Language Learning Strategies: A Review On Definitional and Theoretical Conflicts
Language Learning Strategies: A Review On Definitional and Theoretical Conflicts
Language Learning Strategies: A Review On Definitional and Theoretical Conflicts
net/publication/354131191
CITATION READS
1 401
1 author:
Rifat Kamasak
University of the West of England, Bristol
75 PUBLICATIONS 1,142 CITATIONS
SEE PROFILE
All content following this page was uploaded by Rifat Kamasak on 25 August 2021.
Rıfat KAMAŞAK
ORCID: 0000-0001-8768-3569
Prof. Dr., Yeditepe Üniversitesi Ticari Bilimler Fakültesi
Abstract
This paper aims to review a number of recent studies on language learning strategies to
evaluate whether the related criticisms about strategy use in language learning and teaching are
still justified. In the last thirty years researchers have paid considerable attention to use of
strategies in learning (i.e. Cohen, 1998; Dörnyei, 2005; Lin, Zhang, & Zheng, 2017; Macaro,
2006; O’Malley & Chamot, 1990; Oxford, 1990; Oxford et al., 2014; Rose et al., 2018; Teng
& Zhang, 2016; Tseng, Dörnyei & Schmidt, 2006) to explore unexplained variance in second
or foreign language acquisition (SLA) performance. The rationale behind research was that
“learning strategies could make language learning more effective, efficient, and enjoyable”
(Oxford et al., 2014, p. 11). However, LLS research was criticised because of some weaknesses
it possesses (Dörnyei, 2005; Grenfell & Macaro, 2007; Tseng et al., 2006; Woodrow, 2005).
Definitional fuzziness and theoretical inconsistency, and methodology and measurement
problems were frequently mentioned in the language learning strategy literature. The
emergence of these problems will be investigated in details. Finally, based on this review, some
implications by which language learning strategy research can be strengthened are also
recommended.
Keywords: Language Learning Strategy, Self-regulated Learning, Motivation,
Literature Review
1. INTRODUCTION
Language learning strategies (LLS) are generally defined as “activities consciously
chosen by learners for the purpose of regulating their own language learning” (Griffiths, 2013,
p. 36). This definition may not be accepted by every researcher in the field but “it touches the
heart of the matter” (Griffiths & Oxford, 2014, p. 2). There have been repeated and ambitious
attempts to define learning strategies, offer strategy classification schemes and strategy
frameworks in the field. A number of theorists (i.e. Anderson, 1980; O’Malley & Chamot,
1990; Wenden, 1987) have situated learning strategies within the field of cognitive psychology
and have stated that “strategies are located in the brain” Macaro (2006, p. 323) and are produced
through mental processes and structures. O’Malley and Chamot (1990) primarily focused on
the use of cognitive elements (i.e. working memory and mental frameworks) for processing
information and on meta-cognitive elements “such as planning and evaluating for executive
management of language learning” (Oxford et al., 2014b, p. 31). According to O’Malley and
Chamot (1990), LLS are “special thoughts or behaviours that individuals use to help them
comprehend, learn, or retain new information” (p. 1). As we can see different definitions
regarding language learning strategies have led to a vague phenomenon in the field. In
particular, the integration of other social and psychological elements by Rebecca Oxford’s
141
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
model (1989) created more turbulent ground. Thus, the problematic issues in the field of
language learning strategy are discussed with a number of main headings below.
142
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
143
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
improvements on the issue are considerable (Dörnyei & Ryan, 2015; Griffiths & Oxford, 2014;
Teng & Zhang, 2016).
According to Dörnyei (2005), the most fundamental problem of LLS literature was its
“inability to explain the difference between engaging in an ordinary learning activity and a
strategic learning activity” (Grenfell & Macaro, 2007, p. 25). Several researchers who
emphasise the unique characteristics and psychological reality of language learning (i.e.
Dörnyei, 2005; Tseng et al., 2006; Yamamori et al., 2003) link this problem to lack of
psychometrically sound measures in existing LLS research. The rating scales that are based on
frequency of strategy use were “not cumulative and [their] computing mean scale scores were
psychometrically not justifiable” (Dörnyei, 2005, p. 182). Therefore, a new approach that shifts
the direction of related research from “learner’s use of strategies to their self-regulation in
language learning” (Gao, 2006, p. 615) emerged.
144
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
emotional or behavioural, and showed how “the dynamic descriptions of the interplay of
cognitive, motivational and behavioural phenomena” (p. 365) can be facilitated through self-
regulation by explaining the role of information processing in his model. With a
methodological concern in SRL, Rose (2012) conducted a research which employs “a mixed-
method qualitative approach in a case study context” (Rose, 2013, p. 983) on strategic learning
of Japanese writing character, kanji. Considering the idiosyncratic and context-specific self-
directed learning behaviours of learners and using the interview data, his framework
incorporated both LLS-based “notions of cognitive and memory strategies with SRL-based
notions of self-regulation and motivation control” (p. 141). After the emergence of SRL
approach, Oxford (2011) also modified her famous model with the new “self-regulated L2
learning strategies” framework. This framework included new constructs such as “generating
and maintaining motivation” and “overcoming knowledge gaps in communicating” Oxford
(2011, p. 12). However, this conceptual broadening added more complexity to the existing
learning strategy literature (Dörnyei & Ryan, 2015).
In a more recent study, Teng and Zhang (2016) offered the Writing Strategies for Self-
Regulated Learning Questionnaire (WSSRLQ) as a new instrument and tested its validity
through a confirmatory factor analysis CFA). CFA yielded nine SRL strategies and six of them
were associated with EFL writing performance. The results did not only reveal the multi-
faceted structure of SRL but it also confirmed the applicability of new psychometric constructs
such as “peer learning, feedback handling, and interest enhancement” to the SRL approach.
The study provided a robust statistical testing of psychometric constructs, but again new
constructs which require further examination are added to the SRL field.
DISCUSSION
Against the contributions of SRL framework in understanding better the processes
underlying language learning, working with psychometric constructs, using rich qualitative
data through open questions, and bringing a more dynamic model to explore changing strategic
behaviours of learners in response to changing learning environments, the framework does not
seem to resolve the problems of traditional LLS. Firstly, the SRL framework does not
completely address the measurement weakness of the self-reported data. Secondly, as a result
of the synonymous use of the concepts “such as self-management, self-control, action control,
volition, self-change, self-directed behaviour, coping behaviour, and even meta-cognitive and
problem solving” (Dörnyei & Ryan, 2015, p. 167) SRL opens another door to definitional
fuzziness issue of strategy literature. Moreover, some elements of overlaps in action control
strategies (Rose, 2012) and in the relationship between strategic behaviour and achievement
(Ranalli, 2012) can be observed.
Based on the review, it can be concurred that strategy use in language learning is a highly
individualised activity which includes environment and context specificities. Given this
situation, instead of using standard inventories and seeking to add another definition to the
existing ones, researchers should develop individualised research designs that are adaptable to
situational factors. In line with the arguments of Rose et al. (2018) about the “underuse of
qualitative, in-depth measures” (p. 158) in SRL research, this can be achieved through
employing qualitative methods such as stimulated recall and structured observation (Gass &
145
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
Mackey, 2017; Rose, 2013) which can provide rich and insightful data or other unique hybrid
designs which combine qualitative and quantitative methods. The use of self-reported data
about achievement or performance may lead to misleading results besides the performance
related construct may change over time. For this reason, conducting longitudinal studies should
be the target of strategy researchers.
SRL-based strategy use and achievement are generally linked to each other with direct
relationship assumptions (i.e. Filate, 2012; Ping & Siraj, 2012; Seker, 2016) in strategy
research. However, investigation of the mediating or moderating roles of strategy use between
other influential constructs and performance may offer insightful findings. More research to
understand interactions between the dimensions of SRL is also needed. Finally, different
elements of different frameworks (i.e. cognitive strategies of LLS and self-regulation of SRL)
may play complementary roles in explaining strategic language learning of learners. Therefore,
given the strategy selections may vary according to tasks (Cohen & Macaro, 2007), researchers
should further investigate the interplay of strategy combinations and their effects on learning
performance.
REFERENCES
[1] Anderson, J. (1980). Cognitive Psychology and Its Implications. San Francisco: W.H.
Freeman & Co.
[2] Ardasheva, Y., & Tretter, T. (2013). Strategy inventory for language learning–ELL
student form: testing for factorial validity. The Modern Language Journal, 97(2): 474–489.
[3] Cohen, A. D. (1998). Strategies in Learning and Using a Second Language. Harlow:
Longman.
[4] Cohen, A. D. (2011). Strategies in Learning and Using a Second Language (2nd,
ed.). London: Longman.
[5] Cohen, A. D., & Macaro, E. (Eds.). (2007). Language Learner Strategies: 30 Years
of Research and Practice. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[6] Dörnyei, Z. (2005). The Psychology of the Language Learner: Individual Differences
in Second Language Acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.
[7] Dörnyei, Z., & Ryan, S. (2015). The Psychology of The Language Learner Revisited.
New York: Routledge.
[8] Dörnyei, Z., & Skehan, P. (2003). Individual differences in second language learning.
In C. J. Doughty, & M. H. Long (eds), The Handbook of Second Language Acquisition, (pp.
589-630). Oxford: Blackwell.
[9] Filate, Y. A. (2012). The impact of students’ self-regulated language learning on their
reading achievement: Grade 9 students in focus. ELT Research Journal, 1(3), 175–188.
[10] Gao, X. (2006). Has language learning strategy research come to an end? A response
to Tseng, Dörnyei. Applied Linguistics, 28(4): 615–620.
[11] Gass, S. M., & Mackey, A. (2017). Stimulated Recall Methodology in Applied
Linguistics and L2 Research (2nd ed.). New York: Routledge.
[12] Glenn, W. (2000). Language learning strategy use of bilingual foreign language
learners in Singapore. Language Learning, 50(2): 203–243.
146
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
[13] Grenfell, M., & Macaro, E. (2007). Claims and critiques. In A. D. Cohen, & E.
Macaro (eds), Language Learner Strategies: Thirty Years of Research and Practice, (pp. 9-
28). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[14] Griffiths, C. (2013). The Strategy Factor in Successful Language Learning. Bristol:
Multilingual Matters.
[15] Griffiths, C., & Oxford, R. L. (2014). The twenty-first century landscape of
language learning strategies: introduction to this special issue. System, 43(1): 1–10.
[16] Gu, Y. (2012). Learning strategies: prototypical core and dimensions of variation.
Studies in Self-Access Learning Journal, 3(4), 330–356.
[17] Larsen-Freeman, D., & Cameron, L. (2008). Complex Systems and Applied
Linguistics. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
[18] Lin, C-H., Zhang, Y., & Zheng, B. (2017). The roles of learning strategies and
motivation in online language learning: a structural equation modelling analysis. Computers &
Education, 113: 75–85.
[19] Macaro, E. (2006). Strategies for language learning and for language use: revising
the theoretical framework. The Modern Language Journal, 90(3): 320–337.
[20] Mizumoto, A., & Takeuchi, O. (2012). Adaptation and validation of Self-regulating
Capacity in Vocabulary Learning Scale. Applied Linguistics, 33(1): 83–91.
[21] O’Malley, J. M., & Chamot, A. U. (1990). Learning Strategies in Second Language
Acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press.
[22] Oxford, R. L. (1989). Use of language learning strategies: a synthesis of studies with
implications for strategy training. System, 17(2): 235–247.
[23] Oxford, R. L. (1990). Language Learning Strategies: What Every Teacher Should
Know. New York: Newbury House.
[24] Oxford, R. L. (2011). Teaching and Researching Language Learning Strategies.
Harlow: Longman.
[25] Oxford, R. L., Griffiths, C., Longhini A., Cohen, A. D., Macaro, E., & Harris, V.
(2014a). Experts’ personal metaphors and similes about language learning strategies. System,
43(2): 11–29.
[26] Oxford, R. L., Rubin, J., Chamot, A. U., Schrammd, K., Lavine, R., Gunning, P., &
Nel, C. (2014b). The learning strategy prism: perspectives of learning strategy experts. System,
43(3): 30–49.
[27] Ping, A. M., & Siraj, S. (2012). Exploring self-regulatory strategies for vocabulary
learning among Chinese EFL learners. Procedia - Social and Behavioral Sciences, 47, 1211–
1215.
[28] Ranalli, J. (2012). Alternative models of self-regulation and implications for L2
strategy research. Studies in Self-Access Learning Journal, 3(4): 357–376.
[29] Riding, R., & Rayner, S. G. (1998). Cognitive Styles and Learning Strategies:
Understanding Style Differences in Learning and Behaviour. London: David Fulton.
[30] Rose, H. (2012). Reconceptualizing strategic learning in the face of self-regulation:
throwing language learning strategies out with the bathwater. Applied Linguistics, 33(1): 92–
98.
147
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY
[Pearson Journal International Conference on 22-23 January
Social Sciences & Humanities] 2021
[31] Rose, H. (2013). L2 learners’ attitudes toward, and use of, mnemonic strategies
when learning Japanese Kanji. The Modern Language Journal, 97(4): 981–992.
[32] Rose, H. (2015). Researching language learner strategies. In B. Paltridge, & A.
Phakiti (eds), Research Methods in Applied Linguistics: A practical Resource, (pp. 421-437).
New York: Bloomsbury.
[33] Rose, H., Briggs, J. G., Boggs, J. A., Sergio, L., & Ivanova-Slavianskaia, N. (2018).
A systematic review of language learner strategy research in the face of self-regulation. System,
72(1): 151–163.
[34] Seker, M. (2016). The use of self-regulation strategies by foreign language learners
and its role in language achievement. Language Teaching Research, 20(5): 600–618.
[35] Takeuchi, O., Mine, H., Yoshida, H., & Yoshida, S. (1999). Toward the
establishment of country-by-country SILL (Strategy Inventory for Language Learning) norms.
Language Laboratory, 36(1): 51–62.
[36] Teng, L. S., & Zhang, J. L. (2016). A questionnaire-based validation of
multidimensional models of self-regulated learning strategies. The Modern Language Journal,
100(3): 674–701.
[37] Tseng, W. T., Dörnyei, Z., & Schmitt, N. (2006). A new approach to assessing
strategic learning: the case of self-regulation in vocabulary acquisition. Applied Linguistics,
27(1): 78–102.
[38] Wenden, A. (1987). Conceptual background and utility. In A. Wenden, & J. Rubin
(eds), Learner Strategies in Language Learning, (pp. 31-42). London: Prentice Hall.
[39] Woodrow, L. (2005). The challenge of measuring language learning strategies.
Foreign Language Annals, 38(1): 90–100.
[40] Yamamori, K., Isoda, T., Hiromori, T., & Oxford, R. L. (2003). Using cluster
analysis to uncover L2 learner differences in strategy use, will to learn, and achievement over
time. International Review of Applied Linguistics, 41(4): 381–409.
[41] Ziegler, N. (2015). The predictive value of the Self-regulating Capacity in
Vocabulary Learning Scale. Applied Linguistics, 36(5): 641–647.
148
Full Texts Book Ankara, TURKEY