doi:10.1038/nrn3312
Georgopoulos AP, Kalaska JF, Caminiti R, Massey JT (1982) On the relations between the
direction of two-dimensional arm movements and cell discharge in primate motor cortex.
J Neurosci 2:1527–1537
Graybiel A (1998) The basal ganglia and chunking of action repertoires. Neurobiol Learn Mem
70:119–136. doi:10.1006/nlme.1998.3843
Heiney S, Kim J, Augustine G, Medina J (2014) Precise control of movement kinematics by
optogenetic inhibition of Purkinje cell activity. J Neurosci 34:2321–2330. doi:10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.4547-13.2014
Hikosaka O, Nakamura K, Sakai K, Nakahara H (2002) Central mechanisms of motor skill
learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol 12:217–222. doi:10.1016/S0959-4388(02)00307-0
Hoshi E, Tremblay L, Féger J et al (2005) The cerebellum communicates with the basal ganglia.
Nat Neurosci 8:1491–1493. doi:10.1038/nn1544
Huang C-C, Sugino K, Shima Y et al (2013) Convergence of pontine and proprioceptive streams
onto multimodal cerebellar granule cells. eLife 2:e00400. doi:10.7554/eLife.00400
Ichinohe N, Mori F, Shoumura K (2000) A di-synaptic projection from the lateral cerebellar
nucleus to the laterodorsal part of the striatum via the central lateral nucleus of the thalamus in
the rat. Brain Res. doi:10.1016/S0006-8993(00)02744-X
Ishikawa T, Shimuta M, Häusser M (2015) Multimodal sensory integration in single cerebellar
granule cells in vivo. eLife e12916
Ivry R, Schlerf J (2008) Dedicated and intrinsic models of time perception. Trends Cogn Sci
12:273–280. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.04.002
Ivry R, Spencer R (2004) The neural representation of time. Curr Opin Neurobiol 14:225–232.
doi:10.1016/j.conb.2004.03.013
Jazayeri M, Shadlen MN (2015) A neural mechanism for sensing and reproducing a time interval.
Curr Biol 25:2599–2609. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2015.08.038
Jirenhed D-A, Bengtsson F, Hesslow G (2007) Acquisition, extinction, and reacquisition of a
cerebellar cortical memory trace. J Neurosci 27:2493–2502. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4202-
06.2007
Johansson F, Jirenhed D-AA, Rasmussen A et al (2014) Memory trace and timing mechanism
localized to cerebellar Purkinje cells. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 111:14930–14934. doi:10.1073/
pnas.1415371111
Johansson F, Carlsson HA, Rasmussen A et al (2015) Activation of a temporal memory in
Purkinje cells by the mGluR7 receptor. Cell Rep 13:1741–1746. doi:10.1016/j.celrep.2015.10.
047
Karniel A, Mussa-Ivaldi F (2003) Sequence, time, or state representation: how does the motor
control system adapt to variable environments? Biol Cybern 89:10–21. doi:10.1007/s00422-
003-0397-7
Kaufman MT, Churchland MM, Ryu SI, Shenoy KV (2015) Vacillation, indecision and hesitation
in moment-by-moment decoding of monkey motor cortex. eLife. doi:10.7554/eLife.04677
Kelly R, Strick P (2003) Cerebellar loops with motor cortex and prefrontal cortex of a nonhuman
primate. J Neurosci Official J Soc Neurosci 23:8432–8444
Koekkoek SK, Hulscher HC, Dortland BR et al (2003) Cerebellar LTD and learning-dependent
timing of conditioned eyelid responses. Science 301:1736–1739. doi:10.1126/science.1088383
Kornysheva K, Diedrichsen J (2014) Human premotor regions parse sequences into their spatial
and temporal features. eLife 3:e03043:e03043. doi:10.7554/eLife.03043
Kornysheva K, Sierk A, Diedrichsen J (2013) Interaction of temporal and ordinal representations
in movement sequences. J Neurophysiol 109:1416–1424. doi:10.1152/jn.00509.2012
Laje R, Buonomano D (2013) Robust timing and motor patterns by taming chaos in recurrent
neural networks. Nat Neurosci 16:925–933. doi:10.1038/nn.3405
Lewis PA, Miall RC (2003) Distinct systems for automatic and cognitively controlled time
measurement: evidence from neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 13:250–255
Encoding Temporal Features of Skilled Movements … 53
Matell MS, Meck WH (2004) Cortico-striatal circuits and interval timing: coincidence detection of
oscillatory processes. Cognitive Brain Res 21(2): 139–70
Medina J, Mauk M (2000) Computer simulation of cerebellar information processing. Nat
Neurosci 3(Suppl):1205–1211. doi:10.1038/81486
Medina J, Carey M, Lisberger S (2005) The representation of time for motor learning. Neuron
45:157–167. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.017
Merchant H, Perez O, Zarco W, Gamez J (2013) Interval tuning in the primate medial premotor
cortex as a general timing mechanism. J Neurosci 33:9082–9096. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
5513-12.2013
O’Reilly J, McCarthy K, Capizzi M, Nobre A (2008) Acquisition of the temporal and
ordinal structure of movement sequences in incidental learning. J Neurophysiol 99:2731–2735.
doi:10.1152/jn.01141.2007
Overduin SA, d’Avella A, Roh J et al (2015) Representation of muscle synergies in the primate
brain. J Neurosci 35:12615–12624. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4302-14.2015
Penhune V, Steele C (2012) Parallel contributions of cerebellar, striatal and M1 mechanisms to
motor sequence learning. Behav Brain Res 226:579–591. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.044
Perrett SP, Ruiz BP, Mauk MD (1993) Cerebellar cortex lesions disrupt learning-dependent timing
of conditioned eyelid responses. J Neurosci 13:1708–1718
Proville RD, Spolidoro M, Guyon N, Dugué GP (2014) Cerebellum involvement in cortical
sensorimotor circuits for the control of voluntary movements
Repp BH, Steinman SR (2010) Simultaneous event-based and emergent timing: synchronization,
continuation, and phase correction. J Mot Behav 42:111–126. doi:10.1080/00222890903566418
Sakai K, Kitaguchi K, Hikosaka O (2003) Chunking during human visuomotor sequence learning.
Exp Brain Res 152:229–242. doi:10.1007/s00221-003-1548-8
Sakai K, Hikosaka O, Nakamura K (2004) Emergence of rhythm during motor learning. Trends
Cogn Sci. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.10.005
Shenoy K, Sahani M, Churchland M (2012) Cortical control of arm movements: a dynamical
systems perspective. Neuroscience 36:337–359. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150509
Shima K, Isoda M, Mushiake H, Tanji J (2007) Categorization of behavioural sequences in the
prefrontal cortex. Nature 445:315–318. doi:10.1038/nature05470
Shin JC, Ivry RB (2002) Concurrent learning of temporal and spatial sequences. J Exp Psychol
Learn Mem Cogn 28:445–457
Shin JC, Ivry RB (2003) Spatial and temporal sequence learning in patients with Parkinson’s disease
or cerebellar lesions. J Cogn Neurosci 15:1232–1243. doi:10.1162/089892903322598175
Siegel J, Mauk M (2013) Persistent activity in prefrontal cortex during trace eyelid conditioning:
dissociating responses that reflect cerebellar output from those that do not. J Neurosci
33:15272–15284. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1238-13.2013
Spencer RM, Ivry RB (2005) Comparison of patients with Parkinson’s disease or cerebellar lesions
in the production of periodic movements involving event-based or emergent timing. Brain
Cogn 58:84–93. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2004.09.010
Spencer RM, Zelaznik HN, Diedrichsen J, Ivry RB (2003) Disrupted timing of discontinuous but
not continuous movements by cerebellar lesions. Science 300:1437–1439. doi:10.1126/
science.1083661
Studenka BE, Zelaznik HN (2011) Circle drawing does not exhibit auditory-motor synchroniza-
tion. J Mot Behav 43:185–191. doi:10.1080/00222895.2011.555796
Tanji J, Shima K (1994) Role for supplementary motor area cells in planning several movements
ahead. Nature 371:413–416. doi:10.1038/371413a0
Teki S, Grube M, Griffiths T (2011) A unified model of time perception accounts for
duration-based and beat-based timing mechanisms. Front Integr Neurosci 5:90. doi:10.3389/
fnint.2011.00090
Ten Brinke MM, Boele H-JJ, Spanke JK et al (2015) Evolving models of pavlovian conditioning:
cerebellar cortical dynamics in awake behaving mice. Cell Rep 13:1977–1988. doi:10.1016/j.
celrep.2015.10.057
54 K. Kornysheva
Thomson AM, Bannister PA (2003) Interlaminar connections in the neocortex. Cereb Cortex
13:5–14. doi:10.1093/cercor/13.1.5
Timmann D, Watts S, Hore J (1999) Failure of cerebellar patients to time finger opening precisely
causes ball high-low inaccuracy in overarm throws
Ugawa Y, Day BL, Rothwell JC, Thompson PD, Merton PA, Marsden CD (1991) Modulation of
motor cortical excitability by electrical stimulation over the cerebellum in man. J Phys
441(1):57–72
Ullen F, Bengtsson S (2003) Independent processing of the temporal and ordinal structure of
movement sequences. J Neurophysiol 90:3725–3735. doi:10.1152/jn.00458.2003
Zeeuw C, Hoebeek F, Bosman L et al (2011) Spatiotemporal firing patterns in the cerebellum. Nat
Rev Neurosci 12:327–344. doi:10.1038/nrn3011
Zelaznik HN, Rosenbaum DA (2010) Timing processes are correlated when tasks share a salient
event. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 36:1565–1575. doi:10.1037/a0020380
Zelaznik HN, Spencer RM, Ivry RB et al (2005) Timing variability in circle drawing and tapping:
probing the relationship between event and emergent timing. J Mot Behav 37:395–403.
doi:10.3200/JMBR.37.5.395-403
Zhou H, Lin Z, Voges K et al (2014) Cerebellar modules operate at different frequencies.
Elife 3:e02536. doi:10.7554/eLife.02536
Predictability and Robustness
in the Manipulation of Dynamically
Complex Objects
Keywords Theory-based Cart-and-pendulum Energy margin Experimental
paradigm Nonlinear fluid dynamics Hapticmaster Chaos Cup-and-ball
Hand trajectory Dystonia Noise
Introduction
Everyday life is full of actions that involve interaction with objects. Grasping and
lifting a book involves manipulation of a free rigid object; turning a key in a
keyhole involves moving a rigid object against a kinematic constraint. Functional
interaction with objects—tool use—is ubiquitous in activities of daily living and the
basis for our evolutionary advantage. Tools extend and augment fundamental
human capabilities. Surprisingly, how humans interactively control objects or tools
is still little understood. Manipulation requires sensing the mechanics and the
geometry of the object and adjusting one’s movements and forces accordingly to
exploit object properties. Manipulation becomes particularly intriguing when the
objects have internal degrees of freedom that add complex dynamics to the inter-
action. An exotic example is cracking a whip, where the flexible whip creates
challenging dynamics (infinitely many degrees of freedom) that the hand has to
interact with (Bernstein et al. 1958; Goriely and McMillen 2002; Hogan and
Sternad 2012). A more mundane example is leading a cup of coffee to one’s mouth
to drink: the transporting hand applies a force not only to the cup, but also indirectly
to the liquid, which in turn acts back onto the hand. These continuous forces require
sensitive adjustments to avoid spilling the coffee (Hasson et al. 2012a; Mayer and
Krechetnikov 2012; Hasson and Sternad 2014; Sauret et al. 2015). Humans are
strikingly adept at interacting with a large variety of such objects, but most studies
on object manipulation have been confined to either multi-digit grasping of a static
object or grip forces needed for transporting solid objects (Flanagan et al. 1993;
Flanagan and Wing 1997; Santello et al. 1998; Gao et al. 2005; Fu and Santello
2014). This chapter will focus on physical interactions with complex objects that
are largely unchartered territory in motor neuroscience to date.
Over the last two decades motor neuroscience has made advances in under-
standing the control of simple movements, for example straight-line reaches in the
horizontal plane including adaptation to external force fields or visual perturbations.
This research has shed light on significant aspects of adaptation and control, such as
error correction mechanisms and internal models (Shadmehr and Mussa-Ivaldi
1994; Scheidt et al. 2001). This paradigm has continued the long tradition of motor
neuroscience examining elementary behaviors under strict experimental control.
Seminal paradigms range from single-joint wrist movements in primates (Evarts
1968), to the speed-accuracy paradigm (Fitts 1954), to today’s center-out reaching
task for human and primate studies (Kalaska 2009). While these paradigms render
manageable data for analysis and modeling, they are far removed from the richness
of everyday actions and interactions. Unfortunately it is difficult, if not impossible,
to extrapolate insights to more complex movements. For example, when extending
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 57
Hypothesis 1: Predictability
When interacting with complex objects, instantaneous action and reaction is critical.
Control models for artificial systems have posited internal models and inverse
dynamics control plus feedback control, as they are largely devoid of long feedback
delays and with relatively low levels of noise (Flanagan et al. 1999, 2003; Kawato
1999; Takahashi et al. 2001). In contrast, in humans feedback-based corrective control
is virtually irrelevant due to trans-cortical or trans-cerebellar loop delays on the order of
100 ms or more, which requires exact extrapolation from current state estimates
58 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
(Pruszynski et al. 2011). This is difficult as variability and noise in the human system is
high, with an approximate precision in timing of 9 ms (Faisal et al. 2008; Cohen and
Sternad 2012). Instead, intrinsic musculo-skeletal properties augmented by spinal
reflexes deliver essentially instantaneous reaction and can provide stabilization to
counteract noise or instability (Colgate and Hogan 1988; Burdet et al. 2001; Franklin
et al. 2003; Selen et al. 2009; Lee et al. 2014). While mechanical impedance is
essential, dexterous control in the presence of delays nevertheless requires one to
anticipate, preempt, and exploit the forces and motions of an object. Yet, prediction for
continuous nonlinear objects with chaotic, i.e. unpredictable, behavior is challenging or
impossible, even for artificial systems with short delays and low noise. Therefore, rather
than expending the neural resources to learn a complex dynamics model, we suggest an
alternative hypothesis: humans make the interactions with objects more predictable.
This can be achieved by simplifying the interactive dynamics via linearization or
avoidance of chaotic regimes.
Hypothesis 2: Robustness
To test the two hypotheses—humans select movement strategies that make inter-
actions with complex objects predictable and robust—an appropriate test bed is
needed. Transporting a cup of coffee is a good candidate as the cup filled with liquid
has complex dynamics and there are clear consequences for failure, i.e. spilled
coffee. However, transporting a cup with sloshing coffee is a complex problem in
fluid dynamics (Mayer and Krechetnikov 2012; Sauret et al. 2015). Hence, the task
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 59
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
x M
φESC
(e)
Box A Box B
d
plie
Ap
Applied
Force
Fig. 1 From the task to the experiment. a The actual task. b The conceptual model. c The
cart-and-pendulum model underlying the displayed cup and ball. The cup is the arc of the circular
pendulum path, the pendulum bob is the ball. d Virtual implementation with robot arm and visual and
haptic interface. e The display with start and end box targets. The schematic below visualizes the
applied force as arrows in accelerating and decelerating directions. Figure modified from Nasseroleslami
et al. (2014) with permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
was simplified to that of moving a cup with a ball rolling inside, representing the
complex dynamics of the coffee [Fig. 1a, b; (Hasson et al. 2012a)]. Implemented in
a virtual environment the cup was visualized as an arc in 2D and modeled as a point
mass moving along a horizontal axis. The ball’s motion was modeled by a sus-
pended pendulum; the arc of the cup corresponded to the ball’s semi-circular path
(Fig. 1c). This model system was implemented in a virtual environment, where
subjects exert forces on the virtual cup via a robotic manipulandum (Fig. 1d shows
the screen display and Fig. 1e (bottom panel) shows a movement of the cup and
60 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
ball with the applied forces shown at different time points). Importantly, movements
of the cup also accelerate the ball, which in turn acts back on the hand. Despite
these simplifications, the model system retained essential elements of complexity: it
is nonlinear and creates complex interaction forces between hand and object.
In this simplification, the equations of the cup-and-ball system are identical to the
well-known cart-and-pendulum problem (Hinrichsen and Pritchard 2005; Ogata 2010).
The cup is the cart with a point mass M that moves horizontally; the pendulum
comprises a point mass m (the ball) attached to a mass-less rod of length ‘ with one
angular degree of freedom h. Subjects control the ball indirectly by applying forces
to the cup, and the ball can “escape”, i.e. it can be lost from the cup when the
angular distance to the rim is exceeded. The hand moving the cup is represented by
an external applied horizontal force FA . The equations of the system dynamics are:
ðm þ M Þ‘x ¼ m‘ €h cos h þ h_ 2 sin h þ FA ð1Þ
_ and €
where h, h, h are the ball’s angular position, velocity, and acceleration; x, x_ and
‘x are the cart/cup’s position, velocity and acceleration; g is gravitational acceler-
ation; damping to pendulum and cart motion can also be added if desired.
To implement this cup-and-ball system in a virtual environment, the cart and the
pendulum rod were hidden, but the pendulum bob (the ball) remained visible
(Fig. 1e). Subjects manipulate the virtual cup-and-ball system via a robotic arm,
which also exerts forces from the virtual object onto the hand [HapticMaster, Motek
(van der Linde and Lammertse 2003)]. Using admittance control, the HapticMaster
has three controllable degrees of freedom, but was constrained to motion on a
horizontal line for the experiments. The pendulum’s h and h_ were computed using a
4th-order Runge-Kutta-integrator, and on the cup FBall was
the force of the ball
€ _
computed based on Eq. 1: FBall ¼ m‘ h cos h þ h sin h . This force, combined
2
with any forces exerted by a human FA , accelerated the virtual mass ðm þ M Þ. The
robot motors moved the manipulandum according to ‘x and the visual display was
updated. For more details see (Hasson et al. 2012a).
This formalization and its virtual implementation has several advantages. (1) The
focus is on the interaction forces between the hand and the object. Confining the
physical interaction to a single “interaction port” via the robot handle avoids the
complexity of grasp formation (Santello and Soechting 2000; Nowak and Hermsdörfer
2003). (2) Compared to real objects that have dozens of modes, this formalization
reduces the object to two modes that facilitate analytical treatment (Hasson et al.
2012b). (3) The virtual implementation enables versatile manipulation of task param-
eters, including linear and nonlinear aspects. (4) The task involves “skill” and requires
practice to arrive at smooth and stable execution. (5) The virtual implementation of the
task is equivalent to the dynamic model. Hence, the measured human kinematics and
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 61
kinetics lends itself to novel mathematical analyses to assess how humans sense and
exploit the object’s dynamic properties. In sum, the task has manageable but sufficient
richness with multiple routes to increment complexity.
Most studies involving object manipulation have used linear systems, such as
mass-springs (Dingwell et al. 2002; Svinin et al. 2006; Danion et al. 2012). By defi-
nition, such systems display predictable behavior. For example, if one were to oscillate
a linear mass spring with the goal of attaining a given oscillation, the execution
variables, the amplitude A and frequency f of the cup oscillation relate linearly to the
applied forces and the resulting motion of the system: If the system is sinusoidally
forced at 1 Hz, it will oscillate sinusoidally at 1 Hz. However, with a nonlinear
system, such as the cup-and-ball, this mapping becomes non-trivial: the same
forcing input may cause the system to oscillate at an array of frequencies with
unpredictable and chaotic behavior.
To illustrate this chaotic behavior in the cup-and-ball system, we applied inverse
dynamics to obtain the required force FA for a given oscillatory cup motion, specified
by the scalar execution variables cup amplitude A and cup frequency f , with initial
ball angle h0 and ball velocity h_ 0 . Shown in Fig. 2 are two simulated examples with
the same sinusoidal cup movement x. The only difference is in the initial angle of
the ball h0 , with h_ 0 set to zero. In one case ðh0 ¼ 1:0 radÞ, the force required to
produce this motion x is periodic and predictable. In the other case ðh0 ¼ 0:4 radÞ,
the force required to produce the same cup motion shows highly irregular fluctu-
ations. To characterize the pattern of force profiles with respect to the cup dynamics,
FA was strobed at every peak of the cup position x. The marginal distributions of
strobed force values are plotted as a function of ball angle h0 in the bottom panel
(Fig. 2). This input-output relation reveals bifurcations with a pattern similar to the
period-doubling behavior of chaotic systems. This feature has important implica-
tions for control: small changes in initial states can dramatically change the
long-term behavior and lead to unpredictable solutions.
Quantifying Predictability
Fig. 2 Simulated force profiles derived from inverse dynamics with specified cup and ball
trajectories. The profiles are applied force, cup position, and ball angle (from top to bottom). The
left panel was initiated with ball angle h0 = 0.4 rad; the right panel with h0 = 1.0 rad. The
bifurcation diagram below shows the marginal distributions of force values strobed at all peak cup
position (see dots in upper panels). The two initial ball angles are shown and marked as
predictable and unpredictable. The diagram combines 1000 simulations with different initial ball
angles h0 in the range between −π/2 to π/2 rad. The force distributions plotted as a function of ball
angle indicate chaotic dynamics. Figure modified from Nasseroleslami et al. (2014) with
permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
between the two. MI is calculated between FA and the phase of the ball u. This
phase was calculated in phase space, spanned by ball angle and velocity:
ZZ
pðu; FA Þ
MI ðu; FA Þ ¼ pðu; FA Þ loge dudFA ð2Þ
pðuÞpðFA Þ
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 63
where pð:Þ denotes a probability density function. MI can also be calculated for the
phase of the cup. MI presents a scalar measure of the performer’s strategy that can
be calculated for all amplitudes and frequencies of the cup and all initial conditions
of the ball. MI can be summarized for each point of the 4D space of execution
variables: A; f ; h0 ; and h_ 0 .
Experimental Insights
Our recent study provided evidence that subjects increase the predictability of
object dynamics with practice and favor predictable solutions over those that
minimize expended force and smoothness, criteria that are widely supported criteria
for free movements (Nasseroleslami et al. 2014). In this study, subjects (n = 8)
oscillated the virtual cup between two targets with a robotic manipulandum, paced
by a metronome at 1 Hz for 50 trials, each lasting 45 s. They were free to choose
their movement amplitude and relative phase between the ball and cup.
The cup and ball oscillations were analyzed to determine how choices of
movement amplitude and relative phase related to three result variables or costs:
predictability, exerted force, and movement smoothness (Fig. 3). Figure 3a shows
the result space for mutual information; lighter shading indicates that combinations
of cup amplitude and ball angle render higher mutual information (higher pre-
dictability). The large point indicates the strategy with the highest mutual infor-
mation. To compare potential alternative explanations, two other result measures, or
commonly used costs, were derived for the same model: minimum force and
maximum smoothness. The expended force was calculated by the square of the
Fig. 3 Result spaces that combine result variables or costs in the space spanned by the execution
variables initial ball angle, cup amplitude, frequency (fixed at 1 Hz), and initial ball velocity (set to
zero). a Mutual information. b Mean squared force (log transformed). c Mean squared jerk of the
ball motion (normalized for amplitude); the large point in each graph indicates the location of
maximum cost. Importantly, the minimum/maximum values are located in different parts of the
map, providing different predictions. Figure modified from Nasseroleslami et al. (2014) with
permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
64 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
mean integral over FA over the course of the trial, mean squared force MSF.
Figure 3B shows the resulting pattern of force for the same space of execution
variables; lighter shading refers to strategies requiring less force. The point high-
lights that the minimum force solution is obtained at the smallest allowable cup
amplitudes. Lastly, smoothness or jerk was evaluated of the cup trajectory for each
of the strategies defined in the execution space. Figure 3c shows smoothness of the
ball movements for each strategy, with lighter shades denoting higher smoothness.
The point shows that a strategy with high amplitude reaches maximum smoothness
or minimum jerk. Importantly, the three maxima lie in different locations of the
execution space. Therefore, by looking at which amplitude and relative phase
subjects choose, we can infer which of the three costs are most important for
subjects’ movement control.
Following these simulations, equivalent measures for the execution variables
A; f ; h0 ; and h_ 0 had be derived from experimental data. However, the experimental
trajectories were not fully determined by the initial values of ball states as variations
could be due to online corrective changes. Therefore, to estimate the execution
variables from the experimental trajectories, the initial values were extracted at each
cycle k (see Fig. 4). Peak excursions of the cup trajectory served as strobe points to
estimate A; f ; h0 ; h_ 0 and calculate trial averages A; f ; h0 ; h_ 0 that served as correlates
for the variables in the simulations. To exclude transients only the time window
after 25 s was considered for analysis. To evaluate Hypothesis 1, that subjects seek
predictable object interactions, MI, and the alternative costs mean squared force
MSF, and mean squared jerk MSJ were calculated for each measured strategy
k ; fk ;
A hk ; h_ k . Calculation of MI followed the same procedure as in the simulated MI,
except that probability density functions were estimated experimentally (for more
details see (Nasseroleslami et al. 2014). To calculate MSF, the continuous force
profile of each trial was squared and averaged, analogous to the simulated data. MSJ
was calculated according to the standard equations (Hogan and Sternad 2009).
The main experimental results are summarized in Fig. 5; the mutual information
plot is overlaid with contours of selected simulated force values (green). The figure
shows how subjects gravitated towards areas with higher MI, i.e. strategies with
more predictable interactions. In the left panel, each point represents the average
strategy for each 45 s trials for all subjects; darker red indicates early practice and
lighter red indicates late practice. The right panel shows the same data separated by
subject: the red arrows mark how each subject’s average strategy changed from
early practice (mean of first 5 trials) to late practice (mean of last 5 trials). Both
figures clearly show that all subjects increased their movement amplitude, associ-
ated with an increase in overall exerted force. The majority of subjects switched
from low- to high-predictability regions in the result space. None of the subjects
moved toward the minimum force strategy, nor towards a strategy with maximum
smoothness. Analysis of MSF and MSJ over trials shows that indeed exerted force
increased and smoothness decreased with practice, counter to findings in free
unconstrained movements. Overall, the results rejected the two alternative criteria
and were consistent with Hypothesis 1.
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 65
Simulation Experiment
A k+1
T Ak
A
θ0 θ k θ k+1
θ (rr a d)
θ (ra d)
. . .
θ (ra d)
θ (ra d)
θ0 θk θ k+1
.
.
Fig. 4 Exemplary profiles from inverse dynamics simulations and corresponding experimental
data for applied force, cup position, ball angle and velocity. Estimates for the execution variables
in the data were derived for each cycle as shown and then averaged across the trial to obtain scalar
estimates for each trial. Figure modified from Nasseroleslami et al. (2014) with permission under
Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
Robust Interactions
The reviewed results suggest that when there is a choice, humans select a strategy
that increases the predictability of the human-object interaction. More predictable
human-object interactions may lessen the control burden; however, errors in control
undoubtedly exist, especially when only rough approximations of internal models
of object dynamics are available. Thus, keeping interactions predictable may not be
enough—a good strategy should also be robust to control errors. The cup-and-ball
task lends itself to experimental investigation of robustness, as there is a
well-defined threshold for failure, i.e. the ball escapes the cup—coffee is spilled.
Note that in the previous experiment, the ball could not escape, but swung around
following the circular path of the pendulum in situations of varying difficulty. By
66 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
Fig. 5 Main results in the result space for mutual information. The green contours denote
different values of mean squared force superimposed onto the same space. a Data pooled from all
subjects; each point is one trial. Darker red pertains to earlier trials than lighter red. The data show
that in the course of practice, subjects shifted their movement strategies to the area of high mutual
information (high predictability). b Averaged data from eight subjects; each arrow represents one
subject, the tail of the arrow is the mean of the first 5 trials, the tip of the arrow is the mean of the
last 5 trials. Notice that none of the subjects shifted down towards the area of minimum force,
indicated by the green point. Figure modified from Nasseroleslami et al. (2014) with permission
under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
introducing a “rim” and also using a shallower cup, we could probe the use of
fragile and robust strategies.
We hypothesized that as a subject learns to manipulate the object, s/he should
find strategies that are more robust to failure. In a risky strategy, the ball gets close
to the rim of the cup and any small error may lead to loss of the ball. Therefore, a
safety margin is critical and might present a sensitive measure distinguishing
“fragile” from robust control. We hypothesized that this safety margin should
increase with practice (Hypothesis 2). Further, we expected that the size of the
safety margin depends on the performance variability. Individuals have different
degrees of variability and those with more variable movements should seek larger
safety margins (Hypothesis 2a). Further, if variability decreases with practice, then
the safety margin should change accordingly (Hypothesis 2b). We will now review
two studies that addressed these questions in young and also older healthy adults.
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 67
Quantifying Robustness
To test these hypotheses, the safety margin needed to be defined. Safety margins
have been most frequently characterized in gait and posture and are typically
quantified by the degree of spatial and/or temporal difference between the body
center of mass/center of pressure and the base of support boundary (Hof et al. 2005;
Hasson et al. 2008). While useful, such measures can be difficult to generalize,
because they are specific to upright stance and can depend on the physical attributes
of the individual. Therefore, we developed a more general formulation, defined in
terms of energy, i.e. an energy margin.
Most objects that we may interact with are initially at rest, and when we pick
them up or handle them, we impart energy to them. For example, we push on a
shopping cart to start moving it or pick up a cup of coffee to drink. If too much
energy is imparted to such objects, an undesirable outcome may occur, such as
overturning the shopping cart or spilling the coffee. We define the energy margin
EM by the difference between the current energy to the energy level that causes
failure [see (Hasson et al. 2012a) for more details].
Specifically for the cup-and-ball system, EM quantifies how close the total
energy of the ball TEBALL is to the energy level that would cause the ball to exceed
the rim, i.e. the escape energy EESC
where KEBALL is the kinetic energy of the ball, PEBALL is the potential energy of
the ball, and PSEBALL is a pseudo-energy because the ball is in an accelerated
reference frame relative to the cup. EESC is defined as
EESC ¼ mg‘ð1 coshESC Þ m‘x‘ sinhESC þ m0 x‘ ð5Þ
In these equations, there are only three time-varying quantities, the ball angle h,
_ and the cup acceleration ‘x. These variables are mea-
the ball angular velocity h,
sured and defined as the execution variables, which jointly determine the result
variable EM. Essentially, EM takes the instantaneous state of the cup and ball,
which includes inputs from the human hand, and extrapolates to determine whether
68 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
the ball will escape, given the current value for ‘x: At the very next instant in time, a
new determination is made based on updated execution values of h, h, _ and ‘x, and
so on for future time points. This analysis approach follows the same logic as for
the rhythmic task described above: identify the execution variables that fully
determinate the result variable. However, instead of mapping to a predictability
measure, MI (alternative measures or MSF and MSJ), the execution variables are
mapped into the energy margin EM. This same analysis strategy was previously
applied to other tasks such as throwing and bouncing a ball (Dijkstra et al. 2004;
Cohen and Sternad 2009; Sternad et al. 2014).
For any movement of the cup and ball, the energy margin fluctuates over the
time of the movement, as shown in Fig. 6a for an exemplar point-to-point trans-
lation of the cup and ball. The normalization of EM to EESC affords an assessment
of the risk at any instant during an ongoing movement. When EM > 0 and the
margin is large, any unexpected disturbance can easily be dealt with or “absorbed”.
However, when EM ≤ 0, the ball will escape in a finite “time-to-escape” (red dotted
lines in Fig. 6a), unless action is taken to increase the EM before the ball reaches
the rim. The exemplary profile shows fluctuations that are concurrent, but not
coincident with the ball excursions, as the applied force is also important. The same
trial can also be plotted as a trajectory in 3D space spanned by the three execution
_ and ‘x. (Figure 6b). The result variable is EM. The critical energy
variables h, h,
EESC defines a closed two-dimensional manifold two oblique cones joined together;
Fig. 6 Exemplary profile of energy margin of one trial during early practice. a The energy margin
EM as a function of time. With the initial high EM, the ball is at rest and is unlikely to escape from
the cup, even when exposed to a disturbance. However, when the EM drops below zero the ball is
in a state where it will escape from the cup in a finite time (shown as the red dotted
“Time-to-Escape” lines). b For the same trial, the three variables that determine EM, ball angle and
angular velocity and cup acceleration, are shown in a three dimensional execution space. The trial
starts in the center (yellow triangle) and moves through the space as the trial progresses until the
cup is stopped at the spatial target (yellow square). The blue mesh represents the escape energy
threshold, EM = 0; whenever the trajectory breaches the manifold there is danger of ball-escape
unless a corrective action is taken. Figure modified from Hasson and Sternad (2014) with
permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 69
see blue mesh. If the trajectory stays within this manifold, EM > 0, the
time-to-escape is infinite, and the ball is never in danger of escaping. If the manifold
is breached, EM < 0 and the ball may escape. If the subject applies a corrective
action to change ‘x in an appropriate way, then failure may be prevented. However,
the available time to make such a correction is finite. If the correction takes too
long, the ball will be lost. Note that the time-to-escape is computed at each instant
in time, assuming constant ‘x, but is then updated at the next instant in time when a
new set of execution variables (h, h, _ and ‘x) is available.
Experimental Insights
A prior study sought to test the hypothesis that humans seek robust movement
strategies with appropriate safety margins (Hasson et al. 2012a). Subjects were
asked to make a discrete point-to-point translation of the cup, and to complete the
movement in a target time of 2 s without losing the ball from the cup. This com-
pletion time was comfortable and afforded selection among several strategies. For
comparison, a separate group of subjects performed a minimum-time movement,
translating the cup as fast as possible over the same distance. Both groups improved
their performance, i.e. the timing error and movement time decreased for the
target-time and minimum-time groups, respectively. As hypothesized, subjects in
the target-time condition increased their energy margin over practice (Fig. 7a). In
contrast, the energy margin decreased in the minimum-time task (Fig. 7b).
Accordingly, the minimum-time group lost the ball about 10 times as often as the
target-time group at the end of practice. These changes in the energy margin typ-
ically occurred throughout the entire movement profile, as highlighted by the
shading in Fig. 7a, b, although some portions of the movement tended to show
larger changes than others. These findings suggest that when urged to move as fast
as possible, subjects “live dangerously” and use small energy margins. However,
when multiple movement options are available humans prefer those that are more
robust to errors in control. This result supported Hypothesis 2a.
For a different view on how the energy margin changed with practice, a number
of trials from one representative subject are shown in execution space in Fig. 7c.
The blue mesh again represents the EESC manifold; two perspectives on the same
data are shown for clarity. Early in practice, the movement trajectories are variable
and frequently break through the EESC manifold by a significant amount, often
leading to loss of the ball. This happened mostly near the end of the movement
when subjects tried to stop the cup (seen as high cup deceleration). However, after
practicing the task, a clear structure becomes visible and the trajectories conform to
the EESC manifold. This “contraction” of the trajectories raises the energy margin,
increasing robustness. As long as the trajectory is within the EESC manifold there is
no chance of the ball escaping from the cup. This could be advantageous, as minor
70 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
Fig. 7 Changes in the energy margin EM with practice. a Difference in the energy margins
between an early and a late trial in four subjects of the target-time group. b Difference in the
energy margins between an early and a late trial in four subjects of the minimum-time
group. c Examples of early and late practice trials for one subject. Trajectories are plotted in
execution space, defined by ball angle and velocity and cup acceleration. Two different views of
the three-dimensional execution space are shown. Trials in which the ball escaped are shown in
red. Note that not all trials that exit the manifold result in failure. Figure modified from Hasson and
Sternad (2014) with permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
errors in control would not cause failure, which could free up cognitive resources
for higher–level movement planning operations.
Motivated by the robustness hypothesis, we also predicted that the size of the
safety margin should depend on subjects’ motor variability (Hypothesis 2a). This
follows previous work suggesting that variability plays a central role in movement
control such that the motor system optimizes movements to minimize the effects of
variability on task goals (Harris and Wolpert 1998; Trommershäuser et al. 2005;
Gepshtein et al. 2007; Cohen and Sternad 2009; Hudson et al. 2010; Sternad et al.
2011; Chu et al. 2013). Specifically, individuals with greater trial-to-trial variability
should choose a larger energy margin, and vice versa. To test this hypothesis, the
degree of correlation between the energy margin and trial-to-trial variability was
assessed for both the target-time and minimum-time tasks. Consistent with
Hypothesis 2a, results showed a positive correlation, i.e. subjects with high vari-
ability at the end of practice also had large safety margins at the end of practice
(Fig. 8a). There was no correlation for the target-time task. This could be ascribed
to the individual variations in strategies in the target-time group, while subjects in
the minimum-time group displayed more similar strategies. When examining
potential correlations across practice within each individual, there was a significant
correlation for the target-time group. Consistent with Hypothesis 2b, subjects with
large decreases in variability also changed their strategies to smaller energy mar-
gins, and vice versa (Fig. 8b). Those subjects who developed a consistent move-
ment pattern may have been more confident in their ability, and therefore did not
need large energy margins. Conversely, subjects with greater trial-to-trial variability
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 71
Fig. 8 Changes in energy margin EM as a function of trial-to-trial variability and task condition.
a Correlations between EM and trial-to-trial variability of the total ball-and-cup system energy
TESTD over the last 30 trials for the Target-Time group (black triangles) and the Minimum-Time
group (green circles). b Correlations between the change in EM and variability from early (first 30
trials) to late (last 30 trials) practice within each subject. Figure redrawn from Hasson et al. (2012a,
2012b)
Fig. 9 Cup and ball kinematics in early and late practice for young (blue) and older adults (red).
Individual subjects are shown as thin lines and the group means are the thicker lines. Variability
decreases in both groups, but no other evident differences are discernible. Figure modified from
Hasson and Sternad (2014) with permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY) license
the kinematic profiles, i.e. the position and velocity of the cup and ball, only minor
differences between the two age groups were discernable (Fig. 9). It was only the
energy margins that revealed the differences between the two groups: early in
practice, the older adults performed with a significantly lower energy margin
(Fig. 10a), supporting Hypothesis 3a. Nonetheless, the older adults were able to
increase their energy margin with practice, although not to the level of the younger
adults (Fig. 10b). This implies that as older adults learned to interact with the
cup-and-ball dynamics, they were able to increase the robustness of their movement
strategies and, consequently, lost the ball less frequently (Fig. 10c), supporting
Hypothesis 3b. However, while the energy margins continued to increase in the
younger adults, it plateaued in the older adults. This suggests that sensorimotor
limitations in older adults limit their ability to keep the cup and ball in a regimen
with high safety margins.
How do humans successfully manipulate tools in daily life, an ability that has a long
evolutionary history? Manipulation of complex dynamic objects presents daunting
challenges, although more for the scientist than the human actor. Extrapolating our
current understanding of human control of free movements to those involving
object manipulation may not be an incremental process. For example, feedback
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 73
Fig. 10 Changes in the energy margin EM and number of ball escapes with practice for young
and older adults. a The energy margin as a function of normalized movement time in early and late
practice: individual subjects are shown as thin lines and the group means are the thicker lines. Note
that in early practice the older subjects had a lower energy margin for most of the movement.
b Group average EM across four blocks of practice. Both young and older adult increase their
energy margins, but older adults have significantly smaller energy margins. c Group average
percentage of trials in which the ball was dropped across four practice blocks. Older adults show
visibly more failures than young subjects, but they also improve with practice. Figure modified
from Hasson and Sternad (2014) with permission under Creative Commons Attribution (CC BY)
license
control based on internal models of the object dynamics appears problematic given
the long delays and high levels of noise in the human neuromotor system. Void of
knowing the control architecture, we adopted a task-based approach. We analyzed
the task and derived the solution space with no assumptions about the human actor
and control. Starting with a physical model of the object dynamics and the task, we
first identified execution and result variables. Mapping execution to selected result
variables rendered a space of solutions. Based on this understanding of the physics,
we could formulate quantitative hypotheses about potential strategies and objective
functions that humans might use. Implementing the task in an interactive virtual
74 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
Unlike the body’s own limbs, interactions with objects in the external world can be
quite unpredictable. This is particularly true for objects with complex dynamics that
cannot be directly controlled, such as a cup of coffee or a jostling baby carriage.
Using the cup of coffee as a model task, we reviewed studies showing that with
practice humans learn to control such objects by making the interaction both pre-
dictable and robust. These criteria are important for all individuals, however they
may be of special importance to individuals with disabilities, where unpredictable
and fragile interactions with the world incur marked effects on the quality of life.
For these populations, it would be beneficial to develop interventions that promote
predictability and robustness and thereby complement traditional movement criteria
from free unconstrained movements such as movement smoothness and economy.
The current ecological task may be a first step in this direction.
References
Begg RK, Sparrow WA (2000) Gait characteristics of young and older individuals negotiating a
raised surface: implications for the prevention of falls. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci 55:
M147–M154
Bernstein B, Hall DA, Trent HM (1958) On the dynamics of a bull whip. J Acoust Soc Am
30:1112–1115
Burdet E, Osu R, Franklin D, Milner T, Kawato M (2001) The central nervous system stabilizes
unstable dynamics by learning optimal impedance. Nature 414:446–449
Charles SK, Hogan N (2011) Dynamics of wrist rotation. J Biomech 44:614–621
Chu WTV, Sternad D, Sanger TD (2013) Healthy and dystonic children are sensitive to their motor
variability. J Neurophysiol 109:2169–2178
Chu WTV, Park S, Sanger TD, Sternad D (2016) Dystonic children can learn a novel motor skill:
strategies that are tolerant to high variability. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 8:847–858
Cluff R, Riley MA, Balasubramanian R (2009) Dynamical structure of hand trajectories during
pole balancing. Neurosci Lett 464:88–92
Cohen R, Sternad D (2009) Variability in motor learning: relocating, channeling and reducing
noise. Exp Brain Res 193:69–83
Cohen RG, Sternad D (2012) State space analysis of intrinsic timing: exploiting task redundancy
to reduce sensitivity to timing. J Neurophysiol 107:618–627
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 75
Colgate J, Hogan N (1988) Robust control of dynamically interacting systems. Int J Control
48:65–88
Cover TM, Thomas JA (2006) Elements of information theory. Wiley, Hoboken
Danion F, Diamond J, Flanagan J (2012) The role of haptic feedback when manipulating nonrigid
objects. J Neurophysiol 107:433–441
Dijkstra TM, Katsumata H, De Rugy A, Sternad D (2004) The dialogue between data and model:
passive stability and relaxation behavior in a ball bouncing task. Nonlinear Stud 11:319–344
Dingwell JB, Mah CD, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2002) Manipulating objects with internal degrees of
freedom: evidence for model-based control. J Neurophysiol 88:222–235
Dingwell J, Mah C, Mussa-Ivaldi F (2004) Experimentally confirmed mathematical model for
human control of a non-rigid object. J Neurophysiol 91:1158–1170
Evarts E (1968) Relation of pyramidal tract activity to force exerted during voluntary movement.
J Neurophysiol 31:14–27
Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:
292–303
Fitts PM (1954) The information capacity of the human motor system in controlling the amplitude
of movement. J Exp Psychol 47:381–391
Flanagan J, Wing A (1997) The role of internal models in motion planning and control: evidence
from grip force adjustments during movements of hand-held loads. J Neurosci 17:1519–1528
Flanagan J, Tresilian J, Wing A (1993) Coupling of grip force and load force during arm
movements with grasped objects. Neurosci Lett 152:53–56
Flanagan J, Nakano E, Imamizu H, Osu R, Yoshioka T, Kawato M (1999) Composition and
decomposition of internal models in motor learning under altered kinematic and dynamic
environments. J Neurosci 34:1–5
Flanagan J, Vetter P, Johansson R, Wolpert D (2003) Prediction precedes control in motor
learning. Curr Biol 13:146–150
Foo P, Kelso J, de Guzman G (2000) Functional stabilization of unstable fixed points: human pole
balancing using time-to-balance information. J Exp Psychol Human Percept Perform 26:
1281–1297
Franklin D, Osu R, Burdet E, Kawato M, Miner T (2003) Adaptation to stable and unstable
dynamics achieved by combined impedance control and inverse dynamics model.
J Neurophysiol 90:3270–3282
Fu Q, Santello M (2014) Coordination between digit forces and positions: interactions between
anticipatory and feedback control. J Neurophysiol 111:1519–1528
Gao F, Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2005) Internal forces during object manipulation. Exp Brain
Res 165:69–83
Gawthrop P, Lee K-Y, Halaki M, O’Dwyer N (2013) Human stick balancing: an intermittent
control explanation. Biol Cybern 107:637–652
Gepshtein S, Seydell A, Trommershäuser J (2007) Optimality of human movement under natural
variations of visual–motor uncertainty. J Vision 7, 13. doi:10.1167/7.5.13
Goriely A, McMillen T (2002) Shape of a cracking whip. Phys Rev Lett 88:244301
Hadjiosif AM, Smith MA (2015) Flexible control of safety margins for action based on
environmental variability. J Neurosci 35:9106–9121
Hamel KA, Okita N, Higginson JS, Cavanagh PR (2005) Foot clearance during stair descent:
effects of age and illumination. Gait Posture 21:135–140
Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature
394:780–784
Hasson C, Sternad D (2014) Safety margins in older adults increase with improved control of a
dynamic object. Front Aging Neurosci 6, 158. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2014.00158
Hasson CJ, Van Emmerik RE, Caldwell GE (2008) Predicting dynamic postural instability using
center of mass time-to-contact information. J Biomech 41:2121–2129
Hasson C, Shen T, Sternad D (2012a) Energy margins in dynamic object manipulation.
J Neurophysiol 108:1349–1365
76 D. Sternad and C.J. Hasson
Hasson CJ, Hogan N, Sternad D (2012b) Human control of dynamically complex objects. In: 4th
IEEE RAS & EMBS International Conference on Biomedical Robotics and Biomechatronics
(BioRob). IEEE, pp 1235–1240
Hinrichsen D, Pritchard A (2005) Mathematical systems theory: modelling, state space analysis,
stability and robustness. Springer, New York
Hof A, Gazendam M, Sinke W (2005) The condition for dynamic stability. J Biomech 38:1–8
Hogan N (1985) Impedance control: An approach to manipulation: Part II—implementation. J Dyn
Syst 107:8–16
Hogan N, Sternad D (2009) Sensitivity of smoothness measures to movement duration, amplitude,
and arrests. J Motor Behav 41:529–534
Hogan N, Sternad D (2012) Dynamic primitives of motor behavior. Biol Cybern 106:727–739
Huang FC, Gillespie RB, Kuo AD (2002) Haptic feedback and human performance in a dynamic
task. In: 10th symposium on haptic interfaces for virtual environmental and teleoperating
systems (Haptics ’02). IEEE, Orlando, pp 24–31
Huang FC, Gillespie RB, Kuo AD (2007) Visual and haptic feedback contribute to tuning and
online control during object manipulation. J Motor Behav 39:179–193
Hudson TE, Tassinari H, Landy MS (2010) Compensation for changing motor uncertainty. PLoS
Comput Biol 6:e1000982
Insperger T, Milton J, Stepan G (2013) Acceleration feedback improves balancing against reflex
delay. J R Soc Interface 10(79), 20120763
Kalaska J (2009) From intentions to actions: motor cortex and the control of reaching movements.
In: Sternad D (ed) Progress in Motor Control V: a multidisciplinary approach. Springer,
New York
Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol
9:718–727
Lee H, Krebs H, Hogan N (2014) Multivariable dynamic ankle mechanical impedance with active
muscles. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 22:971–981
Leib R, Karniel A (2012) Minimum acceleration with constraints of center of mass: a unified
model for arm movements and object manipulation. J Neurophysiol 108:1646–1655
Loeb GE (2012) Optimal isn’t good enough. Biol Cybern 106:757–765
Mayer HC, Krechetnikov R (2012) Walking with coffee: why does it spill? Phys Rev E 85:046117
McFadyen BJ, Prince F (2002) Avoidance and accommodation of surface height changes by
healthy, community-dwelling, young, and elderly men. J Gerontol Ser A Biol Sci Med Sci 57:
B166–B174
Mehta B, Schaal S (2002) Forward models in visuomotor control. J Neurophysiol 88:942–953
Milton J (2011) The delayed and noisy nervous system: implications for neural control. J Neural
Eng 8:065005
Milton J, Fuerte A, Bélair C, Lippai J, Kamimura A et al (2013) Delayed pursuit-escape as a model
for virtual stick balancing. Nonlinear Theor Appl 4:129–137
Nagengast A, Braun D, Wolpert D (2009) Optimal control predicts human performance on objects
with internal degrees of freedom. PLoS Comput Biol 5:e1000419
Nasseroleslami B, Hasson C, Sternad D (2014) Rhythmic manipulation of objects with complex
dynamics: predictability over chaos. PLoS Comput Biol 10:e1003900
Nowak D, Hermsdörfer J (2003) Selective deficits of grip force control during object manipulation
in patients with reduced sensibility of the grasping digits. Neurosci Res 47:65–72
Ogata K (2010) Modern control engineering. Prentice Hall, New York
Pruszynski JA, Kurtzer I, Scott SH (2011) The long-latency reflex is composed of at least two
functionally independent processes. J Neurophysiol 106:449–459
Santello M, Soechting JF (2000) Force synergies for multi-fingered grasping. Exp Brain Res
133:457–467
Santello M, Flanders M, Soechting J (1998) Postural hand synergies for tool use. J Neurosci
18:10105–10115
Sauret A, Boulogne F, Cappello J, Dressaire E, Stone H (2015) Damping of liquid sloshing by
foams. Phys Fluids 27:022103
Predictability and Robustness in the Manipulation … 77
Mark L. Latash
Abstract The equilibrium-point hypothesis and its more recent version, the ref-
erent configuration hypothesis, represent the physical approach to the neural control
of action. This hypothesis can be naturally combined with the idea of hierarchical
control of movements and of synergic organization of the abundant systems
involved in all actions. Any action starts with defining trajectories of a few referent
coordinates for a handful of salient task-specific variables. Further, referent coor-
dinates at hierarchically lower levels emerge down to thresholds of the tonic stretch
reflex for the participating muscles. Stability of performance with respect to salient
variables is reflected in the structure of inter-trial variance and phenomena of motor
equivalence. Three lines of recent research within this framework are reviewed.
First, synergic adjustments of the referent coordinate and apparent stiffness have
been demonstrated during finger force production supporting the main idea of
control with referent coordinates. Second, the notion of unintentional voluntary
movements has been introduced reflecting unintentional drifts in referent coordi-
nates. Two types of unintentional movements have been observed with different
characteristic times. Third, this framework has been applied to studies of impaired
movements in neurological patients. Overall, the physical approach searching for
laws of nature underlying biological movement has been highly stimulating and
productive.
Keywords Equilibrium-point hypothesis Motor equivalence Variance Tonic
stretch reflex Control theory Neural computation Hierarchical control
Synergy Selective stability Back-coupling Equifinality Referent coordinates
The idea that biological systems should be studied with the same scientific method
as all other material objects in the Universe is rather old. The great physicist, Erwin
Schrödinger wrote over 70 years ago a seminal book “What is life? The physical
aspect of the living cell” (Schrödinger 1948). About 15 years later, a rather unique
Department named Physics of Living Systems was created in the Moscow Institute
of Physics and Technology (MIPT, known also as Fiztekh). This was a very small
program with fewer than 10 students graduating each year. It did, however, produce
a visible group of researchers in the field of motor control including Sergei
Adamovich, Alexander Frolov, Yuri Ivanenko, Yuri Levik, Konstantin Popov, and
a few others including the author of this Chapter, and Anatol Feldman—the creator
of the equilibrium-point (EP) hypothesis.
From its inception, the physical approach to biological systems was mostly
limited to problems at the molecular and cellular levels. Applications of this
approach to whole organisms, up to humans, were all but nonexistent, with the field
of movement studies dominated by descriptions of behavior and a few formal
models that had no explicit links to laws of nature. One notable exception was
biomechanics, with examples of successful application of the apparatus of classical
mechanics to animal movements. However, even those examples applied existing,
well-known laws of physics (mechanics) to biological objects without trying to
discover new laws that would be specific to biological motion (cf. Gelfand 1991;
Gelfand and Latash 1998). It has been obvious to any external observer that motion
of biological objects is special. For example, knowing the mechanical parameters,
initial conditions, and external forces is sufficient to predict motion of an inanimate
object. Biological objects, however, are much less predictable; in particular, they
frequently move uphill, fly against the wind, and swim against the current.
At the time of its creation, the name of the mentioned Fiztekh Department was
largely a promise that at some time in future physics of living systems would
become reality. In the subjective opinion of the author, it became reality in 1965
with the publication of the first in the series of three papers (Asatryan and Feldman
1965; Feldman 1966a, b) that introduced the basics of the EP-hypothesis. The
approach accepted in those studies amounted to searching for parameters (invari-
ants) of unknown laws that linked physical variables such as muscle length and
force. Based on the accumulated experimental foundation, Feldman suggested that
muscle force and length were linked by the mechanism of the tonic stretch reflex
with only one parameter that could be modified by the central nervous system,
namely threshold (λ) of the tonic stretch reflex.
Currently, two approaches compete in the field of motor control, physical (based
on laws of natural science) and computational (based on engineering and control
theory). The former approach tries to discover laws of nature and their salient
parameters that dictate the production of natural movements. The latter approach
assumes that the brain is a computational device that solves the complex problems
of interactions among the numerous elements within the body and between the
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 83
body and the environment (e.g., in the form of internal models, Wolpert et al. 1998;
Kawato 1999; Shadmehr and Wise 2005). The postulate of neural computation is
rejected by the physical approach as incompatible with natural science. On the other
hand, the importance of the computational approach for such fields as robotics may
be viewed as well established.
Within the physical approach, one may define a living system as a system able to:
(1) unite basic physical laws, those common across animate and inanimate object,
into chains and clusters leading to new stable and pervasive relations among
physical variables—new physical laws—involving new parameters; and (2) modify
these parameters in a purposeful way. In other words, inanimate systems are slaves
of the basic physical laws, while living systems create new physical laws and then
modify parameters of those new laws to achieve their goals. Within this definition,
the EP-hypothesis was revolutionary in its formulation of a new law of physics
specific for a living muscle with its reflex connections and identification of a salient
parameter for that law, λ.
It took 50 years for the EP-hypothesis to become recognized as one of the (or
arguably the only) hypothesis in the field of motor control that makes it a subfield of
physics of living systems. Over this time, it had to deal with numerous criticisms. It
was at some point fashionable to criticize or even disprove the EP-hypothesis
(Gottlieb 1998; Popescu and Rymer 2000; Hinder and Milner 2003; for review see
Feldman and Latash 2005; Feldman 2015). All these criticisms were based on
misinterpretations of some of the basic postulates of the EP-hypothesis and/or
accepting its simplified (and hence incorrect) versions, making predictions, and then
showing them to be false. The fashion to disprove the EP-hypothesis has fortunately
passed. Now trying to prove or disprove this hypothesis may be safely viewed as
waste of time. The true task is developing the hypothesis and there are plenty of
actively explored directions of such developments including generalization of the
EP-hypothesis to natural multi-muscle movements, linking it to perception,
applying it to disordered movements, and, possibly most importantly, linking it to
neurophysiological mechanisms.
Generalization of the EP-hypothesis to the control of natural multi-muscle
movements has led to the emergence and development of the idea of control with
referent coordinates (RC) sometimes addressed as the RC-hypothesis (reviewed in
Feldman 2015). This is a quickly developing field, and the following brief and
simplified description may be viewed as a snapshot of the current view on the
control of natural movements.
Any goal-directed action starts with the formulation of a goal, which may be
viewed as a relatively low-dimensional set of salient coordinates (Task in Fig. 1).
For example, a pointing movement to a target involving numerous muscles may be
associated with setting only three parameters (RCs) for the pointing effector
84 M.L. Latash
(e.g., the tip of the index finger) corresponding to the three coordinates of the target
in space. Implementation of the action requires setting RCs for individual joints and
muscles leading to a series of few-to-many transformations (the upper level and
lower level transformations only are shown in Fig. 1). Such transformations have
been commonly addressed as redundant (Prablanc et al. 2003; Karniel 2011), while
recently, a different term has been suggested—abundant (Latash 2012)—reflecting
a particular attitude to the problem of motor redundancy (Bernstein 1967).
According to the principle of motor abundance, apparently redundant transforma-
tions are not the sources of computational problems for the CNS (solved, for
example, with optimization approaches, Prilutsky and Zatsiorsky 2002) but useful,
rich designs that allow ensuring stability of salient performance variables in the
presence of unpredictable changes in both intrinsic body states and external forces,
and necessity to perform secondary tasks using the same sets of elements (reviewed
in Latash et al. 2007; Latash 2008).
This chapter addresses the recent progress in some of the areas within this
general scheme of control. First, I will review the links of the general scheme of
control with RCs to such notions as hierarchical control, theory of synergies, and
Fig. 1 A multi-level scheme for the production of a natural movement with several few-to-many
mapping organized in a synergic way. Action starts with defining a low-dimensional set of referent
coordinates for task-specific salient variables, RCTASK. It leads to sets of λs for the involved
muscles compatible with the RCTASK. At each step, the feedback may be organized similarly to the
central back-coupling scheme in Latash et al. (2005)
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 85
uncontrolled manifold (UCM) hypothesis. Then, a few recent results are reviewed
related to such issues as synergies within spaces of hypothetical control variables
(such as r- and c-commands within the EP-hypothesis, Feldman 1980, 1986), the
nature of unintentional movements and drifts in force observed when a person stops
receiving visual information on an ongoing action (e.g., Slifkin et al. 2000;
Vaillancourt and Russell 2002; Shapkova et al. 2008; Ambike et al. 2015a), and
attempts at translating the recent progress into clinical studies (reviewed in Latash
et al. 2010; Latash and Huang 2015).
The hierarchical scheme in Fig. 1 is readily compatible with the idea of synergies as
neural organizations that lead to stabilization of salient performance variables by
covaried adjustment within an abundant set of elemental variables, i.e., the outputs
of elements (reviewed in Latash et al. 2002, 2007). This is not the only meaning of
the word synergy in movement science. At least two more meanings are used
broadly in the literature. Among clinicians, synergy frequently has a negative
connotation; it implies stereotypical patterns of muscle activations (forces and joint
rotations) interfering with purposeful movements that are commonly seen in
patients after stroke (DeWald et al. 1995). In motor control literature, synergy
frequently means something like “a set of variables showing parallel changes”
(d’Avella et al. 2003; Ivanenko et al. 2004; Ting and Macpherson 2005). Uniting
elemental variables into such synergies has been viewed as a means of alleviating
the problem of motor redundancy by decreasing the number of variables manipu-
lated by the CNS.
One of the crucial features of natural movements, which allow them to be
efficient in the changing environment, is their stability. This term has been used in
the motor control literature in different meanings. Here, under stability, I imply
ability of a time-varying (dynamic) system to return to a state or trajectory fol-
lowing a small, transient perturbation. Gregor Schöner (1995) introduced the
important notion of task-specific stability, which is a characteristic of biological
systems but not of inanimate objects. This notion implies that a multi-element
system is able to reorganize interactions among elements leading to stability of
some performance variables but not others, depending on the task and intention of
the actor. This notion is applicable to abundant systems only.
The introduced definition of stability suggests that there is only one method to
assess stability directly, that is, to apply a small, transient perturbation and quantify
behavior of the system. While this method seems straightforward, its application to
the human motor system is far from trivial because of the poorly controlled phasic
reflexes and reflex-like reactions to perturbations (Hammond 1955; Tatton et al.
1978; reviewed in Shemmell et al. 2010). Two indirect methods have been
developed. The first is based on the idea that, over a set of repetitive trials, a
dynamic system deviates from average trajectory more in directions of low stability
86 M.L. Latash
and less in deviations of high stability. As a result, computing variance across trials
at comparable phases is expected to show lower values in relatively stable direc-
tions. If a particular performance variable is selectively stabilized, variance within a
subspace that leads to no change in that variable (its uncontrolled manifold, UCM,
Scholz and Schöner 1999) is expected to be relatively large as compared to variance
in the orthogonal to the UCM directions (ORT). The inequality VUCM > VORT has
been used as a signature of a synergy stabilizing that performance variable,
sometimes reduced to a single metric, an index of synergy ΔV = (VUCM − VORT)/
VTOTAL, where all variance indices are computed per dimension in the corre-
sponding spaces.
Another indirect method of assessing stability in different directions of a
multi-element system is based on the notion of quick corrective actions as brief
perturbations into the system. If a person is asked to perform a quick corrective
action, much of the motion is expected to be in directions of lower stability. For
example, imagine that you hold a small metal spring from a typical pen between the
thumb and the index finger. If you now try to squeeze the spring quickly, very
likely it will buckle and jump away, i.e., move not in the desired direction but in an
orthogonal direction (cf. Venkadesan et al. 2007; Lawrence et al. 2014). This is due
to the fact that the spring is relatively stable along its main axis and is relatively
unstable in orthogonal directions. If the CNS stabilizes a performance variable
selectively, an attempt to change this variable quickly is expected to lead to large
deviations in directions of low stability, i.e., within the UCM for that variable. Such
deviations have been addressed as motor equivalent (ME). A number of studies
have documented very large amounts of ME action during quick corrections of
actions despite the fact that ME action by definition is inefficient in correcting a
deviation of the performance variable (Mattos et al. 2011, 2013, 2015). Note that
both ΔV and ME indices are proxies of stability.
Mechanisms of selective stability (mechanisms of synergies) are basically
unknown. A number of schemes have been suggested including both computational
(such as optimal feedback control, Todorov and Jordan 2002) and neurophysio-
logical (such as central back-coupling, Latash et al. 2005) schemes. A scheme
uniting the ideas of back-coupling and of control with RCs has been suggested
(Martin et al. 2009). Indeed, irrespective of specific realization, the idea of synergic
control is naturally compatible with hierarchical control with RCs: If an action is
initiated with specifying a low-dimensional task-specific set of RCs at a hierar-
chically high level (with some, relatively low, inter-trial variance), trajectories of
corresponding performance variables are expected to be stabilized against intrinsic
noise (e.g., associated with deviations of RCs at lower levels, see Fig. 1) and
changes in external forces. The latter are due to the length- and velocity-dependence
of muscle force (which is universal, not task-specific) and may get contributions
from other, learned, feedback mechanisms.
While action stability is vital, too much stability may be detrimental, for
example, if a person tries to produce a very quick change in a performance variable
on the background of a steady-state action. Recent studies have shown that humans
possess a feed-forward mechanism that allows changing stability of salient
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 87
Since the original publication (Scholz and Schöner 1999), the UCM hypothesis and
the associated method of analysis of the structure of variance have been used
actively to explore synergies in spaces of elemental variables such as joint rotations,
digit forces, and muscle groups (M-modes). The formulation of the problems in
those studies has always been deficient: They searched for synergies in spaces of
elemental performance variables, not control variables. Merging the ideas of control
with RCs with the UCM-based analysis was elusive until recently. This was partly
due to the fact that measuring RCs (e.g., λs for individual muscles) is by itself a
challenging task. Attempts at reconstructing time profiles of RCs required multiple
trials and were built on simplified mechanical models of the moving effectors
(Latash and Gottlieb 1991; Latash 1992; Ambike et al. 2015b), which were criti-
cized as inadequate (Gribble et al. 1998). Recently, however, a method has been
developed that promises to overcome this limitation (Ambike et al. 2016b).
Consider the control of a joint spanned by two muscles acting in opposite
directions, an agonist–antagonist pair (Fig. 2a). The neural control of each muscle
may be described with a single parameter, λ, threshold of the tonic stretch reflex.
For a given λ, there is a dependence of active muscle torque on joint angle (shown
as curved lines in Fig. 2). Note that λ defines neither muscle force, nor its length,
nor its activation level: All three change along the muscle characteristic. Joint
behavior will be defined by its torque-angle characteristic, which is the algebraic
sum of the two muscle characteristics. Actual joint torque and position in equi-
librium will be defined by interactions of the joint with the external load. The pair
of control variables {λAG, λANT} may be substituted with an equivalent pair {r; c}
(Feldman 1980). The r-command (reciprocal command) corresponds to unidirec-
tional shifts in the two λs; such shifts favor activation of one muscle within the pair
compared to the other muscle. The c-command (coactivation command) leads to
counter-directional changes in the λs leading to parallel changes, an increase or a
decrease, in the activation of both muscles.
The {r; c} pair of control variables can be generalized for any effector. For
example, the neural control of action by a fingertip along one dimension can be
described with two variables, R and C, which are analogous to the r- and c-
commands but represent the action of all the muscles that contribute to the fingertip
action (Fig. 2b). One of the commands, R, defines a referent coordinate for the
88 M.L. Latash
Fig. 2 a The torque-angle characteristics for two muscles, agonist and antagonist, acting at a
joint. Their control can be described with two λs, λAG and λANT or with two equivalent commands,
r-command and c-command; b The control of a fingertip can be described with two equivalent
commands, RCFT (referent coordinate) and kC (apparent stiffness)
fingertip, i.e., a spatial coordinate, at which it produces zero net force on the
environment. If the fingertip is kept away from its RC (for example, by a stop), it
would produce a net force on the stop, and the force magnitude (FFT), in a linear
approximation, would depend on the difference between the fingertip actual coor-
dinate (ACFT) and RCFT with a scaling coefficient kC, apparent stiffness of the
fingertip: FFT = kC(RCFT − ACFT). The coefficient kC reflects the other basic
command, C-command.
Figure 3a illustrates the task of accurate force production. When a person is
asked to produce a particular force level by pressing on a stop with the fingertip, an
infinite number of {RCFT; kC} combinations can be used to produce the required
force (panel A in Fig. 3). So, the apparently nonredundant task of fingertip force
production is in fact redundant (abundant!) at the control level. There is a hyper-
bolic line corresponding to a required force level (Fig. 3b). This line is the UCM for
this task in the space of elemental control variables or, to be more exact, in the
space of mechanical variables that may be viewed as direct reflections of the
hypothetical neural variables.
Now one can formulate a question: Do humans use the abundance afforded by
the presence of two commands, R and C, during single-finger accurate force pro-
duction tasks? Are there {R; C} synergies stabilizing the fingertip force reflected in
the {RCFT; kC} synergies? This is not a trivial question. Indeed, deviations from the
prescribed force level depend only on deviations from the UCM in Fig. 3 and not
on deviations along the UCM. So, one can perform the task very accurately, i.e.,
with small deviations from the UCM with equal deviations along the UCM, larger
deviations, or smaller deviations. This is illustrated in the right panel of Fig. 3,
which shows three data distributions with about the same dispersion orthogonal to
the UCM (about the same accuracy of performance). They are associated with
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 89
Fig. 3 a The solution space (UCM) for the task of accurate fingertip force production on the
{RCFT; kC} plane; b A close-up showing possible data distributions compatible with a particular
level of accuracy in the task. Note that deviations from the UCM are about the same for the three
data distributions, while deviations along the UCM may be smaller (crosses), about the same (open
circles), or larger (filled circles) that deviations from the UCM. Distributions similar to the one
illustrated with filled circles were observed in experiments
smaller (filled circles), about equal (crosses), and much larger (open circles) dis-
persion along the UCM.
So far, only one experimental study explored {RCFT; kC} synergies during
fingertip force production (Ambike et al. 2016b). In that study, the subjects pressed
on a force sensor and produced fingertip force to match a visual target. Then, the
“inverse piano” (Martin et al. 2011) device was used to lift the finger smoothly over
a small amplitude (1 cm at 2 cm/s), while the subject was instructed not to interfere
with possible fingertip force changes. Visual feedback was turned off to help the
subject not to correct the force changes that emerged naturally when the finger was
lifted. Finger force increased during the lift in proportion to the change in the
fingertip coordinate. Note that, according to the idea of control with RCs, in the
initial state, a combination of RCFT and kC led to the actual force generated by the
fingertip and measured by the force sensor. Assuming that the subject did not
change the neural commands, the finger lift led to a change in the difference
between the RCFT and ACFT and, as a result, to a proportional change in the force.
Using linear regression between the force and coordinate allows reconstructing
RCFT and kC as the intercept and the slope of the regression line. Of course, the
method is based on several assumptions including the linear relation between the
fingertip force and coordinate (supported by the observed linear regressions with
R > 0.9) and the questionable extension of the regression line beyond the range of
observations.
Experiments have shown that subjects indeed use relatively broad ranges of both
RCFT and kC, and the two are nearly perfectly covarying to keep force deviations
from the desired value low (as in the distribution with filled circles in Fig. 3b). The
90 M.L. Latash
cutaneous receptors in the hand (Augurelle et al. 2003) and when the handle
grasped by the subject expanded and contracted very slowly (Ambike et al. 2014).
The drop in force means that there is a drift in RC for the fingertip and/or a drift in
the apparent stiffness kC. This force drop also illustrates the limitations of the
somatosensory system in providing feedback on kinetic variables such as fingertip
force.
The instruction “continue doing what you have been doing” implies that the
subject should not change the intentional input into the hierarchically lower
structures involved in the task performance. Removing visual feedback is another
method to avoid corrective changes in this descending input. Spontaneous drift in
performance under such conditions is a reflection of natural behavior of the physical
system (including both neural and muscular components) that participates in the
performance. The aforementioned unintentional force drift suggests that this
physical system drifts toward a state with minimum of its potential energy (as all
physical systems do).
More examples of unintentional changes in RCs leading to violations of equi-
finality were observed in experiments with external transient perturbations applied
on the background of static tasks, such as holding a hand position against a force
vector (Zhou et al. 2014, 2015a, b) and producing a constant force by the four
fingers of a hand (Wilhelm et al. 2013; Reschechtko et al. 2014, 2015). These
examples form two groups. First, a transient change in the external force applied
during the positional task leads to relative equifinality in the final hand position and
orientation, but equifinality is violated at the level of joint configurations (Zhou
et al. 2014). In other words, if the external force increases and then decreases
smoothly, the hand comes to a new position, which is more or less the same as the
initial hand position. In contrast, the joint configuration shows large variance,
which is compatible with the unchanged hand position and orientation. In the
multi-finger tasks, lifting and lower a finger (a transient positional perturbation)
during the steady-state phase of the task leads to relatively low variance in the total
force while sharing of the force among the four fingers shows large variability.
Taken together, these studies show that application of external transient per-
turbations leads to relative equifinality at the task level but not at the level of
elemental variables. This is not a completely unexpected result given the existence
of synergies (in a sense VUCM > VORT) stabilizing the task-specific variables, such
as hand position and orientation in the kinematic task and total finger force in the
kinetic task (cf. Wilhelm et al. 2013; Zhou et al. 2014). Such synergies reflect lower
stability of the involved elemental variables in directions leading to no change in
the task-specific performance variables, i.e., along the corresponding UCM.
Naturally, a perturbation is expected to lead to larger deviations of the elemental
variables along the directions of low stability; this is true for both segments of a
transient perturbation, its application, and removal. Hence, these results confirm
directly (i.e., with perturbations) one of the main postulates of the UCM-based
method, i.e., that the inequality VUCM > VORT indeed reflects different stability of
the system along the UCM and ORT subspaces.
92 M.L. Latash
Fig. 4 Schematic illustration of the main results in an experiment with transient perturbations
interrupted by a brief dwell time. In the initial state, the subject produces active force against an
external load (F1) at a certain coordinate (X1). An increase in the load to F2 leads the hand to a new
coordinate (X2) where the system is again at equilibrium. After a few seconds, the load returns to
F1, the hand moves toward the initial coordinate but stops short of X1 (X3). Note the different
slopes of the trajectories during the application and removal of the force perturbation
Another example of violations of equifinality was observed at the task level. Such
violations take place if a transient perturbation is interrupted by a dwell time interval
between the perturbation application and removal (Zhou et al. 2014, 2015a, b). This
process is much faster than the slow force drift observed without perturbations, with
a time constant of about 1 s. Such effects were observed both in multi-joint posi-
tional tasks with the application of force perturbations and in multi-finger pressing
tasks under the application of positional perturbations with the help of “inverse
piano.” These processes show that a perturbation triggers a drift of the RC at the task
level toward the new actual coordinate of the effector. The drift is never complete
such that, when the perturbation is removed, the effector moves back to its initial
state but with an undershoot that may cover up to 50 % of the displacement (force
change) of the effector caused by the perturbation (Fig. 4).
Within the EP (RC) hypothesis, there are two types of movements, voluntary and
involuntary. The former are associated with shifts of the RC at the task level (λ at
the single-muscle level); their mechanical effects and muscle activation changes
depend on both the RC shift and the external force field. For example, the same
shift of λ can lead to a movement in isotonic condition, to force generation in
isometric conditions, and to both in intermediate loading conditions. Involuntary
movements are associated with changes in the external load without a change in
RC. Typical examples include joint motion in response to unloading as in the
classical experiments of Feldman (1966a).
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 93
Fig. 5 Within the EP (RC) hypothesis, changes in RC lead to voluntary movements (and/or active
force production). If external forces (load) change while RC stays unchanged, movements are also
observed (involuntary movements). Recent studies have suggested the existence of unintentional
voluntary movements, i.e. those associated with a change in RC, which happens without subject’s
intention. There are two types of such movements, slow (with characteristic times of ≈ 10–20 s)
and fast (with characteristic times of ≈ 1 s)
i.e., toward higher forces (Ambike et al. 2015a). When a subject is asked to perform
not a steady-state task but a cyclical task, turning visual feedback off reveals both
drifts: A slow drift in the midpoint of the force cycle to lower values and a fast drift
of the amplitude of the cyclical force changes towards higher values (Ambike et al.
2016a). When a perturbation moves the system not away from its RC but towards
the RC, no visible drift is seen (Qiao et al. 2015). Taken together, these observa-
tions suggest that unintentional movement characteristics are defined by a number
of factors that may include, in particular, properties of the somatosensory system,
preferred (maybe optimal in some sense) magnitude of performance, and reactions
of the muscle to lengthening and shortening perturbations (cf. Archambault et al.
2005). This is an exciting field for future experimental and theoretical studies.
The EP (RC) hypothesis offers a fruitful and consistent framework based on natural
science to analyze movement disorders in a variety of neurological patients. One of
the most challenging and important tasks of researchers is to define terms used by
clinicians in a way that allows incorporating them into a general scheme of the
neural control of movement. For example, such commonly used terms as increased
or decreased muscle tone, impaired joint coordination, impaired postural or
movement stability, and others should be defined explicitly and operationally; the
last word means that methods of objective quantitative assessment of the corre-
sponding phenomena have to be developed.
One of the commonly used notions in clinical movement studies is muscle tone.
A detailed discussion of this notion has been published recently (Latash and
Zatsiorsky 2016). Here I would only like to mention the definition proposed by
Bernstein and Kots (1963) who emphasized the importance of reflexes for muscle
tone and linked the notion of muscle tone to tuning the neuromotor apparatus to
tasks of active postural or movement control. In contrast, consider how muscle tone
is measured during clinical examinations (the person is asked to relax and the
effector is moved over its range of motion) or tools that are claimed to measure
muscle tone (applying deformation orthogonal to the surface of the body and
measuring the resistive force and deformation). None of these methods addresses
the Bernstein understanding of muscle tone as a contributor to active movement and
postural tasks, since the subject is always asked to relax and do nothing.
Consider Fig. 6, which illustrates the behavior of a muscle during its passive
stretch. If in the original state the muscle is relaxed at a particular length L0, its
threshold of the tonic stretch reflex value (λ) is undefined. We only know that
λ > L0. For λ1 ≈ L0, any motion stretching the muscle would trigger its activation
and relatively strong resistance. For λ3, which is beyond the anatomical range of
muscle length, motion over the whole range would lead to only weak resistance due
to properties of passive tissues. For an intermediate λ2, the resistance would be
weak until muscle length reaches λ2 and then will increase substantially. Should one
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 95
Fig. 6 A scheme of the relation between muscle active force and length. If the muscle is relaxed,
its tonic stretch reflex threshold (λ) is longer than its actual length (L0). Passive stretching the
muscle to L1 may lead to its different resistance depending on the distance between λ and L0 F1 for
λ1, F2 for λ2, and F = 0 for λ3). Muscle tone may be associated with the distance between L0 and λ.
Passive length-dependent resistance of the tissues is ignored
call the case with λ1 hypertonic, the case with λ3 hypotonic, and the case with λ2—
normal tone? This does not seem to make sense because all three subjects in this
experiment were healthy and simply interpreted the instruction “to relax” differ-
ently. Does it make sense to associate the notion of muscle tone with the distance
from actual muscle length to λ when the subject is instructed to relax? This question
is open to research and discussion among clinicians.
The framework of the EP-hypothesis has been effectively used in the analysis of
spasticity, a common motor disorder following spinal cord injury, stroke, multiple
sclerosis, and other neurological impairments. Traditionally, spasticity has been
associated with so-called positive signs (increased muscle tone and uncontrolled
contractions) and negative signs (weakness up to complete paralysis) (Landau
1974). A number of more recent studies have suggested, however, that the positive
and negative signs interact with each other. For example, effective suppression of
uncontrolled contractions, e.g., with intrathecal baclofen, is associated with
unmasking of better voluntary movements (Corcos et al. 1986; Latash et al. 1990;
Latash and Penn 1996).
These and other observations led to a scheme that considers spasticity as
impairment in the control of control of RCs within the whole natural range (Levin
and Feldman 1994; Musampa et al. 2007; Calota and Levin 2009). At the muscle
level, this means impaired control of shifts of λs of affected muscles. A healthy
person is able to change λ beyond the anatomical range of muscle length values,
{LMIN; LMAX}. This allows producing high forces at short muscle length values
(λ < LMIN) and relaxing the muscle when it is stretched to its maximal length
(λ > LMAX) (Fig. 7). Patients with spasticity are assumed to have a limited range of
λ changes, {L−; L+}. This limits the voluntary control of the muscle to the narrow
range of length values; at shorter muscle length (L < L−), the muscle is paralyzed,
while at longer muscle length (L > L+), the muscle shows involuntary contraction.
This scheme leads to multiple questions regarding, for example, changes in the
96 M.L. Latash
Fig. 7 An illustration of a spastic muscle. The subject lost his/her ability to move the tonic stretch
reflex threshold (λ) over its full range. If the range of λ is limited by L− and L+ it is impossible to
activate voluntarily the muscle when its length is shorter than L−. It also becomes impossible to
relax the muscle when its length is longer than L+. The biomechanical range of muscle length
changes in shown with vertical dashed lines, LMIN to LMAX
Another aspect of movement disorders that has recently been studied actively
within the physical approach is disorders of movement stability (reviewed in Latash
and Huang 2015). Note that stability of natural actions is crucial for their success
given the changing unpredictable environment and intrinsic states of the body. As a
result, loss of stability of motor patterns leads to major impairments in the motor
function. Well-known examples include ataxia in patients with cerebellar disorders
and loss of postural stability in Parkinson’s disease (reviewed in Fahn and Jankovic
2007).
The framework of the UCM hypothesis has been used to quantify task-specific
stability in a number of neurological disorders across a variety of motor tasks, from
multi-finger accurate pressing and manipulation (Park et al. 2012, 2013) to
multi-joint reaching (Reisman and Scholz 2003) and multi-muscle whole-body
actions (Falaki et al. 2016). Overall, these studies revealed strikingly different
changes in stability of motor patterns in patients with cortical and subcortical
disorders.
Patients with subcortical disorders including those with Parkinson’s disease
(Park et al. 2012; Jo et al. 2015), multi-system atrophy (Park et al. 2013), and
multiple sclerosis affecting subcortical loops (Jo et al. 2016a) showed significantly
decreased indices of motor synergies during steady-state phases and significantly
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 97
Fig. 9 Two groups of neural variables are specified at the hierarchically higher control level, NV1
and NV2. A change in NV1 leads to changes in the referent coordinates at the task level (RCTASK)
for salient performance variables, while a change in NV2 leads to changes in stability of those
variables (transformations from RCTASK to RCs at lower hierarchical levels, RCLOW). Clinical
studies suggest that both NV1 and NV2 are generated with a crucial role played by cortical
structures. An impairment in NV1 leads to overall impaired performance including weakness and
low accuracy as seen in averaged across trials patterns. An impairment in NV2 leads to decreased
and delayed ASAs
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 99
The contrasting results in different patient groups suggest that both NV1 and
NV2 are generated with a crucial role played by cortical structures. An impairment
in NV1 leads to overall impaired performance including weakness and low accu-
racy as seen in averaged across trials patterns. An impairment in NV2 leads to
decreased and delayed ASAs. In contrast, the circuitry involved in the few-to-many
transformations seems to depend crucially on subcortical loops. This interpretation,
while obviously simplistic, maps well on the idea of distributed processing modules
based on subcortical loops involving the cerebellum and the basal ganglia (Houk
2005).
The EP (RC) hypothesis is unique in its longevity and influence. Fifty years ago it
signified the first step toward turning motor control into a subfield of physics of
living systems. Over the recent years, the EP-hypothesis has been developing
rapidly. In particular, the main idea of control with referent coordinates merged
with the ideas of hierarchical control, synergic control, and naturally incorporated
the apparatus of the uncontrolled manifold hypothesis. This approach has been
productive not only in the field of the neural control of movement but also with
important implications for kinesthetic perception, clinical studies, and effects of
practice (reviewed in Feldman 2015). From the subjective optimistic view of the
author, we are moving toward turning motor control into a subfield of natural
science, physics of living systems.
Acknowledgments The author would like to thank his younger colleagues who played central
roles in the reviewed studies, Satyajit Ambike, Ali Falaki, Hang Jin Jo, Daniela Mattos, Florent
Paclet, Jaebum Park, Mu Qiao, Sasha Reschechtko, Stanislaw Solnik, Luke Wilhelm, and Tao
Zhou. Preparation of this paper was in part supported by a grant R01 NS035032 from the National
Institutes of Health, USA.
References
Ambike S, Paclet F, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2014) Factors affecting grip force: anatomy,
mechanics, and referent configurations. Exp Brain Res 232:1219–1231
Ambike S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015a) Processes underlying unintentional finger force
changes in the absence of visual feedback. Exp Brain Res 233:711–721
Ambike S, Zhou T, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015b) Moving a hand-held object:
reconstruction of referent coordinate and apparent stiffness trajectories. Neurosci 298:336–356
Ambike S, Mattos D, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2016a) The nature of constant and cyclic force
production: unintentional force-drift characteristics. Exp Brain Res 234:197–208
Ambike S, Mattos D, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2016b) Synergies in the space of control
variables within the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Neurosci 315:150–161
100 M.L. Latash
Archambault PS, Mihaltchev P, Levin MF, Feldman AG (2005) Basic elements of arm postural
control analyzed by unloading. Exp Brain Res 164:225–241
Asatryan DG, Feldman AG (1965) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of
movements or maintenance of a steady posture. I. Mechanographic analysis of the work of the
limb on execution of a postural task. Biophysics 10:925–935
Augurelle AS, Smith AM, Lejeune T, Thonnard JL (2003) Importance of cutaneous feedback in
maintaining a secure grip during manipulation of hand-held objects. J Neurophysiol
89:665–671
Bernstein NA (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, Oxford
Bernstein NA, Kots YM (1963) Tone. Grand Medical Encyclopaedia, vol 32. State Encyclopaedia,
Moscow, pp 418–422
Bizzi E, Polit A, Morasso P (1976) Mechanisms underlying achievement of final head position.
J Neurophysiol 39:435–444
Calota A, Levin MF (2009) Tonic stretch reflex threshold as a measure of spasticity: implications
for clinical practice. Top Stroke Rehab 16:177–188
Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL, Penn RD, Myklebust B, Agarwal GC (1986) Movement deficits caused
by hyperexcitable stretch reflexes in spastic humans. Brain 109:1043–1058
d’Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (2003) Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a
natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6:300–308
DeWald JP, Pope PS, Given JD, Buchanan TS, Rymer WZ (1995) Abnormal muscle coactivation
patterns during isometric torque generation at the elbow and shoulder in hemiparetic subjects.
Brain 118:495–510
DiZio P, Lackner JR (1995) Motor adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of reaching
movements: endpoint but not trajectory adaptation transfers to the nonexposed arm.
J Neurophysiol 74:1787–1792.
Fahn S, Jankovic J (2007) Principles and practice of movement disorders. Churchill Livingstone
Elsevier, Philadelphia, PA, USA
Falaki A, Huang X, Lewis MM, Latash ML (2016) Impaired synergic control of posture in
Parkinson’s patients without postural instability. Gait Posture 44:209–215
Feldman AG (1966a) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or
maintenance of a steady posture. II Controllable parameters of the muscle. Biophysics
11:565–578
Feldman AG (1966b) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement or
maintenance of a steady posture. III. Mechanographic analysis of execution by man of the
simplest motor task. Biophysics 11:667–675
Feldman AG (1980) Superposition of motor programs. I Rhythmic forearm movements in man.
Neurosci 5:81–90
Feldman AG (1986) Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (λ-model) for motor control.
J Mot Behav 18:17–54
Feldman AG (2015) Referent control of action and perception: challenging conventional theories
in behavioral science. Springer, NY
Feldman AG, Latash ML (2005) Testing hypotheses and the advancement of science: recent
attempts to falsify the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Exp Brain Res 161:91–103
Gelfand IM (1991) Two archetypes in the psychology of man. Nonlinear Sci Today 1:11–16
Gelfand IM, Latash ML (1998) On the problem of adequate language in movement science. Mot
Control 2:306–313
Gottlieb GL (1998) Rejecting the equilibrium-point hypothesis. Mot Control 2:10–12
Gribble PL, Ostry DJ, Sanguineti V, Laboissiere R (1998) Are complex control signals required
for human arm movements? J Neurophysiol 79:1409–1424
Hammond PH (1955) Involuntary activity in biceps following the sudden application of velocity to
the abducted forearm. J Physiol 127:23P–25P
Hinder MR, Milner TE (2003) The case for an internal dynamics model versus equilibrium point
control in human movement. J Physiol 549:953–963
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 101
Hoehn MM, Yahr MD (1967) Parkinsonism: onset, progression and mortality. Neurology
17:427–442
Houk JC (2005) Agents of the mind. Biol Cybern 92:427–437
Ivanenko YP, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F (2004) Five basic muscle activation patterns account for
muscle activity during human locomotion. J Physiol 556:267–282
Jo HJ, Park J, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2015) Prehension synergies and hand function in
early-stage Parkinson’s disease. Exp Brain Res 233:425–440
Jo HJ, Mattos D, Lukassen EB, Huang X, Latash ML (2016a) Changes in multi-digit synergies and
their feed-forward adjustments in multiple sclerosis. J Mot Behav (in press) e-pub: PMID:
27715488
Jo HJ, Maenza C, Good DC, Huang X, Park J, Sainburg RL, Latash ML (2016b) Effects of
unilateral stroke on multi-finger synergies and their feed-forward adjustments. Neurosci
319:194–205
Karniel A (2011) Open questions in computational motor control. J Integr Neurosci 10:385–411
Kawato M (1999) Internal models for motor control and trajectory planning. Curr Opin Neurobiol
9:718–727
Kelso JA, Holt KG (1980) Exploring a vibratory systems analysis of human movement
production. J Neurophysiol 43:1183–1196
Lackner JR, DiZio P (1994) Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory.
J Neurophysiol 72:1–15
Landau WM (1974) Spasticity: the fable of a neurological demon and the emperor’s new therapy.
Arch Neurol 31:217–219
Latash ML (1992) Virtual trajectories, joint stiffness, and changes in natural frequency during
single-joint oscillatory movements. Neurosci 49:209–220
Latash ML (2008) Synergy. Oxford University Press, New York
Latash ML (2012) The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp Brain
Res 217:1–5
Latash ML, Gottlieb GL (1990) Compliant characteristics of single joints: preservation of
equifinality with phasic reactions. Biol Cybern 62:331–336
Latash ML, Gottlieb GL (1991) Reconstruction of elbow joint compliant characteristics during fast
and slow voluntary movements. Neurosci 43:697–712
Latash ML, Huang X (2015) Neural control of movement stability: lessons from studies of
neurological patients. Neurosci 301:39–48
Latash ML, Levin MF, Scholz JP, Schöner G (2010) Motor control theories and their applications.
Medicina 46:382–392
Latash ML, Penn RD (1996) Changes in voluntary motor control induced by intrathecal baclofen.
Physiother Res Intern 1:229–246
Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schöner G (2002) Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of
motor variability. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30:26–31
Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schöner G (2007) Toward a new theory of motor synergies. Mot Control
11:276–308
Latash ML, Penn RD, Corcos DM, Gottlieb GL (1990) Effects of intrathecal baclofen on voluntary
motor control in spastic paresis. J Neurosurg 72:388–392
Latash ML, Shim JK, Smilga AV, Zatsiorsky V (2005) A central back-coupling hypothesis on the
organization of motor synergies: a physical metaphor and a neural model. Biol Cybern 92:
186–191
Latash ML, Zatsiorsky VM (2016) Biomechanics and motor control: defining central concepts.
Academic Press, NY
Lawrence EL, Fassola I, Werner I, Leclercq C, Valero-Cuevas FJ. (2014) Quantification of
dexterity as the dynamical regulation of instabilities: comparisons across gender, age, and
disease. Front Neurol 5:53
Levin MF, Feldman AG (1994) The role of stretch reflex threshold regulation in normal and
impaired motor control. Brain Res 657:23–30
102 M.L. Latash
Martin V, Scholz JP, Schöner G (2009) Redundancy, self-motion, and motor control. Neural
Comput 21:1371–1414
Martin JR, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2011) Multi-finger interaction during involuntary and
voluntary single finger force changes. Exp Brain Res 208:423–435
Mattos D, Latash ML, Park E, Kuhl J, Scholz JP (2011) Unpredictable elbow joint perturbation
during reaching results in multijoint motor equivalence. J Neurophysiol 106:1424–1436
Mattos D, Kuhl J, Scholz JP, Latash ML (2013) Motor equivalence (ME) during reaching: Is ME
observable at the muscle level? Mot Control 17:145–175
Mattos D, Schöner G, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015) Task-specific stability of abundant
systems: structure of variance and motor equivalence. Neurosci 310:600–615
Müller H, Sternad D (2003) A randomization method for the calculation of covariation in multiple
nonlinear relations: illustrated with the example of goal-directed movements. Biol Cybern
89:22–33
Musampa NK, Mathieu PA, Levin MF (2007) Relationship between stretch reflex thresholds and
voluntary arm muscle activation in patients with spasticity. Exp Brain Res 181:579–593
Olafsdottir H, Yoshida N, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2005) Anticipatory covariation of finger
forces during self-paced and reaction time force production. Neurosci Lett 381:92–96
Park J, Wu Y-H, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2012) Changes in multi-finger interaction and
coordination in Parkinson’s disease. J Neurophysiol 108:915–924
Park J, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2013) Effects of olivo-ponto-cerebellar atrophy (OPCA)
on finger interaction and coordination. Clin Neurophysiol 124:991–998
Park J, Lewis MM, Huang X, Latash ML (2014) Dopaminergic modulation of motor coordination
in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinson Rel Disord 20:64–68
Popescu FC, Rymer WZ (2000) End points of planar reaching movements are disrupted by small
force pulses: an evaluation of the hypothesis of equifinality. J Neurophysiol 84:2670–2679
Prablanc C, Desmurget M, Gréa H (2003) Neural control of on-line guidance of hand reaching
movements. Prog Brain Res 142:155–170
Prilutsky BI, Zatsiorsky VM (2002) Optimization-based models of muscle coordination. Exer
Sport Sci Rev 30:32–38
Qiao M, Zhou T, Latash ML (2015) Positional errors introduced by transient perturbations applied
to a multi-joint limb. Neurosci Lett 595:104–107
Raptis H, Burtet L, Forget R, Feldman AG (2010) Control of wrist position and muscle relaxation
by shifting spatial frames of reference for motoneuronal recruitment: possible involvement of
corticospinal pathways. J Physiol 588:1551–1570
Reisman D, Scholz JP (2003) Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in
persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain 126:2510–2527
Reschechtko S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2014) Stability of multi-finger action in different
spaces. J Neurophysiol 112:3209–3218
Reschechtko S, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015) Task-specific stability of multi-finger
steady-state action. J Mot Behav 47:365–377
Sangani SG, Raptis HA, Feldman AG (2011) Subthreshold corticospinal control of anticipatory
actions in humans. Behav Brain Res 224:145–154
Schmidt RA, McGown C (1980) Terminal accuracy of unexpected loaded rapid movements:
evidence for a mass-spring mechanism in programming. J Mot Behav 12:149–161
Scholz JP, Schöner G (1999) The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for
a functional task. Exp Brain Res 126:289–306
Schöner G (1995) Recent developments and problems in human movement science and their
conceptual implications. Ecol Psychol 8:291–314
Schrödinger E (1948) What is life? The physical aspect of the living cell. Cambridge University
Press, Cambridge
Shadmehr R, Wise SP (2005) The computational neurobiology of reaching and pointing. MIT
Press, Cambridge, MA
Fifty Years of Physics of Living Systems 103
Shapkova EYu, Shapkova AL, Goodman SR, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2008) Do synergies
decrease force variability? A study of single-finger and multi-finger force production. Exp
Brain Res 188:411–425
Shemmell J, Krutky MA, Perreault EJ (2010) Stretch sensitive reflexes as an adaptive mechanism
for maintaining limb stability. Clin Neurophysiol 121:1680–1689
Shim JK, Olafsdottir H, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2005) The emergence and disappearance of
multi-digit synergies during force production tasks. Exp Brain Res 164:260–270
Slifkin AB, Vaillancourt DE, Newell KM (2000) Intermittency in the control of continuous force
production. J Neurophysiol 84:1708–1718
Tatton WG, Bawa P, Bruce IC, Lee RG (1978) Long loop reflexes in monkeys: an interpretive
base for human reflexes. Prog Clin Neurophysiol 4:229–245
Ting LH, Macpherson JM (2005) A limited set of muscle synergies for force control during a
postural task. J Neurophysiol 93:609–613
Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat
Neurosci 5:1226–1235
Vaillancourt DE, Russell DM (2002) Temporal capacity of short-term visuomotor memory in
continuous force production. Exp Brain Res 145:275–285
Venkadesan M, Guckenheimer J, Valero-Cuevas FJ (2007) Manipulating the edge of instability.
J Biomech 40(8):1653–1661
Wilhelm L, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2013) Equifinality and its violations in a redundant
system: multi-finger accurate force production. J Neurophysiol 110:1965–1973
Wolpert DM, Miall RC, Kawato M (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends Cogn Sci
2:338–347
Zhou T, Wu Y-H, Bartsch A, Cuadra C, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2013) Anticipatory synergy
adjustments: preparing a quick action in an unknown direction. Exp Brain Res 226:565–573
Zhou T, Solnik S, Wu Y-H, Latash ML (2014) Unintentional movements produced by
back-coupling between the actual and referent body configurations: violations of equifinality in
multi-joint positional tasks. Exp Brain Res 232:3847–3859
Zhou T, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015a) Unintentional changes in the apparent stiffness of the
multi-joint limb. Exp Brain Res 233:2989–3004
Zhou T, Zhang L, Latash ML (2015b) Characteristics of unintentional movements by a multi-joint
effector. J Mot Behav 47:352–361
The Relationship Between Postural
and Movement Stability
Anatol G. Feldman
Keywords Reflexes Spatial threshold Joint angles Reafference Mechanical
reductionism Equilibrium point (EP) hypothesis Referent body Frame of
reference (FR) Motoneurons (MN) Coactivation Posture-movement problem
Introduction
Basic ideas on how the brain controls posture and movement have been originated
about two centuries ago (Steinbuch 1811; von Uexküll 1926; von Helmholtz 1866)
and in a most clear form were presented in seminal papers by von Holst (von Holst
and Mittelstaedt 1950; von Holst 1954). Neurophysiological data were compara-
tively limited and many scientists focused on the mechanical output of motor
actions. It seemed natural to assume that the brain is able to preprogram kinematic
and kinetic characteristics and directly specify the requisite motor commands to
muscles (i.e., EMG patterns or forces) to get the desired motor outcome. I call this
approach to motor control mechanical reductionism (Feldman 2015). Many
researchers still take for granted this approach despite indications that it runs into
several problems in the explanation of essential features of motor behavior and
despite experimental findings refuting this approach 50 years ago (Asatryan and
Feldman 1965). Among the problems unresolved in the framework of mechanical
reductionism is the posture-movement problem of how intentional movement from
a stable posture can be produced without evoking resistance of posture-stabilizing
mechanisms. In papers listed above, von Holst shared the common view that the
brain preprograms motor commands and concluded that postural mechanisms are
incompatible with mechanisms responsible for intentional motion. He further
concluded that posture-stabilizing mechanisms are transiently neutralized when
intentional movements are made and that this is achieved by suppressing
movement-related afferent feedback (reafference). He additionally proposed that the
nervous system uses a copy of predicted motor commands (efference copy) to
suppress the reafference. Since then the notion of efference copy and similar notion
of corollary discharge introduced by Sperry (1950) were broadly used in inter-
pretations of motor behavior.
After discovery of coactivation of α- and γ-motoneurons (MNs) during active
movements (see Granit 1955), it would be appropriate to conclude that the activity
of agonist and antagonist muscles during intentional movement is supported due to
activation of muscle spindle afferents by γ-MNs. This means reafference is
enhanced, rather than suppressed during such movements, thus refuting von Holst’s
proposal. This, however, has not been noticed in many studies in which his pro-
posal was accepted without any critical analysis (e.g., Gallistel 1980; McCloskey
1978; Cullen 2004; Proske and Gandevia 2009).
The inability to solve the posture-movement problem in the framework of
mechanical reductionism led to another problem—the failure to clarify the rela-
tionship between postural and movement stability.
More progressive attempts to consider the control of posture and movement in
the framework of the theory of dynamic systems. However, based on the same idea
of preprogramming of kinematics and motor commands, these attempts run into the
problems characteristic of traditional approaches to motor control (see however,
Shoner Mattos et al. 2015). Applications of computational methods developed in
robotics (Hollerbach 1982; Wolpert and Kawato 1998) did not leave the tenets of
The Relationship Between Postural and Movement Stability 107
mechanical reductionism since these methods also relied on the notion of prepro-
gramming of motor commands.
The tenets of mechanical reductionism were broken with the experimental
identification of physiological parameters underlying the control of posture and
movement in humans (Asatryan and Feldman 1965). This result motivated the
equilibrium point (EP) hypothesis, which after systematic testing and development
for about 50 years, has evolved into the theory of referent control of action and
perception (Feldman 1986, 2011, 2015). The theory retained the original notion of
the EP hypothesis that the brain controls motor actions by changing specific
physiological parameters without the necessity to preprogram mechanical variables
and motor commands. It has been shown that the parameters that the nervous
system is able to change are threshold positions of the body segments at which
muscles begin to be activated or deactivated (Asatryan and Feldman 1965). By
changing such parameters the system shifts the equilibrium state in the interaction
between the organism and the environment. As a consequence, changes in
mechanical characteristics and motor commands emerge, without preprogramming,
due to a natural cooperative tendency of neuromuscular elements to reach equi-
librium. If the emerging motor action does not meet the task demand, the system
adjusts the spatial activation thresholds to minimize the movement error. This
control strategy is used not only during everyday life but also during learning novel
motor skills.
There are different hierarchically organized neural levels and respective forms of
threshold position control (Feldman 2015). In particular, the referent body con-
figuration delimits possible positions of body segments or configurations of the
entire body. The referent body orientation delimits possible body positions with
respect to the direction of gravity. The referent body localization delimits possible
places of the body in the environment. The referent coactivation zone delimits the
range of body configurations at which agonist and antagonist muscles can be
coactivated.
In addressing the stability topics, I will primarily focus on some of those forms
of referent control. Each forms can be considered as the origin points of respective
spatial frame of reference (FR) in which motor actions are produced. In this case,
the EP is a secondary, emergent property of referent control. However, the EP and
its shifts characterize an important feature of the organism-environment interaction
and remain an integral part of the referent control theory.
The knowledge of physiological mechanisms underlying the organization of
motoneuronal pool of a single muscle is a departure point of our understanding of
how posture and movement are controlled. Therefore, before addressing the
question of stability, I will describe how the motoneuronal pool of a single muscle
is controlled. After that I clarify the EP concept in relation to stability of posture and
movement. Finally, I will use different forms of referent control to clarify the
relationship between postural and movement stability in several motor tasks. The
reader may note that, in several important aspects, the present description of how
the motoneuronal pool is organized and controlled is different from that in standard
textbooks on motor control.
108 A.G. Feldman
x k [ 0; ð1Þ
where x is muscle length and k is the threshold muscle length, i.e., the length at
which MN recruitment is initiated. In dynamics, the threshold length depends on the
speed of muscle lengthening (v ˃ 0) or shortening (v < 0). It also depends on
intermuscular interaction and history-dependent state of MNs (ρ):
k ¼ klv þ q ð2Þ
Spatial n
threshold
x x
= v+ Muscle length (x)
Reversal
(c) (d)
of MN recruitment
2 Range of
˃
+
1 Biomechanical
x- range of x x+ +
1 2
Fig. 1 Basic principles underlying the organization and control of the motoneuronal pool. See
text
muscle stretching within the entire biomechanical range of muscle length (unless
the stretch speed is very high, as during the knee reflex).
Threshold defines where, in the biomechanical range, the muscles begins to be
activated and generates nonlinear force-length characteristic (Fig. 1b). Defined by
fixed value of k , this characteristic is called invariant one (IC). This notion does
not imply that the shape of characteristic is the same—it may be different for
different activation thresholds. The ability to relax and activate the muscle at any
length within the biomechanical range implies that the range of regulation of λ
exceeds the biomechanical range of changes in the muscle length, x (Fig. 1d).
One can say that by specifying the threshold, the nervous system predetermines
the spatial range within which the intrinsic muscle and reflex properties manifest
position and velocity-dependent changes in the muscle force. These properties are
essential for stability of posture and movement.
Not less essential is that, by changing λ, the system not only predetermines the
position at which the muscle can balance the external load, but also transfer stability
properties to that position. This notion is fundamental for the understanding of why
intentional motion from a stable position does not evoke resistance of
posture-stabilizing mechanisms as would occur if motion were produced by direct
specification of motor commands to muscles—the posture-movement problem
110 A.G. Feldman
identified but not resolved by von Holst (von Holst and Mittelstaedt 1950; von
Holst 1954). In other words, in the framework of referent (threshold) control the
problem does not exist.
Several important conclusions can additionally be derived from the above
description.
1. MNs of a single muscle function in one-dimensional spatial FR with the muscle
length (x) as the only coordinate of this FR. There is the origin (referent) point in
this FR: k . MNs begin to be recruited at this point and the activity of MNs
increases depending on x k . The origin point can be shifted by different
spinal and supraspinal systems. It also depends on the speed of changes in the
muscle length as well as on the intermuscular interaction.
2. The notion that MNs function in a spatial FR can be referred to many other
neurons and the latter can be classified according to the spatial FR frames they
function. Each FR is defined by the type of position-dependent information
received by the respective neurons. Control levels target neurons associated with
the FR that is most appropriate for the motor task and predetermine the spatial
boundaries in this FR in which MNs can work. EMG patterns (motor commands
to muscles) are not a matter of concern of neural control levels. These patterns
emerge automatically depending on the deflection of the actual position of the
body from the centrally specified spatial boundaries. Control levels determine
whether the emergent motor action is adequate for reaching the goal and, if not,
they adjust the spatial boundaries or change the type of FR to eventually reach
the goal. This control strategy is equally applicable to everyday actions and
motor learning.
3. The k can be considered as the dynamic threshold of the stretch reflex and thus
the muscle is controlled by shifting the spatial threshold of this reflex.
4. By increasing the threshold, the nervous system can deactivate the muscle but
this does not mean that the stretch reflex is blocked as such. Unless the threshold
is increased to fully relax the muscle, functional properties of the reflex are fully
preserved but in another part of the biomechanical range. This means that gating
of reflexes usually considered in rhythmic movements results from modulation
of the spatial reflex thresholds. In addition, the absence of reflex reactions
should not be considered as resulting from blocking of reflexes. Threshold
control implies that reflexes are not suppressed as such but remains fully
functional in another part of biomechanical range. Similar reservations should
be made for the notion of reflex gain modulation usually considered as a way the
nervous system controls reflexes. In contrast, gain modulation should be con-
sidered as an emerging, not directly controlled by the nervous system property
of threshold control. By focusing on gain modulation and reflex gating one
considers effects, rather than causes of motor control.
The Relationship Between Postural and Movement Stability 111
Additional aspects of stability of posture and movement are associated with the EP
concept. Mechanically, each EP represents a specific combination of the positions
of body segments at which muscle and external torques are balanced. Since EP
characterizes the interaction between the organism and environment, the nervous
system can only influence but cannot fully predetermine the EP: its shifts can also
result from changes in external forces, eliciting involuntary motion, as occurs
during the unloading reflex. This reflex is illustrated by asking a subject to hold a
book on the palm of the arm at certain position. When the book is suddenly
removed by an assistant, the arm automatically moves to another EP.
The EP is a dynamic concept in the sense that it also implies certain reactions of
muscle and external torques to deflections of the system from the EP: the latter can
be stable (an attractor), semi-stable, or unstable (repellent) (Fig. 2). In some cases,
the threshold control allows the nervous system to convert an unstable or
semi-stable to a stable EP (Fig. 2). The neural circuitry and the neuromuscular
properties allow the nervous system to mostly operate in the space of stable EPs,
which means that deflections from an EP are met with resistance increasing with
increasing deflection and its speed from the EP. If perturbations are transient, these
reactions may be sufficient to regain the same EP (equifinality). However, the
system may not tolerate strong or destabilizing perturbation even if they are tran-
sient and involves additional reactions to regain stability by transition to a new,
stable EP. For example, during quiet standing, position- and velocity-dependent
resistance might be insufficient to prevent body falling in response to sudden tilting
of the platform on which the subject stands. In response to such a destabilizing
perturbation, a step can be made to bring the body to a new, stable posture. In this
case, step should be considered as a part of posture-stabilizing mechanisms, which
further emphasizes the dynamic content of the EP concept. It also shows that while
predicting the possibility of equifinality, the EP hypothesis does not predict that
equifinality should be observed in response to any transient perturbation. Because
of misunderstanding of this aspect, many researchers falsely rejected the EP
hypothesis based on observations of equifinality violation in responses to pertur-
bations (e.g., Lackner and Dizio 1994).
The EP concept is associated with an important physical principle not mentioned
in standard textbooks on neural control of motor actions. This principle originated
from analysis of stability in the theory of dynamics systems (Glansdorff and
Prigogine 1971). Consider pointing arm motion from one stable EP to another.
Muscle and external torques are balanced at the initial and final EPs, but, according
to the physical principle, torques or any variables related to them (like forces, EMG
activity) are not responsible for the choice of the arm position at which the balance
is achieved. This statement becomes obvious when changes in the arm position are
produced in isotonic conditions, when torques are the same at the initial and final
EPs, implying that the choice between these EPs is not predetermined by torques. In
contrast, according to the physical principle, the choice is predetermined by specific
112 A.G. Feldman
(a)
(b)
IC
Extensor Flexor
b Gravitational
torque
torque
a
0
c R
(c)
a
0
Rf Re
Fig. 2 Stabilizing equilibrium points by combining R and C commands. When the human
forearm is oriented vertically while shoulder lays on a horizontal surface (a), the gravitational
torque at the elbow joint is zero but increases (in terms of the absolute value) with a deviation of
the forearm from this position. To balance the zero gravitational torque the net muscle torque
should also be zero. This condition can be met by setting the common threshold (R) for activation
of elbow flexors and extensors at this position of the forearm. b Equilibrium point for this arm
position will be unstable, since the gradient (slope) of the gravitational torque at this position is
higher than muscle stiffness (slope) of invariant torque-angle characteristic, IC, at this position. In
other words, the muscle torque generated in response to perturbations will be insufficient in
overcoming the increasing gravitational torque to return the arm to the point a. When perturbed,
the forearm will arrive at stable equilibrium point b or c, depending on the direction of
perturbation. c To stabilize equilibrium point a, the system can facilitate flexor and extensor MNs
to create a zone coactivation (between Rf and Re), thus increasing the net muscle stiffness.
Reproduced with permission from Feldman (2015), Copyright Springer
The Relationship Between Postural and Movement Stability 113
C command
Torque
C command
R & C command
Thus, the C command is used to increase movement speed beyond the limit
defined by maximal rate of shift in the R command. The system converts posture
stabilizing to movement-producing mechanisms even in the cases when muscle
coactivation is used. In contrast, if MNs were activated directly, muscle coactiva-
tion would increase resistance to motion.
This example also illustrates that with referent control, mechanisms that stabilize
the initial posture are used to stabilize motion and the final posture.
Consider the case when the platform at which a subject quietly stands is suddenly
tilted (toes up) by about 10° and remained tilted after that. After the transitional
period, the subject continues to stand on the tilted platform. With the onset of
tilting, ankle extensors are stretched. The stretch reflex tends to rotate the body
backward and the subject is forced to do something to prevent falling. Naturally,
with repetition of the perturbation, the stretch reflex is attenuated (Horak et al. 1989;
Nashner 1976; Schieppati and Nardone 1995). One question has not been
116 A.G. Feldman
addressed in the cited studies. After adaptation to perturbation, the stretch reflex
was suppressed. Was the reflex continued to be suppressed during standing on the
tilted platform? The stretch reflex should be restored to stabilize posture during
standing on the tilted platform, but how this is possible if the stretch reflex was
suppressed with the onset of platform tilting after several trials? The notion of
referent control is helpful in answering these questions. A after several trials sub-
jects were able to increase the stretch reflex threshold of ankle extensors in response
to tilting the platform to prevent stretch responses of these muscles. During the
platform tilting, they continued to increase the threshold to minimize stretch
responses. With the end of platform tilting¸ the threshold shifts were ceased such
that the stretch reflex became fully functional during standing at the tilted platform
(Fig. 4a). This example illustrates the general rule that by resetting the activation
thresholds, the nervous system does not suppress the stretch reflex as such but
transfer its properties to the final posture. Indeed, the validity of this description in
terms of referent control can be tested by perturbation methods.
Consider how intentional leaning of the body can be produced. Referent body
orientation indicates the position of the body with respect to the direction of gravity.
At this position all skeletal muscles reach activation thresholds or, in the presence
of C command, are activated but generate net zero torques. Activity of muscles
changes with deflection of the body from the referent orientation.
Using the concept of referent body orientation, consider the case when a subject
stands on a horizontal surface. One can assume that during quiet standing the
referent body orientation coincides with the direction of gravity. The COM is
located in front of referent line and the torque of body weight leans the body
forward until this torque becomes balanced by the torque of activated ankle
extensors. This is the initial situation during quiet standing before intentional
leaning of the body.
Now consider how the subject can lean the body forward. The gravitational
torque at the new body position is higher than at the initial body posture and, to
balance the torque, extensor muscle torque should be enhanced. However,
increasing the muscle torque at the initial posture would deflect the body backward,
not forward. Again, referent control is helpful in the explanation of how the system
performs the task. The system leans the referent body orientation forward thus
initially diminishing the deflection of the actual body orientation from the new
referent orientation. As a consequence, the activity of ankle extensors will initially
drop, letting the gravitational torque lean the body forward. During leaning, ankle
extensor muscles will be restretched and reactivated. The leaning will cease at a
new body position at which the increasing gravitational torque becomes balanced
by joint active and passive extensor torques (Fig. 4b). Again, the validity of the
proposed referent control of body posture can be tested by perturbation methods.
The Relationship Between Postural and Movement Stability 117
(b) R Q RQ
118 A.G. Feldman
The notion that posture and movement are stabilized by common mechanisms is
helpful in the explanation of how, in principle, locomotion is initiated, maintained,
and terminated. This analysis will brings us to the conclusion that the traditional
claim that locomotion is associated with falling and catching of the body in each
cycle is misleading.
Consider neurons that receive sensory inputs depending on the current local-
ization of the body in the environment. Then according to the general rule, inde-
pendent central inputs predetermine the referent (threshold) body localization at
which these neurons begin to be recruited. Like in MNs, these neurons will generate
activity depending on the deflection of the current from the referent body local-
ization. Transmitted to MNs, this activity will affect threshold muscle lengths. It is
assumed that these projections are organized such that the elicited changes in
lambdas will elicit muscle activity tending to diminish the deflection of the body
from its referent localization. These previous explanations will help us in the
description of how a step or continuous locomotion is produced.
During quiet standing, the localization of the body coincides with the referent
body localization. By sifting the referent body localization, the nervous system
transfers body balance to a new place in the environment. Muscles are activated and
tend to diminish the deflection of the body from the place to which the body balance
is shifted. If the shift in the body localization is small, a single step will be sufficient
to relocate the body to the new place. The speed of the step will depend on the rate
of referent shift. With prolongation of the referent shifts, a continuous locomotion
(gait or running, depending on the rate of referent shifts) will be performed until the
referent shifts are ceased and the body reaches a new place in the environment.
In standard mechanical descriptions of locomotion, it is usually emphasized that
when a step is made the COM moves outside the base of support (BOS) and the
body begins to fall. In the framework of referent control this is not a problem:
While leaving the initial BOS, the COM moves toward the new BOS to which the
body balance was transferred. In non-perturbed locomotion, the speed of motion of
the COM is always sufficient to move the COM toward the shifted BOS and reach
the final BOS when referent shifts are ceased.
Some consequences of referent control of locomotion have been formulated and
tested (Feldman et al. 2011; Krasovsky et al. 2014). Transient perturbations may
temporarily slowdown the motion of COM such that the referent BOS will go too
far to be reached. To prevent body falling, the system can shortly diminish the rate
or even stop the referent shifts of the body localization such that the BOS will be
reached. After that regular referent shifts can be resumed but the phase of steps will
permanently be changed (phase resetting). This prediction was confirmed in the
studies cited above.
To summarize, in addition to solving several problems in the explanations of
motor actions, the notion of referent control clarifies the relationship between
The Relationship Between Postural and Movement Stability 119
stability of posture and movement with the conclusion that posture and movements
are stabilized by common mechanisms.
For a long time, many researchers were puzzled by the question of how intentional
movements can be produced without resistance of reflexes that tend to minimize
deflections from an initial posture (the classical posture-movement problem). By
shifting the spatial domain in which reflexes function, the nervous system forces
them to produce movements without any posture-movement problem.
References
Asatryan DG, Feldman AG (1965) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of
movement or maintenance of a steady posture: I. Mechanographic analysis of the work of the
joint on execution of a postural task. Biophysics 10:925–934
Cullen KE (2004) Sensory signals during active versus passive movement. Curr Opin Neurobiol
14: 698–706
Feldman AG (1986) Once more on the equilibrium-point hypothesis (lambda model) for motor
control. J Mot Behav 18:17–54
Feldman AG (2011) Space and time in the context of equilibrium-point theory. Wiley Interdiscip
Rev Cogn Sci 2(3):287–304
Feldman AG (2015) Referent control of action and perception: challenging conventional theories
in behavioral neuroscience. Springer, New York
Feldman AG, Krasovsky T, Banina MC, Lamontagne A, Levin MF (2011) Changes in the referent
body location and configuration may underlie human gait, as confirmed by findings of
multi-muscle activity minimizations and phase resetting. Exp Brain Res 210:91–115
Gallistel CR (1980) The organization of action: a new synthesis. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates,
Inc, Hillsdale, p 432
Glansdorff P, Prigogine I (1971) Thermodynamics theory of structure, stability and fluctuations.
Wiley-Interscience, London
Granit R (1955) Receptors and sensory perception. Yale University Press, New Haven
Henneman E, Somjen G, Carpenter DO (1965) Functional significance of cell size in spinal
motoneurons. J Neurophysiol 28:560–580
Hollerbach JM (1982) Computers, brains, and the control of movement. Trends Neurosci 5:
189–192
Horak F, Diener H, Nashner L (1989) Influence of central set on human postural responses.
J Neurophysiol 62:841–853
Kanda K, Burke RE, Walmsley B (1977) Differential control of fast and slow twitch motor units in
the decerebrate cat. Exp Brain Res 8:57–74
Krasovsky T, Lamontagne A, Feldman AG, Levin MF (2014) Effects of walking speed on gait
stability and interlimb coordination in younger and older adults. Gait Posture 39:378–385
Lackner JR, Dizio P (1994) Rapid adaptation to Coriolis force perturbations of arm trajectory.
J Neurophysiol 72:299–313
120 A.G. Feldman
Matthews PB (1981) Evolving views on the internal operation and functional role of the muscle
spindle. J Physiol 320:1–30
Mattos D, Schoner G, Zatsiorsky VM, Latash ML (2015) Motor equivalence during multi-finger
accurate force production. Exp Brain Res 233:487–502
McCloskey DI (1978) Corollary discharges: motor commands and perception. In: Brooks V
(ed) Handbook of physiology, motor control, vol 2. American Physiological Society, Bethesda,
MD, pp 1415–47
Nashner LM (1976) Adapting reflexes controlling the human posture. Exp Brain Res 26:59–72
Pilon JF, Feldman AG (2006) Threshold control of motor actions prevents destabilizing effects of
proprioceptive delays. Exp Brain Res 174:229–239
Proske U, Gandevia SC (2009) The kinaesthetic senses. J Physiol 587(Pt 17):4139–4146
Schieppati M, Nardone A (1995) Time course of ‘set’-related changes in muscle responses to
stance perturbation in humans. J Physiol 487:787–796
Sperry RW (1950) Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual
inversion. J Comp Physiol Psychol 43:482–489
Steinbuch J (1811). Beytrag Zur Physiologie Der Sinne (Contribution to the Physiology of
Senses). Verlag, Nuremberg, p 408
von Helmholtz H (1866) Handbuch der Physiologischen Optik. Voss, Leipzig
Von Holst H (1954) Relations between the central nervous system and the peripheral organs. Br J
Anim Behav 2:89–94
Von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950/1973) Das reafferezprincip. Wechselwirkungen zwischen
Zentralnerven-system und Peripherie, Naturwissenschaften 37: 467–476 (The reafference
principle. In: Martin R (Trans), The behavioral physiology of animals and man. The collected
papers of Erich von Holst. University of Miami Press, Coral Gables, FL, pp 139–173,
176–209)
von Uexküll J (1926) Theoretical Biology. Harcourt, Brace & Co, New York
Wolpert DM, Kawato M (1998) Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control.
Neural Netw 11(7–8):1317–1329
Zhang L, Straube A, Eggert T (2016) Under threshold position control, peripheral mechanisms
compensate efficiently for small perturbations of arm movements. Mot Control 20:87–108
Principles of Motor Recovery After
Neurological Injury Based on a Motor
Control Theory
Mindy F. Levin
Keywords Joint angle International Classification of Function (ICF) Stroke
Spasticity Hand dysfunction Trunk restraint Virtual reality
Introduction
wide (Kwakkel et al. 2003). The most widely recognized problem in survivors of
stroke is motor impairment that restricts voluntary, well-coordinated, and effective
movements predominantly on one side of the body (hemiparesis). In particular,
long-term impairment of the upper limb on the side of the body contralateral to the
brain lesion affects up to 75 % of people with stroke (Clarke et al. 2002), which
limits the ability of an individual to perform the full range of daily life tasks and
restricts their level of participation in society. Despite the large amount of research
focusing on identifying underlying post-stroke sensorimotor deficits, it is still
unclear which deficit or combination of deficits limits motor recovery.
Consequently, there is a paucity of good evidence linking motor retraining
approaches to effective upper limb functional recovery (Teasell et al. 2006).
Descriptions of post-stroke sensorimotor deficits are related to different levels of
the International Classification of Functioning (ICF; Fig. 1) developed by the World
Health Organization (WHO 2001). The ICF is an established globally-accepted
framework to describe, assess, and compare function and disability which has been
used to guide research and clinical practice. Based on the health condition, the ICF
framework categorizes the spectrum of disability into three levels: Body Structures
and Function, Activity, and Participation. It also identifies interactions between these
levels associated with personal and environmental factors.
Fig. 1 The International Classification of Function as a framework for the description, assessment,
and comparison of function and disability, in which three levels are identified: (1) Body Structures
and Function, (2) Activity, and (3) Participation, also termed, respectively, as impairments,
limitations, and restrictions. These levels are modified by contextual factors with respect to the
environment and the person. Modified from http://www.who.int/classifications/icf/en/
Principles of Motor Recovery After Neurological Injury … 123
Deficits at the first two levels of the ICF model (Body Structures and Function,
Activity) are of interest when discussing the basic motor control factors that may
influence motor recovery.
Measures characterizing motor behaviors at the ICF Body Structures and Function
(impairment) level include spasticity, decreased range of motion, muscle weakness,
and impaired ability to isolate movements of individual joints and to perform
coordinated movement of multiple joints (e.g., Brunnstrom 1966; Fugl-Meyer et al.
1975; Gowland et al. 1993).
Spasticity, defined as a motor disorder characterized by a velocity-dependent
increase in tonic stretch reflexes (“muscle tone”) with exaggerated tendon jerks
(Lance 1980), develops gradually after the acute phase of stroke and persists into
the chronic phase of recovery. The prevalence of post-stroke spasticity is highly
variable, ranging from 17 to 42.6 % in chronic stroke (Wissel et al. 2013). The
presence of spasticity may leads to secondary complications such as pain and
muscle contractures as well as to difficulties in performing activities of daily living
and to a reduced quality of life (Zorowitz et al. 2013). In standard clinical
assessment, spasticity is characterized as the amount of resistance felt by the
examiner when the muscle is stretched from a shortened to a lengthened position,
regardless of the speed of stretch. Measured in this way, however, few studies have
related the amount of spasticity to the functional motor deficit in the upper limb.
However, for the lower limb, one study in post-stroke patients showed that
velocity-sensitive stretch responses in the calf were correlated with low plan-
tarflexor activation levels during the stance phase of gait. These responses were
positively related (r = 0.47, p < 0.01; r = 0.57, p < 0.001) to the amount of muscle
resistance evoked by stretch of the plantarflexors at rest, and negatively related to
gait speed (r = −0.47 to −0.53, p < 0.01; Lamontagne et al. 2001). Although
described behaviorally, the precise relationship between hypertonicity, hyperactive
reflexes, weakness, and the production of voluntary movement has not been elu-
cidated. Since disorders of muscle tone and movement deficits have been tradi-
tionally been considered as separate phenomena, the controversy is likely related to
the lack of a coherent view of this relationship within the context of a motor control
theory (see below).
During voluntary movement production, individuals with hemiparesis have
varying degrees of spatial and temporal coordination deficits between movements
of adjacent joints even for simple arm movements (elbow/shoulder, Beer et al.
2000; Cirstea et al. 2003a, b; Dewald et al. 1995; Levin 1996), when synchronizing
hand orientation with hand opening/closing during reach-to-grasp movements to
stationary targets (Michaelsen et al. 2004, 2006; Roby-Brami et al. 2003), and in
modulation of precision grip forces according to task demands (Hermsdorfer et al.
1999; Wenzelburger et al. 2005). An example of how simple reaching movement to
Principles of Motor Recovery After Neurological Injury … 125
a midline target is disrupted in patients with stroke is shown in Fig. 2. The figure
illustrates two aspects of disrupted reaching in the spatial domain in a subject with
moderately severe hemiparesis: excessive use of the trunk for reaching targets
located both close and far from the body (Fig. 2a) and a deficit in the spatial
coordination between movements of adjacent shoulder and elbow joints (Fig. 2b;
Michaelsen et al. 2001). Temporal relationships between elbow and shoulder
movements are also disrupted during rapid alternating pointing movements with
both joints being recruited simultaneously and stereotypically compared to a more
temporally sequential recruitment in age-matched healthy control subjects
(Rodrigues et al. 2015).
One mechanism that may underlie problems in movement production of the upper
limb is disruption in the ability of the central nervous system to regulate stretch
reflex thresholds of muscles during single (Levin et al. 2000) and multi-joint
126 M.F. Levin
Fig. 3 Principle of Threshold Control. a In the healthy nervous system, the tonic stretch reflex
threshold (TSRT) can be regulated in the range TSRT− to TSRT+ that exceeds the biomechanical
range of the joint (Ɵ− to Ɵ+). Torques can be balanced at any muscle length by shifting the TSRT
along with the joint torque/angle characteristic (diagonal black line) into the biomechanical range
and beyond. b After neurological injury, one or both limits of the TSRT abnormally lie inside of
the biomechanical range. In the example shown, the range of joint angles between TSRT− and
TSRT+ is the angular zone in which normal muscle activation occurs in both flexors and extensors
(Active control zone). In the angular zone, to the right of the TSRT+, muscles cannot be relaxed
(Spasticity zone) and in the area to the left of the TSRT−, muscle weakness occurs
inhibitory mechanisms may be preserved in the forearm of patients with stroke that
can reduce the angular range in which spasticity occurs during voluntary
movement.
Deficits in threshold regulation may also partially explain movement slowness in
people who have had a stroke. Aside from decreases in the speed of cognitive
processes, movements may be slowed when attempts to extend the arm beyond the
threshold for activation of the elbow flexors result in coactivation of antagonists,
due to movements into the spasticity zone.
Deficits in spatial threshold regulation may also account for some of the
observed deficits in interjoint and/or intersegment coordination. Thus, it is likely
that active reaching movements may be affected by the limitation in the range of
regulation of the spatial thresholds in some or all of the muscles moving the limb,
resulting in loss of normal interjoint coordination and/or the appearance of new
interjoint coupling relationships (e.g., abnormal synergies).
Activity Level
made for obstacle avoidance (Alberts et al. 2002; Saling et al. 1998) while
movement time is prolonged (Biegstraaten et al. 2003). Thus, the CNS uses hand
and object spatial information in hand transport planning to produce closely
coordinated grasp and transport components (for review, see Sangole and Levin
2007).
Effects of distracting objects flanking a target have been studied during reaching
in healthy subjects. Changes in transport trajectories occur even when flanking
objects do not block the movement path (Chapman and Goodale 2008; Tresilian
1998), but their orientation and location influence reaching behavior differently
(Chapman and Goodale 2008; Mon-Williams and McIntosh 2000; Mon-Williams
et al. 2001). Grasp apertures, however, are only affected when the object is directly
obstructed. Changes are speed dependent and occur even in children as young as
8-years old (Tresilian et al. 2005). However, very few studies have characterized
obstructed reaching behavior in patients with stroke. The few studies in patients
with neglect (McIntosh et al. 2004), optic ataxia (Schindler et al. 2004) or visual
agnosia (Rice et al. 2006) suggest that hand guidance to reach around an obstacle
relies on dorsal stream visual processing while reaching between obstacles (bi-
secting) requires more visual attention involving ventral stream processing. In
healthy subjects, even complex movements made in real environments are rela-
tively automatic, requiring limited attention (Doyon and Benali 2005). Movement
automaticity has been studied using dual-task paradigms, in which a motor task
competes with a secondary (often cognitive) task for the same attentional resources
(Huang and Mercer 2001). Most studies of dual-tasking involve locomotor activ-
ities combined with increased cognitive loading. When the motor task requires
disproportionate attention, decreased performance in the form of slower or less
accurate movement occurs due to dual-task interference (Kahneman 1973). In one
of the few studies of the effects of dual-tasking on upper limb function in stroke,
Houwink et al. (2013) found that increasing cognitive load via an auditory Stroop
task impaired circle drawing performance in a small group of subjects with mod-
erate upper limb paresis. More severely affected stroke patients required more
attentional resources to control the limb causing a detriment in dual-task perfor-
mance. They suggested that improvement in dual-task performance occurred due to
improved movement automaticity, but this motor element was not objectively
quantified. They also noted a discrepancy between motor capacity and actual arm
use and indicated the need to identify underlying processes responsible for spon-
taneous arm use.
We recently identified spatiotemporal coordination deficits during an obstacle
avoidance task (Baniña et al. 2016) in patients with chronic stroke resulting in
mild-to-moderate hemiparesis. Patients had persistent deficits in their ability to
avoid hitting a sliding door that partially obstructed a target object (juice can) on a
shelf behind it when reaching from sitting. They also had smaller margins of error
when reaching around the obstacle and lower success rates than healthy
age-matched controls. These results suggested that diminished arm use in patients
with stroke may be related to reduced arm and hand coordination to produce
effective interactions with a changing environment (Baniña et al. 2016; Dancause
130 M.F. Levin
et al. 2002; Feldman et al. 2007). We also identified that the ability to rapidly
correct errors and respond to changing environmental conditions was related to the
person’s level of self-confidence (self-efficacy) in using the upper limb in daily
activities.
Successful obstacle avoidance behavior may rely on many factors including
cognitive skills such as mental flexibility to make rapid decisions and choose
appropriate responses. Knowledge gaps remain about higher order motor control
deficits in stroke and how deficits in multiple systems (motor, cognitive, psy-
chosocial) relate to diminished arm use.
The ability to interact with one’s arm in a meaningful way in real-world, complex
and/or unpredictable environments, depends on the coordination of multiple
degrees of the freedom (DFs) in a dexterous way (Bernstein 1967; Latash 2012).
Reaching movements involve the coordination of multiple (>7) DFs of the arm to
transport the hand from one position to another in 3D Cartesian space. Rather than
controlling each joint rotation separately, the nervous system shifts the endpoint
(usually the hand for reaching movements) from one stable position to another,
while the coordination between multiple DFs emerges from the biomechanical and
intermuscular neural interactions between body segments and the physical envi-
ronment (Feldman 2011). The action of displacing the endpoint from one position
to another is described by a shift in the referent arm configuration so that movement
emerges due to the deviation of the actual arm configuration from the referent one
specified by the nervous system (Asatryan and Feldman 1965; Feldman 2011; see
Chap. 6).
The concept of referent control has solid empirical support and provides a
solution to the classical posture-movement problem described by Von Holst (1954).
Referent control is based on the predetermination of a “referent body configuration”
by the brain in anticipation of actual movement. It extends the principle of
Threshold Control from a single muscle or joint to a whole body segment or whole
body posture. Muscle activation occurs according to the deviation of the actual
body configuration (Q; i.e., the current position of the body) from the referent body
configuration (R). The R is a tool that allows the nervous system to control multiple
muscles of the body as a coherent unit. In the absence of muscle coactivation, R
represents a common threshold configuration for a system with multiple DFs, i.e.,
the body position at which muscles begin to be recruited or de-recruited. This
common threshold configuration can be shifted by reciprocal central influences on
motoneurons (MNs) of agonist and antagonist muscles. By means of central
co-facilitation of agonist and antagonist MNs, muscles can be coactivated at
position R and throughout the range defined R− to R+ (Asatryan and Feldman
Principles of Motor Recovery After Neurological Injury … 131
b Fig. 5 Referent control of reaching for targets beyond the arm reach. Although they move
simultaneously, the arm and trunk contribute sequentially to the hand motion extent. a, b Subject
moved the hand to target placed beyond the arm reach, which necessitated forward trunk
displacement. In randomly chosen trials, trunk motion was prevented by an electromagnet.
Example shown is for pointing to the ipsilateral target. c–h The hand and trunk began to move
simultaneously (e, f). Blocking the trunk had no effect on the hand trajectory or velocity profile
until about the peak hand velocity. Thus, the trunk began to contribute to the hand movement
extent after the offset of referent arm control but before that it was accelerated in preparation for its
future contribution. Reproduced with permission from Rossi et al. (2002). Copyright 2002 John
Wiley and Sons
path earlier than in healthy subjects. The earlier trajectory deviation was associated
with a later divergence of interjoint coordination patterns indicating a lack of
flexibility in adapting arm joint rotations to account for the added trunk movement.
Using a mathematical analysis of kinematic variability (uncontrolled manifold)
Principles of Motor Recovery After Neurological Injury … 135
during whole arm reaching movements from sitting, Reisman and Schultz (2003)
found that people with mild-to-moderate hemiparesis had deficits coordinating
specific joint movements and were only partially able to rapidly compensate
movement errors. Problems in adapting movements (error correction) to unexpected
external conditions has also been shown for single-joint elbow movements by
Dancause et al. (2002) who further related the error compensation deficits to
impairments in executive functioning in patients with chronic stroke. Deficits have
also been reported in error compensation for regaining interim phase relationships
after arm perturbations during arm swinging (Ustinova et al. 2006).
This chapter provides examples of how transfer of knowledge from basic science to
clinical applications can fill important gaps in our understanding of disordered
upper limb movement, loss of coordinated movement, diminished use of the arm as
well as mechanisms limiting motor recovery after neurological injury or disease.
Impairments in single-joint control at the Body Structures and Function level of the
ICF and deficits in the production of higher order coordinated movement at the
Activity level of the ICF are considered in light of two interrelated principles of the
Equilibrium-Point Theory: Threshold Control and Referent Control. Explanations
of motor deficits based on these principles provide knowledge about the underlying
mechanisms of disordered control of movement beyond simple descriptions of the
consequences of these disorders (e.g., decreased force output) that can be used to
develop better clinical tests to identify and quantify upper limb coordination defi-
cits, which is currently lacking in clinical practice. At the same time, a better
understanding of motor deficits in patients with neurological lesions may also
advance our understanding of normal motor control. Understanding upper limb
motor control deficits as diminished threshold control suggests that interventions
aimed at increasing this range may result in better motor recovery. To this end,
different types of interventions that can be explored include pharmacological and
physical training approaches.
References
Abend W, Bizzi E, Morasso P (1982) Human arm trajectory formation. Brain 105:331–348
Adamovich SV, Archambault PS, Ghafouri M, Levin MF, Poizner H, Feldman AG (2001) Hand
trajectory invariance in reaching movements involving the trunk. Exp Brain Res 138:288–303
Alberts JL, Saling M, Stelmach GE (2002) Alterations in transport path differentially affect
temporal and spatial movement parameters. Exp Brain Res 143:417–425
Archambault P, Pigeon P, Feldman AG, Levin MF (1999) Recruitment and sequencing of different
degrees of freedom during pointing movements involving the trunk in healthy and hemiparetic
subjects. Exp Brain Res 126:55–67
136 M.F. Levin
Asatryan DG, Feldman AG (1965) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of
movements or maintenance of a steady posture: I. Mechanographic analysis for the work of the
joint on execution of a postural tasks. Biophysics 10:925–935
Ballard C, Stephens S, Kenny R, Kalaria R, Tovee M, O’Brien J (2003) Profile of
neuropsychological deficits in older stroke survivors without dementia. Dement Geriatr
Cogn Disord 16:52–56
Blanchette AK, Mullick AA, Moïn-Darbari K, Levin MF (2016) Tonic stretch reflex threshold as a
measure of ankle plantar-flexor spasticity after stroke. Phys Ther 96(5):687–95
Bandura A (1986) Social foundations of thoughts and actions: a social cognitive theory.
Prentice-Hall, New Jersey
Bandura A (1997) Self-efficacy: the exercise of control. Freeman, New York
Baniña MC, Mullick AA, McFadyen BJ, Levin MF (2016) Upper limb obstacle avoidance
behaviour in individuals with chronic stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair, pii:
1545968316662527. [Epub ahead of print], PMID: 27542986
Beer RF, Dewald JP, Rymer WZ (2000) Deficits in the coordination of multijoint arm movements
in patients with hemiparesis: evidence for disturbed control of limb dynamics. Exp Brain Res
131:305–319
Bernstein NA (1967) The coordination and regulation of movements. Pergamon Press, London
Biegstraaten M, Smeets JB, Brenner E (2003) The influence of obstacles on the speed of grasping.
Exp Brain Res 149:530–534
Boyd LA, Quaney BM, Pohl PS, Winstein CJ (2007) Learning implicitly: effects of task and
severity after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 21:444–454
Broeks JG, Lankhorst GJ, Rumping K, Prevo AJH (1999) The long-term outcome of arm function
after stroke: results of a follow-up study. Disabil Rehab 21:357–364
Brunnstrom S (1966) Motor testing procedures in hemiplegia: based on sequential recovery stages.
Phys Ther 46:357–375
Calota A, Feldman AG, Levin MF (2008) Spasticity measurement based on tonic stretch reflex
threshold in stroke using a portable device. Clin Neurophysiol 119:2329–2337
Chapman CS, Goodale MA (2008) Missing in action: the effect of obstacle position and size on
avoidance while reacing. Exp Brain Res 191:83–97
Cirstea MC, Mitnitski AB, Feldman AG, Levin MF (2003a) Interjoint coordination dynamics
during reaching in stroke. Exp Brain Res 151(3):289–300
Cirstea MC, Ptito A, Levin MF (2003b) Arm reaching improvements with short-term practice
depend on the severity of the motor deficit in stroke. Exp Brain Res 152:476–488
Cirstea CM, Ptito A, Levin MF (2006) Feedback and cognition in arm motor skill reacquisition
after stroke. Stroke 37:1237–1242
Cisek P (2007) Cortical mechanisms of action selection: the affordance competition hypothesis.
Phil Trans Roy Soc B 362:1585–1599
Clarke P, Marshall V, Black SE, Colantonio A (2002) Well-being after stroke in Canadian seniors:
findings from the Canadian study of health and aging. Stroke 33:1016–1021
Cos I, Medley F, Cisek P (2012) The modulatory influence of endpoint controllability on decisions
between actions. J Neurophysiol 108:1764–1780
Dancause N, Ptito A, Levin MF (2002) Error correction strategies for motor behavior
after unilateral brain damage: short-term motor learning processes. Neuropsychologia 40:
1313–1323
De Ryck A, Fransen E, Brouns R, Geurden M, Peij D, Marien P, De Deyn PP, Engelborghs S
(2014) Psychosocial problems associated with depression at 18 months poststroke. Int J Ger
Psych 29:144–152
Dean J, Brüwer M (1994) Control of human arm movements in two dimensions: paths and joint
control in avoiding simple linear obstacles. Exp Brain Res 97:497–514
Dewald JPA, Pope PS, Given JD, Buchanan TS, Rymer WZ (1995) Abnormal muscle coactivation
patterns during isometric torque generation at the elbow and shoulder in hemiparetic subjects.
Brain 118:495–510
Principles of Motor Recovery After Neurological Injury … 137
Doyon J, Benali H (2005) Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor
skills. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15:161–167
Feldman AG (1966) Functional tuning of the nervous system with control of movement and
maintenance of a steady posture: II. Controllable parameters of the muscle. Biophysics 11:
565–578
Feldman AG (2011) Space and time in the context of the equilibrium-point theory. Wiley
Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci 2:287–304
Feldman AG (2015) Referent control of action and perception. Springer, New York
Feldman AG, Goussev V, Sangole A, Levin MF (2007) Threshold position control and the
principle of minimal interaction in motor actions. In: Cisek P, Drew T, Kalaska JF
(ed) Progress in brain research, vol 165, pp 267–281
Fugl-Meyer AR, Jääskö L, Leyman I, Olsson S, Steglind S (1975) The post-stroke hemiplegic
patient. I. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand J Rehab Med 7:13–31
Gage M, Valiant Cook J, Fryday-Field K (1997) Understanding the transition to community living
after discharge from an acute care hospital: an exploratory study. Am J Occ Ther 51(2):96–103
Ghafouri M, Feldman AG (2001) The timing of control signals underlying fast point-to-point arm
movements. Exp Brain Res 137:411–423
Gowland C, Stratford P, Ward M et al (1993) Measuring physical impairment and disability with
the Chedoke-McMaster stroke assessment. Stroke 24:58–63
Hacke W (2003) European stroke initiative recommendations for stroke management—update
2003. Cerebrovasc Dis 16:311–337
Hallett M, Pascual-Leone A, Topka H (1996) Adaptation and skill learning: evidence for different
neural substrates. In: Bloedel JR, Ebner TJ, Wise S (ed) The acquisition of motor behavior in
vertebrates, MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 289–30
Hellstrom K, Lindmark B, Wahlberg B, Fugl-Meyer AR (2003) Self-efficacy in relation to
impairments and activities of daily living disability in elderly patients with stroke: a
prospective investigation. J Rehabil Med 35:202–207
Hermsdorfer J, Laimgruber K, Kerkhoff G, Mai N, Goldenberg G (1999) Effects of unilateral brain
damage on coordination, and kinematics of ipsilesional prehension. Exp Brain Res 128:41–51
Heruti RJ, Lusky A, Dankner R et al (2002) Rehabilitation outcome of elderly patients after a first
stroke: effect of cognitive status at admission on the functional outcome. Arch Phys Med
Rehabil 83:742–749
Hochstenbach J, Mulder T, van Limbeek J, Donders R, Schoonwaldt H (1998) Cognitive decline
following stroke: a comprehensive study of cognitive decline following stroke. J Clin Exp
Neuropsychol 20:503–517
Houwink A, Steenbergen B, Prange GB, Buurke JH, Geurts ACH (2013) Upper-limb motor
control in patients after stroke: attentional demands and the potential beneficial effects of arm
support. Hum Movement Sci 32:377–387
Huang JH, Mercer VS (2001) Dual-task methodology: applications in studies of cognitive and
motor performance in adults and children. Pediatric Phys Ther 13:133–140
Hultborn H (2006) Spinal reflexes, mechanisms and concepts: from Eccles to Lundberg and
beyond. Prog Neurobiol 78:215–232
Jaillard A, Naegele B, Trabucco-Miguel S, LeBas J, Hommel M (2009) Hidden dysfunctioning in
subacute stroke. Stroke 40:2473–2479
Jobin A, Levin MF (2000) Regulation of stretch reflex threshold in elbow flexors in children with
cerebral palsy: a new measure of spasticity. Dev Med Child Neurol 42:531–540
Kahneman D (1973) Attention and effort. Prentice-Hall International, New Jersey
Kizony R, Katz N, Weiss PL (2004) Virtual reality based intervention in rehabilitation:
relationship between motor and cognitive abilities and performance within virtual environ-
ments for patients with stroke. In: Proceedings of the 5th international conference on disability,
virtual reality and associated technology. Oxford, UK
Kwakkel G, Kollen BJ, van der Grond J, Prevo AJ (2003) Probability of regaining dexterity in the
flaccid upper limb: impact of severity of paresis and time since onset in acute stroke. Stroke
34:2181–2186
138 M.F. Levin
Lai SM, Studenski S, Richards L, Perera S, Reker D, Rigler S, Duncan PW (2006) Therapeutic
exercise and depressive symptoms after stroke. J Am Geriatr Soc 54:240–247
Lamontagne A, Malouin F, Richards CL (2001) Locomotor-specific measure of spasticity of
plantarflexor muscles after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 82:1696–1704
Lance JW (1980) Pathophysiology of spasticity and clinical experience with baclofen. In:
Feldman RG, Young RR, Koella WP (eds) Spasticity: disordered motor. Year Book Medical
Publisher Control, Chicago, pp 185–220
Lashley KS (1951) The problem of serial order in behavior. In: Cerebral Mechanisms in Behavior.
Wiley, New York
Latash ML (2012) The bliss (not the problem) of motor abundance (not redundancy). Exp Brain
Res 217(1):1–5
Levin MF (1996) Interjoint coordination during pointing movements is disrupted in spastic
hemiparesis. Brain 119:281–294
Levin MF, Feldman AG (1994) The role of stretch reflex threshold regulation in normal and
impaired motor control. Brain Res 657:23–30
Levin MF, Selles RW, Verheul MH, Meijer OG (2000) Deficits in the coordination of agonist and
antagonist muscles in stroke patients: implications for normal motor control. Brain Res
853:352–369
Levin MF, Michaelsen S, Cirstea C, Roby-Brami A (2002) Use of the trunk for reaching targets
placed within and beyond the reach in adult hemiparesis. Exp Brain Res 143:171–180
Mayo NE, Mayo NE, Fellows LK, Scott SC et al (2009) A longitudinal view of apathy and its
impact after stroke. Stroke 40:3299–3307
McIntosh RD, Dijkerman HC, Mon-Williams M, Milner AD (2004) Grasping what is graspable:
evidence from visual form agnosia. Cortex 40:695–702
Michaelsen SM, Luta A, Roby-Brami A, Levin MF (2001) Effect of trunk restraint on the recovery
of reaching movements in hemiparetic patients. Stroke 32:1875–1883
Michaelsen SM, Jacobs S, Roby-Brami A, Levin MF (2004) Compensation for distal impairments
of grasping in adults with hemiparesis. Exp Brain Res 157:162–173
Michaelsen SM, Dannenbaum R, Levin MF (2006) Task-specific training with trunk restraint on
arm recovery in stroke: randomized control trial. Stroke 37:186–192
Mon-Williams M, McIntosh RD (2000) A test between two hypotheses and a possible third way
for the control of prehension. Exp Brain Res 134:268–273
Mon-Williams M, Tresilian JR, Coppard VL, Carson RG (2001) The effect of obstacle position on
reach-to-grasp movements. Exp Brain Res 137:497–501
Mullick AA, Musampa NK, Feldman AG, Levin MF (2013) Stretch reflex threshold measure
discriminates between spasticity and rigidity. Clin Neurophysiol 124(4):740–51
Musampa NK, Mathieu PA, Levin MF (2007) Relationship between stretch reflex thresholds and
voluntary arm muscle activation in patients with spasticity. Exp Brain Res 181:579–593
Platz T, Denzler P (2002) Do psychological variables modify motor recovery among patients with
mild arm paresis after stroke or traumatic brain injury who receive the Arm Ability Training?
Restor Neurol Neurosci 20:37–49
Rand D, Eng JJ (2012) Disparity between functional recovery and daily use of the upper and lower
extremities during subacute stroke rehabilitation. Neurorehabil Neural Rep 26:76–84
Rand D, Eng JJ (2015) Predicting daily use of the affected upper extremity 1 year after stroke.
Stroke Cerebrovasc Dis 24:274–283. doi:10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2014.07.039
Reisman DS, Scholz JP (2003) Aspects of joint coordination are preserved during pointing in
persons with post-stroke hemiparesis. Brain 126:2510–2527
Rice NJ, McIntosh RD, Schindler I, Mon-Williams M, Demonet JF, Milner AD (2006) Intact
automatic avoidance of obstacles in patients with visual form agnosia. Exp Brain Res 174:
176–188
Robinson-Smith G (2002) Self-efficacy and quality of life after stroke. J Neurosci Nursing 34:
91–98
Robinson-Smith G, Johnston MV, Allen J (2000) Self-care, self-efficacy, quality of life, and
depression after stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 81:460–464
Principles of Motor Recovery After Neurological Injury … 139
Won J, Hogan N (1995) Stability properties of human reaching movements. Exp Brain Res
107:125–136
World Health Organization (2001) International classification of functioning, disability and health.
World Health Organization, Geneva
Yang F, Feldman AG (2010) Reach-to-grasp movement as a minimization process. Exp Brain Res
201(1):75–92. doi:10.1007/s00221-009-2012-1
Zorowitz RD, Gillard PJ, Brainin M (2013) Poststroke spasticity: sequelae and burden on stroke
survivors and caregivers. Neurology 80:S45–S52
Part III
Neurophysiological Mechanisms of Motor
Control
What Do TMS-Evoked Motor Potentials
Tell Us About Motor Learning?
Abstract Thirty years ago, the first magnetic device capable of stimulating the
human brain without discomfort through the intact skull was unveiled in Sheffield,
England (Barker et al. in Lancet 1:1106–1107, 1985). Since that time, transcranial
magnetic stimulation (TMS) has become the tool of choice for many scientists
investigating human motor control and learning. In light of the fact that there are
limits to the information that can be provided by any experimental technique, we
first make the case that the necessarily restricted explanatory scope of the TMS
technique—and the motor-evoked potentials to which it gives rise, is not yet
reflected adequately in the research literature. We also argue that this inattention,
coupled with the pervasive adoption of TMS as an investigative tool, may be
restricting the elaboration of knowledge concerning the neural processes that
mediate human motor learning. In order to make these points, we use as an
exemplar the study of cross-education—the interlimb transfer of functional
capacity.
Keywords Transcranial Magnetic stimulation Interlimb transfer Functional
capacity Cross-education
Motor-evoked potentials MEP amplitude
Corticospinal Primary motor cortex
Within the field of motor learning, there is a long tradition of employing metaphors
derived from contemporary technological culture as a means to conceptualise the
work of the brain. Franklin Henry adopted the term memory drum—a magnetic
data storage device invented by Gustav Tauschek in 1932 and used as computer
memory throughout the 1950s and 1960s—to capture the sense of a stored program
moved to a temporary buffer and thereafter translated into motor commands (Henry
and Rogers 1960). In this respect, Henry was echoing Sherrington’s famously
poetic allusion to an enchanted (Jacquard) loom in describing the complex patterns
woven by the cerebral cortex as “the body is up and rises to meet its waking day”
(Sherrington 1942). While such analogies undoubtedly help convey ideas effec-
tively, there is a broader issue that concerns the ways in which technology may
constrain the generation of knowledge (Harrington 1992). In some cases, the
severity of the constraint may be such that hypothesis generation becomes biased.
In an era in which brain imaging has become the tour de force of cognitive neu-
roscience, we are not alone in noting that the application of these technologies
imposes limits (often inadvertently) on theory development and testing. Given tools
that map activity in specific brain regions, the tendency has been to proceed with
model building on the basis that each has its fundamental computation (Anderson
2014). With the contemporary development of imaging technologies and analytical
methods that permit estimates of structural and functional brain connectivity, the-
ories that encapsulate the integration (of neural information) arising from generic
properties of parallel, distributed networks receive fresh impetus (e.g. Sporns 2011).
As each new technology creates its own constituency, there emerges the associated
risk that the technology begins to run the science (Fodor 1999).
Thirty years ago, the first magnetic device capable of stimulating the human
brain without discomfort through the intact skull was unveiled in Sheffield, England
(Barker et al. 1985). Since that time, transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) has
become the tool of choice for many scientists investigating human behaviour,
particularly those for whom their interest extends to the neural processes that
mediate motor control and learning. Among the proliferation of empirical reports
based on the application of this technique, there are many that have made truly
novel contributions to our knowledge of the human motor system. Nonetheless,
there are limits to the information that can be provided by any experimental
technique. The thesis to be developed in the present chapter has two key elements.
Our first point is that the restricted explanatory scope of the TMS technique—and
the motor-evoked potentials to which it gives rise—is seldom reflected in the
research literature. We furthermore contend that this inattention, coupled with the
pervasive adoption of TMS as an investigative tool, is restricting the elaboration of
knowledge concerning the neural processes that mediate human motor learning.
With a view to presenting our case succinctly, we will not attempt to be com-
prehensive with respect to the wide variety of contexts in which motor learning is
expressed, or consider the multitude of paradigms that have been used in its
What Do TMS-Evoked Motor Potentials Tell Us About Motor Learning? 145
Motor-Evoked Potentials
As there are authoritative and comprehensive reviews dealing with the biophysics
and neurophysiology of TMS (e.g. Di Lazzaro and Rothwell 2014; Lemon et al.
2002; Rothwell 1997; Ziemann and Rothwell 2000), we hereby provide only a
summary pertinent to our present purpose that draws in part upon these previous
works. The essence of the technology is that a brief electric current is passed
through a conducting coil (positioned over the scalp), creating a transient
(*200 μs) magnetic field which in turn induces an eddy current in the underlying
cortical tissue (Fig. 1). The induced electrical current tends to flow parallel to the
(a) (b)
(c) (d)
Fig. 1 Schematic of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS). Panel a depicts the TMS coil
placed over the scalp. The current direction generated in the cortex is opposite in direction to that
produced by the coil. b The magnetic field generated by the coil is perpendicular to the current
direction. It traverses skull and other layers between the scalp and cortex, producing an electrical
current opposite in direction to the original coil current. This generates action potentials in
horizontal interneurons synapsing on pyramidal neurons and thus activates the corticospinal tract.
The descending corticospinal tract then synapses on to spinal motor neurons, carrying the signal to
the muscle. c Shows the electromyographic (EMG) recording of the resultant muscle activity via
surface electrodes as the corticospinal volley reaches the periphery. d Shows the amplified EMG
signal as a stereotypic motor-evoked potential (MEP). (Adapted from Brown et al. 2014)
146 R.G. Carson et al.
plane of the TMS coil, and to the horizontal fibres in superficial layers of cortex
(Terao and Ugawa 2002). If the induced current is of sufficient magnitude, cortical
neurons are depolarised and action potentials generated. When the coil is positioned
over the primary motor cortex (M1), pyramidal tract neurons may be stimulated
directly or indirectly, giving rise to a descending corticospinal volley. In some
circumstances, this volley may result in the depolarization of spinal motoneurons,
which can be registered as compound motor action potentials in electromyographic
recordings of skeletal muscle. The short latency component (e.g. 12.9–21.6 ms
LM: latero-medial
I1
Current induced in brain
I1
Low intensity
I1
Intermediate
5uV
PA: posterior-anterior intensity
I1 5ms
High intensity
Late I waves
20uV
AP: anterior-posterior
5ms
PA (late I-waves)
L5
PA (I1)
AP-PA, proximal D wave
Anodal electrical stimulation. LM TMS (D wave)
What Do TMS-Evoked Motor Potentials Tell Us About Motor Learning? 147
onset in flexor carpi radialis (FCR) motor units, Olivier et al. 1995) of this response
is typically referred to as a motor-evoked potential (MEP).
While reasonably taken to reflect the excitability of the corticospinal tract, the
MEP is a compound measure consisting of several descending waves (Fig. 2) that
can be discriminated in epidural recordings (Di Lazzaro et al. 2004). The long-
standing interpretation is that TMS over M1 activates chains of interneurons with
fixed temporal characteristics that produce a periodic bombardment of corticospinal
neurons (Amassian et al. 1987). When a figure-of-eight shaped stimulating coil is
used, and current at the junction of the coil windings flows initially from an
anteromedial to latero-posterior direction, the successive components of
motor-evoked potentials elicited at (“threshold”) intensities required to generate a
descending volley are thought to first reflect the activation of cortico-cortical axons
projecting onto corticospinal neurons, or axon collaterals of corticofugal systems
[e.g. motor areas such as dorsal premotor cortex (PMd), dorsal premotor cortex
(PMv) and supplementary motor area (SMA)] with corresponding projections, i.e.
onto corticospinal neurons (Di Lazzaro et al. 2008), and subsequently transmission
via polysynaptic networks or recurrent synaptic networks (cf. Rusu et al. 2014). As
148 R.G. Carson et al.
Variations in the amplitude of the MEP have been used frequently to draw infer-
ences concerning the contribution of the primary motor cortex to processes
underlying motor learning, in the context of a wide range of behavioural paradigms
What Do TMS-Evoked Motor Potentials Tell Us About Motor Learning? 149
(e.g. Lotze et al. 2003; Muellbacher et al. 2001; Pascual-Leone et al. 1994, 1995).
Should the basis for such inferences be valid, the monitoring of MEPs would appear
to offer particular promise if it can be used to discriminate differences in the level of
engagement of specific circuits local to M1 as learning evolves. The most influential
experimental reports have tended to suggest that in humans, corticospinal
excitability (CSE) is elevated during and following the completion of training
regimes that give rise to an expression of motor learning (Muellbacher et al. 2001;
Pascual-Leone et al. 1994, 1995; Perez et al. 2004). Nonetheless, the opposite
pattern of change, i.e. decreases in MEP amplitude during motor skill learning, has
also been documented (Coxon et al. 2014; Holland et al. 2015). This contrariety
alone suggests that generalisations concerning the relationship between changes in
MEP amplitude and the processes of motor learning (particularly when considered
across task contexts) should be made with caution. There is however a more
substantial concern. While it may be the case that performance of the tasks that give
rise to motor learning is accompanied by systematic and reliable changes in CSE,
evidence of an instrumental relationship between the degree of change in CSE, and
the learning that accrues to an individual, is extremely sparse (Ljubisavljevic 2006).
Among the small proportion of the most prominent studies that have included
measures of association, null findings are the norm (e.g. Liepert et al. 1999).
Although positive correlations between changes in MEP amplitude and short-term
improvements in performance have on occasion been noted (e.g. Muellbacher et al.
2001), we are aware of only a few instances in which retention or transfer tests have
been employed to consider the potential presence of a relationship. Even in studies
in which “follow-up” measurements were obtained, this step is seldom taken (e.g.
Muellbacher et al. 2001). A conspicuous exception in this regard is the positive
finding reported by Lundbye-Jensen et al. (2005). In a carefully crafted investi-
gation, their participants were required to perform 16 min of visuomotor tracking
(by means of elbow flexion-extension movements), in each of 13 training sessions
conducted over a period of 4 weeks. Increases in the maximum amplitude of the
MEP that could be produced by TMS, and decreases in the MEP threshold over this
period (both measures being obtained prior to the training movements), were cor-
related with the retained improvements in performance.
Within the sub-domain of motor learning in which the transfer of acquired
capacity has been the focus of interest, and in particular those investigations in
which interlimb transfer has been examined, measures of association (i.e. between
changes in MEP amplitude and accrued improvements in performance) have been
generated more diligently. This corpus of work therefore provides a helpful basis
upon which to examine critically the assumption that variations in MEPs provide an
index of processes that are instrumentally related to motor skill acquisition.
150 R.G. Carson et al.
Cross-education is the process whereby training of one limb gives rise to increases
in the subsequent performance of its opposite counterpart. Motor learning thus
‘transfers’ from a trained to an untrained effector following a period of practice that
is exclusively unilateral. In view of the fact that the execution of many unilateral
tasks is associated with increased excitability of corticospinal projections to the
opposite limb (Carson et al. 2004; Hortobagyi et al. 2003), it has been proposed that
interlimb transfer of training-induced performance gains may be subserved by
interactions between the primary motor cortices (e.g. Hinder et al. 2011). The more
general conjecture is that bilateral cortical activity generated during unilateral
training drives concurrent neural adaptations in both cerebral hemispheres
(Hellebrandt 1951). This has been dubbed as the ‘cross-activation’ hypothesis (e.g.
Lee et al. 2010) (Fig. 3).
As a consequence, there are a number of studies that have been conducted on the
basis of the (sometimes implicit) assumption that functional adaptations within the
M1 that provides the (majority of) corticospinal projections to the untrained
(transfer) limb, should be evident as increases in the amplitude of MEPs following
completion of the training regime. In the context of ballistic tasks in which
short-term unilateral practice of finger or thumb movements brings about bilateral
increases in movement velocity or acceleration, rises in the excitability of corti-
cospinal projections to the muscles of the untrained limb have indeed been reported "Trained" limb
"Untrained" hemisphere
"Untrained" limb
"Trained" hemisphere
(Carroll et al. 2008; Lee et al. 2010; Hinder et al. 2011; Poh et al. 2013; Dickins
et al. 2015; Reissig et al. 2015). This does not however constitute evidence that any
such changes are instrumentally related to the level of interlimb transfer (Ruddy and
Carson 2013). It has been highlighted, for example, that increases in corticospinal
excitability (CSE) can also be present for projections to homologues of muscles that
do not make a direct mechanical contribution to the action that is trained (Carroll
et al. 2008).
In many of the studies cited above, however, the investigators also took the
trouble to calculate measures of association (typically correlation coefficients) to
determine whether a relationship existed between variations in the degree of
cross-education expressed across participants, and the extent to which elevations in
the excitability of corticospinal projections to the muscles of the untrained limb
were manifested. In all of the instances of which we are aware (Carroll et al. 2008;
Hinder et al. 2011; Dickins et al. 2015; Reissig et al. 2015), null findings were
reported. This was also the case for a recent study conducted in our laboratory
(Ruddy et al. 2016). In circumstances in which the performance (peak acceleration)
of the training limb increased by more than 40 %, and the median level of interlimb
transfer was greater than 80 %, there was no association with individual changes in
the amplitude of MEPs recorded in the transfer limb. In short, with exception of
circumstances in which augmented visual feedback has been provided (Nojima
et al. 2012), there is very little evidence to indicate that changes in MEP amplitude
—at least when obtained at rest (cf. Hortobágyi et al. 2011)—are predictive of the
level of learning exhibited by the transfer limb.
In the context of the particular ballistic training task we employed (Ruddy et al.
2016)—requiring wrist flexion movements, there was no net elevation in the
amplitude of MEPs measured in the untrained limb following training. Thus, a
substantial (≈35 %) improvement in the performance of a task that demanded
maximum engagement of the wrist flexor muscles was entirely dissociated from
variations in the excitability of corticospinal projections to one of the principal
agonists (FCR), as assessed using TMS. A finding of this nature is not without
precedent. Using a variant of the serial reaction-time task (SRTT), Perez et al.
(2007) noted that increases in the performance of the transfer hand were not
accompanied by changes in MEP amplitude (or threshold).
None of these observations precludes the instrumental involvement of other
circuits local to primary motor cortex in mediating interlimb transfer of learning.
Hortobágyi et al. (2011) noted that for a high intensity training task, the level of
cross-education was correlated (across participants) with changes in the
TMS-derived measure of interhemispheric inhibition (IHI)—which is elicited
through paired stimulation of the primary motor cortices. Perez et al. (2007)
reported that following unilateral SRTT training, there was a decrease in IHI from
the M1 contralateral to the training limb, to the M1 contralateral to the transfer limb.
The extent of this decrease was correlated with the amount of non-specific per-
formance transfer to the untrained limb. In the SRTT, this is typically expressed as
decreased reaction times in all aspects of the task, including random blocks that
have no sequential component (Robertson 2007).
152 R.G. Carson et al.
By and large, in the studies referred to above, the magnetic stimulating coil has
been oriented such that current in the coil flows initially in an anteromedial to
latero-posterior direction. The current induced in the brain thus moves in the
opposite direction—from latero-posterior to anteromedial [termed posterior-anterior
(PA)]. It is possible that learning-related changes in the excitability of intracortical
and transcortical networks of interneurons that synapse upon corticospinal neurons
are not readily detected with this configuration. Different populations of fibres are
likely to be excited by anterior–posterior (AP) as opposed to PA currents (Di
Lazzaro et al. 2001). Although the most likely neuronal site for activation by TMS
is at the fibre terminal, it is also possible that large afferent axons from premotor and
somatosensory areas may be especially sensitive to AP currents (Esser et al. 2005).
These fibres constitute the main cortical input to M1 (DeFelipe et al. 1986; Sutor
et al. 2000), entering in a bend approximately 0.5 mm away from layer 5 and
1.5 mm from layer 3 (Rockel et al. 1980).
A more general and critical issue is thus highlighted. In the event that associ-
ations can be shown between the variations in the magnitude of the evoked
response to AP currents, and the degree of cross-education that is expressed across
participants, these are unlikely to be amenable to interpretation in terms of changes
in the state of circuits local to M1. A similar point can be made with respect to
variations in IHI, and indeed in relation to other measures based on paired-pulse
TMS. The origins of the modulatory inputs that ultimately influence the magnitude
of the “test response”—whether to AP current stimulation or following a condi-
tioning stimulus—cannot readily be determined. In most cases, it is impossible to
exclude the involvement of areas other than M1.
Reflection
We are not seeking to imply that the primary motor cortex is not intimately
involved in processes underlying motor learning. Rather, our singular aim is to
demonstrate that changes in MEP amplitude cannot be relied upon as an unam-
biguous indication of variations in the state of specific circuits within M1, and that
there is very little empirical evidence to support the assumption that there exists an
instrumental relationship between modifications in the state of neural elements that
influence the size of the MEP, and the quality of motor learning. Perhaps most
obviously, the modifications of behaviour persist long after the changes in the
amplitude or threshold of the MEP have dissipated (Ljubisavljevic 2006).
The MEP is a compound measure of excitability that is influenced by multiple
neural elements. The majority of the short latency responses to TMS evoked in
healthy adults are mediated by large corticospinal neurons with fast-conducting
axons (Lemon et al. 2002). Slower conducting axons also make monosynaptic
connections with upper limb motoneurons (Porter and Lemon 1995) and it remains
possible that cortical circuits projecting onto these cells exhibit distinct patterns of
adaptation in the mediation of motor learning. In principle, a component of the
What Do TMS-Evoked Motor Potentials Tell Us About Motor Learning? 153
In our view, oft reported variations in MEP amplitude, whether obtained in the
context of interlimb transfer or other manifestations of motor learning, simply
indicate that at least one of the many elements of the motor system that can
influence corticospinal excitability has changed as an accompaniment. There is very
little empirical evidence to support the assumption that there exists an instrumental
relationship between modifications in the state of neural elements that influence the
size of the MEP, and the quality of motor learning. In the absence of further
information—whether derived from extensions of TMS or from complementary
methodologies, variations in MEP amplitude are in themselves entirely ambiguous
with respect to the engagement of specific circuits acting through primary motor
cortex.
154 R.G. Carson et al.
In spite of the fact that this much would be readily acknowledged by most
practitioners of the technique—at least when the points are made in such terms—it
remains the case that an observation of changes in MEP amplitude is most often the
initial finding upon which all subsequent avenues of investigation are predicated.
We believe that this widespread practice may account for the ambiguity and
inconsistency of the research literature as it concerns the outcomes of derivative
investigations in which such methods as paired-pulse TMS have been applied. The
consistent absence of associations in terms of these measures further corroborates
the view that the changes in MEP amplitude fail to exhibit a clear functional
relationship to the processes of adaptation that mediate improvements in perfor-
mance. We are forced to conclude that the motor-evoked potential generated by
single-pulse TMS is the wrong tool for the purpose of discriminating differences in
the level of engagement of specific circuits local to M1, as learning evolves.
Our further argument is that the fixation on TMS as a method of first choice,
coupled with interpretations of TMS outcome measures that lack sufficient con-
straint, has biased recent theorising in the sub-domain of cross-education in par-
ticular, and motor learning in general. We believe that this bias is towards models
that place unwarranted emphasis upon a limited set of the neural elements within
primary motor cortex that is recruited at rest by single-pulse TMS.
References
Amassian VE, Stewart M, Quirk GJ, Rosenthal JL (1987) Physiological basis of motor effects of a
transient stimulus to cerebral cortex. Neurosurgery 20(1):74–93
Anderson ML (2014) After phrenology: neural reuse and the interactive brain. MIT Press,
Cambridge, MA
Barker AT, Jalinous R, Freeston IL (1985) Non-invasive magnetic stimulation of human motor
cortex. Lancet 1(8437):1106–1107
Bestmann S, Krakauer, JW (2015) The uses and interpretations of the motor-evoked potential for
understanding behaviour. Exp Brain Res 233(3):679–689. http://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-014-
4183-7
Brown KE, Neva JL, Ledwell NM, Boyd LA (2014) Use of transcranial magnetic stimulation in
the treatment of selected movement disorders. Degenerative Neurol Neuromuscul Dis 4:
133–151. http://doi.org/10.2147/DNND.S70079
Carroll TJ, Lee M, Hsu M, Sayde J (2008) Unilateral practice of a ballistic movement
causes bilateral increases in performance and corticospinal excitability. J Appl Physiol 104:
1656–1664. doi:10.1152/japplphysiol.01351.2007
Carson R, Riek S, Mackey D, Meichenbaum D, Willms K, Forner M, Byblow W (2004)
Excitability changes in human forearm corticospinal projections and spinal reflex pathways
during rhythmic voluntary movement of the opposite limb. J Physiol-Lond 560(3):929–940
Coxon JP, Peat NM, Byblow WD (2014) Primary motor cortex disinhibition during motor skill
learning. J Neurophysiol 112(1):156–164. http://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00893.2013
DeFelipe J, Conley M, Jones EG (1986) Long-range focal collateralization of axons arising from
corticocortical cells in monkey sensory-motor cortex. J Neurosci 6(12):3749–3766
Di Lazzaro V, Oliviero A, Saturno E, Pilato F, Insola A, Mazzone P et al (2001) The effect on
corticospinal volleys of reversing the direction of current induced in the motor cortex by
What Do TMS-Evoked Motor Potentials Tell Us About Motor Learning? 155
Rothwell JC (1997) Techniques and mechanisms of action of transcranial stimulation of the human
motor cortex. J Neurosci Methods 74(2):113–122
Rothwell JC (2002) Spinal interneurones: re-evaluation and controversy. Adv Exp Med Biol
508:259–263
Ruddy KL, Carson RG (2013) Neural pathways mediating cross education of motor function.
Front Human Neurosci 7:397. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00397
Ruddy KL, Rudolf AK, Kalkman B, King M, Daffertshofer A, Carroll TJ, Carson RG (2016)
Neural adaptations associated with interlimb transfer in a ballistic wrist flexion task. Front
Human Neurosci. http://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00204
Rusu CV, Murakami M, Ziemann U, Triesch J (2014) A model of TMS-induced I-waves in motor
cortex. Brain Stimulation 7(3):401–414. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.brs.2014.02.009
Sherrington CS (1942) Man on his nature. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, p 178
Sporns O (2011) Networks of the brain. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA
Singh AM, Neva JL, Staines WR (2016) Aerobic exercise enhances neural correlates of motor skill
learning. Behav Brain Res 301:19–26. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2015.12.020
Sutor B, Schmolke C, Teubner B, Schirmer C, Willecke K (2000) Myelination defects and
neuronal hyperexcitability in the neocortex of connexin 32-deficient mice. Cereb Cortex
10(7):684–697
Taylor JL (2006) Stimulation at the cervicomedullary junction in human subjects. J Electromyogr
Kinesiol: Official J Int Soc Electrophysiological Kinesiol 16(3):215–223. http://doi.org/10.
1016/j.jelekin.2005.07.001
Terao Y, Ugawa Y (2002) Basic mechanisms of TMS. J Clin Neurophysiol 19(4):322
Ziemann U, Rothwell JC (2000) I-waves in motor cortex. J Clin Neurophysiol 17(4):397–405
Motor Control of Human Spinal Cord
Disconnected from the Brain and Under
External Movement
Abstract Motor control after spinal cord injury is strongly depending on residual
ascending and descending pathways across the lesion. The individually altered
neurophysiology is in general based on still intact sublesional control loops with
afferent sensory inputs linked via interneuron networks to efferent motor outputs.
Partial or total loss of translesional control inputs reduces and alters the ability to
perform voluntary movements and results in motor incomplete (residual voluntary
control of movement functions) or motor complete (no residual voluntary control)
spinal cord injury classification. Of particular importance are intact functionally
silent neural structures with residual brain influence but reduced state of excitability
that inhibits execution of voluntary movements. The condition is described by the
term discomplete spinal cord injury. There are strong evidences that artificial
afferent input, e.g., by epidural or noninvasive electrical stimulation of the lumbar
posterior roots, can elevate the state of excitability and thus re-enable or augment
voluntary movement functions. This modality can serve as a powerful assessment
technique for monitoring details of the residual function profile after spinal cord
injury, as a therapeutic tool for support of restoration of movement programs and as
a neuroprosthesis component augmenting and restoring movement functions, per se
or in synergy with classical neuromuscular or muscular electrical stimulation.
Keywords Injury Sublesional control Spinal reflex Motor unit EMG
Surface polielectromyography Discomplete Supraspinal control
Neurorehabilitation Functional electrical stimulation (FES)
Introduction
Features of the human spinal cord motor control can be described using surface
electrodes and the two spinal injury model: (i) the spinal cord completely separated
from brain motor structures by accidental injury models; (ii) the spinal cord
receiving reduced and altered supraspinal input due to an incomplete lesion.
Systematic studies using surface electrode polielectromyography, i.e., simultaneous
EMG recording from multiple muscle groups, were carried out to assess skeletal
reflex responses to a single and repetitive stimulation in a large number of subjects.
In complete spinal cord, the functional integrity of three different neuronal circuits
below the lesion level is demonstrated: first, simple monosynaptic and oligosy-
naptic reflex arcs and polysynaptic pathways; second, propriospinal interneuron
system with their cells in the gray, the axons in the white matter of the spinal cord,
conducting activity to different spinal cord segments; and third, internuncial gray
matter neurons with short axons and dense neurons in contact within spinal gray
matter. All of these three systems participate continuously in the generation of
spinal cord reflex output activating muscles. The integration of these systems and
the relative degree of excitation and setup produces characteristic functions of
motor control and depends on relative degree of excitation and setup produced
characteristic function of motor control. In incomplete spinal cord injury patients,
the implementation of brain motor control depends on the profile of residual brain
descending input and its integration with functional neuronal circuits below the
lesion. Locomotors patterns of this new structural relationship are expressed as an
alternative, but characteristic and consistent neurocontrol. The more we know about
how brain governs spinal cord networks, the better we can describe human motor
control. On the other hand, such knowledge for restoration of residual functions and
for construction of new cord circuitry is crucial to expend the function of injured
spinal cord (Dimitrijevic et al. 2005).
These observations highlight another long-standing issue as to the nature of a
motor complete injury (SCI). Some evidence suggests that most of the individuals
with clinically motor complete paralysis do not have a complete anatomical sepa-
ration of the proximal and distal ends of the spinal cord at the site of the lesion
(Kakulas 1987). The absence of a clear functional dichotomy of a spinally complete
and incomplete injury has been demonstrated to be dependent on the thoroughness
and procedures of the clinical examination. For example, in individuals that are
considered to be motor complete based on a standard clinical assessment, a more
detailed assessment can reveal some residual motor function (Kakulas and
Bedbrook 1976). Thus, from a functional perspective, there are probably gradations
of completeness of the injury that can easily remain undetected. Could remaining
Motor Control of Human Spinal Cord Disconnected … 161
descending axons that cross the lesion be ‘reawakened’, and if so, what is the
mechanism? Such a scenario would emphasize the importance of there being some
remaining anatomical continuity of axons across the lesion, even though they may
have no remaining functionally detectable synaptic function before beginning of
epidural stimulation. On the other hand, they could have some remaining functional
influence recognizable in clinical assessment. Perhaps, when voluntary control was
recovered in the presence of epidural stimulation, the stimulation modulated the
circuitry to a higher level of excitability, which allowed for some residual
descending motor input to exceed the motor threshold of some motor neurons.
However, even if this was the case there had to be some fundamental reorganization
of the supraspinal and/or spinal circuitry that occurred after months of stimulation
and training. Or was the underlying mechanism recovery of the supraspinal and/or
spinal circuitry that occurred after months of stimulation and training? Or was the
recovery of voluntary control attributable to a combination of stimulation and
repeated voluntary efforts generated simultaneously that induced axons to grow
through or around the lesion and make functional connections in the proximal
stump? If this did occur, then a remarkable ability of the descending axons to form
functional connections to interneurons (probably propriospinal neurons) that can
then generate coordinated movements would be rather impressive and certainly
encouraging.
The term “discomplete spinal cord injury” was proposed on the basis of apparently
isolated spinal cord in which clinical signs indicated complete motor paralysis,
whereas the neurophysiological evaluation documented the presence of spinal
reflex activity, or even volitionally induced facilitation of motor unit activity,
restoring augmentation of motor output (Dimitrijevic 1987; Sherwood et al. 1992).
Human neuroscientists, after epidural or transcutaneous posterior root stimulation
are speculating on posterior lumbosacral stimulation to result in “electro enabling”
motor control, when observing regain of voluntary control over single paralyzed
motor functions in selected cases during administration of continuing trains of
electrical stimuli (Edgerton and Harkema 2011).
By recording EMG activity from leg muscles, changes in segmental excitability of
the plantar reflex elicited by cutaneous stimulation of the plantar surface have been
studied. Using surface polielectromyography, 50 SCI patients were examined for their
ability to volitionally suppress the plantar reflexes by recording motor units activity
from their paralyzed muscles. The patients, who had no voluntary EMG activity in the
monitored muscles, were able to volitionally suppress the plantar reflex responses by
45 % in the tibialis anterior, hamstring, and triceps surae muscles and to suppress the
quadriceps response by 72 %. In this patient group, 73 of 100 tibialis anterior muscle
162 W. Mayr et al.
groups showed suppression of more than 20 % compared with the control response. On
reexamination, these findings were consistent during a period of 2 years in six patients
(Cioni et al. 1986). Thus, it is possible to volitionally suppress excitability in paralyzed
muscles below a complete spinal cord lesion.
The above observation supported the hypothesis that the absence of volitional
motor activity does not preclude the existence of CNS axons traversing the injury
site and influencing the activities below the level of the lesion.
We are proposing to use for this model the term “discomplete” which combines
the notion that in absence of motor control brain excitatory and inhibitory influence
on the nervous system below the level of the lesion is preserved.
Systematic studies of motor units activity from the lower thoracic, lumber and upper
sacral segments of the spinal cord during reflex, automatic and volitional motor tasks in
the chronic SCI subjects, led us to describe in this chapter a clinical neurophysiological
classification of spinal cord injury. Furthermore, we also reviewed supportive
anatomical evidence for the proposed clinical neurophysiological classification and we
especially addressed newly reported clinical-pathomorphological evidence for the
“discomplete” model of the human spinal cord.
In this study of the model of the human spinal cord with “reduced and altered motor
control,” we applied the identical approach used in the “discomplete” model. This
means that we carried out a study of the qualitative features of motor control recorded
by BMCA in subjects with residual volitional control of motor units in paretic and
paralyzed muscles below the spinal cord injury (Dimitrijevic et al. 1997). This “reduced
functional anatomy of spinal cord injury” is just the opposite of the “discomplete” SCI
model in which residual motor control is absent and is replaced by the brain’s facili-
tatory and suppressive influence on segmental motor output.
Results were obtained from 91 chronic SCI patients and 15 neurologically
healthy individuals. Of these 91, 23 were women and 68 were men, with ages
ranging from 11 to 77 years, with a mean of 33.6 ± 1.7 (mean ± SE). All suffered
from post-traumatic closed SCI between C2 and T10. The time between injury and
the recording of motor control ranged from 3 months to 27 years with a mean
duration of 78.5 ± 10.9 months. 21 of these 91 patients showed no signs of
volitional movement or volitionally induced motor unit activity below the level of
injury, 22 could move voluntarily but could not ambulate, and 48 were ambulatory
with a variety of assistive devices.
We analyzed the EMG patterns during gait and two discrete volitional motor
tasks in the supine position: the first consisted of volitional efforts to perform full
flexion and extension of the lower limbs (multi-joint movement) and the second
consisted of dorsal and plantar flexion of single ankle (single-joint) movements.
Motor Control of Human Spinal Cord Disconnected … 163
The main characteristic of integrated EMG activity during gait is the repetitive
pattern of burst discharges followed by silent periods. Another general feature of
EMG activity is that EMG bursts are lower amplitude and shorter duration in
proximal and trunk muscles than in the leg muscles. The third general feature is the
reciprocal relationship of bursts of EMG and activity between antagonistic leg and
thigh muscle groups during the stance and swing phases of gait. Bearing in mind
these general EMG features for the adult patient with an intact nervous system, it is
easy to recognize alterations in motor output (Dimitrijevic et al. 1997).
Unlike the semi-automatic and stereotyped motor control during steady gait, if
we record EMG activity in a SCI patient during a volitional motor task of single- or
multi-joint movements in the supine position, it is possible to expand observations
on motor control in paralyzed patients (Dimitrijevic et al. 2012).
We have found in EMG studies of motor control during volitional motor tasks
that there is a consistency in the different features of motor unit activity. This was
the case in 11 wheelchair-bound and 23 ambulatory patients. In this group of 11 and
23 SCI patients, we analyzed the percentage of the presence of volitional multi-joint
flexion, isolation of single- from multi-joint movement, and isolation of agonist
from antagonist EMG activity. All 3 features indicate discrete motor control and are
observed more frequently in ambulatory patients than in the wheelchair-bound
population. On the other hand, diffuse patterns of motor output and delayed onset
are much more frequent in the wheelchair-bound than in the ambulatory group.
The fact that we can recognize different features of motor control during
single-joint and multi-joint volitional motor tasks and that these features are con-
sistent, repetitive, and related to the severity of the lesion led us to hypothesize that
a new organization of spinal circuits is established and that this new structural and
functional relationship can generate definite patterns of residual motor functions.
In incomplete SCI, the organization of brain motor control depends on the
profile of the residual brain descending system and its integration with the spinal
network. In the past, our understanding was that motor activity after incomplete SCI
was inconsistent and considered abnormal because it differed from normal motor
activity. However, we have shown that locomotor patterns in SCI people are
consistent. Thus, the functional properties of these newly established sensory motor
integration mechanisms are expressed in the repeatable neurophysiological char-
acteristics of motor unit activity during single- and multi-joint volitional tasks.
A thorough understanding of this new motor control will play an important role in
establishing the scientific basis for neurobiological interventions to restore impaired
SCI functions.
Human SCI models based on studies with surface polielectromyography
(BMCA) play an important role in assessing and understanding the underlying
mechanisms of newly developing protocols for the modifications of the already
established clinical output in chronic SCI subjects. An example of this is found in
locomotor training with or without FES for the improvement of walking speed and
capacity (Cardenas 2010).
Furthermore, the new spinal cord organization established as a result of spinal
cord injury can develop different potentials for neuroplasticity and, as a result of
164 W. Mayr et al.
this activity-dependent capacity, the CNS can respond by increasing motor per-
formance (Muir and Steeves 1997). Moreover, another factor, which will influence
the capacity of neuroplasticity will be the profile of residual supraspinal control.
Such residual profile can have components of postural, locomotor and volitional
activity, or only one of these components can prevail. It has been shown, for
instance, that interactive locomotor training using body weight support can improve
walking capabilities in patients and not change the capacity for volitional muscle
contraction (Wernig 2015).
Thus, the assessment of motor control in SCI subjects can reveal subclinical
brain influence below the level of the lesion in otherwise clinically complete SCI, or
it can describe and measure the relation between different components of residual
suprasegmental control in incomplete SCI patients. This new possibility will help in
defining clinical and laboratory inclusion criteria of SCI patients for a variety of
available restorative procedures and neurorehabilitation protocols (Dimitrijevic
et al. 2012).
The emphases in human reflex studies have been on reflex pathways, their latencies
and their reciprocal relation. Thus, repetitively elicited spinal reflex responses in
humans lacking brain motor control can reveal behavioral motor units modification
depending from rate of repetitive afferent stimulation. Epidural stimulation was
carried out with 4-polar linear or 16-polar array electrode configurations (Sayenko
et al. 2014).
action potentials (CMAPs) of short latency, which were approximately half that of
phasic stretch reflex latencies for the respective muscle groups. EMG amplitudes
were stimulus-strength dependent. Stimulation at 5–15 and 25–50 Hz elicited
sustained tonic and rhythmic activity, respectively, and initiated lower limb
extension or stepping-like movements representing different levels of muscle syn-
ergies. All EMG responses, even during burst-style phases were composed of
separate stimulus-triggered CMAPs with characteristic amplitude modulations.
During burst-style phases, a significant increase of CMAP latencies by about 10 ms
was observed.
Angeli et al. (2014) point to a previous report on one individual who had a motor
complete, but sensory incomplete spinal cord injury regained voluntary movement
after 7 months of epidural stimulation and stand training, where they had presumed
that the residual sensory pathways were critical in this recovery. In the same article,
they now reported on three more individuals, where voluntary movement occurred
with epidural stimulation immediately after implant even in two who were diag-
nosed with a motor and sensory complete lesion. They conclude that neuromodu-
lating the spinal circuitry with epidural stimulation, enables completely paralyzed
individuals to process conceptual, auditory, and visual input to regain relatively fine
voluntary control of paralyzed muscles. Further they assume that neuromodulation
of the sub-threshold motor state of excitability of the lumbosacral spinal networks
was the key to recovery of intentional movement in four of four individuals
diagnosed as having complete paralysis of the legs. Finally, they claim to have
uncovered a fundamentally new intervention strategy that can dramatically affect
recovery of voluntary movement in individuals with complete paralysis even years
after injury.
In a recent review article, Grecco et al. (2015) analyze the state of the art of
transcutaneous spinal cord stimulation as therapeutic option after spinal cord injury.
Though they still see limited effects in treatments for spinal cord injury, they found
evidences that electrical stimulation might facilitate plastic changes in affected
spinal circuitries that may be beneficial in improving motor function and spasticity,
and reducing SCI-related neuropathic pain. They critically reviewed the physio-
logical basis and therapeutic action of transcutaneous spinal cord stimulation in
SCI, based on available animal and clinical studies. They analyzed the literature
published on PubMed to date, looking for the role of three main noninvasive
stimulation techniques in the recovery process of SCI and focusing mainly on
transcutaneous spinal stimulation. This review discusses the main clinical appli-
cations, latest advances, and limitations of noninvasive electrical stimulation of the
spinal cord. Although most recent research in this topic has focused on transcuta-
neous spinal direct current stimulation (tsDCS), they also reviewed the technique of
potential methods to modulate spinal cord plasticity. They also developed a finite
element method (FEM) model to predict current flow in the spinal cord when using
different electrode configurations. Identifying gaps in the knowledge on noninva-
sive electrical stimulation for modulation of spinal neuronal networks in patients
with SCI they nevertheless conclude that their is encouraging preliminary evidence
that tsDCS, TENS, and NMES have a positive influence on the promotion of
Motor Control of Human Spinal Cord Disconnected … 167
plasticity in SCI. They see FEMs useful to predict how in tsDCS electrode size and
placement can be used to shape, which spinal segments are modulated and what
detailed areas of spinal anatomy can concentrate current density (e.g., spinal roots).
In conclusion, they see tsDCS as a technique that can influence conduction along
ascending tracts in the spinal cord, so could modulate supraspinal activity.
In their review on technical aspects and therapeutic applications Nardone et al.
(2015) discuss instrumentation for noninvasive spinal cord stimulation. Electrical and
magnetic trans-spinal stimulation can be used to increase the motor output of multiple
spinal segments and modulate cortico-spinal excitability. The application of direct
current through the scalp as well as repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation are
known to influence brain excitability, and hence can also modulate other central ner-
vous system structures including spinal cord. They conclude that noninvasive electrical
and magnetic spinal stimulation may provide reliable means to characterize important
neurophysiologic and pathophysiologic aspects of spinal cord function. Moreover,
transcutaneous direct current stimulation and repetitive magnetic stimulation may hold
therapeutic promise in patients with spinal cord disorders, although future
well-controlled studies are needed to corroborate and extend the preliminary findings.
Even though there is growing evidence from clinical studies that tsDCS could
serve as an effective therapeutic tool for influencing central state of excitability in
the central nervous system, the method raises safety issues that need more attention.
There are non-negligible risks for electrochemical tissue damage at electrode tissue
interfaces, in particular if DC is administered, which needs systematic investiga-
tions and, even more concerning, axon damage under application of similarly low
direct current, as applied in clinical tsDCS, has been reported in a recent animal
study (Ravid and Prochazka 2012).
Clinical studies of FES in subjects with upper motor neuron dysfunction revealed
that in addition to the stimulation of motor fibers of peripheral nerves for “cor-
rection” of central paresis (Mayr 2015). Functional Electrical Stimulation (FES) can
be a versatile tool for support of movement rehabilitation. On basis of noninvasive
(electrodes at the skin surface) or invasive (implanted electrodes) techniques there
are three options for reactivation of lost muscle functions:
(1) Stimulation of efferent nerves—neuromuscular stimulation—as established
method,
(2) Direct activation of muscle fibers—muscle stimulation—in absence of the
motor nerve, which can lead to similar contraction responses, and
(3) Stimulation of afferent nerve structures, which can lead to improvement in
impaired movement patterns by artificially substitution of missing neural
168 W. Mayr et al.
How movements are controlled is just one of many questions and influencing
factors. Of equal importance is how we maintain stability of the motor system. In
abnormal conditions, acquired skills might be disrupted by in part missing feedback
and actuation components as disturbing factors. Learning of compensatory skills
takes time and is strongly depending on autonomous and heteronomous circum-
stances and limited influence on their manifestation.
Besides individually altered physiology, time-dependent due to regenerative and
degenerative processes, and personal live situation and motivation, multiple factors
of rehabilitation environment and conditions are acting and decisive for develop-
ment and outcome. It begins with level of analysis, which is carried by computa-
tional level, proceedings level, and implementation.
Research on human motor control relies on various fields contributing as neu-
rophysiology, medicine, biology, engineering, physics, statistics, behavioral sci-
ence, and cognitive and human factors. Detailed physiological foundations are
essential, e.g., on muscle spindle, Golgi tendon organ, joint receptors,
cutaneous-receptors, spinal reflexes, servo theory, Alfa-Gama coactivator, recurrent
inhibition, reciprocal inhibition, smart spinal cord, tuning of spinal cord, and many
more to be understood and holistically considered.
By decreasing the excitability of spinal reflexes with application of electrical and
magnetic trains, spinal stimulation could be helpful in normalizing reflexes and
treating hypertonia in subject with lesion in the upper motor neuron.
Transcutaneous spinal cord stimulation can influence ascending and descending
pathways, spinal reflex excitability, and functional neurotrophic changes. In addi-
tion, peripheral transcutaneous stimulation can be used to target the spinal cord, and
provide specific afferent inputs; many have potential for neuromodulation in
cord-injured subjects although future well-controlled studies are needed to cor-
roborate and extend the preliminary findings.
170 W. Mayr et al.
Let us finally mention that the review showed that only few reported, eight
studied subjects, on safety or complications of the spinal cord stimulation
(SCS) (Bendersky et al. 2014; Benfield et al. 2016; Walsh et al. 2015). There is an
urgent need for such studies even in the widely applied application of SCS for pain
treatment (Cameron 2004).
References
Angeli CA, Edgerton VR, Gerasimenko YP, Harkema SJ (2014) Altering spinal cord
excitability enables voluntary movements after chronic complete paralysis in humans. Brain
137(Pt 5):1394–1409. And: Angeli C, Edgerton VR, Gerasimenko Y, Harkema SJ (2015)
Reply: no dawn yet of a new age in spinal cord rehabilitation. Brain 138(Pt 7):e363
Bendersky D, Claudio Yampolsky C (2014) Is spinal cord stimulation safe? A review of its
complications. World Neurosurg 82:1359–1368
Benfield J, Maknojia A, Epstein F (2016) Progressive paraplegia from spinal cord stimulator Lead
fibrotic encapsulation. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 95:e30–e33
Cameron T (2004) Safety and efficacy of spinal cord stimulation for the treatment of chronic pain:
a 20-year literature review. J Neurosurg: Spine 100(3):254–267
Cardenas D (2010) Spinal cord medicine, second edition: principles & practice, 2nd edn. Demos
Medical Publishing. ISBN-10: 1933864192
Cioni B, Dimitrijevic MR, McKay WB, Sherwood AM (1986) Voluntary supraspinal suppression
of spinal reflex activity in paralyzed muscles of spinal cord injury patients. Exp Neurol 93
(3):574–583
Dimitrijevic MR (1987) Neurophysiology in spinal cord injury. Paraplegia 25(3):205–208
Dimitrijevic MR, Gerasimenko Y, Pollo FE (1997) Sustained epidural electrical stimulation of the
spinalized lumbar cord in humans can facilitate or suppress spinal reflex and central pattern
generator activities. In: Gurfinkel VS, Levik Yu S (eds) Proceedings of the international
symposium on brain and movement, vol 60. St. Petersburg, Moscow
Dimitrijevic MR, Persy I, Forstner C, Kern H, Dimitrijevic MM (2005) Motor control in the
human spinal cord. Artif Organs 29(3):216–219
Dimitrijevic, MR, Kakulas BA, McKay WB, Vrbová G (eds) (2012) Restorative neurology of
spinal cord injury. Oxford University Press, NY, 336 pp. ISBN: 978-0-19-974650-7
Edgerton VR, Harkema S (2011) Epidural stimulation of the spinal cord in spinal cord injury:
current status and future challenges. Expert Rev Neurother 11(10):1351–1353
Motor Control of Human Spinal Cord Disconnected … 171
Fisher LE, Miller ME, Bailey SN, Davis JA Jr, Anderson JS, Rhode L, Tyler DJ, Triolo RJ (2008)
Standing after spinal cord injury with four-contact nerve-cuff electrodes for quadriceps
stimulation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 16(5):473–478
Grecco LH, Li S, Michel S, Castillo-Saavedra L, Mourdoukoutas A, Bikson M, Fregni F (2015)
Transcutaneous spinal stimulation as a therapeutic strategy for spinal cord injury: state of the
art. J Neurorestoratology 3:73–82
Jilge B, Minassian K, Rattay F, Pinter MM, Gerstenbrand F, Binder H, Dimitrijevic MR (2004)
Initiating extension of the lower limbs in subjects with complete spinal cord injury by epidural
lumbar cord stimulation. Exp Brain Res 154:308–326
Kakulas BA, Bedbrook GM (1976) Injuries of the spine and spinal cord. In: Vinken PJ, Bruyn GW
(eds) Handbook of clinical neurology, Chap. 3. North Holland Publishing Company,
Amsterdam, pp 27–42
Kakulas BA (1987) The clinical neuropathology of spinal cord injury. A guide to the future.
Paraplegia 25(3):212–216
Sherwood AM, Dimitrijevic MR, McKay WB (1992) Evidence of subclinical brain influence in
clinically complete spinal cord injury: discomplete SCI. J Neurol Sci 110(1–2):90–98
Mayr W, Bijak M, Girsch W, Holle J, Lanmüller H, Thoma H, Zrunek M (1993) Multichannel
stimulation of phrenic nerves by epineural electrodes. Clinical experience and future
developments. ASAIO J 39:M729–M735
Mayr W, Bijak M, Rafolt D, Sauermann S, Unger E, Lanmüller H (2001) Basic design and
construction of the Vienna FES implants: existing solutions and prospects for new generations
of implants. Med Eng Phys 23:53–60
Mayr W, Hofer C, Bijak M, Rafolt D, Unger E, Sauermann S, Lanmueller H, Kern H (2002)
Functional electrical stimulation (FES) of denervated muscles: existing and prospective
technological solutions. Basic Appl Myol 12:287
Mayr W (2015) Early and contemporary approaches for application of FES in movement
rehabilitation after spinal cord injury. In: József Laczkó (ed) Progress in motor control X,
Budapest July 22–25, 2015; International Society of Motor Control; Hungarian Sports Science
Booklets—XII; p 43. ISBN: 978-615-5187-07-0
Minassian K, Jilge B, Rattay F, Pinter MM, Binder H, Gerstenbrand F, MR Dimitrijevic MR
(2004) Stepping-like movements in humans with complete spinal cord injury induced by
epidural stimulation of the lumbar cord: electromyographic study of compound muscle action
potentials. Spinal Cord 42(7):401–416
Muir GD, Steeves JD (1997) Sensorimotor stimulation to improve locomotor recovery after spinal
cord injury. Trends Neurosci 20(2):72–77
Nardone R, Höller Y, Taylor A, Thomschewski A, Orioli A, Frey V, Trinka E, Brigo F (2015)
Noninvasive spinal cord stimulation: technical aspects and therapeutic applications.
Neuromodulation 18(7):580–591 (discussion 590-1)
Ravid N, Prochazka A (2012) Nerve lesioning with direct current. In: Proceedings of 17th annual
IFESS conference, Banff, Canada; pp 344–347, 9–12 Sept 2012
Rejc E, Angeli C, Harkema S (2015) Effects of lumbosacral spinal cord epidural stimulation for
standing after chronic complete paralysis in humans. PLoS ONE 10(7):e0133998
Sayenko DG, Angeli C, Harkema SJ, Edgerton VR, Gerasimenko YP (2014) Neuromodulation of
evoked muscle potentials induced by epidural spinal-cord stimulation in paralyzed individuals.
J Neurophysiol 111(5):1088–1099
Walsh KM, Machado AG, Krishnaney AA (2015) Spinal cord stimulation: a review of the safety
literature and proposal for perioperative evaluation and management. Spine J 15:1864–1869
Wernig A (2015) Response to ‘Randomised controlled trials do not always give the results we
want but that doesn’t mean we should abandon randomised controlled trials’. Spinal Cord
53(12):897–898
Anticipation in Object Manipulation:
Behavioral and Neural Correlates
Abstract One way to foster the understanding of the impressively skilled fine
motor control of human object manipulation is to investigate the rules of the
underlying fundamental mechanisms. Examining the action of grasping and lifting
objects of various characteristics reveals that one vital step to handle objects in a
skilled and fluent way is to predict their properties and plan motor actions
accordingly. A large number of behavioral studies have extracted the factors on
which we rely when we interact with objects in an anticipatory mode of action. At
the same time, considerable effort was taken to elucidate the neuroanatomical areas
and networks involved and crucial for anticipatory behavior by conducting func-
tional imaging and stimulation studies and examining the deficits of patients with
localized brain damage. This chapter gives an overview of these studies and tries to
reconcile their findings, in order to provide an insight into the basic principles of
anticipatory motor control and their underlying neural substrates.
Keywords Torques Lift-off Tilts Economic grip Anticipatory control
Load force Object dynamics Sensorimotor memory
Introduction
The ability to dexterously grasp and manipulate a vast variety of objects and tools is
one of the most evident and remarkable skills of humans. One vital step of skilled
interaction with objects is to incorporate the physical properties of the objects into
the planning of the manipulation. Anticipatory planning of upcoming grasps in a
feedforward manner allows us to fluently and economically lift objects according to
task requests and our intentions.
In successfully programmed lifting trials the grip and load force rates form a
smooth, one-peaked, bell-shaped trajectory during the load phase (Johansson and
Westling 1988). Their peaks occur well before lift-off (thus in the absence of
sensory feedback about weight) and have proven to be valid and highly sensitive
indicators of the anticipation of object weight. When lifting an unexpectedly light
object, the load force exceeds gravity too early during still increasing force rates,
which leads to overshooting object loading with inappropriately high and late force
peaks. On the other hand, lifting an unexpectedly heavy object leads to an initially
insufficient force increase necessitating further force increments, which are trig-
gered by the absence of the expected lift-off (Johansson and Westling 1984, 1987).
Besides the above-described task aspects of smoothly lifting objects in a stable,
yet force economic grip, it is also vital to control object orientation after lift-off by
preventing tilts. To that aim subjects must predictively exert torques to compensate
for external torques emerging at lift-off due to an asymmetric object weight dis-
tribution. The exerted torque is hereby the net product of the selected finger
positioning, the exerted GFs and the sharing patterns of load forces between
opposing fingers (Fu et al. 2010).
Sensorimotor Memory
During the repeated grasping and lifting of objects, the central nervous system
encodes and stores information about a variety of experienced object characteristics
(e.g., friction, weight, mass distribution), as well as the motor effort employed to
manipulate them. This so-called sensorimotor memory has a strong impact on the
anticipatory force planning of following trials.
The best way to experimentally address questions concerning sensorimotor
memory processes distinctly is to have subjects lift neutral objects that do not
provide cues allowing to predict physical properties. It has repeatedly been shown
that one trial is often sufficient to establish a sensorimotor memory, which reflects
the experienced friction, grip shape, weight, and mass distribution of the preceding
lift of the object. This allows for rapid trial-by-trial adaptation to changes of object
properties. When participants repeatedly lift an unaltered object, they usually refine
these memory traces within two to three lifts and do not improve significantly
Anticipation in Object Manipulation … 177
thereafter (Johansson and Westling 1984, 1988; Jenmalm and Johansson 1997;
Salimi et al. 2000; Fu et al. 2010).
In order to detect the brain regions involved in encoding, storing and recalling
these sensorimotor memory traces, neuroimaging studies employed fMRI and TMS
setups, which allowed testing the grasping and lifting of unseen objects.
Functional MRI studies revealed a large network of sensorimotor areas involved in
this basic task (Schmitz et al. 2005; Jenmalm et al. 2006; Ehrsson et al. 2007).
During grasping and lifting with the right hand the left contralateral primary
somatosensory and motor cortices (S1/M1), the left dorsal premotor cortex (PMd),
the left parietal operculum, the supplementary motor area (SMA), areas close to the
left lateral fissure, and the right and left cerebellum were activated (Schmitz et al.
2005). Lifting a sequence of objects with varying weights induced additional
activations in the right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG), the left parietal operculum, and
the right supramarginal gyrus (SMG). Lifts of unexpectedly heavy or light objects
in a lifting sequence triggered activity in right SMG, in the contralateral primary
sensorimotor cortex and the ipsilateral cerebellum (Jenmalm et al. 2006). Recent
evidence suggests that also object perception areas in the ventral stream code
information about object weight during lifts of constant objects. In particular,
multivoxel pattern analysis of fMRI data differentiated BOLD activity during
planning lifts of light and heavy objects in object-sensitive regions within the
occipitotemporal cortex, in addition to the expected M1 and PMd responses
(Gallivan et al. 2014).
TMS studies affirmed that primary motor cortex and cortico-spinal system are
not only crucial for finger force generation but also play an important role in the
storage and recall of object-related sensorimotor memory modulating force plan-
ning. Motor evoked potential in hand muscles following a single pulse of tran-
scranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) applied over M1 just before a lifting
movement reflects the weight of the previous lift if there are no other weight cues
(Loh et al. 2010). Additionally, stimulation of the primary motor cortex with per-
turbation protocols seems to deteriorate the memory of object characteristics
obtained in the preceding lift (Chouinard et al. 2005; Nowak et al. 2005; Berner
et al. 2007). Moreover, stimulation of numerous brain regions of the so-called
grasping network apart from M1 effects force scaling. Inhibitory stimulation of the
ventral premotor cortex (PMv) also changes the influence of the last object’s weight
on force scaling (Dafotakis et al. 2008). In addition, stimulation of the left anterior
intraparietal area (AIP), which is strongly involved in object manipulation (Davare
et al. 2011) as well as the stimulation of the left SMA affects the force scaling of
both hands (Davare et al. 2007; White et al. 2013).
Intactness of peripheral sensibility and central sensorimotor integration of tactile
inputs are essential to grasp and lift objects in an energy efficient, economic way by
exerting GFs just high enough to prevent object slippage, and to establish and
utilize sensorimotor representations of previous lifts. Healthy adults initially exert
GFs only about 20 % above the minimum necessary to prevent slippage and further
decrease this safety margin across subsequent trials (Johansson and Westling 1987).
178 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
In contrast, patients suffering from middle cerebral artery (MCI) stroke generally
exhibit excessive GF levels and fail to adapt their force programming to given
surface-friction- and weight-properties when grasping objects with their slightly to
moderately paretic hand. These deficits correlate best with tests evaluating patient’s
surface discrimination and reactions to sensory perturbations (Hermsdörfer et al.
2003; Blennerhassett et al. 2007).
However, sensory loss does not seem to be the sole reason for uneconomic force
programming. Studies investigating anticipatory force control of the ipsilesional
hand of stroke patients suggested that patients suffer from a general force control
deficit, which also extends to the presumably unimpaired, ipsilesional hand. Despite
the absence of clinical sensory or motor deficits, GF levels were significantly
elevated when subjects lifted objects with the ipsilesional hand in a two-finger
precision grip (Quaney et al. 2005; Nowak et al. 2007a). The demonstration of
disrupted anticipatory control in the absence of sensory deficits and the possibility
of long lasting improvements after sensory augmentation interventions (Quaney
et al. 2010; Hsu et al. 2012) are highly suggestive of the existence of a central
sensorimotor integration deficit, causing clumsy motor control of stroke patients
even when using the ipsilesional hand (Sunderland et al. 1999; Schaefer et al.
2012). However, it has to be noted that studies employing a three-finger grip did not
detect elevated GF levels of the ipsilesional hand (Li et al. 2011; Eidenmüller et al.
2014; Buckingham et al. 2015).
Importantly, despite having difficulties in anticipatory force scaling, stroke
patients still generate grip and load forces in parallel to each other. Although some
studies revealed an slightly impaired temporal coupling between grip- and load
forces (Wenzelburger et al. 2005; Blennerhassett et al. 2006; McDonnell et al.
2006), the majority of stroke patients still programs GFs in a feedforward manner
anticipating load forces instead of reacting to them (Hermsdörfer et al. 2003;
Nowak et al. 2003). This shows that fundamental aspects of anticipatory control are
usually preserved after stroke. Different from MCI stroke, lesion of the cerebellum
frequently impair the parallel production of grip and load force (Rost et al. 2005;
Brandauer et al. 2008, 2010). This observation led to the conclusion that the
cerebellum may contribute importantly to the anticipatory control of GF in parallel
to self-produced loads (Nowak et al. 2007c). Neuroimaging paradigms supported
this notion (Kawato et al. 2003; Boecker et al. 2005). Evidence was however also
found in support of a contribution of parietal cortical areas and of the basal ganglia
(Ehrsson et al. 2003; Imbach et al. 2015).
Sensorimotor memories can be updated. Stacking previously lifted objects of
equal appearance but different weight upon each other, participants were able to
predict the weight of the novel, combined object and scale their force accordingly.
This suggests that internal models of object properties may be additive. Similarly,
(Chang et al. 2008) reported that subjects were capable of predictive force scaling
when lifting an object from the palm of the other hand. However, participants were
unable to predict the weight of a water-filled, previously lifted object after having
reduced its weight by drinking from it with a straw (Nowak and Hermsdörfer 2003).
Anticipation in Object Manipulation … 179
Due to these findings, it was proposed that direct haptic contact between the hand
and the object is indispensable to update sensorimotor memories (Nowak et al.
2013).
resulting in the same torque. These results point out that task-related sensorimotor
memories are not represented at the level of the fingers involved.
are only adequate if the weight remains the same. Switching to a lighter object leads
to overshooting force increases and an abrupt lifting movement, whereas after
switching to a heavier object several stepwise force increases and a longer load
phase are observed until lift-off occurs. Concordantly, the direction of initial
compensatory torques is determined by the previously encountered mass distribu-
tion when the center of mass of the object changes randomly across subsequent
trials (Lukos et al. 2013).
Quaney et al. (2003) demonstrated that not only the weight of the previously
lifted object impacts force scaling, but also previous task-unrelated actions.
Particularly, subjects who were instructed to squeeze a pinch force transducer with
a defined GF in between a series of lifting trials subsequently exerted a similar GF
when lifting an object, irrespective of the actual weight of the object and the hand
performing the lift. Although the load force was not influenced by previous
pinching, the findings suggested that not only object properties but also a sense of
effort might be memorized and can influence GF scaling.
Even when subjects are explicitly aware that a 180° rotation of an object with an
asymmetric mass distribution has occurred or even rotate the object themselves,
they fail to change the direction of the previously learned compensatory torque and
thus initially even add to the external torque (Fig. 1b, c bottom row). As a con-
sequence, large initial object tilts occur (Salimi et al. 2000, 2003; Bursztyn and
Flanagan 2008; Fu et al. 2010; Zhang et al. 2010; Marneweck et al. 2015). This
failure of compensatory torque anticipation is both due to not being able to modify
previously learned load force partitioning as well as vertical finger positioning to
the new situation. Such a lack of adaption to a new object orientation also appears
when the subject is rotated around the object instead (Marneweck et al. 2015). The
authors proposed that sensorimotor memories of a learned manipulation of an
object with asymmetrical weight distribution is strictly specific to the frame of
reference in which it is acquired and can basically not be transformed to new
orientations or situations (Marneweck et al. 2015). This implies that object
dynamics are not represented in a single veridical object representation but multiple
representations for distinct task situations of object handling must concurrently
exist from which the sensorimotor system selects the most appropriate one.
Nevertheless, being exposed to several object rotations, subjects eventually learn
to change the torque-direction according to alterations of the object orientation and
are able to at least partially compensate the external torque. They hereby succeed to
alter finger positioning after object rotations, while persistently failing to change the
direction of load force distribution (Fu et al. 2010; Fu and Santello 2015;
Marneweck et al. 2015). This dissociation in learning rates of finger positioning and
182 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
force sharing across context changes led to the assumption that these two aspects
might be governed by independent neural processes.
Studies on intermanual transfer addressed the question if sensorimotor memories
established in lifts with one hand can be accessed with the other hand. Interestingly,
they yielded opposing results depending on the object property under investigation.
When subjects repeatedly lift an object with a symmetric weight distribution with
one hand and subsequently change to the other hand, they employ precise antici-
patory control already during the first lift after the hand switch (Gordon et al. 1994).
Comparable studies conducted with stroke patients and children with cerebral
palsy raised the hope that such transfer mechanisms might aid rehabilitation
(Gordon et al. 1999; Raghavan et al. 2006). While employing inappropriately high
GFs when lifting an object of varying weight with the impaired hand, patients are
able to scale forces correctly according to object weight with their impaired hand
after having learned successful manipulation with the unimpaired hand before.
However, studies examining the lifting of an object with asymmetric weight
distribution contradict these findings. Participants who have learned to compensate
torques with one hand failed to exert any compensatory moment when switching to
the other hand (Fig. 1b, c top row) and therefore behave as if they were completely
naïve to the task (Salimi et al. 2000; Bursztyn and Flanagan 2008; Fu et al. 2014).
Nevertheless, one subject group in the study of (Bursztyn and Flanagan 2008)
constitutes an exception. When subjects simultaneously switched hands and
inverted their hand posture, considerable compensatory torques preventing initial
object tilt to a large degree could be observed. Indeed, this kind of hand and posture
alternations resemble the way in which we pass objects from one hand to another in
daily life more. Therefore, practice might be a decisive factor in transferring
information across hands.
Associative Memory
In order to be able to learn and refine object manipulation skills in the long term, it
is essential to be able to link recently acquired short-living sensorimotor memory of
lifting trials to characteristic object traits. Later, these traits can be used to identify
the object and retrieve the corresponding internal representation of object dynamics
enabling anticipatory object manipulation (Nowak et al. 2007b; Ameli et al. 2008).
The pace at which such a link is established seems to depend upon the predictability
of presentation. For example, children learned to scale force rates according to
different pre-cued object weights at a slower rate when exposed to a random lifting
schedule compared to a more predictable blocked schedule (Gordon and Duff
1999). However, scaling precision after a one-day retention was similar for both
schedule groups. When arbitrary visual or auditory cues signal the weight of an
object to be lifted, healthy participants can easily use these cues to predictively
scale GFs with both hands by building and accessing associations between the cues
and sensorimotor memories of object weight (Ameli et al. 2008).
Anticipation in Object Manipulation … 183
Importantly, both patients with right-sided and left-sided MCI stroke are clearly
hampered in establishing and/or retrieving such associations when using their
affected contralesional hand. While this deficiency is also prominent when grasping
with the ipsilesional, non-paretic hand in the left-brain damage group, right
brain-lesioned patients’ force prediction resembles that of healthy controls when
grasping with the ipsilesional hand (Bensmail et al. 2012). This adds further evi-
dence to the presumed motor dominant role of the left hemisphere.
Employing a similar protocol, a recent fMRI study measured BOLD responses
when subjects prepared to lift a lighter or a heavier weight indicated by a color cue
using the right hand (van Nuenen et al. 2012). Bilateral activity was found in the
PMd, in mesial premotor areas, in the SMA, as well as in the left parietal cortex
when the lifting of the heavier weight was signaled. In this study, the GF deviation
when subjects were first cued for an incorrect weight and then received a valid
second cue for the correct weight was taken as the measure for the strength of the
color association. Interestingly, this measure correlated with left PMd activity
supporting the clinical findings of a more important role of left-brain areas in the
task.
In accordance with the fMRI findings, the authors as well as other groups found
impaired force scaling with arbitrary cues after TMS stimulation of the PMd
(Chouinard et al. 2005; Nowak et al. 2009; van Nuenen et al. 2012). In addition to
the premotor cortex, stimulation of the AIP deteriorated cue-based anticipatory
activity during reaching and grasping movements (Taubert et al. 2010).
The by far most objects we encounter daily are well known and we usually have
gathered extensive lifetime experience in manipulating them. Hence, one might
think it is easy to guess the properties of familiar objects and adapt force devel-
opment accordingly. Indeed, studies investigating the anticipatory control of grasps
of common everyday objects (Fig. 2a) concordantly showed that healthy subject
succeed in increasing their GF (Hermsdörfer et al. 2011) and load force (Gordon
et al. 1993; Hermsdörfer et al. 2011) more rapidly when grasping heavy objects
(like a book) than when lifting a lighter object (like a box of tissues) (Fig. 2b CL).
Thus, the peaks of lift and GF rates before lift-off that are highly indicative of
preplanning of object manipulation were almost linearly associated with the actual
object weights (Fig. 2c CL). In the design of one experiment, objects were chosen
so that object weights could not be derived from their size using a uniform density
estimate. Consequently, a control group who lifted the same objects covered in
white paper showed a by far weaker association between force-parameters and
actual weight (Hermsdörfer et al. 2011). This supported the authors’ thesis that
successful estimation of object properties was due to the identification of the objects
and the retrieval of learned object-specific internal representations of object weight
and not visual size analysis.
Cole (2008) showed that subjects remain sensitive to size when lifting everyday
objects. Subjects lifted two filled bottles, which slightly differed in size and weight,
accordingly. Despite not realizing that they actually lifted two different bottles, they
employed force rates that matched the actually different weights. This showed that
despite consciously identifying the object as being the same, the sensorimotor
184 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
Fig. 2 Anticipatory grip force scaling during grasping and lifting everyday objects. a Grip force
measurement using flexible force-sensor arrays mounted above the finger pads and secured by
rubber caps. b Examples of time courses of grip force, maximum grip force rate (1st derivative of
grip force) and load force during the lifting of four everyday objects in one patient (LBD) and one
control subject (CL). The load force was measured with a scale; decrease of the scale signal reflects
loading of the fingers. Note that the patient scales grip force rate similarly for the book and the
mug, although the book is much heavier. The patient also employed a lower grip force level, which
was not typical for the group. c Relationship between maximum grip force rate and the weight of
everyday objects lifted by a control subject (CL) and two selected patients with left-brain damage
(LBD). While the control subject produced grip force rates with a nearly linear relationship with
the object weight, the relationship was very scattered in the patients indicating an inability to
anticipate object weight when it had to be concluded from object identity. Modified from
Eidenmüller et al. (2014)
system still processes size information to modify force output. Cole proposed that
not object weight is stored in internal memory traces. Instead, the sensorimotor
system integrates a remembered density representation to predict weight based on
visual size analysis.
Patients suffering from MCI stroke who were instructed to lift a set of everyday
objects with their ipsilesional, less impaired hand showed a significantly smaller
and flattened correlation compared to healthy controls (Dawson et al. 2010;
Eidenmüller et al. 2014). Several patients applied clearly inappropriate force pat-
terns, which seemed at random (Fig. 2b, c LBD). This disruption of natural force
control was limited to the group of left-brain-lesioned patients. Since all patients
were able to name or point to the objects, this failure cannot be attributed to general
Anticipation in Object Manipulation … 185
loss to identify objects, but might rather stem from an inability to retrieve or process
object properties from object identity and appearance.
While (Dawson et al. 2010) suggested lesions in the left parietal and adjacent
temporal areas to be related to the deficits, the voxel based lesion symptom analysis
performed by (Eidenmüller et al. 2014) found symptoms-related regions in the left
IFG and premotor cortex which are also frequently impaired in patients with tool
using deficits.
Visual Cues
Even when lifting novel objects for the first time, our brain tries to tune the motor
program according to the expected object properties. We can process various visual
cues like the size, assumed material and shape or form of the object and handle. On
the basis of this information, we compute predictions in order to optimize the first
lift in terms of adequate force control (Gordon et al. 1991; Jenmalm and Johansson
1997; Buckingham et al. 2009; Fu and Santello 2012). These computations rely on
associations between object traits and physical properties that have developed over
a lifetime of experience, e.g., the relation between size and weight based on the
density of an object. We universally compute such visuomotor transformations
when we encounter novel as well as familiar objects and usually obtain reliable
predictions to guide motor control.
Important paradigms to gain insights into the interaction between weight predic-
tions relying on size cues and the sensorimotor system are size–weight illusion
(SWI) tasks. We assume that objects with the same appearance consist of the same
material and thus have the same density. Therefore, we expect the weight of such
objects to increase with size. SWI paradigms investigate the effect of violations of
this size–weight relationship. Asked to alternately lift two cubes of differing size
but identical weight, participants initially base their weight predictions on visual
size cues and increase forces faster when lifting the big cube which is unusually
light than when lifting the small one which is unusually dense (Flanagan and
Beltzner 2000). After lift-off the motor error is signaled to the subjects and it takes
them about 5–10 lifts to gradually adapt force scaling to the true weight and
program their motor output adequately, thus in the same way for both cubes.
Interestingly, despite the adequate force output after a few trials, subjects perceive a
strong illusion that the smaller object is heavier, which does not ameliorate across
trials. Different from the perceptual system, the system controlling GF apparently
learned to block predictive mechanisms based on visual size cues and to only rely
on sensorimotor influences (Flanagan and Beltzner 2000). Importantly, the
186 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
and the superior occipital lobe. Since they also presented with a rare deviation in the
perception of the SWI, the authors suggested the area at the left parieto-occipital
junction to play a vital role in processing object properties for motor control and
perception. While in patients with cerebellar diseases the anticipatory scaling of
load forces according to object size was impaired, the scaling of GFs was preserved
similarly to MCI stroke patients (Rabe et al. 2009). Therefore, the processing of
visual cues to drive force scaling seems to be a highly persistent mechanism which
is resistant to even severe brain damage or can be effectively reorganized in case of
a brain lesion (Li et al. 2011).
A fMRI study used repetition suppression methodology to measure brain activity
during lifting of seen objects in a SWI paradigm as well as other combinations of
different or equal object sizes and weights (Chouinard et al. 2009). From different
contrasts between the conditions the authors concluded that size information was
coded in the contralateral left primary sensory areas (S1) and in the left AIP, weight
information was coded in the left primary motor cortex (M1). Constant density was
reflected by activity changes in the left PMv suggesting that this area integrates
sensory information about the size of objects and the weight of objects. When
information about object texture informed about the weight, brain activity that
discriminates between both objects during lift planning was obvious in the primary
somatosensory cortex (S1) and in the PMd, but also in texture-sensitive brain
regions in the occipitaltemporal cortex (Gallivan et al. 2014).
As summed up above, healthy adults can take advantage of arbitrary- and identi-
fication cues, as well as of visual density and size cues, to establish anticipatory
force scaling when lifting an object with a symmetric mass distribution. However,
disparate results were found in studies focusing on the anticipatory compensation of
external torques.
Although participants were able to precisely localize the center of mass of an
object that consisted of two halves of differently dense materials (leading to an
asymmetric weight distribution) prior to lifting it, they exerted only negligible
torques at the moment of lift-off and considerable tilts occurred (Craje et al. 2013).
Even hefting the object in the one hand and lifting it with the other did not allow for
improvement of anticipative torque planning. A similar dissociation between
explicit knowledge of mass distribution and failure to program compensatory tor-
ques was observed in studies in which a weight block positioned at one side of an
object was either visible or indicated by a color cue (Salimi et al. 2003; Lukos et al.
2007). These findings stand in line with the above-mentioned studies on object
rotations (Salimi et al. 2000; Fu et al. 2010; Marneweck et al. 2015).
188 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
Fig. 3 Factors influencing anticipatory object control and their interaction. Associated brain areas
as revealed by neuroimaging and clinical patient studies are indicated (see text). Abbrevations: M1
primary motor cortex, S1 primary somatosensory cortex, PMv & PMd ventral and dorsal premotor
cortex, AIP anterior intraparietal area, SMA supplementary motor area, IFG inferior frontal gyrus,
SMG supramarginal gyrus, IPS intraparietal sulcus, POJ parietal-occipital junction, object -
sensitive occipitotemporal cortex (OTC-obj), texture-sensitive occipitotemporal cortex (OTC-tex),
CB cerebellum
190 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
References
Ameli M, Dafotakis M, Fink GR, Nowak DA (2008) Predictive force programming in the grip-lift
task: the role of memory links between arbitrary cues and object weight. Neuropsychologia
46:2383–2388
Bensmail D, Sarfeld AS, Ameli M, Fink GR, Nowak DA (2012) Arbitrary visuomotor mapping in
the grip-lift task: dissociation of performance deficits in right and left middle cerebral artery
stroke. Neuroscience 210:128–136. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2012.03.015
Berner J, Schonfeldt-Lecuona C, Nowak DA (2007) Sensorimotor memory for fingertip forces
during object lifting: the role of the primary motor cortex. Neuropsychologia 45:1931–1938
Blennerhassett JM, Carey LM, Matyas TA (2006) Grip force regulation during pinch grip lifts
under somatosensory guidance: comparison between people with stroke and healthy controls.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil 87:418–429. doi:10.1016/j.apmr.2005.11.018
Blennerhassett JM, Matyas TA, Carey LM (2007) Impaired discrimination of surface friction
contributes to pinch grip deficit after stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 21:263–272.
doi:10.1177/1545968306295560
Boecker H, Lee A, Mühlau M et al (2005) Force level independent representations of predictive
grip force—load force coupling: A PET activation study. NeuroImage 25:243–252.
doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.10.027
Brandauer B, Hermsdörfer J, Beck A, Aurich V, Gizewski ER, Marquardt C, Timmann D (2008)
Impairments of prehension kinematics and grasping forces in patients with cerebellar
degeneration and the relationship to cerebellar atrophy. Clin Neurophysiol 119:2528–2537
Brandauer B, Timmann D, Häußler A, Hermsdörfer J (2010) Influences of load characteristics on
impaired control of grip forces in patients with cerebellar damage. J Neurophysiol 103:
698–708
Buckingham G, Bienkiewicz M, Rohrbach N, Hermsdörfer J (2015) The impact of unilateral brain
damage on weight perception, sensorimotor anticipation, and fingertip force adaptation. Vis
Res 115:231–237. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2015.02.005
Buckingham G, Cant JS, Goodale MA (2009) Living in a material world: how visual cues to
material properties affect the way that we lift objects and perceive their weight. J Neurophysiol
102:3111–3118. doi:10.1152/jn.00515.2009
Bursztyn LL, Flanagan JR (2008) Sensorimotor memory of weight asymmetry in object
manipulation. Exp Brain Res 184:127–133. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1173-z
Chang EC, Flanagan JR, Goodale MA (2008) The intermanual transfer of anticipatory force
control in precision grip lifting is not influenced by the perception of weight. Exp Brain Res
185:319–329. doi:10.1007/s00221-007-1156-0
Chouinard PA, Large ME, Chang EC, Goodale MA (2009) Dissociable neural mechanisms for
determining the perceived heaviness of objects and the predicted weight of objects during
lifting: an fMRI investigation of the size-weight illusion. Neuroimage 44:200–212
Chouinard PA, Leonard G, Paus T (2005) Role of the primary motor and dorsal premotor cortices
in the anticipation of forces during object lifting. J Neurosci 25:2277–2284
Cole KJ (2008) Lifting a familiar object: visual size analysis, not memory for object weight, scales
lift force. Exp Brain Res 188:551–557. doi:10.1007/s00221-008-1392-y
Craje C, Santello M, Gordon AM (2013) Effects of visual cues of object density on perception and
anticipatory control of dexterous manipulation. PLoS ONE 8:e76855. doi:10.1371/journal.
pone.0076855
Dafotakis M, Sparing R, Eickhoff SB, Fink GR, Nowak DA (2008) On the role of the ventral
premotor cortex and anterior intraparietal area for predictive and reactive scaling of grip force.
Brain Res in press. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.06.027
Davare M, Andres M, Clerget E, Thonnard JL, Olivier E (2007) Temporal dissociation between
hand shaping and grip force scaling in the anterior intraparietal area. J Neurosci 27:3974–3980.
doi:10.1523/jneurosci.0426-07.2007
Anticipation in Object Manipulation … 191
Davare M, Kraskov A, Rothwell J, Lemon R (2011) Interactions between areas of the cortical
grasping network. Curr Opin Neurobiol 21:565–570. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.021
Dawson AM, Buxbaum LJ, Duff SV (2010) The impact of left hemisphere stroke on force control
with familiar and novel objects: neuroanatomic substrates and relationship to apraxia. Brain
Res 1317:124–136. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2009.11.034
Ehrsson HH, Fagergren A, Ehrsson GO, Forssberg H (2007) Holding an object: neural activity
associated with fingertip force adjustments to external perturbations. J Neurophysiol 97:
1342–1352
Ehrsson HH, Fagergren A, Johansson RS, Forssberg H (2003) Evidence for the involvement of the
posterior parietal cortex in coordination of fingertip forces for grasp stability in manipulation.
J Neurophysiol 90:2978–2986
Eidenmüller S, Randerath J, Goldenberg G, Li Y, Hermsdörfer J (2014) The impact of unilateral
brain damage on anticipatory grip force scaling when lifting everyday objects.
Neuropsychologia 61:222–234. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.06.026
Flanagan JR, Beltzner MA (2000) Independence of perceptual and sensorimotor predictions in the
size-weight illusion. Nat Neurosci 3:737–741. doi:10.1038/76701
Flanagan JR, Bittner JP, Johansson RS (2008) Experience can change distinct size-weight priors
engaged in lifting objects and judging their weights. Curr Biol 18:1742–1747. doi:10.1016/j.
cub.2008.09.042
Flanagan JR, King S, Wolpert DM, Johansson RS (2001) Sensorimotor prediction and memory in
object manipulation. Can J Exp Psychol 55:87–95
Flanagan JR, Merritt K, Johansson RS (2009) Predictive mechanisms and object representations
used in object manipulation. In: Nowak DA, Hermsdörfer J (eds) Sensorimotor control
of grasping: physiology and pathophysiology. Cambridge University Press, Cambrige,
pp 161–203
Fu Q, Choi JY, Gordon AM, Jesunathadas M, Santello M (2014) Learned manipulation at
unconstrained contacts does not transfer across hands. PLoS ONE 9:e108222. doi:10.1371/
journal.pone.0108222
Fu Q, Hasan Z, Santello M (2011) Transfer of learned manipulation following changes in degrees
of freedom. J Neurosci 31:13576–13584. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1143-11.2011
Fu Q, Santello M (2012) Context-dependent learning interferes with visuomotor transformations
for manipulation planning. J Neurosci 32:15086–15092. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2468-12.
2012
Fu Q, Santello M (2015) Retention and interference of learned dexterous manipulation: interaction
between multiple sensorimotor processes. J Neurophysiol 113:144–155. doi:10.1152/jn.00348.
2014
Fu Q, Zhang W, Santello M (2010) Anticipatory planning and control of grasp positions and forces
for dexterous two-digit manipulation. J Neurosci 30:9117–9126. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
4159-09.2010
Gallivan JP, Cant JS, Goodale MA, Flanagan JR (2014) Representation of object weight in human
ventral visual cortex. Curr Biol 24:1866–1873. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2014.06.046
Gordon AM, Charles J, Duff SV (1999) Fingertip forces during object manipulation in children
with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. II: bilateral coordination. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:176–185
Gordon AM, Duff SV (1999) Fingertip forces during object manipulation in children with
hemiplegic cerebral palsy. I: anticipatory scaling. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:166–175
Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Iwasaki N (1994) Formation and lateralization of internal represen-
tations underlying motor commands during precision grip. Neuropsychologia 32:555–568
Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Johansson RS, Westling G (1991) Visual size cues in the programming
of manipulative forces during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 83:477–482
Gordon AM, Westling G, Cole KJ, Johansson RS (1993) Memory representations underlying
motor commands used during manipulation of common and novel objects. J Neurophysiol
69:1789–1796
Hager-Ross C, Johansson RS (1996) Nondigital afferent input in reactive control of fingertip forces
during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 110:131–141
192 T. Schneider and J. Hermsdörfer
Hermsdörfer J, Hagl E, Nowak DA, Marquardt C (2003) Grip force control during object
manipulation in cerebral stroke. Clin Neurophysiol 114:915–929
Hermsdörfer J, Li Y, Randerath J, Goldenberg G, Eidenmuller S (2011) Anticipatory scaling of
grip forces when lifting objects of everyday life. Exp Brain Res 212:19–31. doi:10.1007/
s00221-011-2695-y
Hsu HY, Lin CF, Su FC, Kuo HT, Chiu HY, Kuo LC (2012) Clinical application of computerized
evaluation and re-education biofeedback prototype for sensorimotor control of the hand in
stroke patients. J Neuroeng Rehabil 9:26. doi:10.1186/1743-0003-9-26
Imbach LL, Baumann-Vogel H, Baumann CR, Sürücü O, Hermsdörfer J, Sarnthein J (2015)
Adaptive grip force is modulated by subthalamic beta activity in Parkinson’s disease patients.
NeuroImage: Clin 9:450–457. doi:http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.nicl.2015.09.010
Jenmalm P, Johansson RS (1997) Visual and somatosensory information about object shape
control manipulative fingertip forces. J Neurosci 17:4486–4499
Jenmalm P, Schmitz C, Forssberg H, Ehrsson HH (2006) Lighter or heavier than predicted: neural
correlates of corrective mechanisms during erroneously programmed lifts. J Neurosci 26:
9015–9021. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5045-05.2006
Johansson RS, Flanagan JR (2009) Coding and use of tactile signals from the fingertips in object
manipulation tasks. Nat Rev Neurosci 10:345–359. doi:10.1038/nrn2621
Johansson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in
automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp Brain
Res 56:550–564
Johansson RS, Westling G (1987) Signals in tactile afferents from the fingers eliciting adaptive
motor responses during precision grip. Exp Brain Res 66:141–154
Johansson RS, Westling G (1988) Coordinated isometric muscle commands adequately and
erroneously programmed for the weight during lifting task with precision grip. Exp Brain Res
71:59–71
Kawato M, Kuroda T, Imamizu H, Nakano E, Miyauchi S, Yoshioka T (2003) Internal forward
models in the cerebellum: fMRI study on grip force and load force coupling. Prog Brain Res
142:171–188. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(03)42013-X
Lashley KS (1930) Basic neural mechanisms in behaviour psychological review. Psychol Rev
37:1–24
Li Y, Randerath J, Goldenberg G, Hermsdörfer J (2007) Grip forces isolated from knowledge
about object properties following a left parietal lesion. Neurosci Lett:187–191. doi:10.1016/
j.neulet.2007.09.008
Li Y, Randerath J, Goldenberg G, Hermsdörfer J (2011) Size-weight illusion and anticipatory
grip force scaling following unilateral cortical brain lesion. Neuropsychologia 49:914–923.
doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.02.018
Loh MN, Kirsch L, Rothwell JC, Lemon RN, Davare M (2010) Information about the weight of
grasped objects from vision and internal models interacts within the primary motor cortex.
J Neurosci 30:6984–6990. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6207-09.2010
Lukos J, Ansuini C, Santello M (2007) Choice of contact points during multidigit grasping: effect
of predictability of object center of mass location. J Neurosci 27:3894–3903. doi:10.1523/
JNEUROSCI.4693-06.2007
Lukos JR, Choi JY, Santello M (2013) Grasping uncertainty: effects of sensorimotor memories on
high-level planning of dexterous manipulation. J Neurophysiol 109:2937–2946. doi:10.1152/
jn.00060.2013
Marneweck M, Knelange E, Lee-Miller T, Santello M, Gordon AM (2015) Generalization of
dexterous manipulation is sensitive to the frame of reference in which it is learned. PLoS ONE
10:e0138258. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0138258
McDonnell MN, Hillier SL, Ridding MC, Miles TS (2006) Impairments in precision grip correlate
with functional measures in adult hemiplegia. Clin Neurophysiol 117:1474–1480. doi:10.1016/
j.clinph.2006.02.027
Anticipation in Object Manipulation … 193
van Nuenen BFL, Kuhtz-Buschbeck J, Schulz C, Bloem BR, Siebner HR (2012) Weight-specific
anticipatory coding of grip force in human dorsal premotor cortex. J Neurosci 32:5272–5283
Wenzelburger R, Kopper F, Frenzel A et al (2005) Hand coordination following capsular stroke.
Brain 128:64–74. doi:10.1093/brain/awh317
White O, Davare M, Andres M, Olivier E (2013) The role of left supplementary motor area in grip
force scaling. PLoS ONE 8:e83812. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0083812
Zhang W, Gordon AM, Fu Q, Santello M (2010) Manipulation after object rotation reveals
independent sensorimotor memory representations of digit positions and forces. J Neurophysiol
103:2953–2964. doi:10.1152/jn.00140.2010
Part IV
Learning of Skilled Behavior
Brain Plasticity and the Concept
of Metaplasticity in Skilled Musicians
Abstract Early and extensive musical training provides plastic adaptations of the
nervous system and enhanced sensory, motor, and cognitive functions. Over dec-
ades, neuronal mechanism underlying the plastic adaptation through musical
training has been investigated using neuroimaging and transcranial stimulation
techniques. Recently, plastic changes in neuroplastic functions through musical
training have gradually gained some interest, so-called metaplasticity.
Metaplasticity enables faster and more stable skill acquisition for individuals with a
history of prior musical training. This mechanism may also serve for prevention of
developing maladaptive changes in the nervous system, being pathophysiology of
focal dystonia in musicians. The present chapter introduces neurophysiological
mechanisms and functional significances of brain plasticity and metaplasticity of
the sensory and motor systems of musicians.
Keywords Neuroimaging MRI_imaging Voxel-based morphometry (VBM)
Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) Magnetoencephalography (MEG) Diffusion
tensor imaging (DTI) Dystonia Piano Dexterity
central nervous system is highest. This feature is not unique to music as other
skilled activities, for example, classical ballet, also require an early start. Second,
making music obviously is linked to sound production. As in speech, the auditory
system provides a very precise feedback of the movement effects (van der Steen
et al. 2014), with a temporal resolution superior to kinaesthetic and visual feedback.
Furthermore, in the frame of classical music, which is notated and available as sheet
music, the target parameters are strictly predefined: namely, temporal accurateness
(correct tempo, accuracy of rhythm, swing, beat, pulse, etc.) and spatial accurate-
ness (correct key, or finger position on fingerboard, correct sound quality).
Therefore, a highly reliable reproduction of movements meeting these targets is
required. This feature is unique to music: in classical ballet, for example, visual
feedback is less critical in terms of spatiotemporal precision. Third, most musicians
work at the upper limit of their sensory-motor capabilities and strive to push their
limits in order to be faster, louder, and more expressive. Given the complexity of
music, the demands of composers, especially in the last 100 years, and the role of
outstanding peers as models, such as the Chinese pianist Lang Lang, or the “record
braking” violinist David Garrett, musicians are aware that the only limit of
movement accuracy and speed is the temporal and spatial resolution of the auditory
system. And finally, the societal pressure and expectancies concerning the quality
of musical performances have definitely grown over the last centuries. This is due,
on the one hand, to the ubiquitous availability of music recorded in the media, such
as on YouTube and CDs, but on the other hand, to collective learning processes
leading to higher standards of music appreciation. This process augments anxiety,
tension, and competition among musicians, making their lives increasingly stress-
ful. Frequently even outstanding soloists have to cope with severe performance
anxiety (Wilson 1997). Nevertheless, making music is frequently linked to highly
positive emotions, to feelings of joy, satisfaction, and even to strong emotional
reactions, known as “chill responses” (Altenmüller et al. 2013). These qualities are
known to enhance plastic adaptations of the brain and can even lead to a sort of
addictive behavior, causing younger musicians to over-practice and ignore fatigue
and musculoskeletal pain which show their bodily limits.
In general terms, brain plasticity means that our brain can be molded and formed. It
is brain plasticity what allows us to learn throughout our lifetime. Mechanisms
include not only strengthening or weakening of synaptic connections, but also
structural changes such as growth of neurons, dendrites, myelin sheets, and neu-
roglia and reduced physiological loss (apoptosis) of neurons. Brain plasticity is best
observed in complex tasks with high behavioral relevance for the individual such
that they cause strong emotional and motivational activation. Plastic changes are
more pronounced in situations where the task or activity is intense and the earlier in
life it has been developed. Obviously, the continued activities of accomplished
200 E. Altenmüller and S. Furuya
gray matter (GM) volume in primary motor, somatosensory, and premotor areas,
among other regions in the musician groups. Modeling musical expertise with the
same three-group population (professional musicians, amateur musicians and
nonmusicians), James et al. (2014) reported an intricate pattern of increased/
decreased GM. In particular, musicians showed GM density increases in areas
related to higher order cognitive processes (such as the fusiform gyrus or the
inferior frontal gyrus), whereas GM decreases were found in sensorimotor regions
(as perirolandic and striatal areas). These reductions in GM were interpreted as
reflecting a higher degree of automaticity of motor skills in more expert musicians
(James et al. 2014).
Our understanding of the molecular and cellular mechanisms underlying these
adaptations is far from complete. At the synaptic level, long-term depression
(LTD) and long-term potentiation (LTP) account for changes in synaptic
excitability. Generally speaking, brain plasticity may occur on different time axes.
For example, the efficiency and size of synapses may be modified in a time window
of seconds to minutes, whereas the growth of new synapses and dendrites may
require hours to days. An increase in gray matter density, which mainly reflects an
enlargement of neurons, needs at least several weeks. White matter density also
increases as a consequence of musical training. This effect is primarily due to an
enlargement of myelin cells: the myelin cells, wrapped around the nerve fibers
(axons) are contributing essentially to the velocity of the electrical impulses trav-
eling along the nerve fiber tracts. Under conditions requiring rapid information
transfer and high temporal precision these myelin cells grow and as a consequence
nerve conduction velocity increases. Finally, brain regions involved in specific
tasks may also be enlarged after long-term training due to the growth of structures
supporting the nervous function, for example, in the blood vessels that are neces-
sary for the oxygen and glucose transportation to sustain nervous function.
over the primary motor cortex elevates the cortical excitability, whereas cathodal
one lowers the excitability. Simultaneous anodal and cathodal tDCS over the left
and right motor cortices, respectively, therefore elevates the left motor cortical
excitability (Paquette et al. 2011; Waters-Metenier et al. 2014). The bihemispheric
tDCS while nonmusicians and expert pianists were taking rest resulted in
enhancement and degradation of fine motor control of skilled finger movements,
respectively (Furuya et al. 2014). In addition, the bihemispheric tDCS brought
about changes in fine motor control to smaller extent for pianists who initiated
musical training at earlier age, particularly before the age 8 (Furuya et al. 2013).
These findings are evidence in favor of “early optimization” of the nervous system,
a mechanism stabilizing the brain plasticity so as to retain acquired skills and
minimize undesirable neuronal changes.
Brain Imaging Gaser and Schlaug (2003) could demonstrate enhancement of gray
matter density in cortical sensory-motor regions, auditory regions, the left dorso-
lateral prefrontal cortex, and in the cerebellum in professional instrumentalists as
compared to nonmusicians and amateurs. Interestingly, these plastic adaptations
depend on critical age periods: Vaquero et al. (2016) investigated a group of 36
expert pianists (among them, 21 started their musical training before age 7, and 15
started at age 7 or after that age) and another group of 17 college students without
any musical background.
In line with Gaser and Schlaug, comparing with nonmusicians, pianists showed
more gray matter in regions associated with learning (hippocampus), with sensory
and motor control and processing (putamen and thalamus), with emotional pro-
cessing and the reward system (amygdala), as well as with auditory and language
processing (left superior temporal cortex); but they also showed less gray matter in
regions involved in sensory and motor control (postcentral gyrus), in processing of
musical stimuli (right superior temporal cortex), and structures that have been
related to music-score reading (supramarginal gyrus).
Moreover, among the pianists it was observed that the right putamen correlated
significantly with the age of start of music training (the later they started to play the
piano, the greater was the volume of gray matter in the right putamen). The
putamen is a deep subcortical structure which plays a role in motor control and
automatization of movements. The volume of gray matter in this structure has been
previously related with piano playing skills in the same direction with in the present
study: the higher the pianistic skills (the automaticity), the smaller the volume of
gray matter. One of the interpretations for this result is that pianists who started
earlier in life have been able to perform an optimization process of a circuit highly
trained by these musicians. This could also be the reason explaining why, in the
comparison between musicians and nonmusicians, less gray matter was found in
some areas related to music performance.
Regarding a behavioral scale-playing task done in these pianists, those who have
started their musical training before age 7 played with higher regularity than those
who started after 7 years of age, even though all the pianists participating in the
study practiced the same amount of hours nowadays and have achieved the same
level of proficiency. The findings are summarized in Fig. 1.
204 E. Altenmüller and S. Furuya
Fig. 1 Summary oft the results of the study on pianists by Vaquero et al. (2016). Courtesy of
Lucia Vaquero and Antoni Rodriguez–Fornells, with permission
The findings presented here are summarized in Fig. 2. We propose that early
musical training stabilizes the motor system and provides neuroprotective effect to
stabilize weights in the sensorimotor networks. A plausible biological significance
of this “early optimization” of the nervous system, being termed as homeostatic
metaplasticity (Bienenstock et al. 1982; Muller-Dahlhaus and Ziemann 2015), is to
Brain Plasticity and the Concept of Metaplasticity … 205
Fig. 2 Different time courses of skill acquisition in earlier and later “starters”. The neuronal
networks representing a specific skill will be optimized in early childhood. This will allow a more
proficient skill acquisition during preadolescence and adolescence resulting in a higher skill level.
This skill level will even improve during adulthood and will be more stable with cessation of skill
maintaining activities. Furthermore, these early optimized neuronal networks are more stable and
less susceptible to maladaptive changes, for example in musicians dystonia
References
Abraham WC, Bear MF (1996) Metaplasticity: the plasticity of synaptic plasticity. Trends
Neurosci 19:126–130
Altenmüller E (2003) Focal dystonia: advances in brain imaging and understanding of fine motor
control in musicians. Hand Clin 19:523–538, xi
Altenmüller E, Ioannou CI, Lee A (2015) Apollo’s curse: neurological causes of motor
impairments in musicians. Prog Brain Res 217:89–106. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2014.11.022
Altenmüller E, Kopiez R, Grewe O (2013) A contribution to the evolutionary basis of music:
lessons from the chill. The evolution of emotional communication: from sounds in nonhuman
mammals to speech and music in man, p 313
Altenmüller E, Schlaug G (2015) Apollo’s gift: new aspects of neurologic music therapy. Prog
Brain Res 217:237–252. doi:10.1016/bs.pbr.2014.11.029
Amunts K, Schlaug G, Jancke L, Steinmetz H, Schleicher A, Dabringhaus A, Zilles K (1997)
Motor cortex and hand motor skills: structural compliance in the human brain. Hum Brain
Mapp 5:206–215. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1097-0193(1997)5:3<206:AID-HBM5>3.0.CO;2-7
Bailey JA, Zatorre RJ, Penhune VB (2014) Early musical training is linked to gray matter structure
in the ventral premotor cortex and auditory-motor rhythm synchronization performance.
J Cogn Neurosci 26:755–767. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00527
Bengtsson SL, Nagy Z, Skare S, Forsman L, Forssberg H, Ullen F (2005) Extensive piano
practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development. Nat Neurosci 8:
1148–1150. doi:10.1038/nn1516
Bienenstock EL, Cooper LN, Munro PW (1982) Theory for the development of neuron selectivity:
orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex. J Neurosci 2:32–48
Candia V, Wienbruch C, Elbert T, Rockstroh B, Ray W (2003) Effective behavioral treatment of
focal hand dystonia in musicians alters somatosensory cortical organization. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA 100:7942–7946. doi:10.1073/pnas.1231193100
Elbert T, Candia V, Altenmüller E et al (1998) Alteration of digital representations in
somatosensory cortex in focal hand dystonia. NeuroReport 9:3571–3575
Elbert T, Pantev C, Wienbruch C, Rockstroh B, Taub E (1995) Increased cortical representation of
the fingers of the left hand in string players. Science 270:305–307
Ericsson KA, Krampe RT, Heizmann S (1993) Can we create gifted people? Ciba Found Symp
178: 222–231 (discussion 232–249)
Furuya S, Altenmüller E (2015) Acquisition and reacquisition of motor coordination in musicians.
Ann NY Acad Sci 1337:118–124. doi:10.1111/nyas.12659
Furuya S, Klaus M, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Altenmüller E (2014) Ceiling effects prevent further
improvement of transcranial stimulation in skilled musicians. J Neurosci 34:13834–13839.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1170-14.2014
Furuya S, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Altenmüller E (2013) Early optimization in finger dexterity of
skilled pianists: implication of transcranial stimulation. BMC Neurosci 14:35. doi:10.1186/
1471-2202-14-35
Furuya S, Oku T, Miyazaki F, Kinoshita H (2015) Secrets of virtuoso: neuromuscular attributes of
motor virtuosity in expert musicians. Sci Rep 5:15750. doi:10.1038/srep15750
Gaser C, Schlaug G (2003) Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians.
J Neurosci 23:9240–9245
Granert O, Peller M, Jabusch HC, Altenmüller E, Siebner HR (2011) Sensorimotor skills and focal
dystonia are linked to putaminal grey-matter volume in pianists. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry
82:1225–1231. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2011.245811
Hallam S, Cross I, Thaut M (2016) The Oxford handbook of music psychology. Oxford University
Press, Oxford
Hamada M, Hanajima R, Terao Y et al (2009) Primary motor cortical metaplasticity induced by
priming over the supplementary motor area. J Physiol 587:4845–4862. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.
2009.179101
Brain Plasticity and the Concept of Metaplasticity … 207
Herholz SC, Boh B, Pantev C (2011) Musical training modulates encoding of higher-order
regularities in the auditory cortex. Eur J Neurosci 34:524–529. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.
07775.x
Herholz SC, Zatorre RJ (2012) Musical training as a framework for brain plasticity: behavior,
function, and structure. Neuron 76:486–502. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.011
Hund-Georgiadis M, von Cramon DY (1999) Motor-learning-related changes in piano players and
non-musicians revealed by functional magnetic-resonance signals. Exp Brain Res 125:417–425
James CE, Oechslin MS, Van De Ville D, Hauert CA, Descloux C, Lazeyras F (2014) Musical
training intensity yields opposite effects on grey matter density in cognitive versus
sensorimotor networks. Brain Struct Funct 219:353–366. doi:10.1007/s00429-013-0504-z
Krampe RT, Ericsson KA (1996) Maintaining excellence: deliberate practice and elite
performance in young and older pianists. J Exp Psychol Gen 125:331–359
Meinz EJ (2000) Experience-based attenuation of age-related differences in music cognition tasks.
Psychol Aging 15:297–312
Mosing MA, Madison G, Pedersen NL, Kuja-Halkola R, Ullen F (2014) Practice does not
make perfect: no causal effect of music practice on music ability. Psychol Sci 25:1795–1803.
doi:10.1177/0956797614541990
Münte TF, Altenmüller E, Jancke L (2002) The musician’s brain as a model of neuroplasticity.
Nat Rev Neurosci 3:473–478. doi:10.1038/nrn843
Muller-Dahlhaus F, Ziemann U (2015) Metaplasticity in human cortex. Neuroscientist 21:
185–202. doi:10.1177/1073858414526645
Nitsche MA, Paulus W (2000) Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak
transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol 527(3):633–639
Nitsche MA, Paulus W (2001) Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor
cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology 57:1899–1901
Pantev C, Oostenveld R, Engelien A, Ross B, Roberts LE, Hoke M (1998) Increased auditory
cortical representation in musicians. Nature 392:811–814. doi:10.1038/33918
Paquette C, Sidel M, Radinska BA, Soucy JP, Thiel A (2011) Bilateral transcranial direct current
stimulation modulates activation-induced regional blood flow changes during voluntary
movement. J Cereb Blood Flow Metab 31:2086–2095. doi:10.1038/jcbfm.2011.72
Prinz W (1984) Modes of linkage between perception and action. In: Cognition and motor
processes. Springer, Berlin, pp 185–193
Quartarone A, Rizzo V, Bagnato S et al (2005) Homeostatic-like plasticity of the primary motor
hand area is impaired in focal hand dystonia. Brain 128:1943–1950. doi:10.1093/brain/awh527
Ragert P, Schmidt A, Altenmüller E, Dinse HR (2004) Superior tactile performance and learning
in professional pianists: evidence for meta-plasticity in musicians. Eur J Neurosci 19:473–478
Schlaug G, Jancke L, Huang Y, Staiger JF, Steinmetz H (1995) Increased corpus callosum size in
musicians. Neuropsychologia 33:1047–1055
Schmidt A, Jabusch HC, Altenmüller E, Kasten M, Klein C (2013) Challenges of making music:
what causes musician’s dystonia? JAMA Neurol 70:1456–1459. doi:10.1001/jamaneurol.2013.
3931
Siebner HR, Lang N, Rizzo V, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Lemon RN, Rothwell JC (2004)
Preconditioning of low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation with transcranial
direct current stimulation: evidence for homeostatic plasticity in the human motor cortex.
J Neurosci 24:3379–3385. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5316-03.2004
Steele CJ, Bailey JA, Zatorre RJ, Penhune VB (2013) Early musical training and white-matter
plasticity in the corpus callosum: evidence for a sensitive period. J Neurosci 33:1282–1290.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3578-12.2013
Tierney AT, Krizman J, Kraus N (2015) Music training alters the course of adolescent auditory
development. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 112:10062–10067. doi:10.1073/pnas.1505114112
van der Steen MC, Molendijk EB, Altenmüller E, Furuya S (2014) Expert pianists do not listen:
the expertise-dependent influence of temporal perturbation on the production of sequential
movements. Neuroscience 269:290–298. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.03.058
208 E. Altenmüller and S. Furuya
John J. Buchanan
Abstract The primary goal of this chapter is to merge together the visual per-
ception perspective of observational learning and the coordination dynamics theory
of pattern formation in perception and action. Emphasis is placed on identifying
movement features that constrain and inform action-perception and
action-production processes. Two sources of visual information are examined,
relative motion direction and relative phase. The visual perception perspective
states that the topological features of relative motion between limbs and joints
remains invariant across an actor’s motion and therefore are available for pickup by
an observer. Relative phase has been put forth as an informational variable that
links perception to action within the coordination dynamics theory. A primary
assumption of the coordination dynamics approach is that environmental infor-
mation is meaningful only in terms of the behavior it modifies. Across a series of
single limb tasks and bimanual tasks it is shown that the relative motion and relative
phase between limbs and joints is picked up through visual processes and supports
observational learning of motor skills. Moreover, internal estimations of motor skill
proficiency and competency are linked to the informational content found in rela-
tive motion and relative phase. Thus, the chapter links action to perception and vice
versa and also links cognitive evaluations to the coordination dynamics that support
action-perception and action-production processes.
Introduction
Very young children watch their parents move and interact with objects in the world
on a day-to-day basis. Parents pick things up, toss things about, move around
things, and children will imitate these actions. I have a picture of myself mowing
the grass and my then three-year-old son Shawn pushing a toy mower and fol-
lowing me around the yard. What was my son learning by imitating my behavior?
Was he learning intent, general action, or specific kinematic patterns? From a social
point of view, a person off the street might say my son was learning intent or
socially acceptable behavior and relating it to a general outcome. A savvy bystander
might say my son was extracting general and specific action information about
moving and interacting with a mechanical device to develop appropriate motor
commands so that the societal goal is achieved in an efficient and stable manner.
When it comes to observation as a means to support motor learning, it is the
viewpoint of the savvy bystander which is developed in this chapter. In other
words, how does observation facilitate the development of motor commands to
generate specific kinematic patterns of limb movements without (or with limited)
physical practice? It is this level of analysis that observation or demonstration as a
means to study action can most inform about how the central nervous system maps
perception to action and action to perception with regard to motor skills.
This chapter is organized around into three sections: (1) cognitive and ecological
theories of observation; (2) shared information in the form of coordination
dynamics that support action understanding (perception) and action-production
(control/coordination); and (3) neural areas and the link between action and
action-observation. For the first section, social learning theory (Bandura 1977,
1986), the visual perception perspective (Scully and Newell 1985), and the Theory
of Event coding (Hommel et al. 2001) are briefly outlined to highlight the differ-
ences between cognitive and ecological theories of observational learning. Within
the second section, the attempt is made to merge the visual perception perspective
of observational learning with the coordination dynamics theory of pattern for-
mation in action-perception processes (Kelso 1995). The third section will briefly
outline studies that have identified brain regions that may be functionally linked to
some of the perceptual/behavioral results presented in section two.
in seeking to propose that perception and action are directly linked. For the visual
perspective theory, the informational link is in the form of the topological invariant
features of relative motion, whereas for event coding the information link is in the
form of a single cognitive representation wherein perception and action information
for an event are linked. The goal of this chapter is to show that perception and
action are coded in the same informational representation, wherein this represen-
tation is in the form of the self-organized coordination dynamics of relative phase
and how those dynamics interact with the topological features of relative motion.
variability changes were modeled at both the individual component levels (fingers
or wrist as nonlinear oscillators) and the relative phase level (potential function)
(Haken 1983; Haken et al. 1985; Schöner et al. 1986). In the potential function (the
HKB model), 0° and 180° are stable attractors while patterns such as 90° are
unstable. A primary conclusion was that relative phase was an order parameter that
governed the spatiotemporal coordination of the fingers or wrists in this task and
that the transition was a self-organized phenomenon that resulted from a loss of
stability in the anti-phase pattern. Thus, in-phase and anti-phase coordination pat-
terns are viewed as stable attractors of the order parameter dynamics. The
demonstration of phase transitions in rhythmic human action has also been linked to
single limb multi-joint tasks. With the forearm supine and the arm in the sagittal
plane, an in-phase rhythmic pattern of elbow and wrist motion (wrist flex/ext.
coordinated with elbow flex/ext.) is very stable, whereas an anti-phase rhythmic
pattern (wrist flex/ext. coordinated with elbow ext./flex) is less stable. If movement
frequency is increased, transitions from anti-phase to in-phase occur, but not vice
versa (Kelso et al. 1991). With the forearm supine, the transition is from a
homologous muscle action to a nonhomologous action, just as in the bimanual task.
For this task and other single limb trajectory formation tasks it was concluded that
relative phase acts as an order parameter for the production of rhythmic single limb
multi-joint patterns (Buchanan et al. 1997; de Guzman et al. 1997).
elbow may be described as the wrist lags the elbow (Fig. 1a right side) or the wrist
leads the elbow (Fig. 1a left side) through flexion and extension. The relative phase
patterns from −30° to −150° are linked to the wrist lagging, while the relative phase
patterns in the range 30°–150° are linked to the wrist leading. For the 0° and 180°
patterns, neither the wrist nor the elbow leads. The two segments rotate in the
opposite direction for 180° (as the elbow flexes the wrist extends) whereas for 0°
they rotate in the same direction (both flex and extend together). The 0° and 180°
animations are consistent with the coordination patterns studied by Kelso et al.
(1991).
Participants were asked to categorize the different relative phase patterns as
either 0° in-phase or 180° anti-phase. The categorization procedure revealed that
relative phase patterns in the ranges of 0° ≤ ϕ < ±90° were most often categorized
as in-phase whereas relative phase patterns in the range of ±90° < ϕ < ±150° were
more often categorized as 180° anti-phase. The largest variability across partici-
pants in classifying the patterns occurred for the ±90° patterns, while the variability
in classification for the other patterns was not significant. The finding that the
categories were defined around ±90° is consistent with the HKB model in
that ±90° are symmetry partners and are unstable patterns that require extensive
training to perform without very specific augmented feedback (Schöner et al. 1992;
Zanone and Kelso 1992; Kovacs and Shea 2011). This experiment demonstrated
that a forced choice task can produce a perceptual bias with regard to the differences
in stability of relative phase patterns as defined by the HKB model. This study
shows that the order parameter relative phase act as an informational variable in the
perception of actions that is consistent with the coordination of actions. However,
there are two important aspects of coordination that this study did not examine.
First, the task did not examine the role of pattern stability in the categorization
process, an essential feature of the difference between in-phase, anti-phase and
other phase relationships on the action side. The role of pattern stability in visual
perception of relative phase will be examined in the following paragraph. Second,
the study did not determine if relative motion (wrist lag or lead) played a role in the
perceptual categorization process. It could be argued that relative phase was the
primary informational source in the task because of the symmetry in results. This
issue will be explored later in the chapter.
This link between perception and action with regard to coordination stability
(relative phase variability) was examined in a study that utilized animated dots to
display relative phase patterns, e.g., 0°, 30°, 90°, etc. (Bingham et al. 1999; Zaal
et al. 2000) (see Fig. 1b). Participants in the Bingham et al. (1999) study were
instructed to judge how stable the motion of the dots were under manipulations of
relative phase, relative phase variability, and movement frequency. Overall, the
experiments demonstrated that mean relative phase influenced the judgment of the
stability in the dots motions, with a 90° relative phase pattern judged as the least
stable whereas 0° in-phase was judged as the most stable followed by 180°
anti-phase. Different levels of variability in the visual displays were only detected
for the 0° and 180° patterns. The judgments of coordination were also found to
change as a function of the frequency of the moving dots. The 180° anti-phase
The Coordination Dynamics of Observational Learning … 215
Fig. 1 a Stick figures representing the animated arm display used as a stimulus in several studies.
The left side portrays the wrist leading pattern and the right side portrays the wrist lagging pattern.
b Representation of stimulus to characterize bimanual coordination and used in several perceptual
evaluation studies
pattern and other relative phase patterns were judged as less coordinated as
movement frequency increased, whereas movement frequency did not impact the
judgments of 0° in-phase (Bingham et al. 2001). Different levels of variability in
relative phase were perceived more consistently only for the in-phase pattern across
different movement frequencies. These findings are consistent with the transition in
bimanual coordination and with the predictions of the HKB model regarding the
stability of the various coordination patterns (Bingham et al. 1999; Zaal et al. 2000).
The findings from these perceptual assessments of coordination and variability
suggest that action-perception is based on action capability. When a pattern cannot
be produced in a stable manner, it is then perceived as less stable than patterns that
can be produced. However, the issue of how perception may change with learning
was not examined in these studies. Later in the chapter it is shown that observa-
tional and physical training alter perceptual processes as a function of learning and
changes in coordination stability.
The finding that perceptual evaluations of actions mirror the stability of those
actions when performed provides support for the application of the coordination
dynamics theory to the study of observation based on the idea that order parameters
are informational variables that map perceptual processes to motor processes (Kelso
1994). Application of coordination dynamics theory to the study of motor skill
learning through observation or demonstration must reside in the concepts of the
order parameter and its dynamics, change of stability, loss of stability, etc. To bring
216 J.J. Buchanan
together the visual perspective theory of observation with the coordination dynamics
theory will require the demonstration that some topological features of the relative
motion direction between limbs and joints remains invariant and that such features
support the informational nature of order parameters such as relative phase.
Fig. 2 a The templates shown in the Lissajous plots represent both relative phase and joint
amplitudes required for the initial training ellipse and the transfer tasks. b Percentage of trials
characterized with the same relative motion direction in the observers. The percentage reported are
across all transfer tasks: TA trained arm, UTA untrained arm
upper arm and allow the observers to focus on the elbow and wrist only. Each
observer was yoked to an individual model. Half of the models trained with the right
arm and half with the left arm and they were not instructed to have the wrist lead or
lag the elbow to achieve the 90° relative phase pattern. The task required the elbow
to rotate 90° as the wrist rotated 48°. The models trained for 2 days and observer’s
watched every training trial and were privy to concurrent and terminal feedback
provided to the model. To facilitate learning, a vertically oriented ellipse template
representing the difference in joint amplitudes and the required relative phase of 90°
was displayed in a Lissajous plot feedback display (Fig. 2a). The feedback consisted
of plotting the elbow and wrist angles along the axes of the ellipse. The models were
told that their goal was to make the trace of their elbow and wrist motions create a
trace around the perimeter of the ellipse. Twenty four hours after training, observers
and models performed retention tests separately and only the ellipse template
(without feedback) was shown in the Lissajous plot. The observers performed the
retention test with the same arm that their model trained with and observers had no
exposure to the elbow–wrist motion until the retention test.
218 J.J. Buchanan
A key issue examined with the above study was defining the nature of the
relative motion direction selected by the models and produced by observers. Some
of the models (N = 5) selected a wrist lead relative motion direction (+90° relative
phase) while others selected a wrist-lag (−90°) relative motion direction (N = 11)
during training (Fig. 1a). The retention tests revealed that the models performed
with the same relative motion direction they trained with and that each observer
selected the wrist lead or lag relative motion direction of the model they watched.
The models had smaller error with respect to the target of 90° and performed with
less relative phase variability compared to the observers, however, the observers
had smaller error and performed with less variability compared to a control
group. This experiment demonstrated that observers without any instruction pickup
the relative motion direction between the joints as well as the specific relative phase
pattern between the joints and use that information to help organize initial per-
formance attempts. The results suggest that relative motion direction was important
for organizing the actions, although the results did not really show that the relative
motion direction is a separate source of information from relative phase.
The concept of topology in mathematics implies that the properties of geometric
objects remain the same under transformations, such as stretching or bending.
Within the area of human movement or biological coordination, the idea of topo-
logical features may be viewed as coordination staying the same under various
transfer tasks. Scully and Newell (1985) proposed that relative motion has topo-
logical features, in other words, relative motion—both its perception and produc-
tion—should remain invariant under some form of transformation. This issue was
examined in a series of transfer tests that was an extension of the experiment that
used the elbow–wrist task just described (Buchanan and Wright 2011). The transfer
tests were designed to determine if relative motion direction and relative phase
would transfer across changes in forearm orientation (supine to prone), across limbs
(left-to-right and right-to-left), and changes in component amplitudes (see
Table 1).1 As with the retention test, the Lissajous plot was used to define the
transfer tests which consisted of a horizontal ellipse and a circle along with the
trained vertical ellipse (Fig. 2a). Performance feedback was not provided with the
transfer tests. Overall there were 528 transfer trials performed by the observers, and
in 400 (76 %) of the trials the observers used the same relative motion direction that
their model used during training and that they used during retention. Of the various
transformations, transfer across arms with the forearm supine, just as with the
training, was associated with utilizing the same relative motion direction most
often, in comparison to transfer across forearm positions (Fig. 2b). Thus, through
observation participants picked-up relative motion direction and used that infor-
mation rather consistently when performing variations of the trained task. These
results show more clearly that relative motion direction is acting as an independent
source of information from relative phase.
1
The transfer results reported here are unpublished. The manuscript by Buchanan and Wright
(2011) focused on relative phase performance across the transfer tasks.
The Coordination Dynamics of Observational Learning … 219
Table 1 Transfer tests and associated transformations based on arm, Forearm Orientation (FO),
and Joint amplitude (JA)
Vertical ellipse Horizontal ellipse Circle
a
RA supine Retention Transfer JA Transfer JA
RA prone Transfer FO Transfer FO, JA Transfer FO, JA
LA supine Transfer arm Transfer arm & JA Transfer arm & JA
LA prone Transfer arm & FO Transfer arm, FO, & JA Transfer arm, FO, & JA
a
This table represents the RA training condition. Substitute LA for RA for LA training condition
Fig. 3 The number of trials with a given wrist lead or wrist lag classification for the wrist lagging
and wrist leading test groups (labels at top of each side) are shown as a function of the required
relative phase. Part of the data in this figure is from Fig. 2 in Buchanan (2015) JMB and
reproduced with permission from Taylor and Francis
A more recent study using the single limb task described previously examined
this relationship between various relative phase patterns (0°, ±30°, ±60°,
±90°, ±120°, ±150°, 180°) and relative motion directions using the stick figure
arm animation (Fig. 1a). This task was designed to separate out the different
informational aspects of relative motion direction and relative phase that may
emerge at the very start of observational training (Buchanan 2015). Participants
attempted to match the animated arm motions that consisted of elbow and wrist
motion only in the sagittal plane. Attempts at performance matching occurred by
tracking with the animation or after the animation finished 16 cycles of motion
followed by a five second delay. The findings revealed that participants consistently
selected the correct relative motion direction between the elbow and wrist. Those
individuals exposed to wrist lagging animations (−30° to −150°) produced a neg-
ative mean relative phase (as required) for 92 % of the trials, while the individuals
exposed to the wrist leading animations (+30° to +150°) produced a positive mean
relative phase (as required) for 75 % of the trials (Fig. 3). Within each group, the
inability to use the correct relative motion direction occurred most often in the
delayed tracking groups, wrist leading group 41 of 58 incorrect trials and wrist
220 J.J. Buchanan
lagging group 19 of 19 incorrect trials. The results show that relative motion
information is quickly identified by humans and that it can be used instantaneously
to generate actions. In some of the early point light work, humans actions were
identified in less than 200 ms (Johansson 1976). The results show that relative phase
is perceived independently of relative motion in that the dynamics of the perfor-
mance, stability and attraction, were governed by the intrinsic dynamics of in-phase
and anti-phase more than either relative motion direction.
The ability of observers to distinguish various relative motion patterns has been
demonstrated in a series of bimanual circle tracing tasks. As with the elbow wrist
task, emphasis was placed on showing that observers’ pickup relative motion
direction (independent of relative phase) here defined as tracing direction around a
pair of circles. Several studies have examined symmetric and asymmetric circle
tracing and revealed that these two patterns of coordination have very similar
dynamics to the bimanual finger patterns of in-phase and anti-phase, with asym-
metric coordination (anti-phase) less stable than symmetric coordination (in-phase)
(Carson et al. 1997; Byblow et al. 1999). The circle tracing task offers a means to
study the pickup of relative motion direction in that a pair of circles can be traced in
a variety of clockwise (CW) and counterclockwise (CCW) combinations with a left
or right-hand lead, and based on the absolute value of relative phase are considered
as equivalent (Fig. 4).
Symmetric and asymmetric coordination are defined in the top row for bimanual
circle tracing. From a discovery learning point of view, the task of learning a 90°
relative phase pattern while tracing the two circles has a variety of solutions, many
more than learning a 90° relative phase using just flexion–extension motions of the
two index fingers, which has only two, the left or right finger leads (Fig. 4, ¼ cycle
lead). From an observation point of view, an observer may have many options to
select from if they watch a novice model learning through discovery. When
observers were yoked to discovery models learning a 90° relative phase in this task,
around 50 % of the observers selected the rotation direction and hand lead that their
yoked model practiced with the most often (Buchanan and Dean 2010). When
observers watched an instructional novice learning this task that used only the CW:
The Coordination Dynamics of Observational Learning … 221
CCW right-hand lead strategy or an expert using the same strategy then the
observers selected the CW: CCW 100 % of the time (Buchanan and Dean 2014).
Another big difference between observers that watch a discovery model, an
instructional model, or an expert model is that observers that view a discovery
model have a greater tendency to switch strategies within and between trials as well
as switch hand leads within and between trials compared to observers that watched
an instructional or expert model (Buchanan and Dean 2010).
In this section it was shown that observers detect and pickup relative motion
direction between joints and limbs and use that information to help them organize
outgoing motor commands in order to produce a variety of relative phase patterns.
Previous work has argued that relative motion direction was the specific informa-
tion that supports the coordination of rhythmic movements (Wilson et al. 2005).
Here, the position is taken that relative motion information is important in defining
the movement directions between joints and limbs, but that it does not carry specific
information for relative phase except for the relative phase patterns of 0° (in-phase)
and 180° (anti-phase) (Wilson and Bingham 2008). As the single limb and
bimanual studies revealed, different relative motion directions can be associated
with the same absolute relative phase value. Moreover, many relative phase patterns
can be associated with the same general relative motion direction between joints
and limbs (Buchanan 2015; Buchanan et al. 2015).
relationship of the wrist to the elbow (Buchanan 2015). After attempting to match
the coordination pattern represented in the animation participants were required to
rate their level of proficiency. The proficiency rating was not an estimate of how
well they thought they did at matching the animation or how coordinated they
thought the animated pattern looked (Bingham et al. 1999); instead, it was a rating
of how proficient (or coordinated) they felt while attempting to match the anima-
tion. A scale of 1–10 was used for the proficiency rating with 10 representing very
proficient and 1 not at all proficient. Overall, proficiency ratings correlated with the
relative phase variability found in the attempts at the seven different relative phase
patterns (Fig. 5a) (r2 = 0.93, F = 68.2, p < 0.0001). On average individuals rated
themselves as more coordinated when doing 0° (in-phase), ±30° (strong attraction
to 0°) and 180° (anti-phase), and less coordinated when doing ±60°, ±90°, ±120°
and ±150°. The results revealed an awareness of the dynamics of relative phase in
terms of intrinsic coordination stability, a finding distinct from the perceptual
evaluation work that manipulated stability in the visual displays (Bingham et al.
1999, 2001; Zaal et al. 2000). The proficiency rating results are consistent with
work showing that participants use proprioceptive information to judge phase
variability, being most accurate for bimanual finger patterns mechanically driven at
0° and 180° compared to 90° (Wilson et al. 2003).
The second study compared both physical and observational practice and the
animated arm portrayed two wrist leading to-be-learned patterns, +60° and +120°
(Buchanan et al. 2015). There were two training days with 30 training trials a day.
Participants in the physical group alternated every two trials in moving with the
animation or moving after the animation stopped, while participants in the obser-
vation group watched the animation over the two training days. The participants
were all trained separately and retention tests were performed separately. Prior to
motor skill training, participants took part in a perceptual evaluation task. The task
required them to view the 12 relative phase animations that were used in the Kelso
and Pandya (1991) and Buchanan (2015) studies. After viewing each animated
pattern (3 trials per pattern that were randomized) the participants had to rate how
well they felt they could perform the pattern just animated. This task was designed
to measure motor skill competency in that participants were not evaluating the level
of coordination in the pattern (Bingham et al. 1999) nor were they evaluating their
own performance, but were estimating their own ability to produce the just
observed pattern. The rating scale was 1–10 with 10 indicating very competent and
1 not at all competent. Before practice, the perceptual ratings revealed that par-
ticipants felt competent in their ability to produce the 0° and 180° patterns, but not
the to-be-learned patterns with the competency ratings for all other patterns drop-
ping off from 0° and 180° with the lowest ratings for ±60°, +90° and ±120°. After
practice, the competency rating increased significantly for the two trained relative
phase patterns (+60° and +120°) in both the models and observers (Fig. 5b). In fact,
the observation group’s competency ratings were slightly larger (not significantly)
than the physical group’s competency ratings. One reason the physical practice
group may have had lower post-training competency ratings is because they relied
more on their proprioceptive experience of the task gained through physical
The Coordination Dynamics of Observational Learning … 223
practice, whereas the observation group were relying more on their visual knowl-
edge when evaluating.
The above two studies when taken together show that the self-evaluation of
actions is linked to information about relative motion direction and the variability in
relative phase. It is proposed that any mental representation (or symbolic code or
event code) formed during observation are constrained by the actual coordination
dynamics that are intrinsic to the system and those dynamics that can change
through learning. Thus, proposed symbolic codes or event codes formed about the
kinematics of actions during observation should be entities with dynamics that
provide a direct link between perception and action. If perception and action are
linked through self-organized dynamic processes then there is not a need for
intermediate symbolic codes. This does not mean that cognitive representations do
not exist, since individuals can verbally describe the relative motion relationship
between the joints. What it means is that at the level of movement kinematics there
is a direct link between the production of an action and the perception of that action.
The above conclusion is consistent with the idea of an event code as proposed by
Hommel et al. (2001), except that coding is in the form of the dynamics of relative
phase and topological features of relative motion. A more systematic way to study
this idea of dynamic representations or codes and how they change through
observation would be to evaluate motor skill proficiency and competency evalua-
tions across training session and not just at the end points.
224 J.J. Buchanan
In this last section a brief connection to recent findings from neural imaging studies
will be examined. The idea is to show that neural areas linked to arm movement
kinematics have been identified and that areas linked to performance evaluation,
i.e., competency, have also been identified. Within the area of neuroscience, the
“mirror neurons” in the premotor cortex and the associated mirror system have been
invoked as mechanisms whereby action-observation is linked to motor imitation
and action-observation (Buccino et al. 2004; Rizzolatti and Craighero 2004;
Calvo-Merino et al. 2005; Cattaneo and Rizzolatti 2009; Cross et al. 2009, 2012).
Theoretically, the mirror neuron system and the action observation network
(AON) may allow observers to map a representation of the seen action onto the
motor system and gain knowledge of the action through internal execution
processes.
The results summarized throughout this chapter have shown that observers can
extract two specific types of action information from moving joints and limbs,
relative motion and relative phase and use that information to develop stable
coordination patterns with limited physical practice. Research using fMRI has
demonstrated that the occipitotemporal cortex is a region selective for responding to
static hand and upper limb postural positions with functional connectivity to the
human middle temporal complex an area associated with motion coding (Orlov
et al. 2014). Studies have shown that the human motion complex and posterior
superior temporal sulcus are regions that respond to speed and distance of moving
body parts (hand, arm, and head) (McAleer et al. 2014). Identification of the relative
motion direction between the elbow and wrist in the animated arm studies or the
relative motion directions in circle tracing and hand lead in the bimanual tasks may
occur in the above regions. The rhythmic nature of the motions studied and the
stability of relative phase may be processed through M1 and the cerebellum based
on a MEG study that localized these areas as important for processing frequency
information (Bourguignon et al. 2013). Observation as a training protocol requires
the continual monitoring of a models’ actions (whether human or animation) at the
kinematic level and there are areas in the human nervous system that have been
identified as supporting this monitoring (McAleer et al. 2014).
The proficiency and competency tasks based on the single limb motions
demonstrated that what are often thought of as cognitive processes may be linked to
the order parameter dynamics of relative phase and the relative motion between the
joints. The competency task demonstrated that physical and observational practice
can alter individuals’ cognitive evaluations of their performance. Research using
fMRI has revealed that expert ballet dancers are better at recognizing actions from
their own motor repertoire and this ability was linked to activation differences in the
greater premotor area, the parietal lobe, and the cerebellum (Calvo-Merino et al.
2006). Comparisons between experts and novices suggests that expertise is asso-
ciated with configural action processing, i.e., the ability to identify transformations
The Coordination Dynamics of Observational Learning … 225
in actions that come from an individual’s own motor repertoire (Calvo-Merino et al.
2010). The configural transformation may be viewed as similar to the single limb
transfer tasks mentioned earlier. An fMRI study that examined the observation of
dance moves over 5 sessions found that activity in the ventral premotor and inferior
parietal lobule (areas of the AON) were linked to increases in competency ratings
(Cross et al. 2006). The above studies show a link between self-evaluation and
motor experience, but they did not examine whether this change in evaluation was
linked to specific kinematic information as some behavioral work has demonstrated
(Maslovat et al. 2010; Buchanan 2015; Buchanan et al. 2015). If the evaluation
changes in the fMRI studies were linked to the kinematics, then activity in the
above brain regions may support the proficiency and competency ratings that were
linked to relative phase and relative motion information described in this chapter.
The chapter outlined the merging of the visual perception perspective of observa-
tional learning with the coordination dynamics theory of perception-action pro-
cesses. The studies reviewed revealed that relative motion direction and relative
phase both serve as informational variables that may be picked-up through visual
perception processes and be used to organize and constrain outgoing motor com-
mands with minimal physical practice. Moreover, it was shown that the processes
that support motor skill proficiency evaluations and competency evaluations are
influenced by the same informational variables and that these “more cognitive type
processes” are modified in a manner consistent with changes in pattern stability that
emerge through practice, both observational and physical. Future research needs to
examine more closely the different contributions of physical and observational
training to motor skill acquisition (Buchanan and Park, in press). If the phrase
“Watch how I do this” has any real meaning, then future research needs to deter-
mine when this phrase is most applicable in the learning process. Several studies
reviewed in this chapter suggest embedding proficiency and competency ratings
into training tasks will help reveal the similar and different contributions of
observation and physical practice to motor skill learning.
References
Al-Abood SA, Davids K, Bennett SJ, Ashford D, Marin MM (2001) Effects of manipulating
relative and absolute motion information during observational learning of an aiming task.
J Sports Sci 19:507–520
Bandura A (1977) Social learning theory. Prentice-Hall, Englewood Cliffs, NJ
Bandura A (1986) Social foundations of thought and action: a social cognitive theory. Prentice
Hall, Englewood Cliffs
226 J.J. Buchanan
Bingham GP, Schmidt RC, Zaal FTJM (1999) Visual perception of the relative phasing of human
limb movements. Percept Psychophys 61:246–258
Bingham GP, Zaal FTJM, Shull JA, Collins DR (2001) The effect of frequency on the visual
perception of relative phase and phase variability of two oscillating objects. Exp Brain Res
136:543–552. doi:10.1007/s002210000610
Bourguignon M, De Tiege X, de Beeck MO, Van Bogaert P, Goldman S, Jousmaki V, Hari R
(2013) Primary motor cortex and cerebellum are coupled with the kinematics of observed hand
movements. Neuroimage 66:500–507. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.038
Breslin G, Hodges NJ, Williams AM, Curran W, Kremer J (2005) Modelling relative motion to
facilitate intra-limb coordination. Hum Mov Sci 24:446–463
Breslin G, Hodges NJ, Williams AM, Kremer J, Curran W (2006) A comparison of intra- and
inter-limb relative motion information in modeling a novel motor skill. Hum Mov Sci 25:
753–766
Buccino G, Vogt S, Ritzl A, Fink GR, Zilles K, Freund H-J, Rizzolatti G (2004) Neural circuits
underlying imitation learning of hand actions: an event-related fMRI study
Buchanan JJ (2015) Perceptual estimates of motor skill proficiency are constrained by the stability
of coordination patterns. J Mot Behav 47:453–464. doi:10.1080/00222895.2015.1008687
Buchanan JJ, Dean N (2010) Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice models and
variability in demonstration benefits observational practice. Psychol Res 74:313–320
Buchanan JJ, Dean N (2014) Consistently modeling the same movement strategy is more
important than model skill level in observational learning contexts. Acta Psychol 146:19–27
Buchanan JJ, Kelso JAS, de Guzman GD (1997) Self-organization of trajectory information I.
Experimental evidence. Biol Cybern 76:257–273
Buchanan JJ, Park I (in press) Observation and physical practice: different practice contexts lead to
similar outcomes for the acquisition of kinematic information. Psychol Res. doi:10.1007/
s00426-015-0723-4
Buchanan JJ, Ramos J, Robson N (2015) The perception-action dynamics of action competency
are altered by both physical and observational training. Exp Brain Res 233:1289–1305.
doi:10.1007/s00221-015-4207-y
Buchanan JJ, Ryu YU, Zihlman K, Wright DA (2008) Observational practice of a relative phase
pattern but not an amplitude ratio in a multijoint task. Exp Brain Res 191:157–169
Buchanan JJ, Wright DA (2011) Generalization of action knowledge following observational
learning. Acta Psychol 136
Byblow WD, Summers JJ, Semjen A, Wuyts IJ, Carson RG (1999) Spontaneous and intentional
pattern switching in a multisegmental bimanual coordination task. Mot Cont 3:372–393
Calvo-Merino B, Ehrenberg S, Leung D, Haggard P (2010) Experts see it all: configural effects in
action observation. Psychol Res-Psychol Forsch 74:400–406. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0262-y
Calvo-Merino B, Glaser DE, Grezes RE, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2005) Action observation
and acquired motor skills: an fMRI study with expert dancers. Cereb Cortex 15:1243–1249
Calvo-Merino B, Grezes J, Glaser DE, Passingham RE, Haggard P (2006) Seeing or doing?
Influence of visual and motor familiarity in action observation. Curr Biol 16:1905–1910.
doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.065
Carroll WR, Bandura A (1982) The role of visual monitoring in observational learning of action
patterns: making the unobservable observable. J Mot Behav 14:153–167
Carroll WR, Bandura A (1985) Role of timing of visual monitoring and motor rehearsal in
observational learning of action patterns. J Mot Behav 17:269–281
Carroll WR, Bandura A (1990) Representational guidance of action production in
observational-learning—a causal-analysis. J Mot Behav 22:85–97
Carson RG, Thomas J, Summers JJ, Walters MR, Semjen A (1997) The dynamics of bimanual
circle drawing. Q J Exp Psychol 50A:664–683
Cattaneo L, Rizzolatti G (2009) The mirror neuron system. Neurol Rev 66:557–560
Cross ES, Hamilton A, Grafton ST (2006) Building a motor simulation de novo: observation of
dance by dancers. Neuroimage 31:1257–1267. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.033
The Coordination Dynamics of Observational Learning … 227
Cross ES, Kraemer DJM, Hamilton AFd, Kelley WM, Grafton ST (2009) Sensitivity of the action
observation network to physical and observational learning. Cereb Cortex 19:315–326
Cross ES, Liepelt R, Hamilton AFD, Parkinson J, Ramsey R, Stadler W, Prinz W (2012) Robotic
movement preferentially engages the action observation network. Hum Brain Mapp 33:
2238–2254. doi:10.1002/hbm.21361
Cutting JE, Proffitt DR (1982) The minimum principle and the perception of absolute, common,
and relative motions. Cogn Psychol 14:211–246
Cutting JE, Proffitt DR, Kozlowski LT (1978) A biomechanical invariant for gait perception. J Exp
Psychol Hum Percept Perform 4:357–372
de Guzman GC, Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ (1997) Self-organization of trajectory formation II.
Theoretical model. Biolobical Cybern 76:275–284
Gibson JJ (1979) The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin, Boston
Haken H (1983) Synergetics. Springer, Berlin
Haken H, Kelso JAS, Bunz H (1985) A theoretical model of phase transitions in human hand
movements. Biol Cybern 51:347–356
Hommel B (2004) Event files: feature binding in and across perception and action. Trends Cogn
Sci 8:494–500. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2004.08.007
Hommel B, Musseler J, Aschersleben G, Prinz W (2001) The theory of event coding (TEC): a
framework for perception and action planning. Behav Brain Sci 24:849–878. doi:10.1017/
s0140525x01000103
Johansson G (1973) Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Percept
Psychophys 14:201–211
Johansson G (1976) Spatio-temporal differentiation and integration in visual motion perception.
Psychol Res 38:27–63
Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J
Physiol 246:1000–1004
Kelso JAS (1994) The informational character of self-organized coordination dynamics. Hum Mov
Sci 13:393–413
Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. The MIT Press,
Cambridge
Kelso JAS, Buchanan JJ, Wallace SA (1991) Order parameters for the neural organization of
single limb, multijoint movement patterns. Exp Brain Res 85:432–445
Kelso JAS, Holt KG, Kugler PN, Turvey MT (1980) On the concept of coordinative structures as
dissipative structures: II. Emperical lines of convergence. In: Stelmach GE, Requin EJ
(eds) Tutorials in motor behavior. North-Holland Publishing Company, pp 49–70
Kelso JAS, Pandya AS (1991) Dynamic pattern generation and recognition. In: Badler NI,
Barsky BA, Zeltzer D (eds) Making them move: mechanics, control, and animation of
articulated figures. Morgan Kaufmann, San Mateo, pp 171–190
Kelso JAS, Scholz JP, Schoner G (1986) Nonequilibrium phase-transitions in coordinated
biological motion—critical fluctuations. Phys Lett A 118:279–284. doi:10.1016/0375-9601
(86)90359-2
Kovacs AJ, Shea CH (2011) The learning of 90o continuous relative phase with and without
Lissajous feedback: external and internally generated bimanual coordination. Acta Psychol
136:311–320
Kugler PN, Kelso JAS, Turvey MT, Stelmach GE, Requin J (1980) On the concept of coordinative
structure as dissipative structures: I. Theoretical lines of convergence. In: Tutorials in motor
behavior. North-Holland, New York, pp 1–47
Kugler PN, Turvey MT (1987) Information, natural law, and the self-assembly of rhythmic
movement. Erlbaum, Hillsdale
Maslovat D, Hodges NJ, Krigolson OE, Handy TC (2010) Observational practice benefits are
limited to perceptual improvements in the acquisition of a novel coordination skill. Exp Brain
Res 204:119–130. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2302-7
228 J.J. Buchanan
McAleer P, Pollick FE, Love SA, Crabbe F, Zacks JM (2014) The role of kinematics in cortical
regions for continuous human motion perception. Cognit Affect Behav Neurosci 14:307–318.
doi:10.3758/s13415-013-0192-4
Orlov T, Porat Y, Makin TR, Zohary E (2014) Hands in motion: an upper-limb-selective area
in the occipitotemporal cortex shows sensitivity to viewed hand kinematics. J Neurosci 34:
4882–4895. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.3352-13.2014
Prigogine I, Stengers I (1984) Order out of chaos: man’s new dialogue with nature. Bantam Books,
New York
Rizzolatti G, Craighero L (2004) The mirror-neuron system. Annu Rev Neurosci 27:169–192
Schöner G, Haken H, Kelso JAS (1986) A stochastic theory of phase transitions in human hand
movement. Biol Cybern 53:247–257
Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988a) Dynamic pattern generation in behavioral and neural systems.
Science 239:1513–1520
Schöner G, Kelso JAS (1988b) A synergetic theory of environmentally-specified and learned
patterns of movement coordination. 2 component oscillator dynamics. Biol Cybern 58:81–89.
doi:10.1007/bf00364154
Schöner G, Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1992) Learning as change of coordination dynamics: theory
and experiment. J Mot Behav 24:29–48
Scully DM (1986) Visual perception of technical execution and aesthetic quality in biological
motion. Hum Mov Sci 5:185–206
Scully DM, Carnegie E (1998) Observational learning in motor skill acquisition: a look at
demonstrations. Irish J Psychol 19:472–485
Scully DM, Newell KM (1985) Observational learning and the acquisition of motor skills: toward
a visual perception perspective. J Hum Mov Stud 11:169–186
Turvey MT (1990) Coordination. Amer Psychol 45:938–953
Wilson AD, Bingham GP (2008) Identifying the information for the visual perception of relative
phase. Percept Psychophys 70:465–476. doi:10.3758/PP.70.3.465
Wilson AD, Bingham GP, Craig JC (2003) Proprioceptive perception of phase variability. J Exp
Psychol Hum Percept Perform 29:1179–1190
Wilson AD, Collins DR, Bingham GP (2005) Human movement coordination implicates relative
direction as the information for relative phase. Exp Brain Res 165:351–361
Zaal FTJM, Bingham GP, Schmidt RC (2000) Visual perception of mean relative phase and phase
variability. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 26:1209–1220
Zanone PG, Kelso JAS (1992) The evolution of behavioral attractors with learning: nonequilib-
rium phase transitions. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 18:403–421
Rethinking the Study of Volition
for Clinical Use
Elizabeth B. Torres
Abstract Volition, the acquired voluntary control of our actions (at will), requires
from birth to development and beyond a proper balance across multiple layers of
the nervous systems. These levels range from the autonomic, to the automatic, to
the voluntary control level, providing as well taxonomy with phylogenetic order of
appearance in evolution. In the past few decades of movement research at the
behavioral and systems levels, there has been a paucity of studies focusing on the
possible contributions of involuntary movements to volitional control. Moreover,
the work focusing on voluntary behavior has given us a valuable body of knowl-
edge about constrained and highly over practiced activities while work involving
unrestrained, naturalistic behaviors remains scarce. Perhaps in making theoretical
assumptions about our data acquisition and analyses without properly empirically
verifying, these assumptions have left us with a somewhat skewed notion of how
we think the brain may be realizing the neural control of bodily motions; a notion
that does not exactly correspond to the outcome of the extant empirical work
assessing unrestrained movements as the nervous system acquires them and mod-
ifies skillful behaviors on demand. This chapter takes advantage of new techno-
logical advances and new analytics to invite rethinking some of the problems that
we study in movement science by enforcing somewhat oversimplified assumptions
on the data that we model, acquire, and analyze. I show that by relaxing our a priori
assumptions of normality, linearity and stationarity in data from biophysical
rhythms of the nervous systems, we would gain better insights into the motor
phenomena. Further, we would shy away from a “self-fulfilling prophesy” paradigm
with a tendency to a priori handcraft the outcome of our inquiry. The new lens to
study natural movements and their control includes as well involuntary motions that
Keywords Afference Voluntary Wearable sensor Motor noise Autism
Obstacle avoidance Speed Nonlinear dynamics Bayesian
Introduction
There are three scientists that have inspired the work that I will refer to in this
chapter: Nikolai Bernstein (Russia, 1896–1966), Erich von Holst (Germany, 1908–
1962), and Esther Thelen (US, 1941–2004). The legacy that we inherited in the
body of work that they created can be appreciated when we connect their theoretical
frameworks and examine the concept of volition as the foundation of action
ownership, agency, and cognitive autonomy over our physical actions and mental
thoughts.
Starting with the developmental stages of a newborn baby, Thelen provided the
framework to rethink infant neurodevelopment as a process that could be better
understood in the context of complex nonlinear dynamical systems (Smith and
Thelen 2003). Following up in human development, with research involving par-
ticipants throughout adulthood, Bernstein posed two fundamentally interrelated
problems of motor control that have a direct impact on volition from the start of life
and beyond. One refers to the inherent variability in our actions and the other to the
abundant degrees of freedom (DOF) in our bodies (Bernstein 1967). I posit that the
“glue” connecting these ideas to enable the possibility of casting movement sen-
sation as an integral part of cognitive processes is the principle of reafference by
von Holst and Mittelstaedt (1950) stating that “Voluntary movements show them-
selves to be dependent on the returning stream of afference which they themselves
cause.” This principle is used here to show examples of how to habilitate, reha-
bilitate, and enhance volition across different human populations.
Through the lens of a new statistical platform for the personalized analyses of
physiological rhythms underlying different layers of control within natural behav-
iors, I will underscore the importance of the empirical estimation of variability in
the bodily rhythms, the relevance of scaling up DOF in our experiments and
analyses, and the overall notions that a complex, nonlinear process with multi-
plicative statistics shall not be treated as an oversimplified, linear phenomenon with
assumed additive statistics. Careful consideration should be given to these features
inherently present in our data. This is particularly so when registering physiological
rhythms. Here, we aim at applying the ideas that these three giants of movement
science provided us with in order to further our science and drive clinical research.
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 231
The excess DOF problem posed the question of spatial consistency in the motor
output. How do we achieve a uniquely consistent movement path solution, given a
set of goals and context? How do we do so despite the large number of possibilities
that the body affords its end effectors? Along these lines, one could also ask how to
attain consistency in the temporal profile to travel along a stable spatial path. The
relationship between these two aspects of a movement trajectory is not as trivial as
it has been assumed. The general assumption has been that the output trajectory
already contains information on the systematic path in space and also on the timing
to travel along that path. The implicit assumption of many models of motor control
is that the time duration of a motion to be performed is a priori known and realized
in the motor trajectory (e.g., Flash and Hogan 1985; Uno et al. 1989; Biess et al.
2007, 2011, among others).
If a nervous system is trained to move within a given time constraint, it will do
so with great precision. Yet the very learning process that takes place varies greatly
along the two spatial and temporal components of the motor output trajectory
(Torres and Andersen 2006) and recruits different classes of neurons in the posterior
parietal cortex during the planning stage, before the movement execution (Torres
et al. 2013c). Within the pointing, reaching and reach-to-grasp family of
goal-directed motions, such variations in temporal profiles occur along a highly
conserved spatial hand path (Torres 2010) but also along a conserved postural path
despite the abundant DOF (Torres and Zipser 2002, 2004; Torres and Andersen
2006) (Fig. 1a). Indeed, the speed independence in goal-directed motion paths
(Atkeson and Hollerbach 1985; Nishikawa et al. 1999) has been difficult to rec-
oncile with the theoretical assumption that the timing of the motion is known a
priori. Somehow one would need a ticking clock set somewhere to make that idea
fit the empirical data.
Empirical data collected under unrestrained conditions, or without a priori
constraining the system to move within a predefined time period does not follow
such a pattern. Instead, when the system gradually learns, it seems to arrive at a
notion of time duration for a movement segment in a gradual way, by adjusting
different parameters as a function of task demands. For example, in obstacle
avoidance the distance traveled along an initial segment of the movement that falls
within 200–300 ms, before passing by the obstacle, seems to matter more than the
remaining distances in the initial stages of learning (Torres and Andersen 2006).
Then further adjustments along other sub-movements past the time to the firs
velocity peak, are performed till the endpoint is reached (Fig. 1a). Such adjustments
made at the level of the extrinsic path traveled by the end effector, correspond to
intrinsic postural adjustments. These intrinsic adjustments occur along multiple
DOF, recruiting and releasing them on demand. Under certain conditions (Torres
232 E.B. Torres
Fig. 1 Speed invariance of some kinematics parameters during the learning of obstacle
avoidance. a Straight reaches to a visual target are rather automatic after training. They follow
a bell-shaped speed profile with a very systematic timing whereby the peak velocity (marked by a
black dot on the hand trajectory and on the corresponding speed profile) is reached at a similar time
and special location along the hand path despite random performance across 15 targets where this
is one of them. The curved trajectories to the same target as in the straight reaches conserve their
curvature but have highly variable distances traveled along the initiation of the reach, up to the
peak velocity. Blue curves are early in the first five trials whereas green are later in the last five
trials of the session. During the learning period the movement time was rather variable, yet the
time to reach the peak was stable relative to the variations in the distance traveled during that time.
b Six different targets marked on the surface representing the time to the peak velocity
corresponding to each of 12 spatial locations, with error bars representing the deviations from the
estimated mean. Inset shows a speed invariant symmetry. This symmetry is a geometric index
conserved despite the trial-by-trial dramatic changes in movement dynamics (As in (a), blue is
earlier learning and green is for later trials when the obstacle avoidance skill was acquired).
Panel A previously published in (Torres et al. 2013c); panel B in (Torres 2013b)
and Andersen 2006), it is possible to locally linearize the forward and inverse maps
connecting the intrinsic and extrinsic parameters. This in turn reveals the variable
involvements of the dimensions spanning the kernel and the null subspaces of the
local linear transformation. The moment-by-movement variations in the intrinsic
kernel and the null subspaces have been interpreted as task-relevant and
task-incidental or supplementary subspaces (Torres 2001), respectively; but also as
those corresponding to the controlled or uncontrolled manifolds (Scholz and
Schoner 1999; Latash et al. 2002), respectively (albeit differences in the methods
used by these researchers to decompose the overall space spanned by all the DOF).
Besides the map linearization, there are other ways to examine variability in
movement parameters in light of geometric relationships between desired
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 233
Fig. 2 The breakdown of the geometric symmetry in different patient types along with the
recovery of the symmetry using different types of sensory guidance during a pointing task.
a Pointing task used in this experiment consisted of a continuous stroke forward and back to the
target. Landmarks are the point of peak velocity in each stroke along the hand trajectory. b A
geometric quantity (the area ratio) from the hand trajectory can be obtained in relation to the
Euclidean straight line. The red star represents the point where the motion-curve maximally bends
with respect to the line. The yellow area is then divided by the total area between the two unit
speed curves. An analytical expression (partial differential equation in Torres 2001) gives geodesic
curves for which this quantity is exactly 1/2. Likewise the perimeter ratio is obtained dividing the
partial perimeter around the yellow area and the total perimeter of the two curves combined. This
quantity when generated analytically is 1/2. These symmetries can then be empirically obtained to
ascertain the degree to which the actual data deviates from the ideal geodesic conditions.
c Example from one typical control showing the scatter around the ideal point (1/2, 1/2) from
hundreds of trajectories forward to the target (red) and backwards away from it (blue). d Data from
controls and patients under different forms of visual feedback while performing the task in (a).
First column shows the performance of a representative control (each dot represents a trial). The
tight scatter fit by the line of unity around the symmetry is conserved with similar slope for
forward and back movements across all conditions (no vision, relying on the memory of the target;
vision of the target, relying on the actual continuous visual feedback of the target; finger vision,
relying on the continuous vision of a light emitting diode affixed to the moving finger while the
target is off). Parkinson’s disease patients (along the second column) broke the symmetry but
recovered it with vision of the finger (Torres et al. 2011), as proprioception aligned with vision of
the moving hand/arm linkage. A stroke patient with a left PPC focal lesion (third column) benefits
instead from the vision of the target (Torres et al. 2010)
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 235
Perhaps our ways of thinking about the neural control of movements have been
shaped somehow by the instrumentation available; or perhaps we built the instru-
ments constrained by our ways of simplifying movements that we can model within
some framework that we may think is appropriate to address the problem at hand.
The use of a two-dimensional robotic linkage to probe arm movements prevails
throughout behavioral and systems neuroscience studies that aim at understanding
the mechanisms underlying the neural control of movements. Over the years, I have
asked several questions to many of the researchers behind these studies. These have
been, for the most part, intuitive questions, questions driven by common sense and
seeking to build a solution to the motor control problems that stem from first
principles. I was surprised by the answers I got from everyone I asked. It was as
though the answers to the very questions they were asking were already there,
facilitated by the methods used in data acquisition and/or by the theoretical
framework of choice. In reading the methods of these papers, I knew beforehand
what the answer was going to be under those conditions, and so did those who I
asked, but that, in my eyes, takes away the fun of doing science.
As someone trained first in Mathematics and then in Applied Mathematics, I
knew the dangers of taking a model created to solve a particular problem and
applying that model (somewhat blindly) to another problem domain without
properly verifying its feasibility or the main underlying assumptions. As an
example to illustrate my point, I had already done some work using Pontriagin’s
Optimal Control framework when I started graduate school and took an interest in
the field of motor control. I knew very well that one of the reasons for its invention
was to be able to travel to space and back in optimal time because fuel expenditure
was of the essence. It was a great solution to that problem (Pontriagin 1990).
Bringing that notion to frame the human motor control problem required solving
a time-dependent and time-constrained cost functional. The solution, if one can be
obtained in closed form, is an optimal trajectory providing a movement prescription
in space and time. But that could not possibly be conducive of a solution that a
newborn infant spontaneously came up with. This framework, so good to model the
control of mechanical systems, took away the trial and error element in the whole
learning process of an infant. In my eyes, the newborn using this model would have
to know a priori (or be prompted to learn) how long each movement segment took.
And yet, that is precisely part of what the newborn infant spontaneously comes to
learn. It is an emergent property, rather than an a priori property of that system.
The optimal control framework was not good enough to understand the essence
of the problem at hand or to capture the true nature of the empirical data obtained
from unrestrained motions. Something more fundamental was missing: the missing
ingredients were the variations and their rates of change at all levels of control. How
can one study such variations and characterize their signatures as the person
236 E.B. Torres
behaves throughout the day? And how can one do so as the many segments of the
body spontaneously co-articulate forming different patterns? And as the ebb and
flow of deliberate or automatic autonomy in our actions changes from moment to
moment? I did not think that posing the problem of inverse dynamics the way I had
read it in the literature was going to help me think of the essential problems the
nervous system may face during development. Something else was needed.
The advent of wearable sensing technology that simultaneously registers signals
from various layers of the nervous systems (Fig. 3) made a dent in the research
program in my lab. From cell phone accelerometers (Torres 2013a) to
research-grade wearables (Torres 2013b), we set to develop new analytics and data
types that would enable the integration of multisensory data from different control
levels to create new ways to track the noise-to-signal transitions across the proposed
control taxonomy of the nervous systems (Fig. 3a) in a rather personalized manner.
We set to also do so in social situations, such as those requiring dyadic interactions.
Fig. 3 Proposed taxonomy of control levels defining volition as the proper balance in variability
across the levels (Torres 2011). Different waveforms are registered using contemporary wearable
sensing technology during unrestrained, naturalistic motions. This technology enables the study of
the different levels of control, spanning from autonomic (e.g., heart rate, inter-heartbeat-interval
IBI, temperature, etc.) to automatic (e.g., gait patterns captured with accelerometers and
gyroscopes), and other motion signatures of actively produced movements under deliberate control
(e.g., reaches, ballet, boxing, etc.). The fluctuations in motor performance can be extracted from
each waveform and integrated (e.g., motion and temperature) to estimate in a personalized manner
the transitions from spontaneous random noise to well-structured signals with high statistical
regularities conducive of a predictive code (see next figure)
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 237
We wanted to be able to track those noise-to-signal transitions within the time limits
of the lab experiments (Nguyen et al. 2014; 2015) and longitudinally across several
months at a time (Torres and Lande 2015). We wanted to learn about the temporal
dynamics of the system’s inherent layers of variability without presupposing any-
thing about it. These types of data were not available in the literature of motor
control. Thus, to build our new statistical platform, we needed to build such datasets
ourselves under unrestrained conditions, as people go about their lives any given
day.
b Fig. 4 The new approach to motor control is contrasted to the traditional methods. a Suppose that a
time series of a waveform is gathered with high resolution instrumentation and some 3000 frames are
gathered. The traditional model would epoch the time series (e.g., 100 frames per second each trial)
and average all trials under the a priori assumption of normality. This model examines the motion data
without empirically evaluating the Gaussian assumption and merely assuming the theoretical
Gaussian moments. Data loss is noted by the ellipses as fluctuations are smoothed out as ‘noise’ when
in reality they contain signal. This is the “one size fits all approach” currently dominating the field.
b Suppose now that we do not assume additive statistics a priori, then the same time series is used to
empirically estimate the probability distribution that the fluctuations give rise to along the continuous
stream of motion. To this end one can continuously gather the peaks of the normalized waveform
(values ranging from 0 to 1) and obtain a frequency histogram, then compare across different families
of probability distributions which one fits the data best (e.g., using maximum likelihood estimation).
This estimation can be done for each individual using the same number of frames. Since different
motion types across different components of the nervous systems will span different probability
distribution functions (PDF), a person is really characterized by a whole family of PDFs and the rates
of change of the stochastic signatures from context to context will help empirically define truly
personalized adaptive indexes in a stochastic sense, preserving as well the inherently nonlinear
dynamic nature of the motion data. c Examples of estimated family of PDFs using the continuous
Gamma family and classifying each individual signature on the Gamma parameter plane spanned by
the shape and the scale (dispersion) of the distributions with 95 % confidence intervals. As the
signatures are nonstationary they can be reestimated to track changes on the plane as a stochastic
trajectory. These changes reveal the evolution of the noise-to-signal ratio which on the Gamma case is
the scale parameter. d Computation of the noise-to-signal transitions (changes in amplitude and
frequency) based on the switching between quadrants automatically defined by the median values of
the estimated parameters (black lines are the median values across all the shape and scale empirically
estimated parameters from taking blocks of x number of frames and a sliding window of y number of
frames, where x and y depend on the sampling resolution of the sensors: for example at 240 Hz, with
block size of 1 minute and sliding window size of 1/2 minute it is possible to provide statistical power
for very tight confidence intervals for the estimates, i.e., negligible error relative to the bandwidth of
the data range). Colors are coded as in c. The points are joined by lines in the order in which they occur
to form stochastic trajectories. The insets show the histograms of points in the Upper Quadrant LUQ
(high noise, high skewness toward the exponential limiting case with shape value of 1) versus those in
the Right Lower Quadrant RLQ (low noise, toward symmetric shapes with the Gaussian distribution
as the limiting case). The insets are histograms counting the transitions of the signatures within each
quadrant (stationary transitions) and between each quadrant (nonstationary transitions). The peaks
are another way to visualize the magnitude of the shifts on the Gamma plane. d Quantifications of the
frequency of noise-to-signal transition from high noise-to-signal LUQ to low noise-to-signal RLQ. In
this example, the atypical example had very low percentages of noise-to-signal transitions between
the quadrants indicating stagnation in motor adaptation to physical growth and signaling poor
development of neuromotor control. Note that panel e criteria serve as a classification index of typical
versus atypical neurodevelopment. Panel D published in (Torres et al. 2016)
Sometime in the Spring of 1999, while I was a graduate student at the Department
of Cognitive Science at UCSD, one of my Professors, the late Elizabeth Bates,
introduced me to a lovely lady who did movement-related work in the context of
infant development. I spent an entire afternoon engaged in one of the most
240 E.B. Torres
memorable conversations of my career. She was Esther Thelen. I had never studied
how babies learned to move, yet her ideas instantly resonated with my own way of
thinking about many of the motor control problems that I was interested in.
Thelen’s ideas certainly left such a mark in my way of thinking about these
problems, that today I cannot envision the study of motor control without con-
sidering her dynamical systems perspective on neurodevelopment as a framework
to better understand the intertwined relations between motor control and the
emergence and maintenance of cognition (Thelen and Smith 1994).
Fig. 5 Non-linear dynamics and fast evolving stochastic signatures of early development in
newborn infants shall not be treated as linear processes with a priori assumed stationary normality
a Data from newborn infants (13,362 males and 13,623 females) extracted from the longitudinal
tracking of physical growth to build the World Health Organization (WHO) growth charts
(Kuczmarski et al. 2002). The changes in weight of the newborn infant follow an accelerated rate
of change with initial negative (drop) followed by an accelerated (daily) increase. Inset zooms into
the first month of life and shows the differences between males and females. The black dot signals
a critical point in the statistical regimes of variability according to the reported generalized
coefficient of variation tracked in b. Inset shows an earlier critical point in the first week of life
which separates boys and girls. This separation is even more evident later on at 224 days for
females and 254 days for males (a month later). Notice that this critical change in stochastic
variations so evident in this rate of change data remains hidden to the pediatricians using absolute
rather than incremental weight values. c The WHO charts is built using the percentiles of enforced
normal distributions. Yet the L parameter of the power (Box-Cox) transformation reported in the
methods to construct these charts shows the changes in the skewness of the actual weight
distributions. Notice how improper it is to enforce the normal distribution in the nonlinear physical
growth data of newborn infants. d The rates of change in physical growth of 36 newborn infants
(12 typically born and 24 prelabeled clinically at risk) who were followed for 6 months. The
physical growth parameters (head circumference, body length and body weight) divided by the
number of days since birth are median ranked. The red group is composed of the typically
developing infants with the highest rank in all three parameters. The z-axis is the Alberta Infant
Motor Scale (AIMS) a metric of readiness to walk (higher is better). The size of the circle is the
rate of change in head circumference (cm/day). Blue circles are typically born babies while red
circles are babies at risk (premature). The other surfaces are fit to scatters of infants ranked in
orderly way lower than the first group. The gray scatter is the lowest ranked. e The rates of change
in the frequency of noise-to-signal transitions plotted as a function of the rates of change in
physical growth for three groups: G1 is the Ranked 1 group (red), G2 is the Ranked 2 and 3 groups
combined (blue shades), G3 is the Ranked 4 group (gray). Babies in the highest ranked group G1
of physical growth show a linear relation with the rate of change in stochastic signatures of
neuromotor control. Babies in the group at high risk of neurodevelopmental derail are the lowest
ranked (gray) group in d. They are the ones with the flat slope, denoting stagnation in the
development of motor control (motions measured with inertial measurement units, accelerometers
and temperature at 128 Hz, for 8 h continuously in each of 3 visits every 2 months). The
intermediate group G2 falls between the two extremes (Torres et al. 2016)
242 E.B. Torres
Babies that developed normally had rates of physical growth congruent with the
rates of change of scores indicating the readiness to walk (Fig. 5d). Their patterns
of motion were further characterized by fluctuations that transitioned from noise to
signal with high frequency and large amplitude of shifts between quadrants in the
statistical Gamma parameter plane. As their body grew at an accelerated pace, so
did the changes in neural motor control tending to more frequently organized
patterns with lower rates of spontaneous random noise and higher rates of sys-
tematically predictive signals. Whenever the rates of change in such noise-to-signal
patterns became stagnated in relation to those of the physical bodily growth, the
relationship uncovered for typically born and typically developing babies broke
down in the babies at risk (Group 3 in Fig. 5e ranked 4 in 5d).
As the babies’ motions transitioned from spontaneous and random to more
organized and systematic they denoted the emergence of voluntary control. Then in
due time, we referred to von Holst’s notion that at the motor output level it is also
possible to estimate the nature of “the returning afferent stream,” which these
emergent voluntary movements themselves cause. We know today that this
evolving relationship between noise-to-signal transitions and physical body growth
must occur at congruent rates, changing in tandem if development is to occur along
the typical pathway (Fig. 5e). When this does not occur it is very likely that in due
time we will be in the presence of a neurodevelopmental disorder, perhaps even
more specifically of one labeled autism.
Autism has been described as a disorder of the mind, a cognitive disorder where the
social and communication skills that typically emerge in young children by the age
of 3–4 years old does not appear in a child that goes on to receive this diagnosis.
The child lives in social isolation, often bullied and excluded by peers that do not
understand that this child senses the world differently and as such acts differently.
The child is trapped within routines and rituals that do not correspond to social
expectations. Yet, very likely those odd “ritualistic behaviors” help dampen the
high uncertainty of the external social world. We learned through our research that
most likely such rituals also keep the child safe from the extremely high uncertainty
that his/her self-generated motions contain. These motions persistently have high
random and noisy signatures. The proposition by von Holst that “voluntary
movements show themselves to be dependent on the returning stream of afference
which they themselves cause” is violated in autism. Their signatures—better fit by
the memoryless Exponential distribution—denote the occurrence of every event
anew, with no accumulation of information conducive of a predictive code.
Figure 6a shows the comparison of the empirically estimated PDF’s for autism and
controls. The much wider dispersion and lack of diversification of shapes across the
population of different ages denote the absence of maturation in autism. Unlike
typical development showing the transitions in noise-to-signal ratios and the
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 243
evolution in the shapes of the distributions from highly skewed to symmetric ones,
autism statistical signatures are “stuck.”
Indeed, in autism the signature of well-organized, systematically predictive
signal indicative of voluntary control has not appeared by 4 years of age; is absent
at 10 years of age; is not there either at 25 years of age and beyond. In the context
of 176 individuals whose motor output stochastic patterns we have characterized
during goal-directed reaches (Torres et al. 2016), the presence of high noise and
highly skewed probability distributions tending toward the memoryless (most
random) exponential distribution in autism remains prevalent throughout all ages
(Brincker and Torres 2013). This prevalence remains as well to some extent when
there is some capacity for language, but never reaches the typical ranges of
noise-to-signal ratios quantified in controls. Those children who acquire some
language (some phrases, some echolalia, etc.) may show to some degree less dis-
persion in the distributions empirically estimated from the fluctuations in motor
performance. Yet, even at young adulthood, these signatures resemble more those
of a typically developing 3-year-old child than those of a young adult peer.
Figure 6A shows this result in two panels (taken from Torres et al. 2013a).
Something is amiss in the course of maturation of the sensory-motor systems. As in
the babies at risk of neurodevelopmental derail, these individual’s stochastic sig-
natures of fluctuations in motor performance of the voluntary kind (from
goal-directed motions) lack the proper frequency in the transitions from noise to
signal. There is stagnation in the evolution of their noise patterns. Excess noise and
randomness of those patterns is the norm in this ASD population. According to
these statistical signatures, the individual with ASD must live “in the here and
now.”
b Fig. 6 Stagnation in the noise levels of individuals with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders
across life and meds influence in their motor control. a Typical development of the stochastic
signatures derived from goal-directed behavior shows an evolution from random noise to
systematic signal in typically developing individuals. In particular, the transition from 3 years old
(green) to 4 years old (blue) shows a diversification in the shapes and dispersions of the PDFs that
remains absent from the individuals with ASD, regardless of age, sex, or verbal capabilities. By
college age the controls have very low dispersion and symmetric distributions. The presence of
higher noise (broad dispersion) in ASD does not go away with age. There is no maturation as in
the controls. b Involuntary micro-motions of the head captured in the fMRI and reported in the
ABIDE databases. Actual physical excursions of the head in controls (CT) and ASD are captured
by linear displacement and angular rotations throughout the scanning session. The excess
involuntary head motions and the higher rates of noise are present in ASD whether or not they are
on meds. Yet, the intake of psychotropic medications exacerbates the noise levels systematically
with the number of meds the person is on. This effect is true for both linear displacements and
angular rotations of the head. Panel A figures published in (Torres et al. 2013a); panel B
Figure published in (Torres and Denisova 2016) licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution
4.0 International License
ASD formulated using fMRI data (Deen and Pelphrey 2012). It has been difficult to
assess such controversial data because each lab “scrubs” different number of scans
from their sets, so the best bet to unravel the mystery of excess motions in ASD or
in other disorders of the nervous systems, is to look at all the scans from the original
data. Given the head motion data, the remaining problem is how to extract
meaningful waveforms from these raw data so as to pose the question of possible
motor control differences in ASD.
Using the new statistical platform for personalized analyses of micro-movements, I
analyzed the time series of head linear displacements and head angular rotations
during resting state across a number of sites. Specifically, using methods in Fig. 4b–d,
I was able to extract the micro-movements waveform from the head motions of
1048 participants with ASD, ADHD, and controls. The idea was to identify and
characterize stochastic signatures of involuntary micro-movements embedded in the
head motions of participants within these populations.
Given the conceptualization of a necessary balance across the proposed taxon-
omy of control levels (Fig. 3a) to have proper volition (Torres 2011; Torres et al.
2011), I reasoned that excess involuntary motions of the head may flag impairments
in the ability to remain still at will (a superb accomplishment of volitional control).
But how could we distinguish between inherent issues with the sensory-motor
systems and problems induced by psychotropic meds so heavily prescribed across
the ASD and ADHD populations?
The ABIDE and the ADHD-200 databases list as well the psychotropic meds for
a large number of participants affected by the disorders. These drugs have known
side effects in the motor systems (reported in their labels and acknowledged by their
makers in publicly available websites). Since motor side effects confound the issue
of motor problems in ASD, it has been difficult to address motor issues in the
research arena. I set to test the idea of separating motor issues in ASD and ADHD
by comparing those participants under psychotropic meds and participants that were
medication-naïve, both in relation to controls.
To this end, I first characterized the levels of head micro-movements noise in
controls that did not take meds and were typical. Then I examined the signatures of
those fellows who had a diagnosis of ASD/ADHD but took no psychotropic drugs
and compared the PDF’s to those also in these spectrums who did take psychotropic
drugs. The questions were (1) if medication-naïve individuals with ASD/ADHD
had atypical levels of noise accumulation in the micro-movements of head motions;
(2) if psychotropic drug intake had a systematic impact on those estimated signa-
tures of the fellows under psychotropic drugs.
Several interesting results emerged from these analyses. Individuals with autism
have a prevalence of involuntary head motions, even when their head is padded and
they are lying in a resting position. The frequency of the micro-movements and the
rates of accumulation of their noise are significantly higher from those of neu-
rotypical controls. More important yet, the probability distribution functions
describing the micro-movements systematically increase the noise-to-signal ratio
and the skewness with the number of psychotropic drugs that they take. This can be
seen in Fig. 6b. This figure shows the profound differences in statistical signatures
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 247
The analyses of the fMRI open access databases showed us excess noise and the
prevalence of randomness in the micro-movements’ signatures of involuntary head
motions of 1048 participants. Despite this prevalence in ASD and ADHD partici-
pants, we next asked whether such noisy and random signatures in the motor output
could be reshaped. We reasoned that in the likely absence of proper kinesthetic
reafference, we could close the feedback loops with external input. This form of
sensory substitution had already worked for an extreme case of actual physical
deafferentation: the case of Ian Waterman.
IW is a very special subject who lost his proprioception from the neck down but
managed to walk and move purposefully again by replacing the lost proprioception
with vision and motor imagery (Cole 1995). We had the opportunity to reexamine
IW in the lab recently, as we had studied his motor performance under this sta-
tistical platform and found that his signatures of voluntary and spontaneous motor
performance resembled those of the ASD participants, particularly and more
importantly when IW was moving in the dark with no visual feedback (Torres et al.
2014, 2016). If IW, who had actual physical deafferentation, was able to restore
248 E.B. Torres
volition in his actions using external (visual) feedback, there was a chance that the
children with ASD could do so as well. The challenge was that these children were
nonverbal, or had very limited speech. Communicating with the children was
challenging for us. Instructing or prompting an experiment was not feasible.
We developed an interface to spontaneously, without instructions, evoke voli-
tional control in these children. It had to be done gradually, first dampening the
noise and randomness of the fluctuations in motor performance in supplementary
(involuntary) motions, with the hopes of positively impacting—or even evoking in
some cases—the voluntary level of control. This strategy would bring into play von
Holst’s principle of reafference as soon as they developed motions with the sta-
tistical signatures of voluntary control. To that end we needed to first evoke the
self-discovery of a GOAL, and then encourage the search for a systematic pattern. If
the children’s visual and auditory systems were able to detect change in sensory
input, we could do this.
Using Thelen’s notion of goal self-discovery in infants through a process of trial
and error, we designed a closed-loop interface that achieved the above-mentioned
objectives. Figure 7A shows the idea (Torres et al. 2013d), which is rather simple.
We embedded a region of interest (ROI) in the peripersonal space of the child (this
space around the child moves with the child, as it is defined relative to the child’s
body). We had positional sensors (Polhemus Liberty 240 Hz, Colchester VT) on
the child’s hand coded relative to the child, as well as relative to a world axis. Thus,
every time the child passed the hand through the ROI, even if he naturally moved
around, we could trigger external media to attract his attention. Each of the children
figured out the ROI within minutes. They discovered the GOAL: namely, to move
the hand to the ROI so as to sustain the media playing continuously. The media
could be a movie of their liking with sound of their liking, or simply their own
image projected from a video camera in front of them.
The children’s self-discovery of the GOAL led to trial and error till they pin-
pointed exactly where the hand had to be located in order for the media to play.
They self-discovered cause and effect by turning random motions into
GOAL-directed motions under voluntary control. Then, through that trial and error
process (similar to the arm flailing that an infant would do when learning to reach)
they developed the stochastic signatures of volition in their motions. Specifically we
were able to quantify all the while the frequency and stationarity of noise-to-signal
transitions in their voluntary reaches to the GOAL as well as in the coexisting
spontaneous motion segments continuously supplementing the emergent
GOAL-directed ones. Figure 7b shows snapshots of these noise-to-signal transi-
tions using the empirically estimated probability distributions as well as the Gamma
plane log–log plots, whereby each point is an estimated PDF from continuous
accumulations of measurements of angular velocity with shifting signatures as the
system learns over the time span of minutes. At the end of the session, when they
held the arm in place to keep the hand in the ROI that played the media, their
stochastic patterns had spontaneously acquired the typical signatures that we had
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 249
Fig. 7 Habilitating and enhancing volition in ASD. a Simple paradigm using the notion of body–
machine interfaces to help the child self-discover a goal and pursue it actively to evoke volition in
the motions. The child wears sensors that register the position at all times and the distance to a
virtual region of interest (vRoI) that we create. When the hand passes through the vRoI a movie
and music is triggered on the screen. The child actively searches for that location, first through trial
and error, then systematically till she discovers the goal location and holds the hand in it steadily to
watch the video continuously. b Snapshots of the frequency histograms accumulating the
fluctuations in angular velocity while the hand rotates and translates through space. Motions
outside and inside the vRoI are tracked and the stochastic trajectory shown on the Gamma
parameter plane. c The shifts from typically developing and ASD children converging toward
symmetric probability distributions with low dispersion, i.e., high signal with statistical regularities
characteristic of typical controls with high volition. The 25 nonverbal children with ASD mastered
this task on their own and in the process learned how to control their movements at will. Figure
from (Torres et al. 2013d)
quantified in typical peers at the same school. Were these changes transient? We
had witnessed in those same children that prompting them during Applied
Behavioral Analysis (ABA) therapy led to a somewhat robotic structure in their
actions. Furthermore, over the school breaks, despite daily drilling sessions of
250 E.B. Torres
hundreds of rote repetitions, they would come back from the break needing con-
siderable effort to achieve the level of performance they had left at before the break.
We came back to the school 4–5 weeks after our first and only session using this
setup. We found that despite no practice at all, the children rapidly reacquired the
ROI and the desired well-structured, systematic noise levels with the statistical
regularities of a predictive code.
An important lesson from this study was that the signature of motor noise in the
system of a child with a diagnosis of autism has the potential to change into typical
ranges beyond the transient goals of a controlled experiment. This potential can be
turned into the possibility for generalization and transfer of these motor phenomena
from the lab to the activities of daily living, a future goal for investigation in our
lab.
These children’s CNS can spontaneously learn the bridge between intentional
thoughts and well-controlled actions that follow a purpose. They can gain volition
of their bodily rhythms at the level of motor performance that we measured. That is
a first step into teaching them gestural communication, a form of language that they
could use for social exchange.
The social deficits that the DSM-5 labels these children with at the exclusion of
sensory-motor components keep this disorder as a mental deficit that can be heavily
medicated. The fMRI data showing the dramatic effect that psychotropic meds have
on these children seriously question the validity of a diagnosis system that has
financial ties with pharmaceutical companies (Cosgrove et al. 2006, 2014;
Cosgrove and Krimsky 2012). The question then remains as to whether the
DSM-makers and BigPharma will ever inform the parents of the deleterious effects
that those drugs have on the sensory-motor systems of their children.
There is precious potential in the individuals with autism. All we have to do is
harness it and put it to the service of the neural control of movements. Our field
could make a tremendous contribution to this because researchers in it have the
knowledge and the tools to do it.
Concluding Remarks
This chapter provides a new way to examine the concept of volition and use it in
clinical settings. I revisited some old motor control questions using new data types
and new analytics through the lens of unrestrained behaviors. Several examples of
possible applications of this new statistical platform were also discussed and an
invitation was extended to the Motor Control field at large to delve into the issue of
volition with relation to clinical applications. The work presented here was inspired
by the ever fresh ideas of Bernstein, von Holst, and Thelen. I cannot envision
contemporary motor control without their influence as we renovate the body of
knowledge that we have accumulated in our field and extend it to the neurode-
velopmental arena.
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 251
References
American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force (2013) Diagnostic and statistical manual of
mental disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association, Washington, DC
Atkeson CG, Hollerbach JM (1985) Kinematic features of unrestrained vertical arm movements.
J Neurosci 5:2318–2330
Bernstein N (1967) The co-ordination and regulation of movements. Oxford Press, Oxford
Biess A, Liebermann DG, Flash T (2007) A computational model for redundant human
three-dimensional pointing movements: integration of independent spatial and temporal motor
plans simplifies movement dynamics. J Neurosci 27:13045–13064. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.
4334-06.2007 27/48/13045 [pii]
Biess A, Flash T, Liebermann DG (2011) Riemannian geometric approach to human arm
dynamics, movement optimization, and invariance. Phys Rev E: Stat, Nonlin, Soft Matter Phys
83:031927
Brincker M, Torres EB (2013) Noise from the periphery in autism. Front Integr Neurosci 7:34.
doi:10.3389/fnint.2013.00034
Churchland MM, Afshar A, Shenoy KV (2006a) A central source of movement variability. Neuron
52:1085–1096. doi:S0896-6273(06)00871-3
252 E.B. Torres
Churchland MM, Yu BM, Ryu SI, Santhanam G, Shenoy KV (2006b) Neural variability in
premotor cortex provides a signature of motor preparation. J Neurosci 26:3697–3712.
doi:26/14/3697
Churchland MM, Yu BM, Cunningham JP et al (2010) Stimulus onset quenches neural variability:
a widespread cortical phenomenon. Nat Neurosci 13:369–378. doi:nn.2501
Cole J (1995) Pride and a daily marathon. MIT Press, Cambridge
Cosgrove L, Krimsky S (2012) A comparison of DSM-IV and DSM-5 panel members’ financial
associations with industry: a pernicious problem persists. PLoS Med 9:e1001190. doi:10.1371/
journal.pmed.1001190
Cosgrove L, Krimsky S, Vijayaraghavan M, Schneider L (2006) Financial ties between DSM-IV
panel members and the pharmaceutical industry. Psychother Psychosom 75:154–160.
doi:10.1159/000091772
Cosgrove L, Krimsky S, Wheeler EE, Kaitz J, Greenspan SB, DiPentima NL (2014) Tripartite
conflicts of interest and high stakes patent extensions in the DSM-5. Psychother Psychosom
83:106–113. doi:10.1159/000357499
Deen B, Pelphrey K (2012) Perspective: brain scans need a rethink. Nature 491:S20
Faisal AA, Selen LP, Wolpert DM (2008) Noise in the nervous system. Nat Rev Neurosci 9:
292–303. doi:nrn2258
Flash T, Hogan N (1985) The coordination of arm movements: an experimentally confirmed
mathematical model. J Neurosci 5:1688–1703
Friston KJ, Holmes AP, Poline JB, Grasby PJ, Williams SC, Frackowiak RS, Turner R (1995)
Analysis of fMRI time-series revisited. Neuroimage 2:45–53. doi:0.1006/nimg.1995.1007
Hamilton AF, Wolpert DM (2002) Controlling the statistics of action: obstacle avoidance.
J Neurophysiol 87:2434–2440
Harris CM, Wolpert DM (1998) Signal-dependent noise determines motor planning. Nature
394:780–784. doi:10.1038/29528
Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2004a) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427:
244–247. doi:10.1038/nature02169
Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2004b) The loss function of sensorimotor learning. Proc Natl Acad
Sci USA 101:9839–9842. doi:10.1073/pnas.0308394101
Kording KP, Wolpert DM (2006) Bayesian decision theory in sensorimotor control. Trends Cogn
Sci 10:319–326. doi:S1364-6613(06)00127-6
Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Grummer-Strawn LM et al (2000) CDC growth charts: United States.
Adv Data 1–27
Kuczmarski RJ, Ogden CL, Guo SS et al (2002) 2000 CDC growth charts for the United States:
methods and development. Vital Health Stat 11:1–190
Latash ML, Scholz JP, Schoner G (2002) Motor control strategies revealed in the structure of
motor variability. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 30:26–31
Limpert E, Stahel WA (2011) Problems with using the normal distribution and ways to improve
quality and efficiency of data analysis. PLoS ONE 6:e21403. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.
002140310-PONE-RA-16396
Limpert E, Stahel WA, Abbt M (2001) Log-normal distributions across the sciences: keys and
clues. Bioscience 51:341–352
Nguyen J, Papathomas TV, Ravaliya JH, Torres EB (2014) Methods to explore the influence of
top-down visual processes on motor behavior. J Vis Exp. doi:10.3791/51422
Nguyen J, Majmudar UV, Ravaliya JH, Papathomas TV, Torres EB (2015) Automatically
characterizing sensory-motor patterns underlying reach-to-grasp movements on a physical
depth inversion illusion. Front Hum Neurosci 9:694. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00694
Nishikawa KC, Murray ST, Flanders M (1999) Do arm postures vary with the speed of reaching?
J Neurophysiol 81:2582–2586
Piper MC, Pinnell LE, Darrah J, Maguire T, Byrne PJ (1992) Construction and validation of the
Alberta Infant Motor Scale (AIMS). Can J Public Health 83 Suppl 2:S46–50
Pontriagin LS (1990) Optimal control and differential games: collection of papers. American
Mathematical Society, Providence
Rethinking the Study of Volition for Clinical Use 253
Torres EB, Quian Quiroga R, Cui H, Buneo CA (2013c) Neural correlates of learning and
trajectory planning in the posterior parietal cortex. Front Integr Neurosci 7:39. doi:10.3389/
fnint.2013.00039
Torres EB, Yanovich P, Metaxas DN (2013d) Give spontaneity and self-discovery a chance in
ASD: spontaneous peripheral limb variability as a proxy to evoke centrally driven intentional
acts. Front Integr Neurosci 7:46. doi:10.3389/fnint.2013.00046
Torres EB, Cole J, Poizner H (2014) Motor output variability, deafferentation, and putative deficits
in kinesthetic reafference in Parkinson’s disease. Front Hum Neurosci 8:823. doi:10.3389/
fnhum.2014.00823
Torres EB, Isenhower RW, Nguyen J et al (2016) Toward precision psychiatry: statistical platform
for the personalized characterization of natural behaviors. Front Neurol 7:8. doi:10.3389/fneur.
2016.00008
Uno Y, Kawato M, Suzuki R (1989) Formation and control of optimal trajectory in human
multijoint arm movement. Minimum torque-change model. Biol Cybern 61:89–101
van Beers RJ (2009) Motor learning is optimally tuned to the properties of motor noise. Neuron
63:406–417. doi:S0896-6273(09)00516-9
van Beers RJ, Baraduc P, Wolpert DM (2002) Role of uncertainty in sensorimotor control. Philos
Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 357:1137–1145. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1101
van Beers RJ, Haggard P, Wolpert DM (2004) The role of execution noise in movement
variability. J Neurophysiol 91:1050–1063. doi:10.1152/jn.00652.200300652.2003
van Beers RJ, Brenner E, Smeets JB (2013) Random walk of motor planning in task-irrelevant
dimensions. J Neurophysiol 109:969–977. doi:10.1152/jn.00706.2012
von Holst E, Mittelstaedt H (1950) The principle of reafference: interactions between the central
nervous system and the peripheral organs. In: Dodwell PC (ed) Perceptual Processing: stimulus
equivalence and pattern recognition. Appleton-Century-Crofts, New York, pp 41–72
Part V
Impaired Motor Control and
Rehabilitation
Motor Lateralization Provides
a Foundation for Predicting and Treating
Non-paretic Arm Motor Deficits in Stroke
Keywords Bilateral hemispheric model Contralesional Paresis Ipsilesional
Rehabilitation Dynamic dominance Jebsen-Taylor hand function test Interlimb
transfer
Fig. 1 Multidirection reaching movements in patients with unilateral stroke (Schaefer et al.
2009b). a Reaching movements in the non-paretic arm of patients with left (LHD) and right
(RHD) hemisphere damage. b Variance in hand positions during the initial trajectory phase
(bottom ellipses), and in the final position (top ellipses) of the movement
reaching movements in the non-paretic arm of patients with left and right hemi-
sphere damage. While patients with left hemisphere damage (LHD) made highly
curved movements with accurate final positions, patients with right hemisphere
damage (RHD) made straight movements with poor final position accuracies.
Figure 1b shows the variance in hand positions during the initial trajectory phase
(bottom ellipses), and in the final position (top ellipses) of the movement. These
findings demonstrate a robust double dissociation between hemisphere of damage
and motor control process effected by a unilateral hemisphere lesion: While RHD
patients have deficits in stabilizing accurate final positions across trials, LHD
patients have deficits in stabilizing accurate initial trajectories across trials.
Motor Lateralization Provides a Foundation for Predicting … 261
These results provide evidence that each hemisphere contributes different control
mechanisms to both arms, and have been corroborated by studies that examined the
effects of unilateral brain damage on adaptation to novel visuomotor rotations
(Mutha et al. 2011a, b), and studies that have examined responses to sudden visual
perturbations (Schaefer et al. 2012). This bihemispheric model of arm control also
predicts hemisphere specific deficits in the contralesional arm of stroke patients, a
prediction that has been supported in patients with mild paresis (Mani et al. 2013).
Fig. 2 Data from 72 age and gender matched control participants (36 using the right arm, and 36
using the left arm), 22 LHD survivors, and 29 RHD survivors. Y-axis represents the Jebsen-Taylor
Hand Function Test score. Data are normalized to control subjects’ dominant arm performance
(100 %). X-axis stratified by hand (in the case of stroke survivors, this is always the non-paretic,
ipsilesional arm), and severity of contralesional deficit
the Fugl-Meyer et al. (1975) assessment of motor impairment (mild > 55, moder-
ate > 35, Severe ≤ 35).
As illustrated in Fig. 2, in stroke survivors, JTHFT performance with the
non-paretic arm was impacted substantially by both the severity of paretic arm
impairment and the side of the brain that was damaged. Participants with the most
severe paresis in the contralesional arm had the greatest motor deficit in the
non-paretic arm. This effect depended on the side of the lesion, such that LHD was
associated with greater non-paretic arm deficits than RHD. More specifically, LHD
survivors took, on average, 300 % longer to complete the tasks than age matched
control participants. Thus, stroke survivors who are forced to rely most extensively
on their non-paretic arms for performance of ADL have the greatest deficits in
coordination in the non-paretic arm. The fact that we tested these stroke survivors
1.8 years (±0.3 SE) on average after their stroke suggests that these deficits do not
spontaneously improve over time.
Remediation of the Non-paretic Arm Remediation of the non-paretic arm is so
novel that little empirical evidence exists as to whether such intervention could
affect non-paretic arm control and coordination, and functional independence. We
recently conducted a pilot study to test this hypothesis, which suggests that intense
non-paretic arm training not only improves motor coordination and functional
performance in the trained arm, but also improves functional independence and
modestly improves paretic arm function. This is consistent with another recent pilot
randomized clinical study that indicated that patients who received therapy that
included training of the non-paretic arm improved functional independence greater
than those receiving traditional therapy alone (Pandian et al. 2015). These studies
provide preliminary evidence that non-paretic arm remediation might improve
Motor Lateralization Provides a Foundation for Predicting … 263
Fig. 3 Participants were positioned facing a horizontally positioned mirror that reflected the 55″
monitor. The arm was supported over a horizontal table top, positioned just below shoulder height
(adjusted to each individual’s comfort), by an air-jet system, which reduced the effects of gravity
and friction
Fig. 4 Timeline for the intervention—baseline period of 2 weeks, during which no intervention
was done. Two baseline tests (Test 1 and Test 2) were given (2 weeks apart) prior to Non-Paretic
arm training, and Test 3 was given immediately after the 3-week training period
Fig. 5 Participants were initially exposed to virtual reality games for 40 min, followed by real-life
tasks for another 40 min
266 R.L. Sainburg et al.
Fig. 7 Sample Handpaths prior to and following training, and associated measures (Mean ± SE)
for the two pretests (test1, test2), and the post test (test3)
The results of this pilot study are shown in Figs. 6 and 7. As predicted, there
were no significant differences between our baseline measures, for Test 1 and Test
2. This demonstrates stability in baseline performance over time. The effect of
training, however, was reflected by a 25.8 % reduction in performance time for the
Jebsen-Taylor Hand Function Test, and a 9.8 % reduction in impairment, as
measured by the Fugl-Meyer Assessement. Most importantly, the FIM score
improved by 22.6 %, representing an increase of 3.3 ± (SE = 0.88) points out of
28 points on the upper extremity self-care components of the FIM.
Our kinematic measures in the non-paretic arm also showed improvements.
Figure 7 shows example hand paths from a left hemisphere damaged patient using
the left non-paretic arm, prior to training (Test 1) and following training (Test 2).
Bar graphs show average kinematic measures (±SE), across all participants and
test-sessions. Movements became straighter and more accurately directed toward
the target (upper circle) following training, but the distance of the movements was
accurate even in baseline movements. These effects were consistent across all
subjects, as shown by the significant reduction in direction error and aspect ratio (a
measure of hand path straightness), only for Test 3, post-training.
In summary, our pilot data showed promising improvements in non-paretic arm
motor coordination and function. In addition, modest improvements in paretic arm
Motor Lateralization Provides a Foundation for Predicting … 267
For the most part, physical rehabilitation of upper extremity function following
stroke has understandably been focused on training movements in the contrale-
sional arm. However, the research discussed above provides compelling evidence
that ipsilesional assessment and intervention also needs to be addressed. When
patients have severe contralesional paresis, the ipsilesional arm is often the primary
manipulator, or even the sole manipulator. Therefore, effective performance of
ADL relies upon efficient coordination of this arm and hand (Haaland et al. 2012).
We provided evidence that this arm shows substantial coordination deficits that
limit performance of ADL and functional independence. Patients who must live
with a severely paretic dominant arm, unfortunately have the most severe
non-paretic arm deficits in coordination and functional performance.
We have provided preliminary evidence that intensive training of this ipsile-
sional, non-paretic arm could substantially improve functional independence in
patients with hemiparesis. Although preliminary, this is among the first empirical
evidence indicating that such intervention might lead to positive effects on motor
performance and functional independence. However, our findings are consistent
with a recent pilot intervention study that compared a group of patients who
received therapy that included training of the non-paretic arm and the paretic arm to
another group who only received “traditional” therapy for only the paretic arm
(Pandian et al. 2015). The results indicated that when non-paretic intervention was
combined with paretic arm training, the speed and accuracy of non-paretic arm
movements improved, and the impairment level of the paretic arm modestly
improved, when compared to patients who received traditional therapy alone.
Combined with our current results, these findings indicate that focused non-paretic
arm training might produce both improvements in non-paretic arm motor perfor-
mance and in paretic arm function, both of which should facilitate improvements in
functional independence. This finding appears at odds with the well-studied phe-
nomenon of learned non-use of the paretic arm, an effect that has been successfully
addressed by constraining the non-paretic arm in patients with moderate to mild
paresis (Wolf et al. 1989, 2002, 2005, 2006). Constraint induced movement therapy
uses constraint of the non-paretic arm in combination with intense practice of the
paretic arm to facilitate recovery of paretic arm function. However, it should be
stressed that this approach is most effective in patients with high-moderate to mild
paresis, who are able to manipulate objects with the paretic arm, and engage in
unilateral functional activities with that arm. The current study addresses recovery
268 R.L. Sainburg et al.
The research presented in this chapter has demonstrated that many stroke patients
with severe moderate to paresis in the contralesional arm have substantial and
functionally limiting motor coordination deficits in the non-paretic ipsilesional arm.
Motor Lateralization Provides a Foundation for Predicting … 269
References
Beebe JA, Lang CE (2009) Relationships and responsiveness of six upper extremity function tests
during the first six months of recovery after stroke. J Neurol Phys Ther 33:96–103
Bisazza A, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (1998) The origins of cerebral asymmetry: a review of
evidence of behavioural and brain lateralization in fishes, reptiles and amphibians. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev 22:411–426
Chestnut C, Haaland KY (2008) Functional significance of ipsilesional motor deficits after
unilateral stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 89:62–68
Dassonville P, Zhu XH, Uurbil K, Kim SG, Ashe J (1997) Functional activation in motor cortex
reflects the direction and the degree of handedness. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94:14015–14018
Desrosiers J, Bourbonnais D, Bravo G, Roy PM, Guay M (1996) Performance of the ‘unaffected’
upper extremity of elderly stroke patients. Stroke 27:1564–1570
Fisk JD, Goodale MA (1988) The effects of unilateral brain damage on visually guided reaching:
hemispheric differences in the nature of the deficit. Exp Brain Res 72:425–435
Fugl-Meyer AR, Jaasko L, Leyman I, Olsson S, Steglind S (1975) The post-stroke hemiplegic
patient. 1. A method for evaluation of physical performance. Scand J Rehabil Med 7:13–31
Gazzaniga MS (1998) The split brain revisited. Sci Am 279:50–55
Grabowski M, Brundin P, Johansson BB (1993) Paw-reaching, sensorimotor, and rotational
behavior after brain infarction in rats. Stroke 24:889–895
Haaland KY (2006) Left hemisphere dominance for movement. Clin Neuropsychol 20:609–622
Haaland KY, Harrington D (1989a) The role of the hemispheres in closed loop movements. Brain
& Cogn 9:158–180
Haaland KY, Harrington DL (1989b) Hemispheric control of the initial and corrective components
of aiming movements. Neuropsychologia 27:961–969
270 R.L. Sainburg et al.
Haaland KY, Harrington DL (1996) Hemispheric asymmetry of movement. Curr Opin Neurobiol
6:796–800
Haaland KY, Harrington DL, Knight RT (2000) Neural representations of skilled movement. Brain
123(Pt 11):2306–2313
Haaland KY, Mutha PK, Rinehart JK, Daniels M, Cushnyr B, Adair JC (2012) Relationship
between arm usage and instrumental activities of daily living after unilateral stroke. Arch Phys
Med Rehabil 93:1957–1962
Haaland KY, Prestopnik JL, Knight RT, Lee RR (2004) Hemispheric asymmetries for kinematic
and positional aspects of reaching. Brain 127:1145–1158
Haaland KY, Schaefer SY, Knight RT, Adair J, Magalhaes A, Sadek J, Sainburg RL (2009)
Ipsilesional trajectory control is related to contralesional arm paralysis after left hemisphere
damage. Exp Brain Res 196:195–204
Jebsen RH, Griffith ER, Long EW, Fowler R (1971) Function of “normal” hand in stroke patients.
Arch Phys Med Rehabil 52:170–174 (passim)
Jebsen RH, Taylor N, Trieschmann RB, Trotter MJ, Howard LA (1969) An objective and
standardized test of hand function. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 50:311–319
Jones RD, Donaldson IM, Parkin PJ (1989) Impairment and recovery of ipsilateral sensory-motor
function following unilateral cerebral infarction. Brain 112(Pt 1):113–132
Kawashima R, Matsumura M, Sadato N, Naito E, Waki A, Nakamura S, Matsunami K, Fukuda H,
Yonekura Y (1998) Regional cerebral blood flow changes in human brain related to ipsilateral
and contralateral complex hand movements—a PET study. Eur J Neurosci 10:2254–2260
Kawashima R, Yamada K, Kinomura S, Yamaguchi T, Matsui H, Yoshioka S, Fukuda H (1993)
Regional cerebral blood flow changes of cortical motor areas and prefrontal areas in humans
related to ipsilateral and contralateral hand movement. Brain Res 623:33–40
Kuypers HG (1978) The motor system and the capacity to execute highly fractionated distal
extremity movements. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol (Suppl): 429–431
Kuypers HG (1982) A new look at the organization of the motor system. Prog Brain Res 57:381–403
Lawrence DG, Kuypers HG (1968) The functional organization of the motor system in the
monkey. I. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Brain 91:1–14
Lin JH, Hsu MJ, Sheu CF, Wu TS, Lin RT, Chen CH, Hsieh CL (2009) Psychometric comparisons
of 4 measures for assessing upper-extremity function in people with stroke. Phys Ther 89:
840–850
MacNeilage PF, Rogers LJ, Vallortigara G (2009) Origins of the left & right brain. Sci Am
301:60–67
Mani S, Mutha PK, Przybyla A, Haaland KY, Good DC, Sainburg RL (2013) Contralesional
motor deficits after unilateral stroke reflect hemisphere-specific control mechanisms. Brain
136:1288–1303
Mani S, Przybyla A, Good DC, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2014) Contralesional arm preference
depends on hemisphere of damage and target location in unilateral stroke patients.
Neurorehabil Neural Repair 28:584–593
Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2012) The effects of brain lateralization on motor control
and adaptation. J Mot Behav 44:455–469
Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2013) Rethinking motor lateralization: specialized but
complementary mechanisms for motor control of each arm. PLoS ONE 8:e58582
Mutha PK, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY (2010) Coordination deficits in ideomotor apraxia during
visually targeted reaching reflect impaired visuomotor transformations. Neuropsychologia
48:3855–3867
Mutha PK, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY (2011a) Critical neural substrates for correcting unexpected
trajectory errors and learning from them. Brain 134:3647–3661
Mutha PK, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY (2011b) Left parietal regions are critical for adaptive
visuomotor control. J Neurosci 31:6972–6981
Mutha PK, Stapp LH, Sainburg RL, Haaland KY (2014) Frontal and parietal cortex contributions
to action modification. Cortex 57:38–50
Motor Lateralization Provides a Foundation for Predicting … 271
Nozaki D, Kurtzer I, Scott S (2006) Limited transfer of learning between unimanual and bimanual
skills within the same limb. Nat Neurosci 9:3
Oujamaa L, Relave I, Froger J, Mottet D, Pelissier JY (2009) Rehabilitation of arm function after
stroke. Literature review. Ann Phys Rehabil Med 52:269–293
Pandian S, Arya KN, Kumar D (2015) Effect of motor training involving the less-affected side
(MTLA) in post-stroke subjects: a pilot randomized controlled trial. Topics in stroke
rehabilitation. doi:10.1179/1074935714Z.0000000022
Pohl PS, Winstein CJ (1999) Practice effects on the less-affected upper extremity after stroke. Arch
Phys Med Rehabil 80:668–675
Poole JL, Sadek J, Haaland KY (2009) Ipsilateral deficits in 1-handed shoe tying after left or right
hemisphere stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 90:1800–1805
Prestopnik J, Haaland K, Knight R, Lee R (2003a) Hemispheric dominance for open and closed
loop movements. Soc Neurosci Abstr 30
Prestopnik J, Haaland K, Knight R, Lee R (2003b) Hemispheric dominance in the parietal lobe for
open and closed loop movements. J Int Neuropsychol Soc 9:1–2
Rapin I, Tourk LM, Costa LD (1966) Evaluation of the Purdue Pegboard as a screening test for
brain damage. Dev Med Child Neurol 8:45–54
Rose DK, Winstein CJ (2005) The co-ordination of bimanual rapid aiming movements following
stroke. Clin Rehabil 19:452–462
Sainburg RL (2002) Evidence for a dynamic-dominance hypothesis of handedness. Exp Brain Res
142:241–258
Sainburg RL (2014) Convergent models of handedness and brain lateralization. Front Psychol
5:1092
Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2007) Ipsilesional motor deficits following stroke reflect
hemispheric specializations for movement control. Brain 130:2146–2158
Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2009a) Dissociation of initial trajectory and final position
errors during visuomotor adaptation following unilateral stroke. Brain Res 1298:78–91
Schaefer SY, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2009b) Hemispheric specialization and functional
impact of ipsilesional deficits in movement coordination and accuracy. Neuropsychologia 47:
2953–2966
Schaefer SY, Mutha PK, Haaland KY, Sainburg RL (2012) Hemispheric specialization for
movement control produces dissociable differences in online corrections after stroke. Cereb
Cortex 22:1407–1419
Selles RW, Michielsen ME, Bussmann JB, Stam HJ, Hurkmans HL, Heijnen I, de Groot D,
Ribbers GM (2014) Effects of a mirror-induced visual illusion on a reaching task in stroke
patients: implications for mirror therapy training. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 28:652–659
Stevens JA, Stoykov ME (2004) Simulation of bilateral movement training through mirror
reflection: a case report demonstrating an occupational therapy technique for hemiparesis.
Topics Stroke Rehabil 11:59–66
Tanji J, Okano K, Sato KC (1988) Neuronal activity in cortical motor areas related to ipsilateral,
contralateral, and bilateral digit movements of the monkey. J Neurophysiol 60:325–343
Wang J, Sainburg RL (2009) Generalization of visuomotor learning between bilateral and
unilateral conditions. J Neurophysiol 102:2790–2799
Wetter S, Poole JL, Haaland KY (2005) Functional implications of ipsilesional motor deficits after
unilateral stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 86:776–781
Winstein CJ, Merians AS, Sullivan KJ (1999) Motor learning after unilateral brain damage.
Neuropsychologia 37:975–987
Winstein CJ, Pohl PS (1995) Effects of unilateral brain damage on the control of goal-directed
hand movements. Exp Brain Res 105:163–174
Winstein CJ, Pohl PS, Cardinale C, Green A, Scholtz L, Waters CS (1996) Learning a
partial-weight-bearing skill: effectiveness of two forms of feedback [published erratum appears
in Phys Ther 1997 Mar; 77(3):328]. Phys Ther 76:985–993
Wolf SL, Blanton S, Baer H, Breshears J, Butler AJ (2002) Repetitive task practice: a critical
review of constraint-induced movement therapy in stroke. Neurologist 8:325–338
272 R.L. Sainburg et al.
Wolf SL, Lecraw DE, Barton LA, Jann BB (1989) Forced use of hemiplegic upper extremities to
reverse the effect of learned nonuse among chronic stroke and head-injured patients. Exp
Neurol 104:125–132
Wolf SL, Thompson PA, Morris DM, Rose DK, Winstein CJ, Taub E, Giuliani C, Pearson SL
(2005) The EXCITE trial: attributes of the Wolf motor function test in patients with subacute
stroke. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 19:194–205
Wolf SL, Winstein CJ, Miller JP, Taub E, Uswatte G, Morris D, Giuliani C, Light KE,
Nichols-Larsen D (2006) Effect of constraint-induced movement therapy on upper extremity
function 3 to 9 months after stroke: the EXCITE randomized clinical trial. JAMA 296:
2095–2104
Wyke M (1967) Effect of brain lesions on the rapidity of arm movement. Neurology 17:
1113–1120
Yadav V, Sainburg RL (2011) Motor lateralization is characterized by a serial hybrid control
scheme. Neuroscience 196:153–167
Yadav V, Sainburg RL (2014) Handedness can be explained by a serial hybrid control scheme.
Neuroscience 278:385–396
Yarosh CA, Hoffman DS, Strick PL (2004) Deficits in movements of the wrist ipsilateral to a
stroke in hemiparetic subjects. J Neurophysiol 92:3276–3285
Control of Cycling Limb Movements:
Aspects for Rehabilitation
Abstract Walking, swimming, cycling, and running are cyclic movements that are
often performed in training programs or rehabilitation protocols by athletes or
people with neuromotor disorders. The muscular and kinematic activities that are
acquired during cyclic movements reveal control principles, especially for the
optimization and stabilization of motor performance, for a given criterion in
rehabilitation processes. The influence of external loads and resistive forces on limb
movements should be considered in rehabilitation protocols and when assessing
physical activity levels or defining activity patterns for the artificial control of limb
movements. This chapter focuses on special cyclic limb movements: lower and
upper limb cycling. Two aspects of this research and applications are discussed.
First, variances of movement patterns are examined at different levels of the motor
system (endpoint, joint configuration, muscle) during unimanual right and left arm
cycling and bimanual cycling movements. Second, it is shown that the muscle
activity patterns that are acquired during lower and upper limb cycling in
able-bodied people may be used to define and improve stimulation patterns for
functional electrical stimulation-driven cycling movements in spinal cord-injured
individuals. This report also discusses the advantages of the application and control
of these types of movements for the rehabilitation of people with paralyzed limbs.
J. Laczko (&)
Department of Information Technology and Biorobotics, Faculty of Science,
University of Pécs, 6, Ifjusag St, Pécs H-7624, Hungary
e-mail: [email protected]
J. Laczko M. Mravcsik
Laboratory of Rehabilitation-Technology, Wigner Research Centre for Physics,
Hungarian Academy of Sciences, 29–33, Konkoly Thege M. St, Budapest H-1121,
Hungary
e-mail: [email protected]
P. Katona
University of Physical Education, 44 Alkotas St, Budapest H-1123, Hungary
e-mail: [email protected]
M. Mravcsik
Faculty of Science, University of Pécs, 6, Ifjusag St, Pécs H-7624, Hungary
Keywords Unimanual Bimanual Crank resistance Upper limb Lower limb
Power production Electromyography Functional electrical stimulation (FES)
Introduction
Cyclic limb movements are often used for the rehabilitation of people with
neural-based motor dysfunctions. Different types of cyclic movements were
investigated. Differences between discrete and cyclic limb movements were ana-
lyzed in healthy people (Hogan and Sternad 2007; Smits-Engelsman et al. 2002,
2006; van Mourik and Beek 2004) and in survivors of stroke (Massie et al. 2014).
Cycling upper or lower limb movements are a special type of movement that is
clearly distinguishable from other rhythmic movements and sequences of discrete
movements. For example, cycling arm movements occur when a circle is contin-
uously and repetitively drawn or tracked by a pen held in the hand. This task may
reveal motor control properties in people with Parkinson’s disease (Keresztenyi
et al. 2009). However, the most often performed cycling limb movement is cycling
on a bicycle, tricycle or an ergometer, which are frequently available at many
rehabilitation centers and in the homes of people with neural-based movement
disorders; but, these types of movements have received less attention from a motor
control perspective. Cycling movements on an ergometer or bicycle constrain the
path of the endpoint of the limb to a plane and a circle within the plane. This circle
is the path on which the foot (placed on the pedal in lower limb cycling) or the hand
(grasping the handlebar during upper limb cycling) moves. The endpoint of the
limb must move on the same path during these movements, even if external con-
ditions, such as resistive forces, are imposed. This constraint makes cycling
movements different than other types of cyclic limb movements, such as walking,
swimming, or repetitively executed unimanual and bimanual reaching movements,
in which the endpoint paths are not restricted (Laczko et al. 2001; Domkin et al.
2002) and may vary accross trials. However, studies of repetitively executed
reaching and object transporting arm movements (Tibold et al. 2011; Tibold and
Laczko 2012) have demonstrated that the effect of load was not significant on the
variances of endpoint trajectory. Furthermore, the load did not affect torque profiles
in the elbow and shoulder joints (Tibold and Laczko 2012). Participants grasped
and held objects of various weights in their hand and placed the object from an
initial to a target position, vertically upward or downward. Movements were exe-
cuted repetitively in a series. The task was performed using two objects, a light,
empty compact disc case, and a load of 20 N, and it was found that the effect of
load was not significant on the variances of endpoint trajectory. The arm config-
uration variances (defined by the variances of intersegmental joint angles in the
shoulder, elbow, and wrist) when transporting the heavy object upward (against
gravity) did not differ significantly from those observed for movements executed
with the light object. The arm configuration variance was significantly higher when
the object was transported downward for movements executed with heavy object
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 275
compared to movements with light object. The load had a significant effect on the
variances of muscle activity patterns (EMG amplitudes) in both movement direc-
tions. A larger load was associated with increased variances in flexor and extensor
activities. Beside external load, neural disorders also influence motor variances, but
not in all motor tasks.
Cyclic reaching movements were examined in stroke survivors (Massie et al.
2012), and variances in the arm movements were compared for the affected and
non-affected arms. Participants were asked to reach continuously between the center
of two targets in a sagittal plane as quickly and accurately as possible. Variance was
a measure of the consistency of fingertip contact points in relation to the average
contact point. The variance of the target contact did not differ for the affected and
non-affected arms. Endpoint position in these cyclic object transporting and
reaching movements was stabilized by neural control, and this control was reflected
in unaffected endpoint variances.
The variance in cycling movements at the endpoint level (foot or hand) is
expected to be small and practically independent from crank resistance if cycling
cadence is constant because of the fixed path of the endpoint. We hypothesized that
the effect of crank resistance on arm configuration variance and muscle activity
variance is significant. The central nervous system may compensate for external
forces by employing altered and more variable motor patterns at joint and muscle
levels to ensure successful execution of the motor task. This effect was previously
proposed by investigating swimming arm movements (Martens et al. 2015).
Here we present results related to the observed variances of movement patterns
obtained in upper limb cycling. We investigated arm configuration variance in a joint
space defined by intersegmental joint angles in the shoulder, elbow and wrist joints
and muscle activity variances in a muscle space defined by elbow and shoulder
muscles. We compared arm configuration variances and muscle activity variances
for the dominant and non-dominant arms and for unimanual and bimanual cycling.
The effect of crank resistance and pedaling cadence on muscle activities for lower
limb cycling are also presented. The application of functional electrical stimulation-
driven cycling movements of spinal cord-injured individuals is also discussed.
Table 1 Mean arm configuration variances (and SE) in various cycling conditions
Crank resistance (Nm) Variance (°2) Variance (°2)
Bimanual Unimanual
Left Right Left Right
1.16 18.4 (0.9) 15.4 (1.9) 16.4 (1.6) 16.5 (1.9)
3.09 15.3 (1.3) 16.7 (2.0) 17.7 (1.3) 15.2 (1.5)
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 277
Fig. 1 Comparison of arm configuration variances in bimanual and unimnual cyling. Average
(across subjects) arm configuration variances in the left arm (upper diagrams) and right arm (lower
diagrams) when cycling was performed against a crank resistance of 1.16 and 3.09 Nm. Vertical
lines above the bars represent standard errors
278 J. Laczko et al.
Fig. 2 Comparison of arm configuration variances in the left and right arms. Upper diagrams
average arm configuration variances (and SE) obtained in bimanual cycling Lower diagrams
average arm configuration variances (and SE) in unimanual cycling
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 279
Fig. 3 Average muscle activity variances obtained during unimanual versus bimanual cycling in
various cycling conditions. Upper panels present results for the left arm lower panels for the right
arm; Left panels for crank resistance of 1.16, Right panels for crank resistance of 3.09
280 J. Laczko et al.
Fig. 4 Comparison of the left and right arms muscle activity variances in bimanual and
unimanual cycling. Results obtained when cycling was performed against crank resistance of
1.16 Nm (left diagrams) and 3.09 Nm (right diagrams)
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 281
Lower limb cycling was studied and analyzed to a greater extent than arm cycling.
This emphasis is partially due to sport applications, and lower limb cycling is more
widely used in rehabilitation protocols when reaching higher power output is the
goal.
Power output while cycling on an ergometer may be changed by altering
pedaling cadence or crank resistance. We investigated these effects in lower limb
cycling and hypothesized that changes in crank resistance and pedaling cadence
would have a significant effect on the stability of neural control of the movement.
When cycling faster and/or against higher crank resistance, then not only the
amplitude of muscle activities (EMG amplitude) increases, but the activity patterns
of individual muscles and muscle coactivation may also change. The effects of
cadence change (Neptune et al. 1997; Brown and Kautz 1999; Chapman et al. 2007;
Katona et al. 2014), and the effect of load (Baum and Li 2003; Hug et al. 2011;
Smits-Engesman 2006) on muscle activity patterns were investigated. The com-
bined effect of cadence change and crank resistance change on the amplitude of
EMG signals and the size of range of muscle coactivation was analyzed (Katona
et al. 2013, 2014). Able-bodied participants performed cycling movements on an
ergometer at 2 pedaling cadences, 60 and 45 rpm, and 3 levels of crank resistances.
Surface EMG signals were recorded on the left quadriceps and hamstrings at a
1000 Hz sampling frequency.
Figure 5 presents EMG signals from the quadriceps and hamstrings muscle
groups during cycling with slow (45 rpm) and fast (60 rpm) cadences. The
quadriceps exhibited a periodic activity in both cadence conditions, which is related
to its role in this movement as the main power output provider. The EMG of
hamstrings exhibited a rather calm activity with a small periodic increase in the
amplitude (Fig. 5).
Raw sEMG signals were filtered (fourth-order Butterworth filter) and smoothed
(RMS) before further data processing and analyses. The average EMG amplitudes
for each participant and cycling conditions were computed.
The ratio of average EMG amplitudes obtained in the two cadence (fast/slow)
conditions was higher when cycling against a higher resistance. This holds for knee
flexors and extensors (Table 2). This result indicates that if resistance is increased,
then not only muscle activities (EMG) are increased, but their rates of change with
respect to changes in cadence are higher when cycling is performed against higher
crank resistance. Notably, the ratio of cadence (fast versus slow cycling) and the
ratio of power output was independent of resistance conditions in this study, but the
ratio of average muscle activities (EMG amplitudes) depended on crank resistance
and was significantly higher in higher RCs.
Comparing of the ratios of EMG amplitudes of flexors and extensors during fast
versus slow cycling demonstrated that hamstring activity increased generally at a
higher rate than quadriceps, but this difference was not significant.
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 283
Fig. 5 Raw (recorded) sEMG data of a participant cycled for 15 s in moderate resistance
conditions (RC2). The graphs on the left and right present data obtained in slow (45 rpm) and fast
(60 rpm) cycling respectively. (Adapted from Katona et al. 2014, Fig. 1)
Table 2 Crank resistance, power output, ratio of power outputs and the ratio of average EMG
amplitudes obtained in the two cadence conditions are presented for the three resistance conditions
(low—RC1, moderate—RC2, high—RC3)
Dimension Resistance Resistance Power Power Ratio Ratio of Ratio of
(at 60 rpm) (at 45 rpm) (fast) (slow) of EMGmean_Q EMGmean_H
[Nm] [Nm] [W] [W] power [fast/slow] [fast/slow]
[ratio]
RC1 2.51 2.12 15.76 9.99 1.58 1.23 1.22
RC2 3.76 3.18 23.61 14.98 1.58 1.31 1.49
RC3 5.02 4.24 31.53 19.97 1.58 1.46 1.51
Fast represents cycling with a cadence of 60 rpm, and slow represents cycling at 45 rpm. The mean
(across participants) ratio of average EMG amplitudes with the standard deviations is provided for the
two muscle groups (Q—quadriceps and H—hamstrings) separately (Katona et al. 2014)
The hamstrings generate relatively small power compared to the quadriceps, and
when the cadence increases it may co-contract with the quadriceps. Muscle
co-contraction may modify and regulate power output. We established angular
positions of the crank in which each muscle in a pair (Quadriceps-Hamstrings) were
active simultaneously. These angular positions defined the range of coactivation of
the muscle pair. A muscle was considered active when its EMG amplitude
exceeded 30 % of its mean EMG amplitude, as assessed in the actual RC (Ozgünen
et al. 2010). The size of coactivation range of hamstring and quadriceps muscle
groups increased with increased cycling cadence (Katona et al. 2013). In contrast,
coactivation decreased with increased crank resistance. These findings reflect the
284 J. Laczko et al.
Upper limb and lower limb cycling is a special motor task that is used in functional
electrical stimulation-based rehabilitation protocols. Cycling has an advantage
compared to other types of locomotion, such as walking, because the participant
does not have to hold the weight of the entire body. There are several centers where
FES-driven cycling is used in rehabilitation protocols. The aim of these rehabili-
tation training programs is not only muscle strengthening by active muscle force
generation, but also to improve cardiovascular and respiratory functions. The
psychological affect is also very important because the SCI participants generate
active forces using their own muscles. The participants use their own muscles to
actively cycle for tens of minutes continuously, even if the motor command is
coming not from the brain but from an artificial device (stimulator and controller).
The physiological benefits of functional electrical stimulated cycling are presented
in many papers (Frotzler et al. 2009; Lai et al. 2010; Perret et al. 2010; Dolbow
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 285
et al. 2013; Brurok et al. 2013) and there are several studies about the effects of
electrical stimulation (Theisen et al. 2002; Szecsi et al. 2014) and stimulation
parameters (as current amplitude, pulse width and stimulation frequency) on energy
and power output of FES-driven lower limb cycling (Eser et al. 2003; Hunt et al.
2006). We investigated the effect of various stimulation frequencies on power
output (Laczko et al. 2012) and found that higher stimulation frequency did not
necessarily result in higher performance and the dependence of performance on
stimulation frequency varied among individuals.
Other crucial questions, from a motor control view, are the timing of muscle
activations and muscle co-contractions. One possible method to establish proper
artificial stimulation patterns is based on measured muscle activity patterns in
able-bodied cycling people. The stimulation pattern derived from EMG data
acquired from hamstrings and quadriceps muscle groups of 41 able-bodied persons
lead to a stimulation pattern for our SCI participants (Pilissy et al. 2008; Laczko
2011). Two pairs of surface electrodes on each leg, one for the quadriceps, and one
for the hamstring muscle groups were applied.
Recently we established a new stimulation pattern when three parts of the
quadriceps (vastus medialis, vastus lateralis, and rectus femoris) were stimulated
separately by three pairs of electrodes and the hamstrings with one pair of elec-
trodes. In this case the power output increased significantly and this was achieved
applying lower stimulation current amplitude per muscles (Mravcsik et al. 2016b).
Our observations on upper and lower limb cycling performed by able-bodied
people may be used to improve and adjust rehabilitation protocols and artificial
stimulation patterns to various cycling conditions to attain the desired power out-
puts when SCI people or stroke survivors are cycling and their movements are
generated by FES.
Translation to Rehabilitation
The presented features of upper limb cycling suggest some mechanisms to define
rehabilitation protocols depending on the goals of the rehabilitation. The larger
variability of muscle activities across cycles during unimanual upper limb cycling
compared to bimanual cycling supports that it is more difficult to maintain con-
sistent, periodic muscle activity patterns via central control in unimanual cycling
than it is in bimanual cycling. This result is not surprising, but it is remarkable
that the control of the non-dominant arm seems to be as stable as the control of the
dominant arm in arm cycling, even without training. Arm cycling offers an ideal
rehabilitation procedure when stability is important and strengthening the muscles
in both arms is the aim. Joint angular variances as well as muscle activity variances
are similar and stable control of the movement is maintained equally well in the
dominant and non-dominant arm. This result is obtained from able-bodied unim-
paired participants, but this idea should be translated to arm cycling in stroke
survivors in future studies. Whether arm cycling is a motor task in which the
286 J. Laczko et al.
affected and non-affected arms are not as different as in the performing of other
motor tasks is a question for future research.
Functional electrical stimulation (FES)-driven cycling is in demand for lower
limb muscle strengthening, and additionally it is advantageous for the cardiovas-
cular and respiratory systems. Analyses of cycling limb movements of able-bodied
people are profoundly useful for establishing stimulation patterns for
FES-controlled movements of spinal cord-injured (SCI) individuals. Adequate
stimulation patterns are required in SCI individuals to allow them to drive the crank
of an ergometer using their own (electrically stimulated) muscles at various cycling
cadences and crank resistances. The above-mentioned investigations impact the
potential stimulation patterns that are used in FES-assisted cycling, namely,
stronger individual muscle stimulation is needed to increase cycling cadence. If the
aim is to cycle against higher crank resistance, then the time duration should be
decreased in which hamstring and quadriceps muscles are stimulated in parallel. We
suggest increasing quadriceps stimulation at a higher rate compared to the ham-
strings when cycling against a higher resistance. If the aim is to cycle with a higher
cadence, then the hamstrings activity should be increased at a higher rate compared
to the quadriceps. FES-driven limb cycling apparatuses are commercially available
and used mainly by SCI individuals, but stroke survivors may also use this method.
Stimulation patterns varying between apparatuses and the user can define stimu-
lation patterns in most cases. Results obtained from investigations of the motor
control of limb cycling facilitate the efficient use of these resources.
Beside FES applications, lower limb cycling may also be used in the rehabili-
tation of patients with cerebral palsy (CP). Near-maximal adaptation of CP bodies
toward healthy levels was demonstrated as electromyographic mean amplitude and
frequency changed similarly both in the affected and non-affected legs in athletes
with CP and able-bodied athletes who performed cycling training (Runciman et al.
2015).
and non-affected limbs of stroke survivors. Control principles from upper and lower
limb cycling movements of able-bodied people should be further identified to
improve rehabilitation protocols and stimulation patterns for the efficient control of
functional electrical stimulation-driven cycling training in spinal cord-injured
people and stroke survivors with paralyzed limbs.
Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank the National Institute for Medical
Rehabilitation and the Pázmány Péter Catholic University in Budapest for the possibility to per-
form measurements reported in this paper.
References
Katona P, Pilissy T, Laczko J (2013) Speed and crank resistance affects coactivation of knee
muscles during cycling movements. In: San Juan, Puerto Rico, society for the neural control of
movement, 23rd annual meeting abstracts, p 1
Katona P, Pilissy T, Tihanyi A, Laczko J (2014) The combined effect of cycling cadence and crank
resistance on hamstrings and quadriceps muscle activities during cycling. Acta Physiol Hung
101(4):505–516
Keresztenyi Z, Cesari P, Fazekas G, Laczkó J (2009) The relation of hand and arm configuration
variances while tracking geometric figures in Parkinson’s disease—“aspects for rehabilitation”.
Int J Rehabil Res 32(1):53–63
Laczko J (2011) Modeling of human movements, neuroprostheses. Clin Neurosci/Ideggyogy Szle
64(7–8):162–167. IF 0.488
Laczko J, Jaric S, Domkin D, Johansson H, Latash ML (2001) Stabilization of kinematic variables
in the control of bimanual pointing movements. In: International joint conference on
neural networks, Washinghton, DC. July 14–19 2001, ISBN 0-7803-7044-9/ ©2001 IEEE,
pp 1256–1260
Laczko J, Katona P, Waszlavik E, Klauber A (2012) Dependence of cycling performance on
training time and stimulation frequency during FES driven cycling. In: IFESS 2012, 17th
annual meeting—International functional electrical stimulation society, pp 448–451
Laczko J, Katona P, Valy A (2014) Muscle co-activation as function of crank angle when cycling
on an ergometer with altered power output. Program No. 630.06, 2014 Neuroscience Meeting
Planner. Society for Neuroscience
Lai CH, Chan WH, Chan WP, Peng CW, Shen LK, Chen JJ, Chen SC (2010) Effects of functional
electrical stimulation cycling exercise on bone mineral density loss in the early stages of spinal
cord injury. J Rehabil Med 42:150–154
Martens J, Daly D, Deschamps K, Fernandes RJP, Staes F (2015) Intra-individual variability
of surface electromyography in front crawl swimming. PLoS ONE 10(12):e0144998.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0144998
Massie CL, Malcolm MP, Greene DP, Browning RC (2012) Kinematic motion analysis and
muscle activation patterns of continuous reaching in survivors of stroke. J Motor Behav
44(3):213–222
Massie CL, Malcolm MP, Greene DP, Browning RC (2014) Biomechanical contributions of the
trunk and upper extremity in discrete versus cyclic reaching in survivors of stroke. Top Stroke
Rehabil 21(1):23–32
Mravcsik M, Laczkó J (2014) Muscle co-activation during arm cycling against altering crank
resistances. In: 7th World Congress of Biomechanics, Control Number 3864
Mravcsik M, Laczko J (2015) Variances of joint configuration and muscle activity patterns during
arm cycling against external resistances. Program Number 609.06, 2015 Neuroscience Meeting
Planner. Society for Neuroscience, Chicago IL, USA (Online)
Mravcsik M, Zentai N, Botzheim L, Laczko J (2016a) Unimanual versus bimanual arm cycling
movements - muscle activity variances. Program Number 886.01, 2016 Neuroscience Meeting
Planner. Society for Neuroscience, San Diego CA, USA (Online)
Mravcsik M, Klauber A, Laczko J (2016b) FES driven lower limb cycling by four and eight
channel stimulations—a comparison in a case study. In: Proceedings of the 12th Vienna
International Workshop on Functional Electrical Stimulation, pp. 89–93, ISBN:
978-3-900928-12-4
Neptune RR, Kautz SA, Hull ML (1997) The effect of pedaling rate on coordination in cycling.
J Biomech 30(10):1051–1058
Ozgünen KT, Celik U, Kurdak SS (2010) Determination of an Optimal Threshold Value for
Muscle Activity Detection in EMG Analysis. J Sports Sci Med 9(4):620–628. eCollection 2010
Perret C, Berry H, Hunt KJ, Donaldson N, Kakebeeke TH (2010) Feasibility of functional
electrical stimulated cycling in subjects with spinal cord injury: an energetics assessment.
J Rehabil Med 42:873–875
Control of Cycling Limb Movements: Aspects for Rehabilitation 289
Andrew M. Gordon
Abstract Cerebral palsy is caused by early damage to the developing brain, as the
most common pediatric neurological disorder. Hemiplegia (unilateral spastic
cerebral palsy) is the most common subtype, and the resulting impairments, later-
alized to one body side, especially affect the upper extremity, limiting daily func-
tion. This chapter first describes the pathophysiology and mechanisms underlying
impaired upper extremity control of cerebral palsy. It will be shown that the severity
of impaired hand function closely relates to the integrity of the corticospinal tract
innervating the affected hand. It will also shown that the developing corticospinal
tract can reorganize its connectivity depending on the timing and location of CNS
injury, which also has implications for the severity of hand impairments and
rehabilitation. The mechanisms underlying impaired motor function will be high-
lighted, including deficits in movement execution and planning and sensorimotor
integration. It will be shown that despite having unimanual hand impairments,
bimanual movement control deficits and mirror movements also impact function.
Evidence for motor learning-based therapies including Constraint-Induced
Movement Therapy and Bimanual Training, and the possible pathophysiological
predictors of treatment outcome and plasticity will be described. Finally, future
directions for rehabilitations will be presented.
Keywords Sensorimotor integration Constraint-induced movement therapy
Bimanual Jense-Taylor test Hand function Corticospinal pathways (CST)
Object-release coordination Motor planning Thalamocortical Stereognosis
Mirror movements
Introduction
Corticospinal (CST) pathways from motor areas, especially the primary motor
cortex (M1), develop in a corticofugal manner, approaching the spinal cord by the
20th week of gestation during typical development (see Staudt 2010). Thereafter
these pathways undergo synaptogenesis, with targeted projections to the spinal–
segmental level. The motor cortices are initially believed to develop bilateral pro-
jections (i.e., projections to both the contralateral and ipsilateral UEs) (Eyre et al.
2001). There is a gradual weakening of ipsilateral projections, and strengthening of
contralateral projections via synaptic competition during continuing development
(Staudt 2010), which is driven by M1 activity (see Friel et al. 2013). This is
evidenced by studies of the developing CST in kittens, whereby blockade of M1
activity either by injection of a GABA-agonist or restraint of a forelimb, prevents
the CST from establishing spinal connectivity, and results in permanent motor
impairments (Friel et al. 2013). In humans, this intricate process is susceptible to
prenatal and perinatal brain damage. Since the CST directly innervate hand
motoneurons, which provide the capacity for selective UE movement control
(Lawrence and Kuypers 1968), permanent impairments in manual dexterity may
occur following damage to this developing system.
USCP is often the result of periventricular lesions, middle cerebral artery infarct,
hemi-brain atrophy, brain malformation or posthemorrhagic porencephaly (e.g.,
Uvebrandt 1988; Cioni et al. 1999). As described above, motor areas and CST
integrity are often compromised (e.g., Staudt et al. 2004; Bleyenheuft et al. 2007).
The specific etiology may drastically influence subsequent development of CST
pathways (Staudt 2010). The severity of upper extremity impairments largely
depends on the extent of damage to the CST (e.g., Bleyenheuft et al. 2007; Scheck
et al. 2012). The damage can be approximated using both conventional MRIs and
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 293
Fig. 1 a Peduncle asymmetry predicts baseline unimanual dexterity (Jebsen-Taylor Test). Inset
t1-weighted MRI showing cerebral peduncles, outlined. Modified from Friel et al. (2014).
b Comparison of hand motor dysfunction scores among patients with congenital brain injuries
acquired during the trimesters of gestation (malformations of cortical development [MCD], first
and second trimester of pregnancy; periventricular [PV] lesions, early third trimester of pregnancy;
middle cerebral artery [MCA] infarctions, late third trimester of pregnancy). Open circles,
individuals with preserved crossed corticospinal projections from the affected hemispheres; filled
circles, individuals with reorganized ipsilateral projections from the contralesional hemispheres;
half-filled circles, individuals with both preserved contralateral and reorganized ipsilateral
corticospinal projections to the more affected hand. Modified from Staudt et al. (2004). c Relation
between box and blocks score (higher score represents greater dexterity) using the more affected
hand and the CST motor projection pattern to the hand, assessed using transcranial magnetic
stimulation (TMS). Modified from Holmström et al. (2010)
with CP are capable of adjusting their fingertip forces to the object’s weight and
texture, their forces are often excessive and variable, with reduced adaptation to the
object’s weight and texture compared to TD children (Eliasson et al. 1992). Object
release coordination is also impaired (Eliasson and Gordon 2000), which is exac-
erbated when speed and accuracy constraints are imposed (Gordon et al. 2003).
Fortunately, precision grip in children with CP does improve with development
(Eliasson et al. 2006) and extensive practice (Gordon and Duff 1999a), which has
helped inspired the development of intensive rehabilitation protocols (e.g.,
constraint-induced movement therapy; CIMT) (Gordon 2011, 2014).
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 295
Sensorimotor Impairments
Children with unilateral spastic CP have deficits in bimanual coordination above and
beyond unimanual dexterity deficits (e.g., Steenbergen et al. 1996, 2000; see Gordon
and Steenbergen 2008). Similar to motor planning, the relation between the type and
timing of the lesion and bimanual coordination impairments are not known. During
symmetrical, bimanual reaching tasks, children with unilateral spastic CP do coordinate
their bimanual movements by compensating with their noninvolved hand, as long as
the accuracy demands or task complexity are not increased. In one recent study,
participants were instructed to hold a grip device in each hand and place one device on
top of the other while the grip and load force were recorded in each hand (Islam et al.
2011). Children with USCP initiated the task by decreasing grip force in the releasing
hand before increasing the grip force in the holding hand during the preparation phase,
with the subsequent grip force increase in the holding hand being reduced and
occurring later (transition phase) than that of TD children. The impairments were
unrelated to presence of mirror movements, and the impairment was greater when the
less affected hand served as the holding hand.
In another series of studies (Hung et al. 2004, 2010), children were asked to open
a drawer with one hand and manipulate its contents with the other hand. Children
with USCP were less coordinated, with reduced movement overlap of the drawer
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 297
Fig. 2 Tractography of the corpus callosum (genu blue, midbody orange, splenium red) in a a TD
child and b a child with unilateral CP. c Relationship between # of fibres in the splenium of the
corpus callosum and Assisting Hand Assessment scores. Modified from Weinstein et al. (2013)
opening hand and manipulating hand (and sequential completion of opening the
drawer and manipulating its contents (Hung et al. 2010). Interestingly, as described
later in this chapter, bimanual training improved some aspects of this coordination
more than unimanual training (Hung et al. 2011).
While emphasis has been placed on CST connectivity to the affected hands in
relation hand function (e.g., Bleyenheuft et al. 2007; Friel et al. 2014). However,
the cause of hand impairments may not be as simple. For example, one study
showed that reduced structural integrity of the corpus callosum in USCP was
associated with reduced function of both the more- and less affected hands (Green
et al. 2013). Interestingly, the corpus callosum integrity was also related to the
quality of bimanual hand use (Fig. 2), which as described below, may be func-
tionally more important.
Mirror Movements
unknown whether mirror movements impact the ability to treat impaired hand
function, or whether treatments can diminish their impact on function.
CIMT is an obvious choice for eliciting intensive unimanual practice. There was a
strong scientific rationale based on animal models underlying the application of
intensive practice-based approaches to human upper extremity (UE) rehabilitation
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 299
(Tower 1940; Taub and Shee 1980). Starting with ‘forced use’ in adult stroke
patients, in which the less affected UE was physically restrained to incentivize
practice of the more affected UE (Wolf et al. 1989), subsequent efforts incorporated
principles of behavioral psychology (shaping) and motor learning to elicit active
practice of the affected UE. This active training approach along with the restraint
become known as ‘Constraint-induced movement therapy’ (CIMT) (Taub and Wolf
1997). The first adaptation of the approach for the pediatric population leading to a
case study was published more 15 years ago (Charles et al. 2001). Since then there
have been many studies of CIMT to date, including more than 30 RCTs, with
nearly all studies providing evidence of efficacy (see Dong et al. 2013; Eliasson
et al. 2014).
The best age to conduct CIMT is unknown. Most studies include children from 2
to 7 years, although CIMT studies have included infants as young as 7 months
(e.g., Lowes et al. 2014) to adolescence (Eliasson et al. 2003; Gordon et al. 2006a,
b; Sakzewski et al. 2011a). Given the very early age in which brain damage occurs
in children with CP, we could surmise that there is tremendous potential for
recovery (Kennard 1936) and that “earlier treatment is better”. However, the rela-
tionship is not as clear as one might hope (Eliasson et al. 2005; Hoare et al. 2013;
Gordon et al. 2006a, b; Sakzewski et al. 2011a). Nevertheless, the studies to date
indicate that children of all ages may benefit.
Animal models suggest that the best time to start treatments eliciting movements
of the more affected UE, such as CIMT, may well be during the first few months of
life. The as described above (Martin et al. 2011; Eyre 2007). Development of the
CST requires activity-dependent competition between the two sides of the devel-
oping motor system. Transcranial magnetic stimulation studies in human infants
(Eyre 2003, 2007; Eyre et al. 2001; Staudt et al. 2004) are consistent with these
findings. Pharmacologically inhibiting activity of the contralesional side, thus
balancing activity of the two hemispheres, immediately after unilateral brain injury
in the kitten, restores motor function, normal anatomical connectivity of the CST
and the motor representational map in primary motor cortex (Martin et al. 2011).
A feline model of CIMT (Friel et al. 2012) examined the effects of CIMT at age 8 to
13 weeks and 20-24 weeks (Fig. 3). The earlier training restored motor function,
CST connections in the spinal cord and the motor cortical representation of the
forelimb, and increased cholinergic spinal interneuron density on the contralateral
side. The later training (age 20–24 weeks) did not restore motor function or con-
tralateral spinal cholinergic neuronal density, although it did increase CST con-
nectivity and the motor cortical representation of the forelimb. Restraint alone
(forced use without specific, skilled training) only restored CST connectivity (Friel
et al. 2012). This alludes to the importance of increasing skilled activity of the
involved UE, a principle embedded in both CIMT and bimanual training (see
below), to balance the neural activity between the two hemispheres before the less
affected CST “outcompetes” the affected CST (i.e., at an early age).
300 A.M. Gordon
Fig. 3 Summary of effects constraint only (forced use), constraint plus motor training in a in a
feline model of unilateral spastic cerebral palsy induced by inactivation of the motor cortex. Early
training was in a kitten age 8–13 weeks, late training 20–24 weeks. Gray rectangles, conditions in
which an effect was noted. Corticospinal tract (CST) plasticity is defined as the presence of
axons/varicosities within the spinal intermediate gray matter or more ventrally. An effect of
treatment on the M1 motor map is defined as an increase in the number of sites from which
stimulation evoked a limb motor response. A filled light gray rectangle in the cholinergic
interneuron (INs) column indicates the presence of robust increases in the ratio of spinal
cholinergic interneurons on the affected and unaffected sides. The dark rectangle in the motor
recovery column indicates am improvement in stepping accuracy while walking over a horizontal
ladder. From Friel et al. (2013)
The efficacy CIMT lead to the obvious question of whether similar improvements
could be achieved without physically restraining the less affected upper extremity.
Bimanual training is developmentally focused and takes into account principles of
motor learning (Thorndike 1914) (practice specificity) to train use of the more affected
hand as a functional (nondominant) assist rather than as a dominant hand as done in
CIMT. It uses the environment (task) to “force use the more affected hand”. One
child-friendly form of intensive bimanual training was developed, “Hand-arm bimanual
intensive therapy” (HABIT) (Charles and Gordon 2006), with the focus improving the
amount and quality of involved UE use during bimanual tasks. HABIT retains the
intensive structured practice of CIMT and typically it is provided in a day camp
environment 6 h/day for 10–15 days (i.e., 60–90 h). A small randomized control trial
of HABIT was initially conducted (Gordon et al. 2007) demonstrating that children
who received HABIT had improved quality of affected hand use during bimanual
activities and frequency of UE use. HABIT has also been shown to be effective using a
themed (learning magic tricks) approach (Green et al. 2013).
Bimanual training and CIMT result in similar improvements in both unimanual
capacity and bimanual performance outcomes in CP populations (Sakzewski et al.
2011a, b; Hoare et al. 2013; Gordon et al. 2008, 2011; Facchin et al. 2011; Deppe
et al. 2013; Dong et al. 2013) (Fig. 4). Bimanual training has been shown to result
in better improvement in functional activities and play goals meaningful to par-
ticipants and caregivers (Gordon et al. 2011; Brandão et al. 2012) largely since most
goals are bimanual (Gordon et al. 2011). During a bimanual task where children are
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 301
asked to open a drawer with one hand and manipulate its contents with the other
hand, improvements in both the CIMT and HABIT groups were observed despite
not being practiced during the training (Hung et al. 2011). However greater
improvements in movement overlap (the percentage of time with both hands
engaged in the task) and goal synchronization (reduced time between each hand
completing the task goals) were found for the HABIT group. Together these
findings are in agreement with the principle of practice specificity.
Despite the advantages and disadvantages of each approach, CIMT and HABIT
are not mutually exclusive. They can be performed concurrently with sufficient
intensity (Aarts et al. 2010; Cohen-Holzer et al. 2011; Taub et al. 2007) or CIMT
could be followed with a bimanual training program (i.e., sequentially) where gains
in unimanual capacity are translated into bimanual activities (Case-Smith et al.
2012; Taub et al. 2007). These might combine the benefits of both approaches,
although comparison studies to either isolated approach have not been conducted
(but see Gordon 2011).
Although the above intensive training protocols have demonstrated great promise, a
major limitation is that the neurological damage and subsequent recovery in cere-
bral palsy is extremely diverse. Studies of these approaches have largely ignored
Fig. 4 a Mean ± SEM time to complete the 6 timed items (writing excluded) of the
Jebsen-Taylor Test of Hand Function. Faster times correspond to better performance.
b. Mean ± SEM scaled logit scores (AHA units) on the Assisting Hand Assessment; higher
scores represent better performance. Abbreviations: CIMT Constraint-induced movement therapy;
HABIT Hand-arm intensive bimanual therapy; AHA Assisting hand assessment. From Gordon
et al. (2011)
302 A.M. Gordon
this issue and tend to lump participants together and look at the group effects. There
has been large variability in the response to treatment as well (e.g., Sakzewski et al.
2011b). It may be that depending on the specific lesions and brain reorganization,
one approach may be more efficacious than another. For example, Staudt and
colleagues (Kuhnke et al. 2008) showed that children with a contralateral CST
increased manual dexterity after CIMT, whereas children with an ipsilateral CST
did not. The two groups also showed differences in neuroplasticity associated with
the training (Juenger et al. 2013). In contrast, Islam et al. (2014) concluded that
improvements in hand use after CIMT were present in all participants, irrespective
of their CST connectivity pattern. Only one case study of two children with USCP
has examined the effect of CST connectivity pattern on efficacy of bimanual
therapy, with improvements seen regardless of CST organization (Bleyenheuft et al.
2015) (Fig. 5).
A recent study of the neural predictors of bimanual training showed that children
with greater overall severity of brain injury at baseline (higher radiological score)
and decreased cortical activation of the lesional hemisphere on fMRI, showed
greater improvement on bilateral hand function but less improvement on unimanual
function (Schertz et al. 2016). Similarly, greater white matter damage (DTI of the
Fig. 5 Determinist diffusion tensor imaging tracking was made from the 3 mm spheres (123
voxels) created symmetrically in both CST tracts, with their middle centered on the CST fibers as
visualized in a transversal plane passing through the middle cerebellar peduncle. a child 1 with
ipsilateral connectivity, b child 2 with contralateral connectivity. Pretest and posttest data for each
child for the c Jebsen-Taylor test of hand function (JTTHF), d ABILHAND-Kids and e Canadian
occupational performance measure (COPM) performance rating. Modified from Bleyenheuft et al.
(2015)
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 303
posterior limb of the internal capsule and the CST were also associated with greater
improvement on bimanual function but no improvement on unimanual assessment.
However, the amount of damage to the CSTs alone did not relate improvement after
CIMT (Rickards et al. 2014) or bimanual training (Friel et al. 2014).
One study showed increases in activation and size of the motor areas controlling
the affected hand using TMS, DTI and fMRI (Bleyenheuft et al. 2015). The later
study found increased activation in cortical areas involved in reward circuits,
suggesting an intriguing possibility that the training may engage circuits involved
with achieving self-efficacy. Another study showed that changes in DTI and fMRI
parameters were seen when comparing pre- and post intervention following
bimanual training. Despite variable patterns of brain plasticity, a change toward a
more unilateral brain activation pattern was consistently associated with greater
motor improvements. Another (TMS) study showed that the map size and ampli-
tude of motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitudes of the affected hand motor map
increased significantly after bimanual training regardless of whether the represen-
tation was in the lesional or contralesional hemisphere (Friel et al. 2016) (Fig. 6).
The amount of change in these measures correlated with improvements in hand
function. Finally, one study examined differences in cortical reorganization fol-
lowing CIMT depending on CST organization (Juenger et al. 2013). Both groups
exhibited increases in sensorimotor cortical activation, which is consistent with a
report of better sensory function after intensive training (Kuo et al. 2016).
Interestingly TMS showed a decrease in M1 excitability in children with an ipsi-
lateral CST pattern, whereas it showed an increase in M1 excitability in children
with preserved contralateral CST organization.
Recently the question of training ingredients, specifically progressive skill train-
ing, was addressed. Skill progression has been shown to be important for inducing
plasticity in animal models of learning (Kleim et al. 2002; Nudo 2003). Furthermore,
in a feline model of unilateral CP, restriction of the nonparetic forelimb alone (anal-
ogous to forced use) did not drive CST development, cortical reorganization or
improvements in stepping accuracy, whereas the addition to active training (analo-
gous to CIMT) did. Children with USCP were either provided intensive bimanual
training (HABIT) that included progressive increases in the difficulty of activities as
hand function improved, or practice of the same activities without skill progression
(bimanual play). At first glance, it appeared that both approaches lead to equal
improvements in hand function as assessed with standard clinical assessments
(Brandao et al. 2014). However, upon examination of progress on functional and play
goals identified by caregivers, children in the skilled practice group had greater
improvements (Brandao et al. 2014) and improved their movement coordination
patterns as seen using kinematic analysis on the functional drawer opening task
described above. Furthermore, we used TMS to probe the hand representation plas-
ticity (Friel et al. 2016). It was shown that children in the skill progression group that
the size and motor evoked potential (MEP) amplitudes of the affected hand motor map
increased significantly after therapy, whereas training of the same activities without
skill progression did not (Fig. 6). Thus, skill progression appears to be an important
ingredient for changing motor patterns and inducing plasticity in M1.
304 A.M. Gordon
Fig. 6 Changes in magnitude of motor evoked potentials in TMS maps after structured training in
child A and unstructured practice in child B. Maps are of the affected hand located contralateral to
the affected hemisphere. Red color indicates stronger MEP response. Not that the maps indicate a
larger hand representation with stronger MEPS after structured practice only. Plotted from data in
Friel et al. 2016
Together these studies highlight the variations in CNS damage underlying CP. It
is essential to understand the underlying behavioral and neural mechanisms as to
better target them with interventions. However, the variability complicates story
and may limit the extent to which new treatments may be generalized across
individuals. In fact, they highlight the notion that there cannot be a “one-size-fits-all
approach” to rehabilitation.
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 305
Brain Stimulation
References
Charles J, Gordon AM (2006) Development of hand-arm bimanual intensive therapy (HABIT) for
improving bimanual coordination in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child
Neurol 48:931–936
Cioni G, Sales B, Paolicelli PB, Petacchi E, Scusa MF, Canapicchi R (1999) MRI and clinical
characteristics of children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Neuroped 30:249–255
Cohen-Holzer M, Katz-Leurer M, Reinstein R et al (2011) The effect of combining daily restraint
with bimanual intensive therapy in children with hemiparetic cerebral palsy: a self-control
study. NeuroRehab 29:29–36
Deppe W, Thuemmler K, Fleischer J, Berger C, Meyer S, Wiedemann B (2013) Modified
constraint-induced movement therapy versus intensive bimanual training for children with
hemiplegia—a randomized controlled trial. Clin Rehabil 10:909–920
Dong VA-Q, Tung IH-H, Siu HW-Y et al (2013) Studies comparing the efficacy of
constraint-induced movement therapy and bimanual training in children with unilateral
cerebral palsy: a systematic review. Dev Neurorehabil 16:133–143
Eliasson AC, Bonnier B, Krumlinde-Sundholm L (2003) Clinical experience of constraint induced
movement therapy in adolescents with hemiplegic cerebral palsy—a day camp model. Dev
Med Child Neurol 45:357–359
Eliasson AC, Forssberg H, Hung YC, Gordon AM (2006) Development of hand function and
precision grip control in individuals with cerebral palsy: a 13-year follow up study. Pediatrics
118:e1226–e1236
Eliasson AC, Gordon AM, Forssberg H (1991) Basic coordination of manipulative forces of
children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 33:659–668
Eliasson AC, Gordon AM, Forssberg H (1992) Impaired anticipatory control of isometric forces
during grasping by children with cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 34:216–225
Eliasson AC, Gordon AM (2000) Impaired force coordination during object release in children
with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 42:228–234
Eliasson A-C, Krumlinde-sundholm L, Shaw K et al (2005) Effects of constraint-induced
movement therapy in young children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: an adapted model. Dev
Med Child Neurol 47:266–275
Eliasson AC, Krumlinde-Sundholm L, Gordon AM, Feys H, Klingels K, Aarts P, Rameckers E,
Autti-Rämö I, Hoare B (2014) Guidelines for future research in Constraint-induced movement
therapy for children with unilateral cerebral palsy: an expert consensus. Dev Med Child Neurol
56:125–137
Eyre JA (2003) Development and plasticity of the corticospinal system in man. J Neural
Transplant Plast 10:93–106
Eyre JA (2007) Corticospinal tract development and its plasticity after perinatal injury. Neurosci
Biobehav Rev 31:1136–1149
Eyre JA, Smith M, Dabydeen L et al (2007) Is hemiplegic cerebral palsy equivalent to amblyopia
of the corticospinal system? Ann Neurol 62:493–503
Eyre JA, Taylor JP, Villagra F et al (2001) Evidence of activity-dependent withdrawal of
corticospinal projections during human development. Neurol 57:1543–1554
Facchin P, Rosa-Rizzotto M, Dalla Pozza LV et al (2011) Multisite trial comparing the efficacy of
constraint-induced movement therapy with that of bimanual intensive training in children with
hemiplegic cerebral palsy: Postintervention results. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 90:539–553
Farmer SF, Harrison LM, Ingram DA, Stephens JA (1991) Plasticity of central motor pathways in
children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Neurology 41:1505–1510
Friel K, Chakrabarty S, Kuo H-C et al (2012) Using motor behavior during an early critical period
to restore skilled limb movement after damage to the corticospinal system during development.
J Neurosci 32:9265–9276
Friel KM, Chakrabarty S, Martin JH (2013) Pathophysiological mechanisms of impaired limb use
and motor systems repair strategies after unilateral injury of the developing brain. Dev Med
Child Neurol Suppl 4:27–31
308 A.M. Gordon
Friel KM, Kuo HC, Carmel JB, Gordon AM (2014) Improvements in hand function after intensive
bimanual training are not associated with corticospinal tract dysgenesis in children with
hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Exp Brain Res 232:2001–2009
Friel K, Kuo H-C M, Ferre CL, Brandao M, Carmel J, Bleyenheuft Y, Gowatsky JL, Stanford AD,
Rowny SB, Luber B, Bassi B, Murphy DLK, Lisanby SH, Gordon AM (2016) Skilled
bimanual training drives motor cortex plasticity in children with unilateral cerebral palsy.
Neural Rehab Neural Repair 30(9):834–844
Gentile AM (2000) Skill acquisition: action, movement, and neuromotor processes. In: Carr JH,
Shepherd RD (eds) Movement science: foundations for physical therapy, 2nd edn. Aspen,
Rockville, MD, pp 111–187
Gillick BT, Krach LE, Feyma T et al (2014) Primed low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation and constraint-induced movement therapy in pediatric hemiparesis: a randomized
controlled trial. Dev Med Child Neurol 56:44–52
Gordon AM (2001) In: Kalverboer AF, Gramsbergen A (eds) Handbook of brain and behaviour in
human development. Kluwer, Dordrecht, pp 513–537
Gordon AM (2011) To constrain or not to constrain, and other stories of intensive upper extremity
training for children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 53(Suppl. 4):56–61
Gordon AM (2014) In: Shepherd R (Ed) Cerebral Palsy in infancy. targeted activity to optimize
early growth and development. Elsevier, pp 305–322
Gordon AM, Charles J, Schneider JA, Chinnan A (2007) Efficacy of a hand-arm bimanual
intensive therapy (HABIT) for children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a randomized control
trial. Dev Med Child Neurol 49:830–838
Gordon AM, Charles J, Duff SV (1999) Fingertip forces during object manipulation in children
with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. II: Bilateral coordination. Dev Med Child Neurol Neurol
41:176–185
Gordon AM, Charles J, Steenbergen B (2006a) Movement planning is disrupted by impaired
sensorimotor integration in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Ped Res 60:587–591
Gordon AM, Charles J, Wolf SL (2006b) Efficacy of constraint-induced movement therapy on
involved-upper extremity use in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy is not age-dependent.
Pediatrics 117:e363–e373
Gordon AM, Chinnan A, Gill S, Petr E, Hung YC, Charles J (2008) Both constraint-induced
movement therapy and bimanual training lead to improved performance of upper extremity
function in children with hemiplegia. Dev Med Child Neurol 50:957–958
Gordon AM, Duff SV (1999a) Fingertip forces during object manipulation in children with
hemiplegic cerebral palsy. I: Anticipatory scaling. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:166–175
Gordon AM, Duff SV (1999b) Relation between clinical measures and fine manipulative control in
children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 41:586–591
Gordon AM, Hung YC, Brandao M et al (2011) Bimanual training and constraint-induced
movement therapy in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy: a randomized trial. Neuro Rehab
Neural Repair 25:692–702
Gordon AM, Lewis S, Eliasson AC, Duff SV (2003) Object release under varying task constraints
in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 45:240–248
Gordon AM, Magill, RA (2012) Motor learning: Application of principles to pediatric
rehabilitation. In: Campbell SK, Palisano RJ, Orlin MN (eds) Physical therapy for children.
Elsevier (Saunders), St. Louis, pp 151–174
Gordon AM, Steenbergen B (2008) In: Eliasson AC and Burtner P (eds) Improving hand function
in children with cerebral palsy: theory, evidence and intervention. Clinics in developmental
medicine. Mac Keith Press, London, pp 160–75. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/
24237277
Green D, Schertz M, Gordon AM, Moore A, Schejter M, Farquharson Y, Ben Bashat D,
Winstein M, Lin J-P, Fattal-Valevski A (2013) A multi-site study of functional outcomes
following a themed approach to hand-arm bimanual intensive therapy (HABIT) for children
with hemiplegia. Dev Med Child Neurol 55:527–533
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 309
Harrison A (1988) Spastic cerebral palsy: possible spinal interneuronal contributions. Dev Med
Child Neurol 30:769–780
Himmelmann K, Hagberg G, Beckung E, Hagberg B, Uvebrant P (2005) The changing panorama
of cerebral palsy in Sweden. IX. Prevalence and origin in the birth-year period 1995–1998.
Acta Paediatr 94:287–294
Hoare B, Imms C, Villanueva E et al (2013) Intensive therapy following upper limb botulinum
toxin A injection in young children with unilateral cerebral palsy: a randomized trial. Dev Med
Child Neurol 55:238–247
Holmström L, Vollmer B, Tedroff K et al (2010) Hand function in relation to brain lesions and
corticomotor-projection pattern in children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child
Neurol 52:145–152
Hsu WY, Cheng CH, Liao KK, Lee IH, Lin YY (2012) Effects of repetitive transcranial magnetic
stimulation on motor functions in patients with stroke: a meta-analysis. Stroke 43:1849–1857
Hung Y-C, Casertano L, Hillman A, Gordon AM (2011) The effect of training specificity on
bimanual coordination in children with hemiplegia. Res Dev Dis 32:2724–2731
Hung Y-C, Charles J, Gordon AM (2004) Bimanual coordination of arm movements in children
with hemiplegia. Dev Med Child Neurol 46:746–753
Hung YC, Charles J, Gordon AM (2010) Influence of accuracy constraints on bimanual
coordination during a goal-directed task in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Exp Brain
Res 201:421–428
Islam M, Gordon AM, Forssberg H, Sköld A, Eliasson AC (2011) Grip force coordination during a
bimanual task in unilateral cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 53:920–926
Islam M, Nordstrand L, Holmström L, Kits A, Forssberg H, Eliasson AC (2014) Is outcome of
constraint-induced movement therapy in unilateral cerebral palsy dependent on corticomotor
projection pattern and brain lesion characteristics? Dev Med Child Neurol 56:252–258
Johansson RS, Westling G (1984) Roles of glabrous skin receptors and sensorimotor memory in
automatic control of precision grip when lifting rougher or more slippery objects. Exp Brain
Res 56:550–564
Juenger H, Kuhnke N, Braun C, Ummenhofer F, Wilke M, Walther M, Koerte I, Delvendahl I,
Berweck S, Mall V, Staudt M (2013) Two types of exercise-induced neuroplasticity in
congenital hemiparesis: a TMS, fMRI and MEG study. Dev Med Child Neurol 55:941–951
Kennard MA (1936) Age and other factors in motor recovery from precentral lesions in monkeys.
Am J Physiol 115:138–146
Kirton A, Chen R, Friefeld S, Gunraj C, Pontigon AM, deVeber G (2008) Contralesional repetitive
transcranial magnetic stimulation for chronic hemiparesis in subcortical paediatric stroke: a
randomised trial. Lancet Neurol 7:507–513
Kirton A, Andersen J, Herrero M, Nettel-Aguirre A, Carsolio L, Damji O, Keess J, Mineyko A,
Hodge J, Hill MD (2016) Brain stimulation and constraint for perinatal stroke hemiparesis:
The PLASTIC CHAMPS trial. Neurology 3(86):1659–1667
Kleim JA, Barbay S, Nudo RJ (1998) Functional reorganization of the rat motor cortex following
motor skill learning. J Neurophysiol 80:3321–3325
Kleim JA, Barbay S, Cooper NR, Hogg TM, Reidel CN, Remple MS, Nudo RJ (2002) Motor
learning-dependent synaptogenesis is localized to functionally reorganized motor cortex.
Neurobiol Learn Mem 77:63–77
Klingels K, Jaspers E, Staudt M, Guzzetta A, Mailleux L, Ortibus E, Feys H (2016) Do mirror
movements relate to hand function and timing of the brain lesion in children with unilateral
cerebral palsy? Dev Med Child Neurol 58:735–742
Koert I, Pelavin P, Kirmess B, Fuchs T, Berweck S, Laubender RP, Heinen F (2011) Anisotropy of
transcallosal motor fibres indicates functional impairment in children with periventricular
leukomalacia. Dev Med Child Neurol 53:179–186
Kuhnke N, Juenger H, Walther M, Berweck S, Mall V, Staudt M (2008) Do patients with
congenital hemiparesis and ipsilateral corticospinal projections respond differently to
constraint-induced movement therapy? Dev Med Child Neurol 50:898–903
310 A.M. Gordon
Kuo H-C, Gordon AM, Henrionnet A, Hautfenne S, Friel K, Bleyenheuft Y (2016) The effects of
intensive bimanual training with and without tactile training on tactile function in children with
unilateral spastic cerebral palsy. Res Dev Disabil 49–50:129–139
Kuo H-C, Ferre CL, Carmel JB, Gowatsky JL, Stanford AD, Rowny SB, Lisanby SH,
Gordon AM, Friel KM (2016) Using diffusion tensor imaging to identify corticospinal tract
projection patterns in children with unilateral spastic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol.
doi: 10.1111/dmcn.13192
Kuhtz-Buschbeck JP, Sundholm LK, Eliasson AC, Forssberg H (2000) Quantitative assessment of
mirror movements in children and adolescents with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child
Neurol 42:728–736
Latash ML, Anson JG (1996) What are “normal movements” in atypical populations? Behav Brain
Sci 19:55–106
Lawrence DG, Kuypers HGJM (1968) The functional organization of the motor system in the
monkey. I. The effects of bilateral pyramidal lesions. Brain 91:1–14
Lefaucheur J-P, André-Obadia N, Antal A et al (2014) Evidence-based guidelines on the
therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS). Clin Neurophysiol
125:2150–2206
Lowes LP, Mayhan M, Orr T, Batterson N, Tonneman JA, Meyer A, Alfano L, Wang W,
Whalen CN, Nelin MA, Lo WD, Case-Smith J (2014) Pilot study of the efficacy of
constraint-induced movement therapy for infants and toddlers with cerebral palsy. Phys Occup
Ther Pediatr 34:4–21
Martin JH, Chakrabarty SS, Friel KM (2011) Harnessing activity-dependent plasticity to repair
the damaged corticospinal tract in an animal model of cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol
53:9–13
Moberg E (1962) Criticism and study of methods for examining sensibility in the hand. Neurol
12:8–19
Novak I, McIntyre S, Morgan C, Campbell L, Dark L, Morton N, Stumbles E, Wilson SA,
Goldsmith S (2013) A systematic review of interventions for children with cerebral palsy: state
of the evidence. Dev Med Child Neurol 55:885–910
Nudo RJ (2003) Functional and structural plasticity in motor cortex: implications for stroke
recovery. Phys Med Rehabil Clin N Am 14(Suppl):S57–S76
Plautz EJ, Milliken GW, Nudo RJ (2000) Effects of repetitive motor training on movement
representations in adult squirrel monkeys: role of use versus learning. Neurobiol Learn Mem
74:27–55
Prabhu S, Diermayr G, Gysin P, Gordon AM (2011) Impaired anticipatory control of grasp during
gait in children with hemiplegic cerebral palsy. Dev Med Child Neurol 53:865–869
Reis J, Schambra HM, Cohen LG, Buch ER, Fritsch B, Zarahn E, Celnik PA, Krakauer JW (2009)
Noninvasive cortical stimulation enhances motor skill acquisition over multiple days through
an effect on consolidation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 106:1590–1595
Rickards T, Sterling C, Taub E, Perkins-Hu C, Gauthier L, Graham M, Griffin A, Davis D,
Mark VW, Uswatte G (2014) Diffusion tensor imaging study of the response to
constraint-induced movement therapy of children with hemiparetic cerebral palsy and adults
with chronic stroke. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 95:506–514
Sakzewski L, Ziviani J, Abbott DF et al (2011a) Randomized trial of constraint-induced movement
therapy and bimanual training on activity outcomes for children with congenital hemiplegia.
Dev Med Child Neurol 53:313–320
Sakzewski L, Ziviani J, Boyd RN (2011b) Best responders after intensive upper-limb training for
children with unilateral cerebral palsy. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 92:578–584
Salimi I, Friel KM, Martin JH (2008) Pyramidal tract stimulation restores normal corticospinal
tract connections and visuomotor skill after early postnatal motor cortex activity blockade.
J Neurosci 28:7426–7434
Scheck SM, Boyd RN, Rose SE (2012) New insights into the pathology of white matter tracts in
cerebral palsy from diffusion magnetic resonance imaging: a systematic review. Dev Med
Child Neurol 54:684–696
Impaired Voluntary Movement Control and Its Rehabilitation … 311
John C. Rothwell
Keywords Paired associative stimulation (PAS) Synaptic plasticity (STDP)
Transcranial direct current stimulation (TDCS) Magnetic stimulation Cortical
TMS employs a rapidly changing magnetic field to induce a very short pulse of
electrical current in the underlying brain that is designed to be very similar to the
electrical pulse delivered by conventional peripheral nerve stimulators (Barker et al.
1987). It is thought that this activates the axons of neurones near the surface of the
brain, producing an action potential that travels to the synaptic terminal where it
releases transmitter that could excite or inhibit further neurones. In the motor
cortex, several lines of evidence suggest that a single TMS pulse causes repetitive
Broadly speaking, we can distinguish two main types of motor learning: skill
learning and adaptation learning (Krakauer and Mazzoni 2011). Skill learning, such
as learning to putt a golf ball into a hole, involves incremental improvements in
performance that accrue over an extended period of training. This is sometimes
termed ‘non-rule-based’ learning since participants do not know in advance how to
improve their performance. It can be distinguished from a more rapid form of
learning in which a previously learnt movement is performed in the presence of a
visuomotor or somatosensory perturbation—motor adaptation. In this case, par-
ticipants are aware of the error and can use this information to update a previously
learned skill. From a rehabilitation perspective, skill learning is perhaps more
important since this is what we imagine patients have to do when re-learning how to
use their affected limbs after a stroke.
There is good evidence that the motor cortex is involved in the early stages of
skill learning. When volunteers learn a new motor skill, the improvement in per-
formance during training (skill acquisition) is associated with a rapid increase in
motor cortex excitability (Muellbacher et al. 2001; Nudo et al. 1996). After training
has finished, a second process, known as consolidation occurs, in which the motor
memories are strengthened further so that they persist for longer and become more
resistant to interference by subsequent motor activity. This process may involve the
motor cortex, at least for a short period of time after completing training. Thus,
consolidation can be blocked by using TMS to interfere with activity in primary
motor cortex immediately after training but not 6 h later (Muellbacher et al. 2002).
There may also be offline performance gains so that performance is better on the
day after training than it was at the end of training on the previous day
(Brashers-Krug et al. 1996; Walker et al. 2002) Finally, it should be noted that an
already consolidated motor memory may become susceptible to further alteration
when it is reactivated (Nader and Hardt 2009) (reconsolidation) and modulation of
motor cortex activity also influences this process, suggesting that this region may
transiently interact with the stored memory trace during movement execution.
Several experiments have shown that rTMS and TDCS can influence both online
and offline learning. Most studies using rTMS are designed to ‘prime’ the motor
cortex for subsequent learning, such that training occurs during a period of
increased excitability. For example, 10 Hz rTMS delivered to the motor cortex for
2 s immediately before each training block enhanced training in a sequential motor
learning task (Kim et al. 2004). Similarly, Teo et al. tested the influence of inter-
mittent TBS on subsequent training in a ballistic thumb movement task and found
that iTBS enhanced the rate and extent of performance gains (Teo et al. 2011).
Reis and colleagues studied the effect of applying anodal tDCS to the active
motor cortex during training in an isometric pinch task over 5 days, and found that
learning was enhanced over sham and that the benefit was sustained at 3 months
(Reis et al. 2009). Interestingly, tDCS in this study specifically enhanced offline
Can Motor Recovery in Stroke Be Improved by Non-Invasive Brain … 317
Application in Rehabilitation
There have been a large number of relatively small scale trials of the effect of brain
stimulation on motor recovery after stroke (Lefaucheur et al. 2014). The majority
have studied effects in chronic stroke patients >6 months post stroke; only a few
have examined effects in acute and sub-acute stages. Some trials have involved only
a single session of stimulation and examined the immediate after-effects whereas
others have employed multiple treatment sessions, usually once per day for 10
consecutive weekdays.
The general design has been similar to studies of brain stimulation on healthy
learning: stimulation has targeted motor cortex with the aim of improving the
response to standard physical therapy. rTMS is given immediately prior to therapy
whereas TDCS is given either before or during therapy. Finally, stimulation is
either excitatory to the stroke hemisphere, with the intention of improving its
response to therapy; or inhibitory to the non-stroke hemisphere on the assumption
that this may reduce competitive inhibition from the non-stroke to the stroke
hemisphere (Lefaucheur et al. 2014).
There have been many recent summaries and meta-analyses of these trials, and
in general most papers report a small positive effect; a few papers report a null
effect; and no papers report negative effects. The conclusions are limited by the
heterogeneity of the patients studied as well as the differences in stimulation pro-
tocols that have been used. However, the conclusion is that there may be a possible
benefit but more work needs to be done to confirm this and to estimate whether it is
clinically meaningful.
As an example of the variability of approaches and final outcomes, consider two
very recently reported trials from the UK from Triccas et al. (TT trial) and Altman
et al. (AL trial) (Allman et al. 2016; Triccas et al. 2015), both of which examine the
effect of TDCS on the response to physical therapy for arm movement after stroke.
The headline result emphasised in the two papers is that TDCS had no additional
Can Motor Recovery in Stroke Be Improved by Non-Invasive Brain … 319
effect in a robotic therapy (TT trial) whereas TDCS significantly enhanced the
response to a form of graded manual therapy (AL trial). The impression one gets is
that one trial has a null result whereas the other is strikingly positive. However, a
comparison of both trials shows that the results are in fact quite similar. It also
highlights the difficulties in attempting to compare such trials, even when the TDCS
protocol was the same in each trial.
The trials share some important similarities (see Table 1) such as the parameters
of TDCS and the duration of each training session. They are also similar in terms of
the baseline status of the patients, although the TT study contains lower functioning
patients than the AL study. However, they differ in the type of therapy and (robotic
versus graded manual therapy) and the total number of therapy sessions.
The TT was designed with a single outcome measure, which was the change in
Fugl-Meyer (FM) score 3 months after treatment, whereas the AL trial had three
different outcome measures, FM, ARAT (Action Research Arm Test) and WMFT
(Wolf Motor Function Test). The FM scores were common to both trials and
therefore can be directly compared. At baseline the scores are similar and the
overall change at 3 months is around 7–8 points, which is above the usual clinically
relevant threshold of 5 points. Thus, both trials were successful from the patient
viewpoint.
In terms of the real versus sham comparisons, there was no difference between
groups in the TT trial, whereas there was a (non-significant) tendency for the real
group to benefit more in the AL trial (approx. 2.5 points more, see Table 1). This
(FM) result was not the one highlighted in the paper; the emphasis was placed
instead on the scores of the ARAT and WMFT which were significantly greater in
the real TDCS group than sham, and it is on this basis that the AL trial authors
conclude that TDCS improves the response to therapy after stroke.
Thus a detailed comparison of the two studies shows that in fact they have very
similar results, with the effect of therapy clearly dominating any additional effect of
TDCS.
The main reason for this is that, as in the examples above, the additional effects of
stimulation, if they exist, are small. This does not mean that they may not be
relevant, but that they are difficult to observe within the large variability in patients’
individual responses to treatment. The result of this is that small trials may by
chance detect a clear effect, and these trials are the ones most likely to reach
publication.
One common way around this problem is to perform a meta-analysis of many
small studies in order to obtain some estimate of the true effect size. However, as
can be seen in the examples above, trials can differ in very many ways that could
influence the results, and that makes it very difficult indeed to perform meaningful
meta-analyses of published material. At present it seems as if the only solution is to
perform a large scale and well-controlled trial.
In the meantime is there any possibility that brain stimulation methods can be
made more effective so that therapeutic effects are improved? At the present time
many groups are attempting to devise new protocols, for example, quadruple rTMS
or multi-electrode focal TDCS to improve their effectiveness (Hamada and Ugawa
2010; Kuo et al. 2013). However, the protocols are usually evaluated in terms of
their effect on excitability of motor cortex; experiments on behavioural effective-
ness have yet to be performed. Other groups have pointed out that a problem with
all non-invasive methods is that they cannot discriminate very well between the
types of neurone that are activated: some may be inhibitory, some excitatory, some
are interneurons, and some projection neurones. If all are activated simultaneously
(and the evidence suggests that they are) then they may have very different overall
effects. These groups are using modified types of stimulation in an attempt to target
more clearly subsets of neurones at the site of stimulation (D’Ostilio et al. 2016).
Finally, there is increasing interest in the prospect of “brain state controlled”
stimulation. Data show that the effect of non-invasive methods depends on the state
of the brain at the time the stimulation is applied (Gharabaghi et al. 2014). The new
approach is to use EEG recording to identify brain states so that stimulation can be
applied at specific times when it might prove more effective.
Can Motor Recovery in Stroke Be Improved by Non-Invasive Brain … 321
The responses to all present forms of non-invasive brain stimulation are highly
variable between individuals as well as day-to-day within an individual. This is a
stumbling block to using the methods as effective therapeutic treatments. Two new
approaches are presently being trialled to achieve better targeted stimulation. One
employs adjustable pulse parameters for stimulation while the other uses EEG
monitoring to produce stimulation in optimal brain states. Both seem to provide a
greater reliability than conventional methods, but further testing is required.
References
Allman C, Amadi U, Winkler AM, Wilkins L, Filippini N et al (2016) Ipsilesional anodal tDCS
enhances the functional benefits of rehabilitation in patients after stroke. Sci Transl Med 8:
330re1
Barker AT, Freeston IL, Jalinous R, Jarratt JA (1987) Magnetic stimulation of the human brain and
peripheral nervous system: an introduction and the results of an initial clinical evaluation.
Neurosurgery 20:100–109
Biernaskie J, Chernenko G, Corbett D (2004) Efficacy of rehabilitative experience declines with
time after focal ischemic brain injury. J Neurosci 24:1245–1254
Bindman LJ, Lippold OC, Redfearn JW (1962) Long-lasting changes in the level of the electrical
activity of the cerebral cortex produced bypolarizing currents. Nature 196:584–585
Bindman LJ, Lippold OC, Redfearn JW (1964) The action of brief polarizing currents on the
cerebral cortex of the rat (1) during current flow and (2) in the production of long-lasting
after-effects. J Physiol 172:369–382
Brashers-Krug T, Shadmehr R, Bizzi E (1996) Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature
382:252–255
D’Ostilio K, Goetz SM, Hannah R, Ciocca M, Chieffo R et al (2016) Effect of coil orientation on
strength-duration time constant and I-wave activation with controllable pulse parameter
transcranial magnetic stimulation. Clin Neurophysiol 127:675–683
Dan Y, Poo MM (2006) Spike timing-dependent plasticity: from synapse to perception. Physiol
Rev 86:1033–1048
Gharabaghi A, Kraus D, Leao MT, Spuler M, Walter A et al (2014) Coupling brain-machine
interfaces with cortical stimulation for brain-state dependent stimulation: enhancing motor
cortex excitability for neurorehabilitation. Front Hum Neurosci 8:122
Hamada M, Ugawa Y (2010) Quadripulse stimulation–a new patterned rTMS. Restor Neurol
Neurosci 28:419–424
Huang YZ, Edwards MJ, Rounis E, Bhatia KP, Rothwell JC (2005) Theta burst stimulation of the
human motor cortex. Neuron 45:1–6
Kim YH, Park JW, Ko MH, Jang SH, Lee PK (2004) Facilitative effect of high frequency
subthreshold repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation on complex sequential motor learning
in humans. Neurosci Lett 367:181–185
Krakauer JW, Mazzoni P (2011) Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond. Curr
Opin Neurobiol 21:636–644
Kuo HI, Bikson M, Datta A, Minhas P, Paulus W et al (2013) Comparing cortical plasticity
induced by conventional and high-definition 4 × 1 ring tDCS: a neurophysiological study.
Brain Stimul 6:644–648
322 J.C. Rothwell
Werk CM, Klein HS, Nesbitt CE, Chapman CA (2006) Long-term depression in the sensorimotor
cortex induced by repeated delivery of 10 Hz trains in vivo. Neuroscience 140:13–20
Woods AJ, Antal A, Bikson M, Boggio PS, Brunoni AR et al (2016) A technical guide to tDCS,
and related non-invasive brain stimulation tools. Clin Neurophysiol 127:1031–1048
Zaaimi B, Edgley SA, Soteropoulos DS, Baker SN (2012) Changes in descending motor pathway
connectivity after corticospinal tract lesion in macaque monkey. Brain 135:2277–2289
Ziemann U, Paulus W, Nitsche M, Pascual-Leone A, Byblow WD et al (2008) Consensus: motor
cortex plasticity protocols. Brain Stimulat 164–82
Part VI
Human-Machine Interface
Organizing and Reorganizing
Coordination Patterns
Abstract Understanding how the nervous system learns to coordinate the large
number of degrees of freedom in the body to produce goal-directed movement is not
only one of the central questions in theoretical movement neuroscience, but also has
direct relevance for movement rehabilitation. In spite of the centrality of this issue,
the literature on how a new coordination pattern is acquired and refined when first
learning a novel task remains surprisingly small relative to studies that focus on
modifications of already well-learned coordination patterns. In this chapter, we
outline some of the reasons behind why the study of coordination continues to pose
a serious challenge for movement neuroscience, particularly when it comes to
systematically studying and testing hypotheses on how new coordination patterns
are organized and reorganized with practice. We then describe a novel experimental
paradigm—the body–machine interface (BoMI)—that has been developed and used
over the last decade to examine this issue. The paradigm combines the control of a
large number of degrees of freedom along with a linear mapping, which makes it
appealing to examine how coordination of these high degrees of freedom is orga-
nized in a systematic fashion. Finally, we outline some of the new insights that this
paradigm has provided into classic issues of motor learning such as the learning of
high-dimensional spaces, generalization, and transfer.
Keywords Body–machine interface (BoMI) Motor learning Principal compo-
nent (PC) Task space Null space Glove-cursor task Redundant movements
Muscle synergies
R. Ranganathan
Department of Kinesiology, Michigan State, 203 IM Circle, East Lansing, MI, 48824, USA
R.A. Scheidt (&)
Department of Biomedical Engineering, Marquette University, Olin Engineering Center,
Room 206, P.O. Box 1881, Milwaukee, WI 53201, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
The second major challenge in the study of movement coordination is the nature of
the experimental data that is typically recorded. Motor learning studies, borrowing
heavily from their roots in experimental psychology (for a review, see Adams
1987), have traditionally focused on the analysis of “responses” or movement
outcomes (e.g., movement time, performance errors, etc.) with very little emphasis
on the how the movement itself unfolds in time (i.e., in terms of movement kine-
matics, forces, or muscle activities). Given that movement data can be very high
dimensional (depending on the number of degrees of freedom involved), how do we
both measure and compress massive amounts of data to test specific hypotheses?
What tools are available to measure the acquisition of a “coordination pattern,” and
whether coordination has changed dramatically (i.e., qualitatively) with learning?
Here we argue that there are two related issues with this challenge—the first issue
deals with describing and quantifying coordination, and a second issue is to explain
the extent to which coordination depends on specific features of the motor task such
as the spatial geometry of movements and forces to be produced.
To address the issue of quantifying coordination patterns, early work on coor-
dination (specifically locomotion) focused on using angle–angle plots (e.g., for a
330 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
review see Wheat and Glazier 2006). A separate, large body of literature offered a
“dynamical systems” perspective of motor coordination, based on the idea that even
though coordination itself is high dimensional, it can be captured in terms of
low-dimensional collective variables (Kelso 1995). Perhaps the most well known of
these collective variables was the use of relative phase between two effectors in
rhythmic movements to capture transitions in bimanual coordination (Kelso 1984).
However, in spite of its immense impact, the experimental strategy of trying to
identify collective variables has been much harder to generalize to complex
multi-joint movement, especially in nonrhythmic activities.
More recently, there have been advances in using dimensionality reduction
methods to understanding high-dimensional data obtained during experimental
study of motor coordination. One hypothesis that has received considerable atten-
tion is the idea that the nervous system coordinates complex, multidegree of free-
dom movement by combining smaller, temporally stable, building blocks or
modules (sometimes referred to as “motor primitives” or “synergies”). Principal
component analysis (PCA) has been extensively used in the identification of
kinematic synergies (Santello et al. 1998), whereas nonnegative matrix factorization
(NNMF) and factor analysis have been preferred for understanding muscle syn-
ergies in activities like posture (Ting and McKay 2007), reaching (Tresch et al.
1999), and locomotion (Ivanenko et al. 2004). Although there persists vigorous
debate as to what gives rise to these synergies (Kutch and Valero-Cuevas 2012; de
Rugy et al. 2013), these studies demonstrate that regularities in the performance of
goal-directed behaviors can be captured by describing coordination as combinations
of a small number of kinematic, kinetic, and/or electromyographic patterns of
behavior.
The second major challenge in the study of movement coordination is that even
if coordination patterns can be fully described and quantified by such motor
primitives, it is critical to have a framework in which to explain how the experi-
mentally identified motor primitives relate to—and depend on—specific features of
the motor task (e.g., the spatial geometry of movements and forces). In other words,
how does one determine whether observed changes in movement coordination are
potentially relevant or irrelevant to task performance? Such definitions are usually
difficult to define in real-world tasks like throwing a baseball or playing the violin
due to a lack of sufficient understanding of the coordination patterns that differ-
entiate skilled and unskilled performance. To address this challenge, several related
approaches (Scholz and Schoner 1999; Müller and Sternad 2004; Cusumano and
Cesari 2006; Torres et al. 2011) have provided a way to unambiguously define a
priori “task-space” and “null-space” performances, that is, patterns of coordination
that do versus do not contribute to task performance. We advocate for the idea that
advancing understanding of how task- and null-space performances change with
learning will provide new insights into in how patterns of multi-joint coordination
evolve during learning and adaptation. More specifically, using this framework will
help move the study of coordination from an account that simply describes changes
in coordination into an account of being to explain why these coordination changes
occur.
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 331
The experimental paradigm we currently use to address this critical need is called a
body–machine interface. Similar to its more well-known counterpart, the brain–
machine interface, a body–machine interface takes a large number of input signals
(in this case, motions of various body segments) and maps them onto a task space
(cf. Mosier et al. 2005; Liu et al. 2011; Casadio et al. 2012). Take for example the
task of moving a mouse to control the motion of a cursor on a computer display;
people readily and immediately respond to instructions such as “move the cursor
rapidly 5 cm to the left,” despite the fact that this action requires very different
patterns of muscle activations and interjoint coordination depending on the hand’s
initial position relative to the torso. This simple ability demonstrates that the central
nervous system is competent to capture geometric properties of the space in which
actions take place (e.g., measures of “distance” and “direction”) and to use that
information to plan and execute arbitrary movements. How then, does the spatial
geometry of a task influence the patterns of multi-joint coordination used to learn
and adapt that task? We describe below one form of a BoMI that is particularly well
suited to address this question: an instrumented data glove that maps finger motions
into cursor motions on a computer display.
We asked participants to don an instrumented glove (CyberGlove, Immersion
Technologies, Inc.) and to then practice using finger motions to move a computer
cursor between several different target locations on a computer screen (Mosier et al.
2005; Liu and Scheidt 2008; Liu et al. 2011; Ranganathan et al. 2013) (cf., Fig. 1a).
This is a novel and challenging task that most people can learn to perform with
some proficiency within the span of 1 to 2 hours. Glove signals, H, encoding finger
joint angles are mapped onto two-dimensional cursor motions, P, using an affine
transformation matrix A:
x a a1;2 ... a1;19 a1;20
P¼ ¼ A H T ¼ 1;1 ½ h1 h2 . . . h19 1 T ;
y a1;1 a2;2 ... a2;19 a2;20
where a1,20 and a2,20 are translation parameters used to center the projection of the
hand’s resting posture onto the center of the display screen. The remaining ai,j
coefficients determine how changes in each finger joint sensor value contributes to
332 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
Fig. 1 a Schematic of the glove-cursor interface. Hand postures are measured and mapped to
position of a cursor on the screen. b Representation of task space and null space in the glove-cursor
task—the hand posture H can be separated into a task-space component HT that influences the
position of the cursor, and a null-space component HN which has no influence on the cursor
position
motion along the x and y axes of the screen. It is important to note that this task is
redundant in the sense that capturing a single target location on the screen can be
achieved with multiple finger joint configurations. So whereas the screen has a
well-defined Euclidean metric (i.e., the distance between any two points P1 and P2
is the length of the straight segment that joins them), there is no obvious or natural
definition of distance within the “signal space” of finger articulations because
A partitions articulation space (the 19-dimensional vector of glove signals) into a
task space within which the cursor moves, and a complementary null space that
includes all combinations of finger motions that do not contribute to cursor motion
(Fig. 1b).
Fig. 2 Construction of the map between the hand posture and the cursor. a Participants use letters
from the American Manual Alphabet (AMA) when wearing the cyberglove. b PCA reveals that a
majority of the variance can be explained by a small number of (4–7) eigenvectors. c Repeatability
of the coefficients of PC1 and PC2 when the glove is taken off and on multiple times
the AMA is accounted for by the first 8–10 Principal Components (PCs). This
suggests that forming hand gestures as in fingerspelling requires at minimum 8–10
control dimensions, depending on one’s choice of threshold differentiating signal
334 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
from noise. The A matrix is then composed by stacking one or more of the first
several PCs on top of each other as row vectors. Using just one PC yields a
one-dimensional task, using two PCs yields a two-dimensional task, and so on. The
A matrix we have used most often to define a 2D task is the one that uses the first 2
fingerspelling PCs (i.e., those that capture the most variability within the AMA data
set).
Figure 2c plots the PC1-PC2 A matrix coefficients corresponding to each sensor
for 10 participants. Solid lines correspond to the average values for each subject;
dashed lines show the values computed for a single subject in each of 10 don/doff
cycles. The A matrix coefficients were similar across participants and across don-
ning and doffing cycles. Consequently, all participants using the PC1-PC2 A matrix
perform virtually identical tasks, even on different days.
Mosier and colleagues (Mosier et al. 2005) developed the glove-cursor BoMI to
determine specifically the extent to which the central nervous system acquires
patterns of coordination that capture the Euclidean properties of task space (the
space in which actions take place), and to explore how that representation con-
strains subsequent planning and execution of movements in high-dimensional
spaces.
In a first set of experiments, two groups of participants trained for 4 days to
move the cursor between pairs of screen targets on a 2 × 3 grid (cf., Fig. 3a). Here,
the A matrix was composed first by associating arbitrary hand postures with the four
corners of the display screen and then using linear least squares regression to fit the
matrix coefficients to glove signals recorded with the hand in those postures.
Participants could practice the task under two different feedback conditions. In No
Vision trials (NV), participants received no visual feedback of cursor motion during
movement. After each target capture attempt however, the cursor reappeared and
participants were required to bring the cursor to the desired target before the next
movement could start. In Vision trials (V), participants did receive visual feedback
of cursor motion during movement on training trials 2, 3, 5, 6, 8, and 9. Testing
trials 1, 4, 7, and 10 were identical to NV trials, and were used to provide a fair
comparison of performance under the two different training conditions.
At the beginning of practice on Day 1, all participants exhibited tremendous
performance variability. Subjects were slow to acquire the targets because cursor
paths were long, convoluted, and as likely to be directed orthogonal to the ideal,
straight line path to the target as they were to be directed along that path (Fig. 3a).
After about an hour’s practice in both subject groups, the time between target
presentation and target capture decreased, final position errors decreased, and cursor
paths straightened (i.e., they were more likely to be directed straight to the target
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 335
Fig. 3 Improvements in
cursor control with practice.
a Cursor paths initially in
learning (the corner pictures
indicate calibration postures
for the corners of the
workspace). b Cursor paths
after learning. c Aspect ratio
(a measure of linearity of
cursor paths) as a function of
practice and visual feedback
than not). Further straightening of cursor paths and improvements in target capture
accuracy were seen on subsequent days of training (cf., Fig. 3b), suggesting that
consolidation of motor memories enhances these aspects of performance (Fig. 3c).
Observation of practice-related performance changes—decreased movement times,
increased target capture accuracy, increased cursor path rectilinearity—was con-
firmed in subsequent glove-cursor studies performed with distinct, independently
sampled participant groups and with A matrices defined as described in Fig. 1 (Liu
and Scheidt 2008; Liu et al. 2011; Ranganathan et al. 2013; Farshchiansadegh et al.
2015).
336 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
Mosier and colleagues analyzed how behavior evolved within the task and null
spaces by first projecting changes in finger posture from the start to the end of each
movement into components residing in the task and null subspaces (Mosier et al.
2005), and then comparing the amount of motion (i.e., path length) within each
subspace as practice progressed. Consistent with an increase in cursor path
straightness, Mosier observed that task-space motion decreased significantly across
days, with the presence of visual feedback leading to a smaller amount of task-space
motion overall (Fig. 4a). Mosier also observed a similar reduction in null-space
motion across days in both feedback conditions (Fig. 4b). This observation is
consistent with the idea that when confronted with a sensorimotor task with
redundant degrees of freedom, the nervous system learns the geometric structure of
the task (i.e., a representation of the Euclidean space on which hand movements
were remapped) and improves performance by selectively operating within that
space. Counter to the predictions of optimal feedback control and the “minimum
intervention principle” (Todorov and Jordan 2002), which proposes that deviations
from the average trajectory are only corrected when they interfere with task per-
formance, the results of Mosier and colleagues show that when learning a novel
coordination task, null-space motions also decrease with practice, even though they
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 337
are irrelevant to task performance. This suggests the formation of an inverse map
(i.e., one that maps desired cursor position onto appropriate hand posture), a con-
struct that is discussed in greater detail below in the section on coordination in
articulation space.
Fig. 5 Generalization of learning a Participants practiced on the shaded targets (in the cardinal
directions), and generalization of learning was tested using both an interpolation set and an
extrapolation set. b Euclidean error at the beginning, end of training, and a delayed period showed
that generalization occurred for both interpolation and extrapolation sets. Blue training target set;
Orange interpolation set; Red extrapolation set
reaching (Torres and Zipser 2002; Biess et al. 2007; Zipser and Torres 2007), the
structure of the glove-cursor task permits us to determine how participants do so in
a novel task.
In a very real sense, each participant’s solution to the problem of how to invert
matrix A reflects how he or she has imported the geometry of task space (as defined
by matrix A) into articulation space. Essentially, we seek to estimate from the
experimental data a matrix B, which maps desired displacements of cursor positions
onto appropriate changes in finger joint configurations:
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 339
DH ¼ B DP
We can estimate the B matrix using a least squares fit to the data (Liu et al. 2011;
see also Farshchiansadegh et al. 2015):
The other group performed three blocks of movements wherein they experienced
a stepwise, scaling distortion of visual performance feedback:
xscaled 1:8 0 x
PT ¼ ¼ TSP ¼
yscaled 0 1:8 y
In both groups, training with the distorted visual feedback progressed for about
one-half hour, after which time the baseline mapping was reinstated for two more
blocks of trials.
During Day 1 training, initial target capture attempts were inefficient and
dominated by exploratory behavior whereas cursor trajectories late in training were
generally straight and terminated close to the goal (Fig. 6a). This ability was readily
recalled during baseline practice on Day 2 in both groups (Fig. 6b).
Both groups of participants exhibited an adaptive feedforward strategy com-
pensating for the rotation or scaling distortions. For the rotation group, imposing the
stepwise counterclockwise rotation caused cursor trajectories to initially deviate
counterclockwise, but later curve back to the desired final position (Fig. 6b, top).
With practice under the altered mapping, trajectories regained their original recti-
linearity with a learning rate constant of 33 ± 5 trials. When the baseline map was
340 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
Fig. 6 Adapting to task geometry. a Cursor paths at the beginning and end of Day 1 showing
learning of the task. b Cursor paths on Day 2 during baseline, adaptation (with either rotation or
scaling), and aftereffects. c Changes in null-space and task-space variability for both scaling
(black) and rotation (red). d Change in the estimated inverse map showing reorganization in the
rotation, but not the scaling perturbation
restored, the initial portion of the cursor trajectories deviated clockwise relative to
trajectories made during baseline practice. These aftereffects of training with rotated
cursor feedback were transient. For the scaling group, initial exposure to the visual
distortion resulted in cursor trajectories that far overshot their goal (Fig. 6b, bot-
tom). Further practice under the altered mapping reduced these extent errors with a
learning rate time constant of 12 ± 3 trials. Restoring the baseline map resulted in
initial cursor movements that undershot their goal. As for the rotation group,
aftereffects of training were largely eliminated by the end of the washout period.
These task-space adaptations are similar to those observed during studies of
adaptation to rotations and scalings during traditional motor adaptation tasks such
as reaching and pointing (cf., Krakauer et al. 2000).
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 341
Unlike reaching and pointing, however, the glove-cursor task permits analysis
within the complete space of highly redundant control variables that impact task
performance. This capability allowed (Liu et al. 2011) to ask how patterns of coor-
dination within articulation space reorganize when a geometrical transformation TR or
TS is imposed on the geometry of cursor motion. Do participants preserve their
original (baseline) inverse map or do they interpret the transformed environment as a
new task geometry to be learned? Both are viable options. In the first case, participants
would maintain their baseline model of the map (i.e., matrix B) and apply an inverse
transformation T−1 to the targets. In the second case, participants would abandon their
baseline model and create a whole new model of the map. The first option requires
participants to interpret the new map as a transformation of the old one (as was actually
the case). The second option requires participants to learn the new map de novo.
Liu and colleagues (Liu et al. 2011) found that performance variability in both
the task and null subspaces increased transiently after imposing and removing the
rotation (but not the scaling) distortion (Fig. 6c). This finding suggests that the
rotation and scaling distortions induced categorically different reorganizations of
coordination. In a follow-on analysis, Liu et al. (2011) examined the extent to
which Best changed during adaptation to the rotation and scaling distortions. To do
so, they computed Best using values of ΔH and ΔP from trials at the end of the
adaptation trial block (ADAPT) as well as at the beginning (BL1) and end (BL2) of
baseline trials. They evaluated how well the Best obtained after adaptation (BADAPT)
was predicted by rotation (TR) or scaling (TS) of the Best obtained at the end of
baseline practice (BBL2) by computing the difference magnitude ΔBADAPT:
DBADAPT ¼ BADAPT BBL2 T1
To test the significance of this change, they compared ΔBADAPT to the difference
magnitude estimated using movements recorded at the beginning (BBL1) and end
(BBL2) of baseline training:
For participants exposed to the rotation distortion, Best after adaptation could not
reasonably be characterized as a rotated version of the baseline mapping because
ΔBADAPT far exceeded ΔBNOISE for these participants (Fig. 6d, red). The
within-subject difference between ΔBADAPT and ΔBNOISE was significantly greater
than zero (Fig. 6d, red solid bar). Thus the rotational distortion induced these
participants to reorganized patterns of coordination within articulation space to
form new inverse hand-to-screen mappings that differed substantially from the
original mapping. By contrast, ΔBADAPT did not exceed ΔBNOISE for scaling
participants; this suggests that scaling participants simply contracted their baseline
inverse map to compensate for the imposed scaling distortion. Taken together, the
results reveal that people invoke distinct compensatory strategies when confronted
with rotation and scaling distortions of visual feedback, thereby demonstrating
342 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
power of the BoMI paradigm to provide insight into how the human brain organizes
and reorganizes patterns of coordination in the production of goal-directed actions.
Due to redundancy of control in the BoMI paradigm, there are multiple options or
solutions that can be used to achieve task goals. Ranganathan and colleagues
(Ranganathan et al. 2014) sought to characterize the extent to which it is possible to
influence the participant’s solution in articulation space by exploiting the phe-
nomenon of “transfer” (i.e., how practice on a prior task influences the coordination
pattern used when learning a novel task). There exist common cases of positive
transfer (e.g., learning to roller skate facilitates subsequent learning of ice skating)
as well as negative transfer (e.g., learning to play tennis interferes with subsequent
learning of badminton). Transfer (positive or negative) can be explained by
invoking concepts of “similarity” or “dissimilarity” between the tasks involved
(Osgood 1949). However, experimental testing of this explanation in multi-joint
movements has been challenging because heretofore there has been no obvious way
to either quantify similarity between coordination patterns or to design motor tasks
that require the use of similar or dissimilar coordination patterns.
The BoMI paradigm provided a novel way to examine this question.
Ranganathan and colleagues (Ranganathan et al. 2014) used the glove-cursor task
to construct several tasks that had different degrees of overlap or similarity with a
specific “criterion” task. By requiring participants to train on pairs of tasks that
either do or do not share task-space dimensions, the authors aimed to address
questions about how the brain represents extrinsic task space within the sensori-
motor system, and how such representations determine whether prior learning will
facilitate or interfere with subsequent task learning. While the phenomenon of
transfer has been explored in adaptation studies under the idea of structural learning
(Braun et al., 2009, 2010), similarity between tasks in those studies was based on
the type of visuomotor perturbations experienced, not in terms of movement
coordination patterns themselves.
Ranganathan and colleagues selected a two-dimensional criterion task where the
A matrix was based on PC1 and PC2 from the fingerspelling task. All study
participants had to learn the criterion task. The authors then constructed several
“prior” tasks that had different degrees of overlap with the criterion task. Given that
principal components are orthogonal to each other, the prior tasks were designed to
either share or not share the criterion task dimensions. Three of the prior tasks had
significant overlap with the criterion task as they shared PC projection vectors with
the criterion task’s task space; the A matrix for the “1D task” was based on PC1, the
A matrix for the “2D task” was identical to the criterion task (the experimental
control condition), whereas the A matrix for the “3D task” was based on PC1, PC2,
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 343
and PC3. Two additional prior tasks had no overlap with the criterion task’s task
space; the A matrix for the “1D-Null task” was based on PC3, whereas the A matrix
for the “2D-Null task” was based on PC3 and PC4. Participants were split into five
different groups: the 2D group did not have a “prior” task and trained only on the
criterion task (serving as the control group), whereas the remaining four groups
trained on a different prior task on Day 1 (1D, 3D, 1D-Null, or 2D-Null) before
training on the criterion task during the next 2 days (Fig. 7a).
Transfer to the criterion task depended on the amount of overlap between prior
task and the criterion task hand-to-screen mappings (Fig. 7b). Participants who
initially learned a mapping that overlapped with the mapping in the criterion task
(i.e., the 1D and the 3D tasks) showed improved initial task performance (i.e.,
positive transfer) on Day-2 training in the criterion task compared to participants
who learned the task from scratch. By contrast, participants who learned a prior
mapping in the null space of the criterion task (the 1D-Null and 2D-Null groups)
had longer movement times (i.e., negative transfer). Moreover, and unlike adap-
tation tasks where interference is washed out in a few trials, interference effects in
the 2D-Null group were sustained over 2 days of practice. This interference mimics
real-life phenomena where learning can result in atypical movement habits that can
sometimes be maladaptive (e.g., the “chickenwing” backswing in golf, or com-
pensatory trunk movements during reaching after a stroke).
The extent to which the task space of the prior and criterion tasks did or did not
overlap resulted in positive or negative transfer, respectively. Participants exhibited
a strong tendency to explore preferentially along the task space of the prior task
even when performing the criterion task; they did not learn the criterion task
entirely de novo, but instead appeared to adapt their prior mapping to accommodate
the new criterion mapping. Persistence of null-space coordination patterns acquired
through prior practice of tasks that did not enforce the geometry of the criterion task
impeded future improvements in criterion task performance, and this interference
appears to be difficult to unlearn. We suggest that the ability of the glove-cursor
task to quantify and manipulate the degree of similarity between tasks may enable
future studies to gain additional insight into the phenomenon of transfer and how it
may be possible to modify movement coordination through specific training
schedules and programs, an idea that has relevance for movement rehabilitation
science.
In addition to the issues raised so far, BoMIs have also been used to provide
theoretical insight into other motor control issues. For example, an unresolved
question in the field of motor control is whether control of the large number of
muscles in the body is simplified by grouping muscles into a smaller number of
“synergies” (also referred to as “motor modules”). Approaches range from detailed
musculoskeletal modeling (Valero-Cuevas et al. 2015) to intraspinal and intracor-
tical stimulation (Giszter et al. 2007; Overduin et al. 2015). A specific point of
contention is whether the low dimensionality observed in goal-directed behavior is
indicative of a control strategy adopted by the nervous system (Tresch et al. 1999;
d’Avella et al. 2003), or whether they simply arise from the biomechanical con-
straints of the body when performing the task (Kutch and Valero-Cuevas 2012).
Answering this question is methodologically challenging because: (i) it is difficult
to measure muscle activity from all of the independently controlled muscle ele-
ments surrounding multiple joints, and (ii) even if this could be measured, the exact
mapping of how muscle activity determines (and is determined by) kinematic and
kinetic performance is complex and difficult to disentangle empirically.
BoMIs provide a unique opportunity to answer this question, as it is possible to
create “virtual bodies” where the exact mapping between muscle activity and
behavior can be determined by the experimenter (Nazarpour et al. 2012; Berger
et al. 2013; de Rugy et al. 2013). For example, using surface EMG recordings from
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 345
the superficial muscles in the arm, it is possible to drive a virtual arm whose
behavior is determined only by those muscle recordings. Moreover, because the
interface is virtual, the biomechanics of the virtual body can also be altered by
performing “virtual surgeries” (Berger et al. 2013). For example, agonistic muscles
generating similar mechanical actions about a joint (such as the biceps and bra-
chioradialis) normally are activated simultaneously. However, to examine if these
muscles are controlled as a “single unit” (i.e., a muscle synergy), it is possible to
create a virtual arm where these two muscles are functional antagonists (e.g., the
biceps could drive the virtual arm into elbow flexion, whereas the brachioradialis
could drive the elbow into extension). A strong case for fixed muscle synergies can
be made if the muscles continue to be activated together, even when their function
in the virtual world is made to conflict. In one study, Berger and colleagues found
that it was possible to learn novel patterns of muscle coordination when learning to
control a virtual object, but learning was faster when that control required muscle
activity consistent with preexisting patterns of intermuscular coordination (Berger
et al. 2013). In this way, BoMIs can give unique insight into the neural and
biomechanical bases of muscle synergies.
In addition to the theoretical impact, BoMIs also have practical implications in
the field of rehabilitation. One of the challenges in developing assistive technolo-
gies for people with limited movement abilities is to provide a single interface that
can help them interact with their environment in multiple ways—such as control-
ling a cursor, a robot arm or even a wheelchair. Although this has traditionally been
the domain of brain–machine interfaces (Serruya et al. 2002; Wolpaw et al. 2002;
Lebedev and Nicolelis 2006)—where neural signals are mapped to the control of an
external device—it is worth noting that brain–machine interfaces are ideally suited
only for a small number of cases where there is almost a complete absence of
movement. On the other hand, a significant portion of people with movement
impairments still possess residual motor function. BoMIs can be designed to exploit
these residual movements to enable these individuals to control external devices. In
addition to being fast and noninvasive, a key advantage of BoMIs over brain–
machine interfaces is that they can be dynamically adapted to the changing
movement repertoire of the individual. This makes this interface especially
appealing in cases where movement abilities may change with time—either due to
growth and physical development (Lee et al. 2016) or due to rehabilitation and
partial functional recovery.
Recently, Mussa-Ivaldi and colleagues have developed a BoMI for participants
with high-level spinal cord injury (SCI) (Casadio et al. 2010; Thorp et al. 2016).
The BoMI consisted of a wearable sensor system, which consisted of four, wireless
inertial measurement units (IMUs) that were attached to the upper body. Similar to
the glove-cursor task, a high-dimensional vector of signals from the IMUs were
mapped to the low-dimensional position of a cursor on a computer screen, which
allowed participants to communicate (type text) and play computer games using
movements of the upper body. The interface also allowed the IMU signals to
directly control the velocity of a powered wheelchair using an IMU-to-wheelchair
mapping. Despite variations in the level of injury and movement abilities,
346 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
participants with SCI were able to learn to control the cursor and the wheelchair
within a few sessions of practice. More importantly, they retained this ability over
time. The ability to use the BoMI for real-time control over virtual and real-world
objects provides a more natural, continuous mode of control that exceeds the
capabilities of discrete methods of control like head switches or sip/puff controllers.
Moreover, using upper body movements instead of other modalities that overlap
with communication such as gaze control or mouth sticks, participants using the
BoMI retain the freedom to communicate while operating the interface.
Acknowledgments The authors thank Prof. Ferdinando Mussa-Ivaldi for his insightful and for-
mative discussions, which were integral to the conception and execution of each of the glove
BoMI experiments described in this chapter.
References
Adams JA (1987) Historical review and appraisal of research on the learning, retention, and
transfer of human motor skills. Psychol Bull 101:41–74
Adolph KE, Cole WG, Komati M, Garciaguirre JS, Badaly D, Lingeman JM, Chan GL, Sotsky RB
(2012) How do you learn to walk? Thousands of steps and dozens of falls per day. Psychol Sci
23:1387–1394
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 347
Bastian AJ (2008) Understanding sensorimotor adaptation and learning for rehabilitation. Curr
Opin Neurol 21:628–633
Berger DJ, Gentner R, Edmunds T, Pai DK, d’Avella A (2013) Differences in adaptation rates after
virtual surgeries provide direct evidence for modularity. J Neurosci 33:12384–12394
Bernstein N (1967) The coordination and regulation of movement. Pergamon Press, Oxford
Biess A, Liebermann DG, Flash T (2007) A computational model for redundant human
three-dimensional pointing movements: integration of independent spatial and temporal motor
plans simplifies movement dynamics. J Neurosci 27:13045–13064
Braun DA, Aertsen A, Wolpert DM, Mehring C (2009) Motor task variation induces structural
learning. Curr Biol 19:352–357
Braun D, Mehring C, Wolpert D (2010) Structure learning in action. Behav Brain Res 206:
157–165
Casadio M, Pressman A, Fishbach A, Danziger Z, Acosta S, Chen D, Tseng HY, Mussa-Ivaldi FA
(2010) Functional reorganization of upper-body movement after spinal cord injury. Exp Brain
Res 207:233–247
Casadio M, Ranganathan R, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2012) The body-machine interface: a new
perspective on an old theme. J Mot Behav 44:419–433
Cusumano JP, Cesari P (2006) Body-goal variability mapping in an aiming task. Biol Cybern
94:367–379
d’Avella A, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (2003) Combinations of muscle synergies in the construction of a
natural motor behavior. Nat Neurosci 6:300–308
de Rugy A, Loeb GE, Carroll TJ (2013) Are muscle synergies useful for neural control? Front
Comput Neurosci 7:19
Farshchiansadegh A, Ranganathan R, Casadio M, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2015) Adaptation to visual
feedback delay in a redundant motor task. J Neurophysiol 113:426–433
Gessell A (1946) The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In: Carmichael L (ed) Manual of child
psychology. John Wiley, New York, pp 295–331
Giszter S, Patil V, Hart C (2007) Primitives, premotor drives, and pattern generation: a combined
computational and neuroethological perspective. Prog Brain Res 165:323–346
Heathcote A, Brown S, Mewhort DJ (2000) The power law repealed: the case for an exponential
law of practice. Psychon Bull Rev 7:185–207
Ivanenko YP, Poppele RE, Lacquaniti F (2004) Five basic muscle activation patterns account for
muscle activity during human locomotion. J Physiol (Lond) 556:267–282
Kelso JAS (1984) Phase transitions and critical behavior in human bimanual coordination. Am J
Physiol 246:R1000–R1004
Kelso JAS (1995) Dynamic patterns: the self-organization of brain and behavior. MIT Press,
Cambridge
Krakauer JW, Mazzoni P (2011) Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond. Curr
Opin Neurobiol 21:636–644
Krakauer JW, Pine ZM, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (2000) Learning of visuomotor transformations for
vectorial planning of reaching trajectories. J Neurosci 20:8916–8924
Kutch JJ, Valero-Cuevas FJ (2012) Challenges and new approaches to proving the existence of
muscle synergies of neural origin. PLoS Comput Biol 8:e1002434
Langendorfer SJ, Roberton MA (2002) Individual pathways in the development of forceful
throwing. Res Q Exerc Sport 73:245–256
Lebedev MA, Nicolelis MA (2006) Brain-machine interfaces: past, present and future. Trends
Neurosci 29:536–546
Lee MH, Ranganathan R, Kagerer FA, Mukherjee R (2016) Body-machine interface for control of
a screen cursor for a child with congenital absence of upper and lower limbs: a case report.
J Neuroeng Rehabil 13:34
Liu X, Scheidt RA (2008) Contributions of online visual feedback to the learning and
generalization of novel finger coordination patterns. J Neurophysiol 99:2546–2557
Liu YT, Mayer-Kress G, Newell KM (2006) Qualitative and quantitative change in the dynamics
of motor learning. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 32:380–393
348 R. Ranganathan and R.A. Scheidt
Liu X, Mosier KM, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Casadio M, Scheidt RA (2011) Reorganization of finger
coordination patterns during adaptation to rotation and scaling of a newly learned sensorimotor
transformation. J Neurophysiol 105:454–473
Mosier KM, Scheidt RA, Acosta S, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2005) Remapping hand movements in a
novel geometrical environment. J Neurophysiol 94:4362–4372
Müller H, Sternad D (2004) Decomposition of variability in the execution of goal-oriented tasks:
three components of skill improvement. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 30:212
Nazarpour K, Barnard A, Jackson A (2012) Flexible cortical control of task-specific muscle
synergies. J Neurosci 32:12349–12360
Newell KM (1991) Motor skill acquisition. Annu Rev Psychol 42:213–237
Newell A, Rosenbloom PS (1981) Mechanisms of skill acquisition and the law of practice. In:
Anderson JR (ed) Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, pp 1–55
Osgood CE (1949) The similarity paradox in human learning; a resolution. Psychol Rev 56:
132–143
Overduin SA, d’Avella A, Roh J, Carmena JM, Bizzi E (2015) Representation of muscle synergies
in the primate brain. J Neurosci 35:12615–12624
Pierella C, Abdollahi F, Farshchiansadegh A, Pedersen J, Thorp EB, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Casadio M
(2015) Remapping residual coordination for controlling assistive devices and recovering motor
functions. Neuropsychologia 79:364–376
Ranganathan R, Adewuyi A, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2013) Learning to be lazy: exploiting redundancy
in a novel task to minimize movement-related effort. J Neurosci 33:2754–2760
Ranganathan R, Wieser J, Mosier KM, Mussa-Ivaldi FA, Scheidt RA (2014) Learning redundant
motor tasks with and without overlapping dimensions: facilitation and interference effects.
J Neurosci 34:8289–8299
Roberton MA, Halverson LE, Langendorfer S, Williams K (1979) Longitudinal changes in
children’s overarm throw ball velocities. Res Q 50:256–264
Santello M, Flanders M, Soechting JF (1998) Postural hand synergies for tool use. J Neurosci
18:10105–10115
Scholz JP, Schoner G (1999) The uncontrolled manifold concept: identifying control variables for
a functional task. Exp Brain Res 126:289–306
Serruya MD, Hatsopoulos NG, Paninski L, Fellows MR, Donoghue JP (2002) Instant neural
control of a movement signal. Nature 416:141–142
Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R (2006) Interacting adaptive processes with different
timescales underlie short-term motor learning. PLoS Biol 4:e179
Thelen E, Corbetta D, Spencer JP (1996) Development of reaching during the first year: role of
movement speed. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform 22:1059–1076
Thorp EB, Abdollahi F, Chen D, Farshchiansadegh A, Lee MH, Pedersen JP, Pierella C, Roth EJ,
Seanez Gonzalez I, Mussa-Ivaldi FA (2016) Upper body-based power wheelchair control
interface for individuals with tetraplegia. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 24:249–260
Ting LH, McKay JL (2007) Neuromechanics of muscle synergies for posture and movement. Curr
Opin Neurobiol 17:622–628
Todorov E, Jordan MI (2002) Optimal feedback control as a theory of motor coordination. Nat
Neurosci 5:1226–1235
Torres EB, Zipser D (2002) Reaching to grasp with a multi-jointed arm. I. Computational model.
J Neurophysiol 88:2355–2367
Torres EB, Heilman KM, Poizner H (2011) Impaired endogenously evoked automated reaching in
Parkinson’s disease. J Neurosci 31:17848–17863
Tresch MC, Saltiel P, Bizzi E (1999) The construction of movement by the spinal cord. Nat
Neurosci 2:162–167
Valero-Cuevas F, Cohn B, Yngvason H, Lawrence E (2015) Exploring the high-dimensional
structure of muscle redundancy via subject-specific and generic musculoskeletal models.
J Biomech 48:2887–2896
Organizing and Reorganizing Coordination Patterns 349
Wheat JS, Glazier PS (2006) Measuring coordination and variability in coordination. In: Davids K,
Bennett SJ, Newell KM (eds) Movement system variability. Human Kinetics, Champaign, IL,
pp 167–181
Wolpaw JR, Birbaumer N, McFarland DJ, Pfurtscheller G, Vaughan TM (2002) Brain-computer
interfaces for communication and control. Clin Neurophysiol 113:767–791
Wright ZA, Rymer WZ, Slutzky MW (2013) Reducing abnormal muscle coactivation after stroke
using a myoelectric-computer interface: a pilot study. Neurorehabil Neural Repair 28:443–451
Zipser D, Torres E (2007) Computing movement geometry: a step in sensory-motor transforma-
tions. Prog Brain Res 165:411–424
A Computational Index to Describe
Slacking During Robot Therapy
Davide Piovesan
Keywords Manipulandum Assistive Rehabilitation Adaptive controllers
Challenge-based Dystonia Rigidity Viscosity Damping Degrees of freedom
(DOF) Reference trajectory Proprioception Neuro-rehabilitation
Introduction
When we move our arm in space from one point to another, a force applied to our
hand may change how we perform the movement and therefore how our brain
controls the motion of the arm. This well-known concept has been extensively used
in motor control studies to investigate how the brain learns to move in unknown,
through predictable environments (Lackner and Dizio 1994; Shadmehr and
D. Piovesan (&)
Department of Mechanical Engineering, Gannon University, Erie, PA, 16501, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
The aim of this study is to investigate the control strategy of stroke survivors
with a training protocol in which the level of assistive forces is always decreased
down to the minimum force that can evoke a voluntary response. As the training
proceeded and the recovery occurred, volunteers were able to perform the same
exercise with lower levels of forces. We develop a metric that could clearly dif-
ferentiate the amount of voluntary movement for each trial correlating the velocity
profile of the subjects’ hand with those of an ideal trajectory in which the assistive
force would interact only with passive arm mechanics. We investigated the effect of
the initial (maximum) assistive force level when subjects were able to move in the
presence of significantly lower forces. We hypothesized that subjects could either
(i) use the same amount of voluntary control they used at the beginning, (ii) relay
less on the assistive force and more on the voluntary control since their movement
abilities were improved, or (iii) be “greedy optimizers” and decrease their voluntary
control exploiting the forces.
(a) between trials at the beginning and at the end of the training with the same force
level
(b) between trials in the last session with decreasing force levels, in such way that
the performance with lower level of force would be similar to the behavior in
the previous sessions with higher force.
A control experiment was run to test the effect of assistive force on voluntary
control in five able bodied individuals with no history of previous neurological
disorder and with normal level of movement skills.
Experimental Task
The task was to hit a set of seven equally spaced targets arranged at distal positions
almost to the workspace limit (C layer). The exercise was composed of blocks
where subjects starting from the three different positions (A Layer) had to reach
each of the seven targets, presented in random order. Each block consisted of 21
outward movements of amplitude about 20 cm. The workspace was centered with
respect to the shoulder joint contralateral to the lesion.
The planar robotic manipulandum provided assistive forces helping stroke sur-
vivors to accomplish the task. In the blocks where visual feedback was provided,
subjects saw their arm, the robot, and a computer monitor (1 m away at eye level)
that displayed the end-effector position and the target to reach (Fig. 1). The target
and the cursor corresponding to the end-effector position were represented as round
disks of different colors and 1 cm radius. In the blocks where visual feedback was
not provided, subjects were blindfolded and they have to reach the targets only
relying on the haptic cue of the assistive force.
Fig. 1 Experimental
setup. A represent the starting
point layer, C represents the
targets layer. All
combinations of targets was
performed for a total of
3 × 7 = 21 reaches per
session
A Computational Index to Describe Slacking During Robot Therapy 355
where terms in bold (e.g., u) represent vectors and terms in italic (e.g., u) represent
scalars. In Eq. (1) Fe is the force provided by the manipulandum, xT is the target
position, xe is the position of the end effector, and Fa is the selected level of the
assistive force in the trial (see Table 1 for maximum level). The term GðFa ; tÞ starts
from 0, increasing linearly to Fa with a rise time of 1 s; therefore, enabling a
smooth activation of the force field. The two additional terms represent a viscous
field to stabilize the arm posture and a rigid wall. While the viscous field is always
active, the rigid wall engages only beyond the targets’ level, which provided a
representation of the boundary of the workspace. The coefficient Be was empirically
determined to be 12 Ns/m as a trade-off between stability and dissipated energy.
The stiff virtual wall was rendered with a 1000 N/m elastic coefficient Kw (Casadio
et al. 2009a) and used only as safety boundary. When the movement was done
properly, the virtual wall was never hit. The training consisted of 10 sessions of a
duration that ranged from 45 to 75 min. Each session started with the same initial
force, selected by the therapist as the minimal force allowing the subject to initiate
the movement. After the first two blocks, the therapist could decide to extend the
exercise with additional blocks. In these blocks, the levels of force were lowered, in
accordance with the subject’s residual ability and fatigue. For each subsequent
session, while starting always with the first imposed force of the first session,
subjects experienced a further decrease of assistive force, where the ultimate goal
would be (when possible) to reach the target with no assistive force. When subjects
reached each target, the assistive force and visual feedback were switched off for 1 s
before the following target appeared on the screen. The kinematic response to this
sudden drop in assistive force was used to estimate the arm impedance using the
time frequency technique described in Piovesan et al. (2009, 2011a, b, 2012).
Nine chronic stroke survivors with different levels of impairment participated in
the experiment and their clinical and anthropometric data are listed in Table 1.
Subjects held with their right impaired hand the end effector of a planar manipu-
landum (Casadio et al. 2006). Their shoulders and wrist movements were restricted
by using custom made holders.
Simulation
A simple schematic of the coupling between robot and human arm mechanics in the
Cartesian space for a single degree of freedom is depicted in Fig. 2. The system is
governed by the following equation:
Mxi €xi þ Cxi x_ i x_ refi þ Kxi xi xrefi þ Dix x_ i þ Rix xi ¼ Fei ð2Þ
where subscript x refers to the properties of the arm in the Cartesian space. The
variable xi is the ith coordinate of the point of contact between the hand and robot’s
Fig. 2 Mechanical
schematics of the interaction
between the human arm and
the robot: [Mx, Cx, Kx] are the
inertia, damping, and stiffness
of the arm. Fe is the assistive
force, xref is the reference
trajectory for the subject
motor plan. [Rx, Dx] are the
rigidity and viscosity of the
arm
A Computational Index to Describe Slacking During Robot Therapy 357
end effector, calculated in the subjects’ reference frame. Fei is the force provided by
the manipulandum from Eq. (1), xrefi is the reference trajectory for the subjects’
motor plan. M, K, C, R, and D, are the matrices that represent inertia, stiffness,
damping, rigidity, and viscosity proprieties of the subjects arm. According to this
formalism stiffness and rigidity, as well as damping and viscosity refers to different
arm properties that we want to clarify in the following paragraph.
One of the consequences of stroke is the development of an intrinsic arm dys-
tonia that might result in a dominant flexion pattern. Dystonia generates a
position-dependent force that can vary with the degree of impairment. Dystonic
forces are relative to a specific equilibrium position and the starting point of the
trajectory was located in its vicinity. With the hand in such position, applying a
displacement to it in the direction away from the body gives rise to a roughly linear
increase in force opposing the movement. Approximating the position-dependent
force with a linear spring is common in the literature, and the angular coefficient of
rigidity for elbow movement has been reported to reach values up to 8 Nm/rad
(Schmit et al. 1999). In this work, we will refer to the feature characterizing dys-
tonia as a linear coefficient generating force with respect to the trajectory starting
point, using the term “coefficient of rigidity” or simply “rigidity.”
Conversely, we will adopt the term “coefficient of stiffness” or simply “stiffness”
to the parameter that generates a position-dependent reaction force with respect to a
moving point on a reference trajectory. Hence, rigidity and stiffness have two
different reference points. During a point-to-point movement, rigidity generates a
force field that hinders the movement. This field is always present even in unim-
paired individuals but tend to increase due to atrophy and muscle contractions.
Stiffness generates a position-dependent force that maintains the hand on the
intended trajectory if a disturbance occurs. The stiffness reference point changes in
time, moving synchronously with the intended trajectory. In general, stiffness is one
order of magnitude larger than rigidity. However, the force at the end effector
associated to them can be comparable since the rigidity is multiplied by the
amplitude of the movement, while stiffness is multiplied by the deviation of the
movement from the intended trajectory.
Similar considerations can be made for viscosity and damping. The former acts
in parallel to rigidity and generates a force field proportional to the instantaneous
velocity of the movement. The latter acts in parallel to stiffness and generates a
force field proportional to the rate of change of the real trajectory with respect to the
intended one.
The reference trajectory based on external force (EF) can be calculated solving the
Eq. (2) for all degrees of freedom (DOF) imposing, Rx , Dx , Fe and assuming that
358 D. Piovesan
the reference trajectory coincide with the actual trajectory. Notice that Be in Eq. (1)
is embedded in Fe and it depends on the velocity of the end effector. Since the arm
is modeled as a double pendulum and not as a point mass, the inertial matrix Mx is
not diagonal. Hence, the EF trajectory is not a straight line due to the effect of
centripetal and Coriolis force about the joints. The trajectory is curved but it is the
most advantageous in order to reach the target in terms of minimum modulation of
joint torque (i.e., the external force is doing the work for you). The solution of
Eq. (2) given the aforementioned constraints will be used as reference trajectory so
that xEF ¼ xref .
From the experimental data, we obtained the reaching time T of each assisted
movement. The time was estimated as the period comprised between the instant in
which subjects increase the hand absolute reaching velocity above 10 % of the
absolute maximum to the instant in which they decrease the velocity below such
threshold, permanently. To estimate the EF trajectory requires some assumption of
limb mechanics. The arm was modeled as a two DOF system where the shoulder
has a fixed center of rotation. Arm inertial parameters for the estimation of MX were
estimated based on a subject’s weight and height (Zatsiorsky and Seluyanov 1983).
The efficacy of this method with respect to others is discussed in (Piovesan et al.
2011c). The endpoint stiffness KX , and damping CX , were measured using a newly
developed technique, which can estimate the parameters from the values of the arm
residual vibration after the target is reached and the assistive force is suddenly
switched off. The description of the technique can be found in Piovesan et al. (2012,
2013b), while experimental results used in the present paper are reported in
Piovesan et al. (2011a, b, 2013a).
The implemented rigidity and viscosity were estimated using an algorithm
described in the next section. Hence, the trajectory of the hand in the Cartesian
space was computed using an inverse dynamic routine implemented in Simulink®.
The assistive force was implemented as described in Eq. (1).
Rigidity estimation
Given the desired reaching time of the movement and the external force applied in
the experiment, we implemented a first simulation to find the maximum allowed
rigidity. Setting the reference position for the rigidity at the starting point, the
farther from such position the hand is displaced, the larger is the force generated by
the rigidity.
Setting the external force Fe , we will iteratively change the rigidity and viscosity
so that the hand would reach the target at the same time T of the real trajectory.
Since the assistive force is switched off when the target is reached, if the set rigidity
is too low, then the movement will be too fast, and a recoil will be present.
Conversely, if the resistive force generated by the rigidity is too high, the
end-effector would not reach the target in the desired time.
We set the starting rigidity and viscosity matrices at the joints to
A Computational Index to Describe Slacking During Robot Therapy 359
R11 R12 3:5 0:5 Nm
Rh ¼ ¼
R21
R22 0:5 2:0 rad
Nms
Dh ¼ 0:1 Rh ð3Þ
rad
Given the assistive force Fe and reaching time T, the parameter q changes the
stiffness in our simulations isotropically so to obtain a trajectory that reaches the
target with no residual recoil. In this work, the maximum value of the coefficient of
“rigidity” was 2, producing a maximum rigidity of 7 Nm/rad, well within the
physiological range reported in the literature (Schmit et al. 1999).
Data Analysis
If the correlation between the simulated and the real performance will change
with training, we plan a further analysis that will verify the effect of force. We
expect that by decreasing the forces in the last session the correlation between the
measured and the predicted trajectories will change.
Specifically, we hypothesize that
(a) the trajectories at the end of the training will be more correlated with the
simulated ones than the trajectories with a same force level at beginning of the
training
(b) the performance with lower level of force in the last session would be similar
with the behavior (lower correlation) in previous sessions with higher assistive
force.
Results
The first two histogram bars from the left in Fig. 3 show the correlation coefficients
between the experimentally acquired velocity profiles and those calculated in sim-
ulation when following a minimum effort trajectory at the maximum level of force.
The distribution of coefficients is calculated at the fourth and last session (mean and
standard deviation) for our sample population of stroke survivors. We can observe a
statistically significant increase in the correlation coefficients at the last session (see
Table 2), supporting the hypothesis that when a substantial improvement in vol-
untary motion is achieved, a level of assistance beyond subjects need induces a
decrease in voluntary control generally referred as “slaking” (Reinkensmeyer et al.
2009; Reschechtko et al. 2015). The third and fourth histogram bars from the left
show a similar behavior when the exercise is executed in absence of visual feedback.
Hence, we can infer that executing the task blindfolded—i.e., without any visual
feedback and only relying on the haptic cue of the assistive forces—does not
introduce any statistically significant changes in the “slaking” behavior.
To verify if in the last session the voluntary control not maintained at high force
level is still present at lower levels, we performed an analysis to assess the change
Fig. 3 Correlation
coefficients between
experimental data and
simulations (mean and
standard error)
A Computational Index to Describe Slacking During Robot Therapy 361
Table 3 Two-way anova Source df <data;EF ð0Þ control <data;EF ð0Þ stroke
with subject as a random 25 N versus 5 N max(Fa) versus
factor among force intensity min(Fa) at 10th
and directions
F(1, df) p F(1, df) p
Force 1 16.12 0.0001 6.83 0.0093
Direction 6 7.55 <0.001 0.65 0.692
Force Direction 6 4.2 0.0005 0.49 0.819
Discussion
In this work, we tested the hypothesis that during robot-mediated therapy stroke
survivors alter their motor plan as they increase movement proficiency. The
hypothesis encompassed the possibility for the subject to use the trajectory gen-
erated by the assistive force on the passive mechanics (inertia, rigidity, and vis-
cosity) as reference.
362 D. Piovesan
We developed a method to account for the limb rigidity and viscosity, based on a
series of inverse dynamics simulations. While the simulations returned a result that
was physiologically plausible, we acknowledge that for multi-degrees of freedom
model the rigidity matrix Rh might not be symmetric. The asymmetry could have an
effect on the final trajectory. We also observed some biomechanical constrains that
can cause the subject to use different strategies when interacting with an assistive
force in Eq. (1). The endpoint velocity of a right handed subject is higher when
moving from the contralateral to the ipsilateral side (left to right), even though the
external force and the movement distance are the same. This observation points out
the possibility to chose different strategies when moving in different directions. One
of the reasons for a subject to use a force generated trajectory lies on the out coming
velocity profiles, which depends on the value of rigidity and viscosity. With a
coefficient of rigidity so that the hand can reach the target, the peak velocity is
larger than the experimental data. The introduction of stiffness and damping also
increases the velocity profile. This aspect might induce the subject to favor the EF
trajectory.
Finally, we confirmed that knowing the real stiffness of the hand is important to
properly model the movements of an impaired individual. While the motor planning
seems to remain intact, the outcome result is strongly influenced by altered stiffness
and damping values.
References
Wiegner AW, Watts RL (1986) Elastic properties of muscles measured at the elbow in man: I.
Normal controls. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 49:1171–1176
Wolbrecht ET, Chan V, Reinkensmeyer DJ, Bobrow JE (2008) Optimizing compliant,
model-based robotic assistance to promote neurorehabilitation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst
Rehabil Eng 16:286–297. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2008.918389
Zatsiorsky V, Seluyanov V (1983) The mass and inertia characteristics of the main segments of the
human body 30. In: HaKK Matsui (ed) International congress of biomechanics: biomechanics
VIII-B. Human Kinetics, Champaign, pp 1152–1159
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface
That Mimics Natural Cortical Activity
Abstract The dramatic advances in efferent neural interfaces over the past decade
are remarkable, with cortical signals used to allow paralyzed patients to control the
movement of a prosthetic limb or even their own hand. However, this success has
thrown into relief, the relative lack of progress in our ability to restore
somatosensation to these same patients. Somatosensation, including proprioception,
the sense of limb position and movement, plays a crucial role in even basic motor
tasks like reaching and walking. Its loss results in crippling deficits. Historical work
dating back decades and even centuries has demonstrated that modality-specific
sensations can be elicited by activating the central nervous system electrically.
Recent work has focused on the challenge of refining these sensations by stimu-
lating the somatosensory cortex (S1) directly. Animals are able to detect particular
patterns of stimulation and even associate those patterns with particular sensory
cues. Most of this work has involved areas of the somatosensory cortex that mediate
the sense of touch. Very little corresponding work has been done for propriocep-
tion. Here we describe the effort to develop afferent neural interfaces through
spatiotemporally precise intracortical microstimulation (ICMS). We review what is
known of the cortical representation of proprioception, and describe recent work in
our lab that demonstrates for the first time, that sensations like those of natural
proprioception may be evoked by ICMS in S1. These preliminary findings are an
important first step to the development of an afferent cortical interface to restore
proprioception.
Keywords Somatosensation
Intracortical microstimulation (ICMS)
Somatosensory cortex Prosthesis
The 15 years since brain–computer interfaces (BCIs) became a reality have seen a
host of technical improvements that have moved the field from two- or
three-dimensional control of a cursor to multidimensional control of a robotic limb
(Hochberg et al. 2006; Collinger et al. 2013), to the controlled activation of multiple
paralyzed muscles (Ethier et al. 2012). However, in nearly all BCIs, the feedback
guiding these movements is exclusively visual. The only exceptions we are aware
of is a study that used passive movements of a monkeys limb to supply proprio-
ceptive feedback (Suminski et al. 2010), and a recent study in which stimulation in
S1 of monkeys encoded the vector from hand to target, allowing the monkey’s to
guide reaching in the absence of other cues (Dadarlat et al. 2015). Researchers are
increasingly recognizing that supplying somatosensory input directly to the user’s
brain is as important, and arguably more difficult a problem, than is extracting
motor command information from the brain (Weber et al. 2012; Bensmaia and
Miller 2014). Most of the experimental work within this area has focused on the
sense of touch in monkeys (Romo et al. 1998; Fitzsimmons et al. 2007; Tabot et al.
2013) or whisking in rodents (Venkatraman and Carmena 2011). In contrast very
little progress has been made in reproducing proprioception to provide kinesthetic
feedback.
There are multiple points at which a somatosensory afferent neural interface
might be implemented. For patients with intact CNS, such as amputees, the nerve
stump and dorsal roots are possible implant sites. One advantage of a peripheral
nerve interface is the further processing of these signals as they propagate centrally.
The relative simplicity of the information carried by peripheral afferent fibers might
be considered another advantage. Since both muscle spindles and Golgi tendon
organs (GTOs) have been accurately modeled (Mileusnic et al. 2006; Mileusnic and
Loeb 2006), in principle, their firing rates can be mimicked. However, mimicking
these inputs is complicated by the unknown state of the descending gamma system
that normally serves to alter both the static sensitivity of spindles to muscle length,
and their dynamic sensitivity to length changes (Eldred et al. 1953; Lennerstrand
1968). Inferring muscle lengths and forces based on measured limb state would also
be a substantial challenge.
Central interfaces on the other hand, present the obvious advantage of applying
not only to persons with limb amputation, but also those with spinal cord injury, for
whom peripheral interface sites are of no use. Furthermore, neurons in S1 integrate
multimodal afferent signals to form a higher-level representation of limb state.
Neurons within area 2 of the primary somatosensory cortex (S1) appear to signal
the direction of hand movement, whether those movements are active reaching
movements, or perturbations of the hand (Soso and Fetz 1980; Prud’homme and
Kalaska 1994; London and Miller 2013). Cortical interfaces have the potential to
tap into this higher-level kinematic representation. However, these same neurons
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 369
Sherrington first used the term proprioception to define the sense of body position
(Sherrington 1906). Proprioception is now commonly defined as the sensory
information that contributes to a sense of joint position and motion. It arises pri-
marily from muscle spindles and GTOs that respond to muscle length and force,
respectively. However, joint receptors and cutaneous afferents responding to skin
stretch also likely contribute (Burke et al. 1988; Macefield et al. 1990; Weerakkody
et al. 2009). Sir Charles Bell called proprioception the “sixth sense” (Bell and Shaw
1865), as it is often overlooked compared to the five main senses of which we are
more consciously aware.
Beyond its somewhat limited role in our conscious sense of limb position and
motion, proprioception plays a critical subconscious role in the planning and
control of limb movement. Chronic loss of proprioception produces cortical
remapping (Sanes et al. 1988) and profound changes in motor ability (Sanes et al.
1984; Sainburg et al. 1993; Gordon et al. 1995). Patients suffering chronic loss of
proprioception due to large fiber neuropathy make looping, discoordinated reaching
movements (Fig. 1). While these patients can compensate to a certain extent
through vision, this compensation is effortful, and normal ease of motion is never
restored. Claude Ghez and his colleagues showed that these deficits arise from a
loss of coordination between musculature at the elbow and shoulder that prevents
these patients from compensating for the intersegmental inertial dynamics of the
limb (Sainburg et al. 1993). They hypothesized that this compensation relies on a
model of limb dynamics, the accuracy of which depends on proprioceptive feed-
back. Interestingly, in some respects the movements of robotic limbs controlled by
current BCIs look similar to those of these deafferented patients. This similarity
suggests that some of the remaining functional inadequacy of BCIs may result from
the lack of somatosensory feedback, including both touch and proprioception.
370 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
Fig. 1 Reaching trajectories in normal subjects and subjects with peripheral deafferentation. Plots
show the trajectories of a series of rapid out and back reaches to targets at several angles. Control
subjects (left) were able to direct their movements accurately and make rapid reversals of direction,
while deafferented subjects made errors in the initial reach direction and large looping motions
(Adapted from Sainburg et al. 1995, Fig. 3)
sinusoidally with the direction of hand motion and increase with the speed of
motion, much like the similar behavior of neurons in M1 (Prud’homme and Kalaska
1994; London and Miller 2013). It is thus useful to represent a given neuron with a
“preferred direction” (PD). These neurons typically discharge during both active
and passive movement, the relative magnitude of the two components varying from
neuron to neuron (Soso and Fetz 1980; Prud’homme and Kalaska 1994; London
and Miller 2013). Neurons that respond strongly during both active and passive
motions typically have PDs for the two types of movement that are well aligned
(London and Miller 2013), thus simplifying the representation of limb movement,
and the requisite mapping from limb state to patterns of ICMS.
On the other hand, in addition to the phasic, movement-related discharge, many
proprioceptive neurons also have a tonic component that varies approximately with
the instantaneous position of the hand. This combined movement and postural
tuning also occurs in varying proportions across neurons (Gardner and Costanzo
1981; Prud’homme and Kalaska 1994; London and Miller 2013). The directionality
of the postural tuning appears to be similar to the movement-related PDs
(Prud’homme and Kalaska 1994). Restoring a full sense of proprioception will
presumably require supplying both these components.
Beyond the single neuron studies described above, we have recently used
chronically implanted arrays to combine information from many simultaneously
recorded neurons in order to reconstruct detailed information about kinematic limb
state. Figure 2a is a scatter plot of the mean firing rate for two of 41 area 2 neurons
measured in a 300 ms window following limb perturbations in four different
directions (indicated by the symbol color). The structure in this plot is caused by the
differing directional sensitivity of these two neurons. Although it is impossible to
depict the corresponding 41-dimensional plot for the entire 41-neuron sample
population directly, there are methods to reduce the dimensionality of this “neural
space” to a much smaller number. Figure 2b shows the results of a factor analysis,
which can be thought of as the particular projection of these 41-dimensional points
onto a plane that retains as much of the original information as possible. Not only
were points corresponding to a particular target tightly clustered in this 2-D neural
space, the geometry of the two spaces was approximately isomorphic. This suggests
that the neurons in area 2 carry information that conveys, approximately linearly,
the position of the hand following perturbations in different directions.
The dynamics of limb movements can also be captured accurately using the
“decoder” methods that have much more commonly been applied to M1 neurons
(Chapin et al. 1999; Serruya et al. 2002; Vargas-Irwin et al. 2010; Wodlinger et al.
2015). Trajectories during planar, random-target reaching movements were pre-
dicted from area 2 neurons with mean R2 between measured and predicted signals
of 0.59 for position and 0.66 for velocity (Weber et al. 2011). Acceleration was
predicted more poorly with R2 = 0.44. In that study, a neuron dropping analysis
revealed that these predictions were as accurate for neurons that had primarily
cutaneous receptive fields as those with more muscle-like responses. This coun-
terintuitive result is nonetheless consistent with earlier observations of the reliable
modulation of area 1 and 3b neurons during center-out reaching movements (Cohen
372 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
Fig. 2 Neural discharge related to limb movements imposed by force perturbations on the
monkey’s hand in different directions. a Each circle represents the mean firing rate of two (of 41)
neurons during a 300 ms window following a limb perturbation. Perturbation directions are coded
by color, as shown in the inset diagram. Some separation of the neuronal response by reaching
target is evident, but there is considerable overlap for the four reach directions. b Factor analysis
applied to the full 41-dimensional space of concurrently measured firing rates. The perturbation
directions are clearly separated in this 2D factor space, which has a geometry very much like that
of the perturbation directions
et al. 1994), the discharge of cutaneous peripheral afferents during rotations of the
knee (Edin 2001), and of the perception of externally imposed finger movements in
the presence of local anesthesia (Edin and Johansson 1995).
In addition to purely kinematic responses, S1 neurons also modulate during
isometric force production with directionally tuned firing rates (Jennings et al.
1983; Prud’homme and Kalaska 1994; London et al. 2011). During reaching while
compensating for loads in different directions, firing rates in S1 are well fit by
sinusoidal functions of force direction as well as movement (Fig. 3). Under these
conditions, the force PDs (“load axis” in that study) tend to be roughly anti-aligned
with the kinematic PDs. Although the depth of modulation during movement and
isometric tasks co-varies significantly across neurons (Jennings et al. 1983; London
et al. 2011), neurons with strong tonic kinematic tuning to position tend to have
stronger force representation (Prud’homme and Kalaska 1994). This differential
sensitivity to force might account for some of the differing responses of S1 neurons
during kinematically similar active and passive movements. (Soso and Fetz 1980;
Prud’homme and Kalaska 1994; London and Miller 2013) These observations raise
important questions about how the CNS resolves these differences to evoke per-
ceptually similar kinesthetic sensations during active and passive movements. It
also raises questions of the impact this sensitivity to muscle activity might have on
attempts to elicit kinesthetic sensations by intracortical microstimulation (ICMS).
While there have been quite a number of studies that have attempted to distin-
guish between different kinematic coordinate representations within M1 (Evarts
1969; Caminiti et al. 1991; Scott and Kalaska 1995; Kakei et al. 1999; Morrow
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 373
Fig. 3 Representation of the combined effect of reach direction and external loads on the activity
of proprioceptive neurons in S1. The plotted surface represents the response of 93 neurons,
averaged after alignment to the movement and force PDs. The marginal curves represent the
neural responses to unloaded reaching and to isometric force (Adapted from Prud’homme and
Kalaska 1994, Fig. 11A)
et al. 2007; Oby et al. 2013), the analogous studies have not been done in S1.
Existing studies have largely assumed a hand-centered, Cartesian representation
and simply described the resulting mappings. It remains possible that neurons in 3a
or 2 might actually be better described in terms of muscle lengths. Alternately,
given the multimodal nature of the receptive fields in Area 2, it may be that a
transformation occurs between the representations of kinematics in these two areas.
Only with an accurate model of the relation between limb state and the cortical
activity in S1, can we hope to recreate naturalistic patterns of cortical activity
through electrical stimulation.
Similar uncertainty exists for the force representation in S1. Although contact
forces applied to the hand and digits are represented in cutaneous areas 3b and 1
(Sinclair and Burton 1991; Tremblay et al. 1996), it seems very unlikely that they
would also be represented in the proximal limb regions of areas 3a and 2. Instead,
the endpoint force-related tuning in these areas seems more likely to represent
muscle forces. Under conditions in which endpoint force is well correlated with
muscle force (for example, isometric contractions) it can be predicted quite accu-
rately from the activity of multiple area 2 neurons (London et al. 2011). Even
during reaching movements against purely inertial loads, predictions remain rea-
sonably accurate. However, when additional random forces are added to dissociate
374 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
the direction of endpoint motion and endpoint force, the predictions become much
worse, suggesting the modulation of area 2 neurons is more directly related to
muscle forces, rather than the measured endpoint force.
Anyone who has placed their tongue on the poles of a 9-V battery knows something
of the odd sensation that can be evoked when the nervous system is activated
electrically. In the late eighteenth century, Alessandro Volta is credited with the first
electrical activation of the auditory system when he inserted two metal rods into his
ears and connected them with a battery. He described a very unpleasant “boom”
within his head. Substantially refined two centuries later, the cochlear implant has
been used to restore hearing to 300,000 deaf patients worldwide as of 2012 (Yawn
et al. 2015).
ICMS has been shown to evoke detectable percepts from a variety of cortical
areas, including human visual cortex (Bak et al. 1990; Schmidt et al. 1996), rat
(Otto et al. 2005a; Koivuniemi and Otto 2012) and cat (Wang et al. 2012) auditory
cortex, rat barrel fields (Talwar et al. 2002; Houweling and Brecht 2008;
Venkatraman and Carmena 2011; Bari et al. 2013; Thomson et al. 2013), and
monkey somatosensory cortex (O’Doherty et al. 2009; Zaaimi et al. 2013; Dadarlat
et al. 2015; Kim et al. 2015b). There is currently tremendous interest in using this
stimulation to replace natural sensation when it has been lost.
The earliest application of direct cortical stimulation to induce sensation was
probably in the primary visual cortex, resulting in the perception of small dots of
light dubbed “phosphenes.” Phosphenes were first produced experimentally in the
late 1920s by neurosurgeons in Germany, Lowenstein and Borchardt (Löwenstein
and Borchardt 1918), Foerster (1929) and Fedor Krause and Heinrich Schum
(Krause and Schum 1932), who described localized sensations of light, the position
of which depended on the location of the stimulus near the occipital pole (Lewis
and Rosenfeld 2016). The possibility that this electrically elicited sensation might
be exploited to provide a form of artificial vision was pursued independently
40 years later in England by Giles Brindley at Cambridge University (Brindley and
Lewin 1968) and in the Untied States at the University of Utah by William Dobelle
(Dobelle and Mladejovsky 1974). These experiments used approximately
1 mm-sized electrodes placed on the surface of the visual cortex, and required
currents as large as 10 mA to evoke the visual effects (Fig. 4a, b).
Artificial vision was pursued further through the neuroprosthesis program at the
NIH, using penetrating intracortical electrodes. Experiments included intraoperative
stimulation of three normally sighted patients undergoing surgical treatment of
epilepsy (Bak et al. 1990) and chronic stimulation over a period of four months in a
patient who had been blind for 22 years due to glaucoma (Schmidt et al. 1996).
Phosphenes were produced in all these patients with threshold currents ranging
from 1 to 10 μA, much lower than those necessary for the surface stimulation of
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 375
Fig. 4 Cortical interfaces used to restore sensation. a Radiograph of electrodes implanted on the
surface of V1 in a blind human patient (Figure a adapted from Dobelle et al. 1976, Fig. 1). b Map
of the location of phosphenes generated by the electrodes in (a). (Figure b adapted from Dobelle
et al. 1974, Fig. 3). c, d Stimulus trains delivered by penetrating electrodes in S1 allow monkeys to
discriminate frequency as they would with vibratory stimulation of the fingers. c Frequency
discrimination task using either mechanical vibration of the fingertip or ICMS in tactile area 3b.
The monkey was presented a baseline mechanical stimulus of fixed frequency and a subsequent
comparison stimulus and asked to report whether the latter was higher or lower frequency.
d Psychophysical responses for mechanical (filled) and ICMS (open) comparisons, which were not
significantly different (Figures c and d adapted from Romo et al. 1998, Figs. 1 and 2). e Color
coded receptive field locations of recordings made by a Utah array implanted in area 2.
f Psychometric performance comparing the relative intensity of two sequential indentations of the
skin on the finger (blue curves) or two sequential ICMS trains applied to the corresponding
electrode (red curves). As in the frequency discrimination task, these curves were statistically
indistinguishable. The ICMS current was computed from indentation amplitude using an
empirically determined mapping function between the natural and artificial percepts (Figures e and
f adapted from Tabot et al. 2013, Figs. 1 and 3)
Brindley and Dobelle. Further work led to development of two different types of
high-density electrode arrays made from silicon, one effort led by Ken Wise at the
University of Michigan (Hoogerwerf and Wise 1994) and another by Richard
Normann at the University of Utah (Jones et al. 1992). The resultant “Michigan”
and “Utah” electrode arrays, currently available from NeuroNexus and Blackrock
microsystems, respectively, have undergone considerable further development, and
continue to be used in a wide range of recording and stimulation studies.
Stimulus intensity can be graded by frequency, pulse width, or current and used
to modulate the reliability with which the subject can detect the stimulus (Fridman
et al. 2010). The increase in detection rate with current intensity (pulse amplitude or
duration) results from the activation of a larger tissue volume, whereas increases in
frequency presumably lead to higher firing rates in recruited neurons. Studies report
detection thresholds between 2 and 40 µA in sensory areas ranging from visual
areas in humans (Bak et al. 1990; Schmidt et al. 1996) auditory areas in rat and cat
(Koivuniemi and Otto 2012; Wang et al. 2012) and somatosensory areas in rats
376 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
(Houweling and Brecht 2008; Bari et al. 2013) and monkeys (Tabot et al. 2013;
Zaaimi et al. 2013; Kim et al. 2015a). There is typically a gradual increase in the
detection rate with increased current. This monotonic relation between stimulus and
perceptual intensity provides a basis for modulating the stimulation to achieve
percepts of different magnitudes.
Kevin Otto and colleagues trained rats to discriminate between ICMS delivered
on different electrodes in auditory cortex. With 68 µA stimuli, rats were unable to
distinguish electrodes with less than 750 µm separation (Otto et al. 2005b). The
effective electrode density is thus coupled to the maximum current, potentially
limiting the variety of distinct sensations that may be delivered. Further, some
researchers have reported loss of efficacy at high stimulus currents. In V1 phos-
phenes lost color and shrank in size as currents were increased (Schmidt et al.
1996). These investigators also noted that high current on a single electrode
occasionally produced multiple phosphenes. Similarly, in experiments stimulating
area MT, effects on perceived visual motion were lost above 80 µA (Murasugi et al.
1993). These effects may reflect a saturation in the recruitment of neurons near the
electrode combined with the recruitment of a more distant, and likely less func-
tionally homogeneous populations. Combined, these findings set a limit on the
maximum currents that may be used for increasing the strength of artificial percepts.
To add to the complexity, electrical stimulation activates not only the soma of
nearby neurons but also axons passing near the electrode. As a consequence, the
evoked activity is not focused exclusively near the electrode, but instead includes
sparse activation of neurons within a several millimeter radius (Histed et al. 2009).
This suggests that the problem of activating a functionally homogeneous population
of neurons may be more severe than that suggested by a simple model of current
spread.
An adjunct method for increasing the robustness of the evoked sensation may be
to stimulate multiple electrodes. In stimulus detection studies, detection threshold
current at each electrode within a group decreased modestly with the use of two or
four electrodes in tactile areas of S1 (Kim et al. 2015b), and more dramatically in a
separate study of area 2, (Zaaimi et al. 2013). In the latter study the effects were
particularly large, even supralinear, with five and seven electrodes. Distributing
stimulation across many electrodes and reducing the current at each one has the
twin advantages of potentially increasing the functional homogeneity of the acti-
vated neurons within a smaller volume (Weber et al. 2011), and reducing the
potential for damage to the surrounding tissue or electrodes. A recent study found
no histologic damage or functional deficits following six months of stimulation on
multiple electrodes with currents up to 100 µA (Rajan et al. 2015). These results
suggest that easily detected currents are well below those expected to cause dam-
age, but it is difficult to extrapolate to the cumulative effect of decades of stimu-
lation that would be required from a useful prosthesis.
Beyond demonstrating the ability to detect stimulation, a number of animal
studies have shown that ICMS can also be used to provide salient information.
Temporally patterned stimulation in rat auditory cortex-enabled rats to perform a
frequency discrimination task in which ICMS replaced an auditory cue (Otto et al.
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 377
2005b). In somatosensory cortex, ICMS has been used to provide salient infor-
mation in a number of different tasks, allowing animals to distinguish between
patterns of stimulation in cortex and to compare the percepts due to ICMS and
natural stimuli. In an early study, cortical barrel field stimulation was used to guide
a rat’s open field locomotion, stimulation in right or left S1 signaling the corre-
sponding rewarded turn (Talwar et al. 2002). Another study showed that rats per-
forming an object detection task were able to locate virtual objects on the basis of
the timing of ICMS delivered to barrel as the whisker passed through the virtual
object (Venkatraman and Carmena 2011). The rats explored the virtual object by
whisking back and forth, much as they would against an actual object, suggesting
that the sensation produced by ICMS was qualitatively similar to that driven by
deflection of the whiskers.
In experiments with monkeys, stimulus frequency within proprioceptive area 3a
provided cues that allowed monkeys to distinguish between rewarded and unre-
warded targets in a simple cued reaching task (London et al. 2008). The group of
Miguel Nicolelis has performed more elaborate experiments in which monkeys
used brain control to move an avatar hand among multiple virtual targets
(O’Doherty et al. 2009). Target contact triggered ICMS within area 1. Two targets
were associated with different temporal patterns of stimulation referred to as “vir-
tual textures.” Monkeys were able to explore the targets and learn the pattern
associated with the rewarded target.
In contrast to the discrimination experiments described above, in at least two
other studies, animals learned to interpret ICMS that provided continuous feedback
during motion. In one case, ICMS was delivered to barrel cortex of rats and
modulated in intensity according to the rat’s distance from an infrared light above a
water port. ICMS intensity increased as rats drew closer to the port, allowing them
to locate it through this artificial somatosensory percept (Thomson et al. 2013).
Having learned the basic task with a visible light cue, the rats learned to use the
IR-driven ICMS cue in about a month. In another experiment, monkeys learned to
reach to invisible targets guided by feedback that represented an error vector from
the current hand position to the unseen target (Dadarlat et al. 2015). Feedback was
provided either by a random moving-dot flow field or the strength of somatosensory
ICMS delivered across eight electrodes. The frequency of simulation was deter-
mined by the projection of the error vector onto eight evenly spaced unit vectors
assigned arbitrarily to the eight electrodes. After 3–4 months of paired visual and
ICMS feedback, the monkeys learned to use ICMS to guide reaches in the absence
of the visual information.
Work in patients with limb amputation has shown that physiologically appropriate
cutaneous feedback improves the patient’s embodiment of the prosthesis (Cincotti
et al. 2007; Marasco et al. 2011). Likewise, it is reasonable to suspect that learning
378 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
to use an ICMS interface that mimics natural sensations would be faster, and
ultimately perhaps more effective than learning arbitrary associations with unnatural
sensations or arbitrarily modulated ICMS (Bensmaia and Miller 2014). However,
the extent to which artificially evoked activity must mimic that of the natural
afferent inputs in order to be useful remains a critical question.
Ranulfo Romo performed a now classic series of experiments in the
somatosensory system, in which monkeys identified which of two sequential
mechanical stimuli applied to the fingertip had a higher frequency (Romo et al.
1998). The monkeys were able to complete the task when one or both of the
mechanical stimuli was replaced with an ICMS train of the same frequency in the
tactile area 3b (Fig. 4c, d). This experiment was the first to demonstrate that ICMS
in somatosensory areas can evoke percepts with some naturalistic components.
Similar experiments have been done more recently using punctate pressure stimuli
(Tabot et al. 2013). The monkeys in that experiment were able to report both the
location and magnitude of effects evoked by area 1 or 3b stimulation, and to
compare them to their mechanical analogs (Fig. 4e, f). ICMS with higher currents
led to a sensation of higher pressure, allowing construction of mapping functions
between the desired perceptual effects and ICMS. These maps, and the receptive
field locations characteristic of each electrode, were ultimately used to generate
discriminable sensations through ICMS based on forces applied to sensors on a
prosthetic limb, demonstrating a plausible method to provide cutaneous feedback to
a patient. These effects rely on the somatotopy of S1, and the fact that neighboring
neurons in both areas tend to have largely overlapping receptive fields.
Extrapolaind from these studies, a reasonable approach to reproducing a sense of
limb movement in a particular direction would be to electrically activate the neu-
rons that naturally signal movement in that direction. Figure 5 suggests the design
of such an interface, using an array of electrodes that activate groups of neurons
with identified PDs. By activating neurons in an appropriate spatiotemporal
sequence, it may be possible to reproduce a sensation of the natural movement that
elicits the same sequence of neural activity.
A potential challenge to using the natural behavior of neurons to design bio-
mimetic interfaces is that the sensations resulting from ICMS do not always cor-
respond to those normally signaled by the activated neurons. Stimulation of V1, for
instance, does not produce the perceptual experience that might be anticipated
based on the physiological characteristics of single neurons recorded in V1. Rather
than the oriented bars that might be anticipated based on the response properties of
complex cells (Hubel and Wiesel 1962), ICMS gives rise primarily to the sensation
of fixed spots of colored light. Perhaps this is the consequence of the combined
activation of many cells with different orientation selectivity. On the other hand,
stimulation of the higher visual areas MT and MST can, in some conditions, elicit a
sensation of visual motion that is consistent with the visual motion sensitivity of the
neighboring neurons (Salzman et al. 1992; Celebrini and Newsome 1995). These
experiments combined ICMS with the moving-dot stimuli in manner that biased the
monkey’s judgment of the actual dot motion toward the directional preference of
the electrically activated neurons. This effect was present only with currents well
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 379
The strong representation of hand velocity by neurons in area 2 and the similarity
between these neurons and the motion sensitivity of neurons in MT/MST suggests a
useful experimental paradigm that we have begun to explore, examining the
hypothesis that stimulating in area 2 will alter the monkey’s perception of hand
motion. More specifically, just as ICMS in MT and MST biases perception of
motion, ICMS in S1 should increase the activity in the neurons near the stimulated
electrodes, biasing the monkey’s perceived hand motion toward the PD of the
stimulated neurons.
380 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
We trained a monkey to perform two reaching tasks, while grasping the handle
of a planar robotic manipulandum. Movement of the handle controlled the position
of a cursor on a screen and servomotors could be used to apply forces to the handle.
The first task required the monkey to reach to a sequence of randomly positioned
targets in order to generate movements with different speeds and in many direc-
tions. We recorded multi-unit activity and used these data to compute preferred
directions for each electrode. We used multi-unit, rather than discriminated single
units, to better represent the set of neurons that would be activated by ICMS.
The monkey also learned a two alternative forced choice task designed to test his
ability to discriminate the direction of force pulses applied to the handle (Fig. 6).
After moving the cursor to a central target, an additional pair of targets appeared,
located symmetrically on a line passing through the central target. After a random
hold period, the robot applied a 500 ms force pulse that displaced the monkey’s
hand a few centimeters. The monkey then reached to the outer target that was closer
to the direction of the force pulse. The monkey was rewarded for a correct choice
and given an audible error signal if the reach was to the wrong target.
To evaluate the monkey’s performance in the direction identification task, we
computed the proportion of trials in which he chose one of the targets and fit these
responses as a sigmoidal function of the perturbation direction (black curve in
Fig. 7a). The monkey could judge perturbations near either target quite accurately.
Between these extremes, the psychometric function passed through the point of
subjective equality (PSE), the angle for which the monkey perceived the pertur-
bation to be intermediate between the two targets. In this example, PSE fell very
close to 90° in the coordinate system aligned to the target axis.
Fig. 6 Direction
identification task. The
monkey held its hand in a
central location (green circle)
while a physical perturbation
(black arrow) was applied to
the hand. The monkey was
required to reach to the red
target that was nearer to the
pertubation direction (blue
arrow). If the monkey
reached to the correct target
he received a liquid reward.
This task increased in
difficulty as the angle of the
perturbation (θ) approached
90°, midway between the two
targets
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 381
Fig. 7 Biased perception of perturbation direction resulting from concurrent ICMS in area 2.
a Psychometric curves showing the monkey’s performance on the direction identification task of
Fig. 6. Perturbation direction is relative to the target axis, where 0° corresponds to the PD of the
stimulated electrodes. In this example, data were collected over seven days, with the PD target at
211° in absolute coordinates. Black circles and curve indicate the proportion of the monkey’s
choice of the PD (0°) target as a function of the direction of the mechanical perturbation. Red
curves indicate the monkey’s responses with progressively increasing stimulus currents.
Stimulation caused more frequent reaches to the PD target, shifting the psychometric curve up
and to the right. b Summary of the PD groupings of the seven electrode sets tested during these
experiments. Filled circles around the perimeter indicate sets for which stimulation biased the
monkey’s movements toward the corresponding PD target. A single set of electrodes (open cyan
circle) failed to produce the predicted result
On any given ICMS session, we selected four electrodes that had similar PDs
and aligned the target axis to their mean PD. Thus one target was in the direction of
the PD and the second in the anti-PD direction. We delivered ICMS to all four
electrodes concurrently with the perturbation, using currents of 5, 10, 15, or 20 μA.
The sensation caused by the ICMS appeared to combine with the natural sensation
of the force pulse, producing a biased perception of the force direction (red curves
and symbols in Fig. 6a). The psychometric curves shifted up and to the right,
introducing a large bias toward the zero-degree PD target for 90° perturbations, and
shifting the monkey’s PSE toward the PD. Furthermore, the bias was graded with
current. 5 µA stimulation had no significant effect, while 20 µA was strong enough
that the monkey was never fully confident in selecting the anti-PD target, even
when the perturbation was aimed directly toward it.
In a series of experiments over a period of 18 weeks we tested seven different
groups of electrodes (Fig. 7b). Six of these resulted in an effect essentially like that
shown in Fig. 7a, with the bias direction congruent with the PD of the stimulated
electrodes. The seventh set of electrodes produced no detectable effect. Subsequent
analysis showed that these electrodes had larger PD variability over time than the
other sets, suggesting that the failure to induce a bias may have been the result of
instability across the several days of testing.
It is important to note that the ICMS-induced bias was not the result of training,
as was thye case in several other studies. We rewarded the monkey based on
382 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
accurate discrimination of the physical perturbation, rather than any feature of the
stimulation. Since the monkey’s performance was roughly optimal prior to stimu-
lation, the bias had the effect of increasing the monkey’s error rate, thereby low-
ering the amount of reward. This is the opposite of studies that train monkeys to
associate arbitrary ICMS patterns with specific actions in order to receive rewards.
The fact that reward rate decreased, suggests that the monkey was unable to learn to
ignore the sensation due to ICMS, as doing so would have allowed him to return to
the baseline level of reward. These observations suggest that the ICMS generated
sensations that were sufficiently naturalistic that they combined predictably with the
mechanical stimuli.
Future Directions
Only recently, the focus of neural interfaces turned to the critical need to restore
somatosensation, which is lost along with the ability to execute movement, as a
consequence of SCI or limb amputation. Most efforts to restore somatosensation at
the cortical level have relied on learned associations between arbitrary stimulation
patterns and either reward (O’Doherty et al. 2011; Venkatraman and Carmena
2011; Thomson et al. 2013) or other feedback modalities (London et al. 2008;
Dadarlat et al. 2015). In a few studies attempting to restore cutaneous sensation,
ICMS has been shown to reproduce naturalistic sensations (Romo et al. 1998; Tabot
et al. 2013). However, no such evidence has previously been presented for pro-
prioception. By delivering ICMS coincidently with force pulses applied to a
monkey’s hand, we biased the monkey’s perception of the direction of the resulting
movement. We suggest that the combination of ICMS-driven cortical activity and
that due to the actual perturbation, altered the monkey’s perception in a predictable
manner. The biases shown in these experiments required no learning, and appear to
reflect naturalistic sensations of arm movement. These preliminary findings are a
first step toward the development of an afferent cortical interface to restore
proprioception.
References
Bak M, Girvin JP, Hambrecht FT, Kufta CV, Loeb GE, Schmidt EM (1990) Visual sensations
produced by intracortical microstimulation of the human occipital cortex. Med Biol Eng
Comput 28:257–259
Bari BA, Ollerenshaw DR, Millard DC, Wang Q, Stanley GB (2013) Behavioral and
electrophysiological effects of cortical microstimulation parameters. PLoS ONE 8:e82170.
doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0082170
Bell C, Shaw A (1865) The hand: its mechanism and vital endowments, as evincing design. Bell &
Daldy, London
Bensmaia SJ, Miller LE (2014) Restoring sensorimotor function through intracortical interfaces:
progress and looming challenges. Nat Rev Neurosci 15:313–325. doi:10.1038/nrn3724
Brindley GS, Lewin WS (1968) The sensations produced by electrical stimulation of the visual
cortex. J Physiol 196:479–493
Burke D, Gandevia S, Macefield G (1988) Responses to passive movement of receptors in joint,
skin and muscle of the human hand. J Physiol 402:347–361
Caminiti R, Johnson PB, Galli C, Ferraina S, Burnod Y (1991) Making arm movements within
different parts of space: the premotor and motor cortical representation of a coordinate system
for reaching to visual targets. J Neurosci 11:1182–1197
Celebrini S, Newsome W (1995) Microstimulation of extrastriate area MST influences
performance on a direction discrimination task. J Neurophysiol 73:437–448
Chapin JK, Moxon KA, Markowitz RS, Nicolelis MA (1999) Real-time control of a robot arm
using simultaneously recorded neurons in the motor cortex. Nat Neurosci 2:664–670.
doi:10.1038/10223
Cincotti F, Kauhanen L, Aloise F et al (2007) Vibrotactile feedback for brain-computer interface
operation. Comput Intell Neurosci
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 385
Cohen DAD, Prud’homme MJL, Kalaska JF (1994) Tactile activity in primate primary
somatosensory cortex during active arm movements: correlation with receptive field properties.
J Neurophysiol 71:161–172
Collinger JL, Wodlinger B, Downey JE et al (2013) High-performance neuroprosthetic control by
an individual with tetraplegia. Lancet 381:557–564. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)61816-9
Collins DF, Refshauge KM, Todd G, Gandevia SC (2005) Cutaneous receptors contribute to
kinesthesia at the index finger, elbow, and knee. J Neurophysiol 94:1699–1706. doi:10.1152/
jn.00191.2005
Dadarlat MC, O’Doherty JE, Sabes PN (2015) A learning-based approach to artificial sensory
feedback leads to optimal integration. Nat Neurosci 18:138–144. doi: 10.1038/nn.3883. http://
www.nature.com/neuro/journal/v18/n1/abs/nn.3883.html (supplementary-information)
Diester I, Kaufman MT, Mogri M et al (2011) An optogenetic toolbox designed for primates. Nat
Neurosci. doi:10.1038/nn.2749 nn.2749 [pii]
Dobelle WH, Mladejovsky MG (1974) Phosphenes produced by electrical stimulation of human
occipital cortex, and their application to the development of a prosthesis for the blind. J Physiol
243:553–576
Dobelle WH, Mladejovsky MG, Girvin JP (1974) Artificial vision for the blind: electrical
stimulation of visual cortex offers hope for a functional prosthesis. Science 183:440–444
Dobelle WH, Mladejovsky MG, Evans JR, Roberts TS, Girvin JP (1976) “Braille” reading by a
blind volunteer by visual cortex stimulation. Nature 259:111–112
Edin BB (2001) Cutaneous afferents provide information about knee joint movements in humans.
J Physiol 531:289–297. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7793.2001.0289j.x
Edin BB, Johansson N (1995) Skin strain patterns provide kinaesthetic information to the human
central nervous system. J Physiol 487:243–251. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.1995.sp020875
Eldred E, Granit R, Merton PA (1953) Supraspinal control of the muscle spindles and its
significance. J Physiol 122:498–523
Ethier C, Oby ER, Bauman MJ, Miller LE (2012) Restoration of grasp following paralysis through
brain-controlled stimulation of muscles. Nature 485:368–371. http://www.nature.com/nature/
journal/v485/n7398/abs/nature10987.html (supplementary-information)
Evarts EV (1969) Activity of pyramidal tract neurons during postural fixation. J Neurophysiol
32:375–385
Fenno L, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K (2011) The development and application of optogenetics. Annu
Rev Neurosci 34:389–412
Fitzsimmons NA, Drake W, Hanson TL, Lebedev MA, Nicolelis MA (2007) Primate reaching
cued by multichannel spatiotemporal cortical microstimulation. J Neurosci 27:5593–5602
Foerster O (1929) Beitrage zur pathophysiologie der sehbahn und der spehsphare. J Psychol
Neurol 39:435–463
Fridman G, Blair H, Blaisdell A, Judy J (2010) Perceived intensity of somatosensory cortical
electrical stimulation. Exp Brain Res 203:499–515. doi:10.1007/s00221-010-2254-y
Friedman D, Jones E (1981) Thalamic input to areas 3a and 2 in monkeys. J Neurophysiol 45:
59–85
Gardner EP, Costanzo RM (1981) Properties of kinesthetic neurons in somatosensory cortex of
awake monkeys. Brain Res 214:301–319
Gordon J, Ghilardi MF, Ghez C (1995) Impairments of reaching movements in patients without
proprioception. I. Spatial errors. J Neurophysiol 73:347–360
Griffin DM, Hudson HM, Belhaj-Saïf A, Cheney PD (2011) Hijacking cortical motor output with
repetitive microstimulation. J Neurosci 31:13088–13096. doi:10.1523/jneurosci.6322-10.2011
Histed MH, Bonin V, Reid RC (2009) Direct activation of sparse, distributed populations of
cortical neurons by electrical microstimulation. Neuron 63:508–522. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.
2009.07.016 S0896-6273(09)00545-5 [pii]
Hochberg LR, Serruya MD, Friehs GM et al (2006) Neuronal ensemble control of prosthetic
devices by a human with tetraplegia. Nature 442:164–171. doi:10.1038/nature04970
Hoogerwerf AC, Wise KD (1994) A three-dimensional microelectrode array for chronic neural
recording. IEEE Trans Biomed Eng 41:1136–1146
386 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
Houweling AR, Brecht M (2008) Behavioural report of single neuron stimulation in somatosen-
sory cortex. Nature 451:65–68. doi:10.1038/nature06447 nature06447 [pii]
Hubel DH, Wiesel TN (1962) Receptive fields, binocular interaction and functional architecture in
the cat’s visual cortex. J Physiol 160:106–154
Huffman KJ, Krubitzer L (2001) Area 3a: topographic organization and cortical connections in
marmoset monkeys. Cereb Cortex 11:849–867
Jennings VA, Lamour Y, Solis H, Fromm C (1983) Somatosensory cortex activity related to
position and force. J Neurophysiol 49:1216–1229
Jones KE, Campbell PK, Normann RA (1992) A glass/silicon composite intracortical electrode
array. Ann Biomed Eng 20:423–437
Kakei S, Hoffman DS, Strick PL (1999) Muscle and movement representations in the primary
motor cortex. Science 285:2136–2139
Kim S, Callier T, Tabot GA, Gaunt RA, Tenore FV, Bensmaia SJ (2015a) Behavioral assessment
of sensitivity to intracortical microstimulation of primate somatosensory cortex. Proc Natl
Acad Sci 112:15202–15207. doi:10.1073/pnas.1509265112
Kim S, Callier T, Tabot GA, Tenore FV, Bensmaia SJ (2015b) Sensitivity to microstimulation of
somatosensory cortex distributed over multiple electrodes. Frontiers Syst Neurosci 9:47.
doi:10.3389/fnsys.2015.00047
Koivuniemi AS, Otto KJ (2012) The depth, waveform and pulse rate for electrical microstim-
ulation of the auditory cortex. In: 2012 annual international conference of the IEEE
Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (EMBC), pp 2489–2492
Krause F, Schum H (1932) Die epileptischen Erkrankungen, ihre anatomischen und physiolo-
gischen Unterlagen sowie ihre chirurgische Behandlung. Enke, Stuttgart
Krubitzer L, Huffman KJ, Disbrow E, Recanzone G (2004) Organization of area 3a in macaque
monkeys: contributions to the cortical phenotype. J Comp Neurol 471:97–111
Lennerstrand G (1968) Position and velocity sensitivity of muscle spindles in the cat. I. Primary
and secondary endings deprived of fusimotor activation. Acta Physiol Scand 73:281–299
Lewis PM, Rosenfeld JV (2016) Electrical stimulation of the brain and the development of cortical
visual prostheses: an historical perspective. Brain Res 1630:208–224
London BM, Miller LE (2013) Responses of somatosensory area 2 neurons to actively and
passively generated limb movements. J Neurophysiol 109:1505–1513. doi:10.1152/jn.00372.
2012
London BM, Jordan LR, Jackson CR, Miller LE (2008) Electrical stimulation of the proprioceptive
cortex (area 3a) used to instruct a behaving monkey. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng
16:32–36. doi:10.1109/TNSRE.2007.907544
London BM, Ruiz-Torres R, Slutzky MW, Miller LE (2011) Designing stimulation patterns for an
afferent BMI: Representation of kinetics in somatosensory cortex. In: 2011 annual international
conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society, EMBC, pp 7521–7524
Löwenstein K, Borchardt M (1918) Symptomatologie und elektrische Reizung bei einer
Schußverletzung des Hinterhauptlappens. Deutsche Zeitschrift für Nervenheilkunde 58:
264–292. doi:10.1007/BF01629694
Macefield G, Gandevia SC, Burke D (1990) Perceptual responses to microstimulation of single
afferents innervating joints, muscles and skin of the human hand. J Physiol 429:113–129
Marasco PD, Kim K, Colgate JE, Peshkin MA, Kuiken TA (2011) Robotic touch shifts perception
of embodiment to a prosthesis in targeted reinnervation amputees. Brain 134:747–758.
doi:10.1093/brain/awq361
McIntyre CC, Grill WM (2000) Selective microstimulation of central nervous system neurons.
Ann Biomed Eng 28:219–233
McIntyre CC, Grill WM (2002) Extracellular stimulation of central neurons: influence of stimulus
waveform and frequency on neuronal output. J Neurophysiol 88:1592–1604
Mileusnic MP, Loeb GE (2006) Mathematical models of proprioceptors. II. Structure and function
of the Golgi tendon organ. J Neurophysiol 96:1789–1802. doi:10.1152/jn.00869.2005
Toward a Proprioceptive Neural Interface that Mimics Natural … 387
Mileusnic MP, Brown IE, Lan N, Loeb GE (2006) Mathematical models of proprioceptors.
I. Control and transduction in the muscle spindle. J Neurophysiol 96:1772–1788. doi:10.1152/
jn.00868.2005
Morrow MM, Jordan LR, Miller LE (2007) Direct comparison of the task-dependent discharge of
M1 in hand space and muscle space. J Neurophysiol 97:1786–1798
Murasugi CM, Salzman CD, Newsome WT (1993) Microstimulation in visual area MT: effects of
varying pulse amplitude and frequency. J Neurosci 13:1719–1729
Navarro X, Krueger TB, Lago N, Micera S, Stieglitz T, Dario P (2005) A critical review of
interfaces with the peripheral nervous system for the control of neuroprostheses and hybrid
bionic systems. J Peripheral Nerv Syst 10:229–258
Oby ER, Ethier C, Miller LE (2013) Movement representation in the primary motor cortex and its
contribution to generalizable EMG predictions. J Neurophysiol 109:666–678. doi:10.1152/jn.
00331.2012
O’Doherty JE, Lebedev MA, Hanson TL, Fitzsimmons NA, Nicolelis MA (2009) A brain-machine
interface instructed by direct intracortical microstimulation. Front Integr Neurosci 3:1–10.
doi:10.3389/neuro.07.020.2009
O’Doherty JE, Lebedev MA, Ifft PJ, Zhuang KZ, Shokur S, Bleuler H, Nicolelis MAL (2011)
Active tactile exploration using a brain-machine-brain interface. Nature 479:228–231
Otto KJ, Rousche PJ, Kipke DR (2005a) Cortical microstimulation in auditory cortex of rat elicits
best-frequency dependent behaviors. J Neural Eng 2:42–51
Otto KJ, Rousche PJ, Kipke DR (2005b) Microstimulation in auditory cortex provides a substrate
for detailed behaviors. Hear Res 210:112–117
Pons TP, Garraghty PE, Cusick CG, Kaas JH (1985) The somatotopic organization of area 2 in
macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol 241:445–466
Prud’homme MJL, Kalaska JF (1994) Proprioceptive activity in primate primary somatosensory
cortex during active arm reaching movements. J Neurophysiol 72:2280–2301
Rajan AT, Boback JL, Dammann JF et al (2015) The effects of chronic intracortical
microstimulation on neural tissue and fine motor behavior. J Neural Eng 12:066018
Raspopovic S, Capogrosso M, Petrini FM et al (2014) Restoring natural sensory feedback in
real-time bidirectional hand prostheses. Sci Transl Med 6:222ra219. doi:10.1126/scitranslmed.
3006820
Romo R, Hernandez A, Zainos A, Salinas E (1998) Somatosensory discrimination based on
cortical microstimulation. Nature 392:387–390
Saal HP, Bensmaia SJ (2015) Biomimetic approaches to bionic touch through a peripheral nerve
interface. Neuropsychologia 79:344–353
Sainburg RL, Poizner H, Ghez C (1993) Loss of proprioception produces deficits in interjoint
coordination. J Neurophysiol 70:2136–2147
Sainburg RL, Ghilardi MF, Poizner H, Ghez C (1995) Control of limb dynamics in normal
subjects and patients without proprioception. J Neurophysiol 73:820–835
Salzman CD, Murasugi CM, Britten KH, Newsome WT (1992) Microstimulation in visual area
MT: effects on direction discrimination performance. J Neurosci 12:2331–2355
Sanes JN, Mauritz KH, Evarts EV, Dalakas MC, Chu A (1984) Motor deficits in patients with
large-fiber sensory neuropathy. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 81:979–982
Sanes JN, Suner S, Lando JF, Donoghue JP (1988) Rapid reorganization of adult rat motor
cortex somatic representation patterns after motor nerve injury. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 85:
2003–2007
Schmidt E, Bak M, Hambrecht F, Kufta C, O’Rourke D, Vallabhanath P (1996) Feasibility of a
visual prosthesis for the blind based on intracortical microsimulation of the visual cortex. Brain
119:507–522
Scott SH, Kalaska JF (1995) Changes in motor cortex activity during reaching movements with
similar hand paths but different arm postures. J Neurophysiol 73:2563–2567
Serruya MD, Hatsopoulos NG, Paninski L, Fellows MR, Donoghue JP (2002) Instant neural
control of a movement signal. Nature 416:141–142
Sherrington C (1906) The integrative action of the nervous system. CUP Archive
388 T. Tomlinson and L.E. Miller
Sinclair RJ, Burton H (1991) Neuronal activity in the primary somatosensory cortex in monkeys
(Macaca mulatta) during active touch of textured surface gratings: responses to groove width,
applied force, and velocity of motion. J Neurophysiol 66:153–169
Soso MJ, Fetz EE (1980) Responses of identified cells in postcentral cortex of awake monkeys
during comparable active and passive joint movements. J Neurophysiol 43:1090–1110
Suminski AJ, Tkach DC, Fagg AH, Hatsopoulos NG (2010) Incorporating feedback from multiple
sensory modalities enhances brain-machine interface control. J Neurosci 30:16777–16787.
doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3967-10.2010 30/50/16777 [pii]
Tabot GA, Dammann JF, Berg JA, Tenore FV, Boback JL, Vogelstein RJ, Bensmaia SJ (2013)
Restoring the sense of touch with a prosthetic hand through a brain interface. Proc Natl Acad
Sci 110:18279–18284
Talwar SK, Xu S, Hawley ES, Weiss SA, Moxon KA, Chapin JK (2002) Rat navigation guided by
remote control. Nature 417:37–38
Tan DW, Schiefer MA, Keith MW, Anderson JR, Tyler J, Tyler DJ (2014) A neural interface
provides long-term stable natural touch perception. Sci Transl Med 6:257ra138 doi:10.1126/
scitranslmed.3008669
Thomson EE, Carra R, Nicolelis MA (2013) Perceiving invisible light through a somatosensory
cortical prosthesis. Nat Commun 4:1482. doi:10.1038/ncomms2497
Tremblay F, Ageranioti-Belanger S, Chapman C (1996) Cortical mechanisms underlying tactile
discrimination in the monkey. I. Role of primary somatosensory cortex in passive texture
discrimination. J Neurophysiol 76:3382–3403
Vargas-Irwin CE, Shakhnarovich G, Yadollahpour P, Mislow JM, Black MJ, Donoghue JP (2010)
Decoding complete reach and grasp actions from local primary motor cortex populations.
J Neurosci 30:9659–9669. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5443-09.2010
Venkatraman S, Carmena JM (2011) Active sensing of target location encoded by cortical
microstimulation. IEEE Trans Neural Syst Rehabil Eng 19:317–324. doi:10.1109/tnsre.2011.
2117441
Wang J, Liu Y, Qin L, Chimoto S, Nakamoto K, Sato Y (2012) Chronic microstimulation of cat
auditory cortex effective to evoke detection behaviors. Neuroscience 206:81–88. doi:10.1016/
j.neuroscience.2012.01.012
Weber DJ, London BM, Hokanson JA et al (2011) Limb-state information encoded by peripheral
and central somatosensory neurons: Implications for an afferent interface. IEEE Trans Neural
Syst Rehabil Eng 19:501–513
Weber DJ, Friesen R, Miller LE (2012) Interfacing the somatosensory system to restore touch and
proprioception: essential considerations. J Mot Behav 44:403–418. doi:10.1080/00222895.
2012.735283
Weerakkody N, Taylor JL, Gandevia S (2009) The effect of high-frequency cutaneous vibration on
different inputs subserving detection of joint movement. Exp Brain Res 197:347–355
Wodlinger B, Downey JE, Tyler-Kabara EC, Schwartz AB, Boninger ML, Collinger JL (2015)
Ten-dimensional anthropomorphic arm control in a human brain−machine interface: difficul-
ties, solutions, and limitations. J Neural Eng 12:016011
Yawn R, Hunter JB, Sweeney AD, Bennett ML (2015) Cochlear implantation: a biomechanical
prosthesis for hearing loss. F1000Prime Reports 7:45 doi:10.12703/P7-45
Yizhar O, Fenno LE, Davidson TJ, Mogri M, Deisseroth K (2011) Optogenetics in neural systems.
Neuron 71:9–34
Zaaimi B, Ruiz-Torres R, Solla SA, Miller LE (2013) Multi-electrode stimulation in somatosen-
sory cortex increases probability of detection. J Neural Eng 10:056013