2243 12569 2 PB

Download as pdf or txt
Download as pdf or txt
You are on page 1of 21

International Journal of Communication 8 (2014), 2340–2342 1932–8036/20140005

Enacted Social Support on Social Media and Subjective Well-Being

HEEWON KIM1
Rutgers University, USA

This study examines supportive interactions on Facebook to understand the role of social
media use in obtaining social support and promoting psychological well-being. By
assessing the amount of social support received through Facebook and by other means,
this study ascertains a direct relationship between Facebook use and social support
reception. Facebook use was the strongest predictor of social support reception, even
stronger than the number of strong ties, although its link to life satisfaction was not
significant. Findings also demonstrated that gender difference in social support reception
was not found in the context of social media and that fixed Internet use had a negative
association with life satisfaction, whereas mobile Internet use had no such relationship.

Keywords: social media, social support, strong tie, life satisfaction, Facebook

Communication scholars have long been interested in whether and how the use of new media
affects our social lives. Reflecting widespread social concern for the effects of new technology, early
studies argued that Internet use might be detrimental to interpersonal relationships and psychological
well-being (for a review, see Baym, 2010). Kraut et al. (1998) reported that heavy Internet use was
associated with lower levels of face-to-face communication with family and friends and higher levels of
depression, stress, and loneliness. Similarly, Nie, Hillygus, and Erbring (2002) and Robinson, Kestnbaum,
Newstadtl, and Alvarez (2002) claimed that Internet use replaced the time formerly devoted to social
interactions or the use of other media.

The displacement hypothesis, which posits that new media use displaces time spent on other
activities, was influential in the early Internet period but has been criticized since then due to a lack of
empirical support. A follow-up study by Kraut et al. (2002) contrasted with the previous research,
reporting generally positive effects of Internet use on social life and psychological well-being. Hampton
and Wellman (2003) demonstrated that Internet users were more likely to communicate with other people
or to meet others in person than non-users. Active Internet users were also more likely to use other
communication tools, to have frequent face-to-face conversations (Baym, Zhang, & Lin, 2004), and to
have contact with a greater number of people (Wang & Wellman, 2010). Furthermore, when people

Heewon Kim: [email protected]


Date submitted: 2013–05–12

1
The author is indebted to Matthew Weber and two anonymous reviewers for their helpful feedback on an
earlier version of this manuscript.

Copyright © 2014 (Heewon Kim). Licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial No
Derivatives (by-nc-nd). Available at http://ijoc.org.
2202 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

become geographically dispersed, the Internet served as a useful way to maintain relationships (Quan-
Haase, Wellman, Witte, & Hampton, 2002). These studies concluded that Internet use supplements or
reinforces face-to-face communication rather than replaces it.

More recently, as a variety of information and communication technologies (ICTs), such as social
media and mobile phones, are incorporated into people’s daily routines, scholars have found that ICT use
provides new affordances for relational maintenance. Chen (2013) showed that online communication is
positively associated with the number and proportion of strong ties in individuals’ core networks. Mobile
Internet use, which enables individuals to stay connected on the move, has been found to be a time-
enhancing activity rather than a time-displacing activity, which indicates a positive effect of mobile
Internet on sociability (Ishii, 2004). Social media have become a significant means of daily conversations
(boyd & Ellison, 2007); also, social media use can reinforce existing social relations by keeping individuals
informed of others’ activities (Hargittai, 2007). Pursuing these scholarly concerns further, researchers
have shown that social media use can play a positive role in enhancing interpersonal relationships and
mental health. Facebook use significantly promotes social capital and subjective mental well-being,
generating greater benefits for people suffering from low levels of self-esteem and life satisfaction (Ellison,
Steinfield, & Lampe, 2007, 2011; Valenzuela, Park, & Kee, 2009). Similarly, the number of Facebook
friends is positively associated with perceived social support, which in turn leads to reduced stress and
increased life satisfaction, particularly in high-stressed groups (Nabi, Prestin, & So, 2013). Hampton,
Sessions Goulet, Rainie, and Purcell (2011) revealed that Facebook users have closer, more trustworthy,
and supportive relationships than the average American, which implies profound impacts of social media
use on interpersonal relationships.

The current study also examines the associations among social media use, interpersonal
relationships, and subjective well-being, particularly focusing on social support exchange. Social support,
one of the most significant benefits from social relationships, is a barometer of one’s relationship quality
and psychological well-being (Cohen, Gottlieb, & Underwood, 2000). As such, it is crucial to better
investigate whether the use of social media contributes to the transaction of social support, particularly
given that younger generations use social media on a daily basis. Specifically, this study expands social
support scholarship in two ways. First, this research looks into social support exchange both in and out of
Facebook to better understand the effects of Facebook use. In doing so, the study captures to what extent
social media use facilitates exchanging social support. Although prior research displayed positive
relationships between social media use and social capital or perception of social support, whether the use
of social media directly affects supportive interactions has not been thoroughly examined. (A notable
exception is Stefanone, Kwon, & Lackaff, 2012.) Second, social support research generally deals with
specific populations, such as people with terminal illness (e.g., cancer), older adults, and online support
group members who are in urgent need of specialized social support. Although extant research on support
groups offered insights into the impact of online social support on health outcomes, other contexts should
be investigated, given that individuals also increasingly turn to online social support owing to the
prevalent use of ICTs.

In aggregate, this study investigates the types of social support that individuals acquire through
social media and by other means (e.g., face-to-face communication) to understand the extent to which
individuals gather social support through social media use. The associations among the amount of
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2203

received social support, the size of one’s social network, and the use of social media have been considered
to determine whether social media can be a significant means of social support exchange. Perceived life
satisfaction is included to assess the impact of mediated social support on individuals’ subjective well-
being. The results demonstrate that social media use has a greater impact on social support reception
than the number of strong ties, indicating a positive role of social media in cementing relationships. The
effects of gender, age, and Internet use are discussed in light of the findings.

Social Support via Social Media and Subjective Well-Being

Conceptualization of Social Support

Many scholars have sought to refine and develop concepts of social support, resulting in
numerous definitions and measures (Barrera, 1986). These definitions can be classified into three broad
categories that reflect specific dimensions of social support: social embeddedness, perceived social
support, and enacted social support (Barrera, 1986; Vangelisti, 2009). First, the social embeddedness
perspective focuses on individuals’ connections to others or available social ties in their social
environments (Stokes, 1983), as social networks are regarded as the “infrastructure of social support”
(Albrecht & Adelman, 1987). Second, perceived social support is germane to psychological or cognitive
appraisals of supportive relationships, and is defined as the social support that people believe is available
to them (Procidano & Heller, 1983). Third, enacted social support is conceptualized as “actions” that
people perform when they render assistance (Barrera, 1986, p. 417). As Vangelisti (2009) points out, a
communication perspective focuses on the “interactions” (p. 40) that occur between the providers and
recipients of social support. Unlike available or perceived social support, the concept of enacted social
support deals with what individuals actually do when they desire to provide social support; namely,
researchers ask about the types of supportive behaviors that people experience.

To capture individuals’ experiences of social support, this study employs the enacted support
perspective through an examination of the occurrence of supportive behaviors on social media. First, by
conceptualizing social support as “interpersonal transactions” (House, 1981, p. 39), this perspective places
an emphasis on investigating everyday practices that constitute social support. As Thoits (2011) proposed,
attentive investigations of ordinary days can reveal individuals’ routine interactions that convey social
support. Instead of dealing with proxies (e.g., perceived number of supportive ties) or measuring support
exchange in experimental settings, the enacted social support perspective allowed researchers to look into
daily behaviors that people use to provide support. Second, in contrast to social embeddedness or
perceived support perspectives, the enacted support approach differentiated particular dimensions of
supportive interactions that may have varied interpersonal or health implications (e.g., a case in which a
person receives frequent informational support but suffers from a lack of emotional support). Third,
scholars have pointed out that perceived social support scales overlap with measures of perceived stress
or other self-reported psychological distress (Barrera, 1986; Gore, 1981; Henderson et al., 1978). Thus,
when examining the associations between social support and subjective well-being, enacted or received
social support, which measures frequency of behaviors, offers enhanced utility and validity. Keeping this
discussion in mind, this study employs the framework of enacted social support to better ascertain the
relationships among individuals’ experiences of exchanging social support and psychological well-being. In
2204 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

the next section, existing literature on social support in mediated environments, the role of strong ties in
supportive communication, and the impact of social support on subjective well-being are reviewed.

Social Support in Mediated Environments

Early studies of online social support investigated the ways in which supportive communication
patterns emerged and evolved in online environments, particularly in comparison with face-to-face
contexts (Baym, 1998). For instance, Braithwaite, Waldron, and Finn (1999) found that people developed
unique features of mediated social support (e.g., employing emoticons and signatures) to deliver
nonverbal cues and facilitate forging relationships. More recent studies of online support groups have
investigated further associations among mediated social support and online participation, online social
networks, and health outcomes (for a review, see Rains & Young, 2009; Wright & Bell, 2003). Individuals
who spend more time communicating with online support group members are more likely to have a larger
online support network and a higher level of support network satisfaction (Wright, 2000a, 2000b). Also,
Bambina (2007) revealed that core members, who most actively engaged in the online group
communication, provided and received more emotional social support than peripheral members. These
studies confirmed that participation in online support groups was positively associated with increased
social support reception and satisfaction.

Extending these findings, scholars have examined the use of social media to ascertain its effect
on social support exchange. As DeAndrea, Ellison, LaRose, and Steinfield (2012) suggested, social media
can be designed and used to enhance individuals’ perception of available social support and support
networks. Ellison et al. (2011) argued that Facebook use may offer more opportunities to give and receive
emotional social support through Facebook friend networks. Indeed, scholars have shown that Facebook
has been used as a meaningful venue for soliciting and receiving social support. Facebook use was
positively associated with nonmaterial forms of support such as provision of information, advice, and
companionship (Sessions Goulet, 2012). Patients with diabetes and their family and friends requested
advice, received emotional support, and often shared unsolicited information on diabetes through
Facebook (Greene, Choudhry, Kilabuk, & Shrank, 2011). People with cancer employed Facebook for
fundraising, patient and caregiver support, and informational support (Bender, Jimenez-Marroquin, &
Jadad, 2011). Furthermore, Stefanone et al. (2012) examined the relationships between social media use
and social support mobilization to reveal that frequency of Facebook use was the strongest predictor of
social support reception, even stronger than emotional closeness. Overall, these studies illustrate that
active Facebook use can promote supportive interactions among individuals.

The Role of Strong Ties in Social Support Exchange

Social ties are conduits of several types of social support, although strong ties and weak ties can
play disparate roles in social support exchange (Lin & Westcott, 1991). The strength of a tie is determined
by a combination of amount of time, emotional intensity, intimacy, and reciprocal services (Granovetter,
1973). Whereas weak ties are considered channels for nonredundant information, strong ties are
characterized by credibility, influence, and a willingness to provide immediate help (Granovetter, 1983);
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2205

thus, social support has been understood as the functions performed typically by strong ties (Thoits,
2011).

Although researchers have employed terms such as primary ties, strong ties, and core networks,
a number of studies have concluded that close relationships are the most effective resources for social
support. Lin, Woelfel, and Light (1985) show that access to social support is positively related to strong
and homophilous ties, rather than weak and heterophilous ties, as well as network density, meaning a
higher level of interactions. Strong ties are greater motivators of assistance and provide comfort in the
face of uncertainty (Krackhardt, 1992). Also, Pearlin, Lieberman, Menaghan, and Mullan (1981) suggested
that the quality of relations, marked by intimate communication, trust, and solidarity, are critical in
acquiring access to social support. Investigating the ways in which core networks help individuals garner
social support, Hurlbert, Hains, and Beggs (2000) showed that individuals embedded in core networks
with higher density and greater diversity activate their core ties to a greater degree than others to
mobilize social support. Stefanone et al. (2012) extended this inquiry to social media contexts to reveal
that strong ties and Facebook communication frequency are positively associated with enacted social
support. Overall, these studies agreed that closer relationships can play a significant role in offering social
support.

Based on the aforementioned literature, this study delves into whether social media use can
affect the amount of social support received. In particular, this study isolates social support received
through social media from social support received via other channels (e.g., face-to-face, phone calls) to
delve into the role of social media in social support exchange. Also, tie strength is added to the model to
examine the extent to which social media affects the reception of social support. Thus, the hypotheses are
proposed as follows.

H1: Intensity of Facebook use is positively associated with the amount of social support received
through Facebook.

H2: Intensity of Facebook use is positively associated with the amount of social support received by
means other than Facebook.

Mediated Social Support and Subjective Well-Being

Scholars have reported that online social support is related to positive health outcomes such as a
lower level of perceived life stress (Wright, 1999) and better coping and lower depression (Beaudoin &
Tao, 2007). Also, although it was uncertain that mediated social support actually improved health
conditions, recipients perceived mediated social support as beneficial and comforting (Vicary & Fraley,
2010). As Rains and Young (2009) discussed, the extant literature displays mixed findings regarding the
health outcomes of online social support; however, their meta-analysis of 28 studies concluded that
participation in a mediated support group generally leads to increased social support, decreased
depression, increased quality of life, and increased self-efficacy in managing one’s health condition.

Regarding social media, Facebook use is positively related to people’s life satisfaction
(Valenzuela, Park, & Kee, 2009); moreover, Facebook users with lower levels of life satisfaction and self-
esteem gain greater benefit from Facebook use than others in terms of social capital (Ellison, Steinfield, &
2206 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

Lampe, 2007). Individuals who have more Facebook friends experience reduced stress, less physical
illness, and greater well-being (Nabi et al., 2013). This study extends this inquiry to delve into the
association between online social support and subjective well-being compared with social support
delivered by other means. This study focuses on life satisfaction, inter alia, which has been viewed as an
important dimension of mental and psychological health and a measure for general health status
applicable to a wide range of populations (Headey et al., 1993; McDowell, 2006). The associations
between received social support and life satisfaction are hypothesized as follows:

H3: The amount of social support received through Facebook is positively associated with life
satisfaction.

H4: The amount of social support received by means other than Facebook is positively associated
with life satisfaction.

Method

Data Collection

An anonymous online survey was conducted in undergraduate classes at a large eastern


university in April 2011. A variety of classes were selected to ensure as heterogeneous a population as
possible, drawing students from different majors and academic years. Participants were informed that the
study was anonymous, participation was voluntary, and individual responses would not be disclosed. The
researcher was not the course instructor; however, participants were granted extra credit as
compensation by their instructor. Of 934 students contacted, 626 participants completed the online
survey, yielding a 67.02% completion rate.

Participant Demographics and Network Size

Among 626 participants, 62% (N = 388) were female and 38% (N = 238) were male. The mean
age was 20 (SD = 1.96). In addition to age and gender, the number of strong ties (or core network size)
was assessed following the approach developed by McCarty, Killworth, Bernard, Johnsen, and Shelley
(2001). This measure asks respondents to calculate the number of people they know in specific relational
categories. In this study, participants were asked to identify “very close” people among family, friends,
coworkers, group members (e.g., sororities, fraternities, sport groups), neighbors, and others,
respectively. The sum of these numbers provided the overall core network size (see also Boase, Horrigan,
Wellman, & Rainie, 2006). The total number of strong ties including family members had a mean of 20.94
(SD = 14.06).2 On average, participants reported 7.63 friends, 6.29 family members, 3.57 group
members, 1.35 coworkers, 1.25 neighbors, and 1.06 others as strong ties. When family members were
excluded from the number of strong ties, the mean was 14.70 (SD = 11.64).

Measures

2
This statistic is similar to the results of Rainie, Horrigan, Wellman, and Boase (2006).
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2207

Facebook Use. An adapted version of the Facebook intensity scale (Ellison, Steinfield, & Lampe,
2007) was used to capture different aspects of Facebook use patterns. Among the items in the original
scale, those that measured the extent to which Facebook was integrated into daily life, the number of
Facebook friends, and Facebook use were included in this study.3 Regarding Facebook use, however,
instead of time spent on Facebook, frequencies of fixed and mobile Facebook use were assessed in a
manner similar to measuring Internet use. Frequencies of Facebook use closely resembled those of
Internet use, indicating that Facebook use was one of the participants’ major online activities (see Table
1). Cronbach’s alpha of the adapted scale was 0.82 (see Table 2).

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Internet Use.

Mean or % (N) SD
Duration of Internet use (years) 10.51 2.94

Number of Internet-enabled devices in use 2.39 1.03

Mobile Internet use 72.2% (452)

Yes

Frequency of fixed Internet use1 5.08 0.98

Frequency of mobile Internet use1 4.07 1.45

Facebook use 97.8% (612)

Yes

Frequency of fixed Facebook use1 4.5 1.2

Frequency of mobile Facebook use1 3.63 1.61

1
Participants were asked to rate the frequency using a 5-point scale: 0 = Never, 1 = A few times per
month, 2 = A few times per week, 3 = Once a day, 4 = 2 to 5 times a day, 5 = 6 to 10 times a day, 6 =
More than 10 times a day.

3
The number of Facebook friends is weakly correlated with the number of strong ties (r = 0.21, p <
0.01). Given this weak relationship, the number of Facebook friends is not removed from the scale
although the number of strong ties is a control variable.
2208 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

Table 2. Summary Statistics for Facebook Intensity Scale.

Mean SD Factor
Loadings3
Facebook Intensity Scale (Cronbach’s alpha = 0.82) 1
– 0.72
0.0004

About how many Facebook friends do you have? 5.00 1.41 0.407
0 = 10 or less, 1 = 11 to 100, 2 = 101 to 200, 3 = 201 to 300, 4 =
301 to 400, 5 = 401 to 500, 6 = more than 500
About how often do you access Facebook in the place where you live? 4.50 1.20 0.769
0 = Never, 1 = A few times per month, 2 = A few times per week, 3
= Once a day, 4 = 2 to 5 times a day, 5 = 6 to 10 times a day, 6 =
More than 10 times a day
About how often do you access Facebook outside of your place or on the 3.63 1.61 0.607
move?
0 = Never, 1 = A few times per month, 2 = A few times per week, 3
= Once a day, 4 = 2 to 5 times a day, 5 = 6 to 10 times a day, 6 =
More than 10 times a day
Facebook is part of my everyday activity.2 4.17 1.05 0.887
Facebook has become part of my daily routine.2 4.15 1.07 0.890
I feel out of touch when I haven’t logged onto Facebook for a while.2
3.51 1.20 0.736

1
Individual items were standardized before analysis.
2
Response categories ranged from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree.
3
The scale was confirmed as a unidimensional measure.

Received Social Support. The Inventory of Socially Supportive Behaviors (ISSB), which is a 40-
item scale, was selected to assess the frequency of experiences of supportive actions from others
(Barrera, Sandler, & Ramsay, 1981). As ISSB measures the frequencies of enacted support rather than
available support or social ties, it can be used with network variables such as number of strong ties.
Furthermore, ISSB allows the researcher to assess the amount of support experienced in different venues
or environments as it is a behavioral measure. The present survey was designed to distinguish supportive
behaviors through Facebook and those by other means (e.g., face-to-face conversations, mobile phone
calls or texts) to isolate social support via social media from other means. Each item on ISSB asks about
the number of times participants were provided with social support in the preceding month, focusing on
supportive behaviors they experienced in daily situations. Participants were instructed to measure the
frequency of each type of social support received through Facebook and by other means separately.
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2209

ISSB aims to capture varied dimensions, including informational, emotional, appraisal, network,
and instrumental social support. In this study, behaviors that cannot be performed in online contexts (i.e.,
instrumental or tangible support) were removed from the scale, concentrating only on emotional and
informational support. This approach contributed to prevention of survey fatigue, which increases missing
values, given that ISSB was employed for two different contexts (Facebook and other channels). After
factor analyses and reliability tests were conducted to confirm the underlying relationships (Fabrigar,
Wegener, MacCallum, and Strahan, 1999), 11 of the 13 items remained in the scale. The two dropped
items were “Communicated with you about some interests of yours” and “Appreciated something you did
well.” Indeed, these two items may be considered general communication behaviors rather than indicating
delivery of particular social support. Cronbach’s alpha of each scale was very good (α = 0.94 for
Facebook, α = 0.91 for other means). After these two items were removed, factor analyses using varimax
rotation also confirmed that only one component was extracted, which indicated the scale could be used
as a unidimensional measure (see Tables 3 and 4).

Table 3. Summary Statistics for ISSB Scale (Facebook).

ISSB (Facebook)1 Mean SD Factor Loadings


(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.94) 29.91 9.55

Expressed esteem or respect for a competency or personal quality 2.88 1.11 0.728
of yours
Suggested some actions/ideas that can be helpful for you 2.80 1.16 0.785
Agreed that what you wanted to do was right 2.66 1.13 0.797
Let you know that she/he will always be around if you need 2.59 1.15 0.800
assistance
Expressed interest and concern in your well-being 2.80 1.14 0.746
Let you know what to expect in a situation that was about to 2.59 1.15 0.801
happen
Joked and kidded to try to cheer you up 3.26 1.67 0.744
Gave you some information on how to do something 2.75 1.14 0.811
Gave you some information to help you understand a situation you 2.53 1.16 0.831
were in
Let you know what she/he did in a situation that was similar to 2.46 1.17 0.816
yours
Let you know that you are OK just the way you are 2.23 1.16 0.743
2210 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

Table 4. Summary Statistics for ISSB Scale (Other Means).

ISSB (Other Means)1 Mean SD Factor Loadings


(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.91) 36.09 8.63

Expressed esteem or respect for a competency or personal quality 3.22 1.01 0.701
of yours
Suggested some actions/ideas that can be helpful for you 3.28 1.07 0.708
Agreed that what you wanted to do was right 3.23 1.02 0.734
Let you know that she/he will always be around if you need 3.26 1.10 0.753
assistance
Expressed interest and concern in your well-being 3.47 1.11 0.711
Let you know what to expect in a situation that was about to 3.21 1.11 0.747
happen
Joked and kidded to try to cheer you up 3.67 1.08 0.684
Gave you some information on how to do something 3.43 1.01 0.708
Gave you some information to help you understand a situation you 3.28 1.04 0.784
were in
Let you know what she/he did in a situation that was similar to 3.07 1.07 0.791
yours
Let you know that you are OK just the way you are 2.89 1.23 0.694

1
Participants were asked to rate the frequency during the preceding month using the following 5-point
scale: 1 = Not at all, 2 = Once or twice, 3 = About once a week, 4 = A few times a week, and 5 = About
every day.

Life Satisfaction. The Satisfaction with Life Scale (SWLS) measured global life satisfaction
(Diener, Emmons, Larsen, & Griffin, 1985). The scale includes five items designed to assess the level of
subjective well-being. All items were measured based on a five-point Likert-type scale (see Table 5). The
level of global life satisfaction was obtained by adding the value of all items (mean=16.44, SD=3.90).

Table 5. Summary Statistics for SWLS.

SWLS 1
Mean SD
(Cronbach’s alpha = 0.84) 16.44 3.90
In most ways my life is close to my ideal. 3.24 0.96
The conditions of my life are excellent. 3.35 0.94
I am satisfied with my life. 3.54 0.97
So far I have gotten the important things I want in life. 3.37 1.01
If I could live my life over, I would change almost nothing. 2.94 1.14
1
Response categories ranged from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree.
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2211

Control variables. Control variables were identified based on the existing literature to examine
whether the effect of social media use is still statistically significant when other factors are controlled.
Studies suggest that individuals with a larger core network, females, and younger people are likely to
receive a larger amount of social support (Hains, Hurlbert, & Beggs, 1996; Hurlbert, Hains, & Beggs,
2000; Wellman & Wortley, 1990). Thus, the size of the core network (i.e., the number of strong ties),
gender, and age were controlled when testing the first set of hypotheses of social support.

Internet use was additionally included in testing the second set of hypotheses as it had been
discussed as a factor that may affect psychological well-being. Given that the population sample of this
study consisted of active users, the researcher created two categories (fixed and mobile), instead of
assessing Internet use at once, to help subjects’ recall and enhance measurement accuracy. The
distinction between fixed and mobile Internet use is not based on technical characteristics but on location:
Fixed Internet use meant the Internet was used at home or in the dormitory, while mobile Internet use
indicated Internet use in other locales or on the move. Furthermore, this provided a general sense of
college students’ Internet usage patterns; namely, the frequencies indicated whether participants stay
connected to the Internet on the move, meaning Internet use had become part of their day-to-day
activities. Descriptive statistics demonstrated that participants accessed the Internet frequently on the
move (2–5 times daily) although they tended to use the Internet more in the place of residence (6–10
times daily). Taken together, this suggests that Internet use was an important part of participants’ daily
routine.

Analysis

Regression analyses were conducted to test proposed hypotheses. In terms of the first set of
hypotheses, the effect of Facebook use on the amount of received support was investigated when
controlling for the number of strong ties, gender, and age. In testing the second set of hypotheses about
the effect of social support through Facebook on life satisfaction, Facebook use and Internet use were
additionally controlled because these variables may affect the level of mental well-being as discussed.

Results

Descriptives on Social Support via Facebook

Descriptive statistics of received social support reveal that participants used Facebook as an
important venue for social support exchange. As seen in Table 3, participants regularly received various
forms of social support through Facebook. The mean of the amount of social support through Facebook
(mean = 29.91, SD = 9.55) is smaller than the amount of social support from all other means (mean =
36.09, SD = 8.63); however, the results clearly indicate that Facebook had become a significant tool for
social support exchange, given its considerably high mean value.

Facebook Use and Social Support Reception


2212 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

As reported in Table 6, Facebook use has a greater impact on social support reception than the
number of strong ties (β = 0.27, p < 0.001). Gender does not affect the amount of social support through
Facebook, but age is negatively associated with received social support, albeit the effect is statistically not
significant (β = –0.07, p < 0.10). Although this is a weak association, this result is still interesting given
the small age variation of the participant sample. It may imply that younger students incorporate
Facebook use more into their daily routines to exchange supportive messages online. In sum, H1 is
supported.

Second, Facebook use does not display a significant effect on social support received by other
means, indicating that Facebook-enabled communication does not facilitate supportive interactions
happening outside Facebook. Thus, H2 is not supported. However, the number of strong ties (β = 0.22, p
< 0.001) and gender (β = –0.13, p < 0.01) had a significant effect on social support received by other
means, echoing existing findings (Hurlbert, Hains, & Beggs, 2000; Wellman & Wortley, 1990). Women and
people with more strong ties are more likely to receive social support than others when they are using
various modes of communication such as face-to-face or mobile phones.

Table 6. Regressions Predicting the Amount of Received Social Support.

H1 H2
Social support on Facebook Social support by other means
Facebook intensity 0.27*** 0.05
Number of strong ties 0.23 ***
0.22***
Gender
–0.02 –0.13**
Female = 0
Age –0.07+ –0.05
F 29.56*** 12.82***
Adjusted R2 0.16 0.07
+
p < 0.10, *
p < 0.05, **
p < 0.01, ***
p < 0.001.

Social Support on Facebook and Life Satisfaction

The results of the regression analysis are displayed in Table 7. Analyses demonstrate that social
support received through Facebook has no significant effect on life satisfaction, rejecting H3. By contrast,
social support received by other means is positively associated with life satisfaction (β = 0.12, p < 0.01).
Hence, H4 is supported, implying that support via Facebook does not necessarily lead to promotion of
mental health, whereas other kinds of support may have positive impacts. Nonetheless, the results do not
explain much about the variance in life satisfaction. This is unsurprising, given that life satisfaction is
determined by a variety of factors such as social ties (Kahneman & Krueger, 2006), personality (Francis,
1999), communication frequency (Diener, Sandvik, & Payot, 1991) as well as the amount of social support
(Leung & Lee, 2005).
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2213

Table 7. Regressions Predicting Life Satisfaction.

H3 H4

Life Satisfaction

Social support on Facebook 0.01


Social support by other means 0.12**
Facebook intensity 0.07 0.07
Fixed Internet use –0.11* –0.11*
Mobile Internet use 0.03 0.03
Number of strong ties 0.21*** 0.20***
Gender
0.01 0.02
Female = 0
Age –0.01 –0.01
F 5.63*** 6.93***
Adjusted R2 0.05 0.06
*
p < 0.10, **
p < 0.01, ***
p < 0.001.

Discussion

This study examined the role of Facebook use in social support exchange as well as associated
health implications to confirm that Facebook use facilitates supportive interactions rather than attenuating
interpersonal relationships. The findings reveal that Facebook is a significant source of social support for
college-aged adults: The intensity of Facebook use contributes to the reception of social support
significantly greater than the number of strong ties. Given that social media allow individuals to stay
connected to a wider range of networks, social media can be a useful means of sharing one’s needs with
different social groups that potentially aid in resource acquisition. Nonetheless, this study does not find
statistical evidence for the impact of mediated social support on the level of life satisfaction, leading to
further inquiries about the health implications of Facebook use.

Specifically, the implications of this study are three-fold. First, it is worth highlighting that
Facebook use was a more influential factor than the number of strong ties in garnering social support. This
suggests that people who have a smaller network may benefit from active use of social media, which
enables them to reach out to others and mobilize support with relatively less effort, particularly in times of
need. In addition, network size should be understood in conjunction with other factors that may affect
social support reception, which calls for further research. Demonstrating that large or diverse networks do
not necessarily predict societal well-being, Hampton and Ling (2013) reject the taken-for-granted notions
of the advantages of a large network and frequent in-person contacts. As such, a nuanced approach is
needed when investigating the effects of media use and interpersonal networks on support reception and
mental well-being.

Second, the findings provoke an interesting inquiry about the role of gender in supportive
communication online. According to the results, there is no significant difference between women and men
2214 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

in terms of the amount of social support received through Facebook; however, women tend to receive
more social support when using other means. Studies have confirmed that women are more likely to show
supportive behaviors (Trobst, Collins, & Embree, 1994; Wellman & Wortley, 1990) and to seek social
support (Ashton & Fuehrer, 1993) compared with men. In a similar vein, men are less likely to focus on
emotions and to undertake the task of providing support when confronted with a distressed person
(Burleson, Holmstrom, & Gilstrap, 2005). The current study reinforces previous findings when it comes to
social support exchange outside of Facebook (e.g., face-to-face). However, this study reveals that gender
difference was mitigated and ineffective on Facebook, which implies that mediated environments may
offer men an opportunity to engage in interpersonal interactions without the psychological burdens related
to social support exchange (High & Caplan, 2009).

Third, the health implications of media use must be understood in light of the properties of the
social fabric to which individuals belong. According to the results, social support on Facebook has no
significant relationship to life satisfaction. This finding is germane to the nature of Facebook
communication, which is open to a wider social circle and often extended to public communication. Given
that supportive communication is concerned in many cases with sensitive topics, profound conversations
may occur in more private contexts through other channels. And whereas Facebook use certainly
increases the amount of social support, it is not necessarily connected to the quality of social support
despite its potential. There is also a possibility that social support received in the moment does not
necessarily lead to improved life satisfaction, although it certainly is a temporary coping aid. Thus, the
level of life satisfaction can be explained by more sustained, long-term support. Echoing this, the size of
one’s core network, which includes enduring ties, consistently has a positive effect on life satisfaction in
this study. Furthermore, the analyses reveal that fixed Internet use shows a negative association with life
satisfaction, whereas mobile Internet use has no such effect. As Ishii (2004) argues, mobile Internet use
has positive effects on sociability, whereas fixed Internet use does not. This study echoes this finding,
implying that Internet use on the move or in social surroundings may contribute to interpersonal
interactions. Although this study cannot claim the causality of these relationships, the findings suggest
that Internet use behaviors can be better understood in context. Rather than handling technology use as a
single variable, considering different contexts may allow for a closer examination and a deeper
understanding of the social outcomes of media use. In this regard, this study calls for more nuanced
approaches that consider various contexts to make sense of complicated patterns of media use and their
implications (see Hampton, Sessions, & Her, 2011; Hampton, Sessions Goulet et al., 2011).

Limitations and Future Research

As with any study, there are limitations in this research. First, this study looks only into college
students’ Facebook use. Although this group is one of the dominant populations on Facebook, future
research would benefit from inviting diverse populations, such as older adults or teenagers, who may
follow different usage patterns, to enhance the generalizability of the results. Second, Facebook non-users
can be included to further examine whether social support received through Facebook is an addition to in-
person social support or a replacement of it (e.g., whether non-users obtain a similar level of social
support). A comparative study of Facebook users and non-users would shed light on the nature of
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2215

supportive communication and the role of social media in social support exchange. Third, a closer
investigation of the characteristics of mediated social support is needed. What are motivations for using
Facebook to solicit and provide social support? How do people evaluate and make sense of mediated social
support? In particular, given that social support through Facebook did not contribute to mental well-being
in this study, research on support strategies and outcomes may offer explications of the psychological
dynamics of supportive communication online. In this regard, different purposes of social media use and
specific use patterns (e.g., commenting behaviors, Facebook group use, “friending” patterns) can be
included in the analysis to elucidate detailed relationships between Facebook use and its effect on
supportive communication. For instance, different activities on Facebook can have varied levels of
contribution to social support reception (Hampton et al., 2012); thus, further examination of use
strategies and behaviors of Facebook members may allow for a better understanding of which
mechanisms of Facebook use promote supportive relationships and psychological well-being.

Conclusion

Taken together, this study underlines the role of social media use in promoting the acquisition of
social aids by demonstrating how Facebook use enables people to obtain various kinds of social support.
Frequent occurrences of supportive communication on Facebook clearly indicate that individuals can
benefit from the active use of social media. Once incorporated into daily routines, social media use can
significantly contribute to lubricating interpersonal communication and diversifying strategies for providing
and receiving social support. To examine these processes, it is crucial to delve into the content and
contexts of use instead of assessing the frequency of use in general. Based on this perspective, this study
focused on supportive communication on social media as well as different contexts of Internet use. The
results highlight the importance of varied contexts of social support exchange, which may lead to different
health outcomes. As such, this study calls for further investigations of the social and health implications of
different patterns of new media use.
2216 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

References

Albrecht, T. L., & Adelman, M. B. (Eds.). (1987). Communicating social support. Newbury Park, CA: SAGE
Publications.

Ashton, W. A., & Fuehrer, A. (1993). Effects of gender and gender-role identification of participants and
type of social support resource on support seeking. Sex Roles, 28, 461–476.

Bambina, A. (2007). Online social support: The interplay of social networks and computer-mediated
communication. Youngstown, NY: Cambria Press.

Barrera, M., Jr. (1986). Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and models. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 14(4), 413–445.

Barrera, M., Jr., Sandler, I. N., & Ramsey, T. B. (1981). Preliminary development of a scale of social
support: Studies on college students. American Journal of Community Psychology, 9, 435–447.

Baym, N. K. (1998). The emergence of on-line community. In S. G. Jones (Ed.), Cybersociety 2.0:
Revisiting computer-mediated communication and community (pp. 35–68). Thousand Oaks, CA:
SAGE Publications.

Baym, N. K. (2010). Personal connections in the digital age. Cambridge, UK: Polity Press.

Baym, N. K., Zhang, Y. B., & Lin, M. (2004). Social interactions across media: Interpersonal
communication on the Internet, telephone, and face-to-face. New Media & Society, 6(3), 299–
318.

Beaudoin, C., & Tao, C-C. (2007). Benefiting from social capital in online support groups: An empirical
study of cancer patients. CyberPsychology & Behavior, 10(4), 587–590.

Bender, J. L., Jimenez-Marroquin, M-C., & Jadad, A. R. (2011). Seeking support on Facebook: A content
analysis of breast cancer groups. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 13(1). Retrieved from
http://www.jmir.org/2011/1/e16

Boase, J., Horrigan, J. B., Wellman, B., & Rainie, L. (2006). The strength of Internet ties. Washington, DC:
Pew Internet & American Life Project.

boyd, d. m., & Ellison, N. B. (2007). Social network sites: Definition, history, and scholarship. Journal of
Computer-Mediated Communication, 13(1), 210–230.

Braithwaite, D. O., Waldron, V. R., & Finn, J. (1999). Communication of social support in computer-
mediated groups for people with disabilities. Health Communication, 11(2), 123–151.
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2217

Burleson, B. R., Holmstrom, A. J., & Gilstrap, C. M. (2005). “Guys can’t say that to guys”: Four
experiments assessing the normative motivation account for deficiencies in the emotional support
provided by men. Communication Monographs, 72(4), 468–501.

Chen, W. (2013). Internet use, online communication, and ties in Americans’ networks. Social Science
Computer Review, 31(4), 404–423.

Cohen, S., Gottlieb, B. H., & Underwood, L. G. (2000). Social relationships and health. In S. Cohen, L. G.
Underwood., & B. H. Gottlieb (Eds.), Social support measurement and intervention (pp. 3–25).
New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

DeAndrea, D. C., Ellison, N. B., LaRose, R., Steinfield, C., & Fiore, A. (2012). Serious social media: On the
use of social media for improving students’ adjustment to college. Internet and Higher Education,
15, 15–23.

Diener, E., Emmons, R. A., Larsen, R. J., & Griffin, S. (1985). The satisfaction with life scale. Journal of
Personality Assessment, 49, 71–75.

Diener, E., Sandvik, E., & Pavot, W. (1991). Happiness is the frequency, not the intensity, of positive
versus negative affect. In F. Strack, M. Argyle, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Subjective well-being: An
interdisciplinary perspective (pp. 119–139). Oxford, UK: Pergamon Press.

Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2007). The benefits of Facebook “friends:” Social capital and
college students’ use of online social network sites. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 12, 1143–1168.

Ellison, N. B., Steinfield, C., & Lampe, C. (2011). Connection strategies: Social capital implications of
Facebook-enabled communication practices. New Media & Society, 13, 873–892.

Fabrigar, L. R., Wegener, D. T., MacCallum, R. C., & Strahan, E. J. (1999). Evaluating the use of
exploratory factor analysis in psychological research. Psychological Methods, 4(3), 272–299.

Francis, L. J. (1999). Happiness is a thing called stable extraversion: A further examination of the
relationship between the Oxford Happiness Inventory and Eysenck’s dimensional model of
personality and gender. Personality and Individual Differences, 26, 5–11.

Gore, S. (1981). Stress-buffering functions of social supports: An appraisal and clarification of research
models. In B. S. Dohrenwend & B. P. Dohrenwend (Eds.), Stressful life events and their contexts
(pp. 202–222). New York, NY: Prodist.

Granovetter, M. S. (1973). The strength of weak ties. American Journal of Sociology, 78(6), 1360–1380.
2218 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

Granovetter, M. S. (1983). The strength of weak ties: A network theory revisited. Sociological Theory, 1,
201–233.

Greene, J. A., Choudhry, N. K., Kilabuk, E., & Shrank, W. H. (2011). Online social networking by patients
with diabetes: A qualitative evaluation of communication with Facebook. Journal of General
Internal Medicine, 26(3), 287–292.

Hains, V. A., Hurlbert, J. S., & Beggs, J. J. (1996). Exploring the determinants of support provision:
Provider characteristics, personal networks, community contexts, and support following life
events. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 37(3), 252–264.

Hampton, K., & Ling, R. (2013). Explaining communication displacement and large-scale social change in
core networks. Information, Communication & Society, 16(4), 561–589.

Hampton, K., Sessions, L. F., & Her, E. J. (2011). Core networks, social isolation, and new media: Internet
and mobile phone use, network size, and diversity. Information, Communication & Society,
14(1), 130–155.

Hampton, K., Sessions Goulet, L., Marlow, C., & Rainie, L. (2012). Why most Facebook users get more
than they give. Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.

Hampton, K., Sessions Goulet, L., Rainie, L., & Purcell, K. (2011). Social networking sites and our lives.
Washington, DC: Pew Research Center.

Hampton, K., & Wellman, B. (2003). Neighboring in Netville: How the Internet supports community and
social capital in a wired suburb. City & Community, 2, 277–311.

Hargittai, E. (2007). Whose space? Differences among users and non-users of social network sites. Journal
of Computer-Mediated Communication, 13, 276–297.

Headey, B., Kelley, J., & Wearing, A. (1993). Dimensions of mental health: Life satisfaction, positive
affect, anxiety and depression. Social Indicators Research, 29, 63–82.

Henderson, S., Byrne, D. G., Duncan-Jones, P., Adcock, S., Scott, R., & Steele, G. P. (1978). Social bonds
in the epidemiology of neurosis: A preliminary communication. British Journal of Psychiatry, 132,
463–466.

High, A. C., & Caplan, S. E. (2009). Social anxiety and computer-mediated communication during initial
interactions: Implications for the hyperpersonal perspective. Computers in Human Behavior, 25,
475–482.

House, J. S. (1981). Work stress and social support. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2219

Hurlbert, J. S., Hains, V. A., & Beggs, J. J. (2000). Core networks and tie activation: What kinds of routine
networks allocate resources in nonroutine situations? American Sociological Review, 65, 598–
618.

Ishii, K. (2004). Internet use via mobile phone in Japan. Telecommunications Policy, 28, 43–58.

Kahneman, D., & Krueger, A. B. (2006). Developments in the measurement of subjective well-being.
Journal of Economic Perspectives, 20(1), 3–24.

Krackhardt, D. (1992). The strength of strong ties: The importance of philos in organizations. In N. Nohria
& R. G. Eccles (Eds.), Organizations and networks: Structure, form and action (pp. 216–239).
Boston, MA: Harvard Business School Press.

Kraut, R., Kiesler, S., Boneva, B., Cummings, J., Helgeson, V., & Crawford, A. (2002). Internet paradox
revisited. Journal of Social Issues, 58(1), 49–74.

Kraut, R. E., Patterson, M., Lundmark, V., Kiesler, S., Mukopadhyay, T., & Scherlis, W. (1998). Internet
paradox: A social technology that reduces social involvement and psychological well-being?
American Psychologist, 53, 1017–1031.

Leung, L., & Lee, P. S. N. (2005). Multiple determinants of life quality: The roles of Internet activities, use
of new media, social support, and leisure activities. Telematics and Informatics, 22, 161–180.

Lin, N., & Westcott, J. (1991). Marital engagement/disengagement, social networks, and mental health. In
J. Eckenrode (Ed.), The social context of coping (pp. 213–237). New York, NY: Plenum.

Lin, N., Woelfel, M. W., & Light, S. C. (1985). The buffering effect of social support subsequent to an
important life event. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 26, 247–263.

McCarty, C., Killworth, P. D., Bernard, R., Johnsen, E. C., & Shelley, G. A. (2001). Comparing two
methods for estimating network size. Human Organization, 60(1), 28–39.

McDowell, I. (2006). Measuring health: A guide to rating scales and questionnaires. New York, NY: Oxford
University Press.

Nabi, R. L., Prestin, A., & So, J. (2013). Facebook friends with (health) benefits? Exploring social network
site use and perceptions of social support, stress, and well-being. Cyberpsychology, Behavior,
and Social Networking, 16(10), 721–727.

Nie, N. H., Hillygus, D. S., & Erbring, L. (2002). Internet use, interpersonal relations, and sociability: A
time diary study. In B. Wellman & C. Haythornthwaite (Eds.), The Internet in everyday life (pp.
215–243). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
2220 Heewon Kim International Journal of Communication 8(2014)

Pearlin, L. I., Lieberman, M. A., Menaghan, E. G., & Mullan, J. T. (1981). The stress process. Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 22, 337–356.

Procidano, M. E., & Heller, K. (1983). Measures of perceived social support from friends and from family:
Three validation studies. American Journal of Community Psychology, 11(1), 1–24.

Quan-Haase, A., Wellman, B., Witte, J., & Hampton, K. N. (2002). Capitalizing on the Net: Social contact,
civic engagement, and sense of community. In B. Wellman & C. Haythornthwaite (Eds.), The
Internet in everyday life (pp. 291–324). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Rainie, L., Horrigan, J., Wellman, B., & Boase, J. (2006). The strength of Internet ties. Washington, DC:
Pew Internet & American Life Project.

Rains, S. A., & Young, V. (2009). A meta-analysis of research on formal computer-mediated support
groups: Examining group characteristics and health outcomes. Human Communication Research,
35, 309–336.

Robinson, J. P., Kestnbaum, M., Newstadtl, A., & Alvarez, A. S. (2002). The Internet and other uses of
time. In B. Wellman & C. Haythornthwaite (Eds.), The Internet in everyday life (pp. 244–262).
Malden, MA: Blackwell.

Sessions Goulet, L. (2012). Friends in all the right places: Social resources and geography in the age of
social network sites (Doctoral dissertation). University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA.

Stefanone, M. A., Kwon, K. H., & Lackaff, D. (2012). Exploring the relationship between perceptions of
social capital and enacted support online. Journal of Computer-Mediated Communication, 17,
451–466.

Stokes, J. P. (1983). Predicting satisfaction with social support from social network structure. American
Journal of Community Psychology, 11, 141–152.

Thoits, P. A. (2011). Mechanisms linking social ties and support to physical and mental health. Journal of
Health and Social Behavior, 52(2), 145–161.

Trobst, K. K., Collins, R. L., & Embree, J. M. (1994). The role of emotion in social support provision:
Gender, empathy, and expressions of distress. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 11,
45–62.

Valenzuela, S., Park, N., & Kee, K. F. (2009). Is there social capital in a social network site?: Facebook
use and college students’ life satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of Computer-Mediated
Communication, 14, 875–901.

Vangelisti, A. L. (2009). Challenges in conceptualizing social support. Journal of Social and Personal
International Journal of Communication 8 (2014) Social Media and Subjective Well-Being 2221

Relationships, 26(1), 39–51.

Vicary, A. M., & Fraley, R. C. (2010). Student reactions to the shootings at Virginia Tech and Northern
Illinois University: Does sharing grief and support over the Internet affect recovery? Personality
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(10), 1–9.

Wang, H., & Wellman, B. (2010). Social connectivity in America. American Behavioral Scientist, 53(8),
1148–1169.

Wellman, B., & Wortley, S. (1990). Different strokes from different folks: Community ties and social
support. American Journal of Sociology, 96(3), 558–588.

Wright, K. (1999). Computer-mediated support groups: An examination of relationships among social


support, perceived stress, and coping strategies. Communication Quarterly, 47, 402–414.

Wright, K. (2000a). Computer-mediated social support, older adults, and coping. Journal of
Communication, 50(3), 100–118.

Wright, K. (2000b). Perceptions of on-line support providers: An examination of homophily, source


credibility, communication and social support within online support groups. Communication
Quarterly, 48, 44–59.

Wright, K., & Bell, S. B. (2003). Health-related support groups on the Internet: Linking empirical findings
to social support and computer-mediated communication theory. Journal of Health Psychology,
8(1), 39–54.

You might also like