Origin of Vertebrate Telencephalon
Origin of Vertebrate Telencephalon
Origin of Vertebrate Telencephalon
Prof. Dr. M. FROTSCHER, Institut für Anatomie und Zellbiologie, Abteilung für Neuroanatomie,
Albert-Ludwigs-Universität Freiburg, Albertstr. 17, 79001 Freiburg, Germany
e-mail: [email protected]
Prof. Dr. D. E. HAINES, Ph.D., Department of Anatomy, The University of Mississippi Med. Ctr.,
2500 North State Street, Jackson, MS 39216-4505, USA
e-mail: [email protected]
Prof. Dr. E. MARANI, Department Biomedical Signal and Systems, University Twente,
P.O. Box 217, 7500 AE Enschede, The Netherlands
e-mail: [email protected]
Prof. Dr. Dr. h.c. T.H. SCHIEBLER, Anatomisches Institut der Universität,
Koellikerstraße 6, 97070 Würzburg, Germany
193
Advances in Anatomy
Embryology
and Cell Biology
Editors
F. F. Beck, Melbourne · F. Clascá, Madrid
M. Frotscher, Freiburg · D. E. Haines, Jackson
H.-W. Korf, Frankfurt · E. Marani, Enschede
R. Putz, München · Y. Sano, Kyoto
T. H. Schiebler, Würzburg
F. Aboitiz · J. Montiel
With 15 Figures
123
Francisco Aboitiz, Dr.
Departamento de Psiquiatría
Escuela de Medicina
Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile
Santiago
Chile
e-mail: [email protected]
ISSN 0301-5556
ISBN 978-3-540-49760-8 Springer Berlin Heidelberg New York
This work is subject to copyright. All rights reserved, whether the whole or part of the material is
concerned, specifically the rights of translation, reprinting, reuse of illustrations, recitation, broad-
casting, reproduction on microfilm or in any other way, and storage in data banks. Duplication of
this publication or parts thereof is permitted only under the provisions of the German Copyright Law
of September, 9, 1965, in its current version, and permission for use must always be obtained from
Springer-Verlag. Violations are liable for prosecution under the German Copyright Law.
Springer is a part of Springer Science+Business Media
springer.com
© Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2007
The use of general descriptive names, registered names, trademarks, etc. in this publication does not
imply, even in the absence of a specific statement, that such names are exempt from the relevant
protective laws and regulations and therefore free for general use.
Product liability: The publisher cannot guarantee the accuracy of any information about dosage and
application contained in this book. In every individual case the user must check such information by
consulting the relevant literature.
Editor: Simon Rallison, Heidelberg
Desk editor: Anne Clauss, Heidelberg
Production editor: Nadja Kroke, Leipzig
Cover design: WMX Design Heidelberg
Typesetting: LE-TEX Jelonek, Schmidt & Vöckler GbR, Leipzig
Printed on acid-free paper SPIN 11881063 27/3150/YL – 5 4 3 2 1 0
List of Contents
1 Introduction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 1
4 Discussion . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.1 An Overview . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.1.1 Early Neuronal Differentiation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 71
4.1.2 Patterning . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 72
4.1.3 Diversification of the Hemispheres and Neocortical Origins . . . . . . . . . . . . . 74
4.1.4 Olfaction, the Hippocampus and the Amygdala . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
4.1.5 Cortical Lamination . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 75
4.1.6 Tangential Expansion and the Origin of New Areas . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
4.2 Issues with Evolutionary Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
4.2.1 Genetic Conservatism Versus Morphological Diversity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 77
4.2.2 Development as a Clue to Evolution . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 78
4.2.3 Developmental Processes and Homology Criteria . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 79
4.2.4 Development, Adaptation, and Behavior . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 81
4.3 Final Comments . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 83
References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 85
Some of the results presented in this work have been financed by the Millenium
Nucleus for Integrative Neuroscience.
Introduction 1
1
Introduction
There is little doubt that the vertebrate brain is the most complex structure we
know. As with any complex structure, there is the immediate question about its
origins. How could such a complex design develop from the simplest multicellular
animals? This problem has pervaded the study of evolutionary biology since its
beginnings. Although Darwin (1859, 1871) proposed an impeccable mechanism
(natural selection) for the gradual transformation of species including human
origins, even he sometimes expressed certain doubts about the origin of highly
complex structures. This issue has been highly debated both within science and
outside it. For instance, a rebirth of the old religious argument of intelligent design
has gained unexpected strength in the last few years. In essence, this argument
follows Paley’s (1802) claim that if we find a clock that has been thrown away we
cannot consider that it was created on its own, but rather has to be the consequence
of conscious design. Today, creationists have developed a modern version of this
argument, that of “intelligent information” (Denton 2002). For example, after
sequencing the human genome in 2001, one of Celera Genomics top computer
scientists claimed that this complexity suggested design. Although he clarified not
to be thinking of God, he asserted that “there’s a huge intelligence there” (quoted
in Witham 2002, p 9). As Witham (2002) says, modern computer-literate believers
may soon ask the question of whether the universe is self-running or functioning
on DOS, a Divine Operating System.
In this volume, we have decided to tackle the problem of the origin and evolution
of the vertebrate brain, from the simplest nervous system-like elements that we
can observe in nature. In doing so, we expect to establish a continuity between
the simplest stages and the elaboration of the highly intricate neuronal network
that is the mammalian cerebral cortex. For lack of space, we will have to leave
aside several other brain structures such as the basal ganglia or the cerebellum,
as well as components perhaps comparable to the mammalian cerebral cortex in
other vertebrates; as will be seen, the cerebral cortex alone is sufficient to fill up
quite an extensive review. Our main point will be to present a case for continuous
evolutionary transformation of the central nervous system, from its very origins
to the elaboration of the most complex structure that exists on earth.
In order to pursue our goal, we will have to discuss some basic concepts of
neuroanatomy, embryology, and developmental genetics. This knowledge was un-
available in Darwin’s time, which further emphasizes his genius. We will follow
an approach that has been termed developmental evolutionary genetics, which
seeks to establish a correspondence between embryological processes and the
phylogenetic history of an organism. In other words, if we observe continuity in
development between a fertilized egg and an adult brain, we should also expect
continuity in its evolutionary history. This approach is not new; its roots can be
found in Von Baer’s biogenetic law (Von Baer 1828; see also Gould 1977), stating
that embryos start from a general condition (the unicellular egg), shared by all
2 Introduction
animals, and during development they progressively acquire characters that in-
clude them in successively restricted taxonomical groups. That is, if at early stages
all embryos are similar among them, they subsequently develop characters that
define them as say, vertebrates, then mammals, then primates and then as humans.
This view was further expressed in a more extreme version by Haeckel (1892), who
considered both evolution and development as a linear chain, embryology reca-
pitulating the phylogenetic history of the animal. Thus, the human embryo passed
through stages in which it was first a unicellular organism, then an undifferentiated
metazoan (morula), then a jellyfish (gastrula), then a fish, then a tailed reptile and
so on until he or she became a human. Modern understanding of these hypotheses
acknowledges that in fact, early embryos are readily distinguishable among them,
and that human embryos are human embryos during all development; they do
not pass from a jellyfish stage to a fish stage and so on (Garstang 1922; Gould
1977; Richardson et al. 1997). However, it is also recognized that embryos pass
through successive stages in which they acquire the characteristics proper to each
of the nested phylogenetic categories to which they belong. Thus, there is a general
concordance between embryonic stages and the phylogenetic history.
Recent expressions of this approach have taken the name of “evo-devo” (from
evolution and development) and have been particularly fruitful after the rise of
molecular embryology. This discipline has revealed an exquisite correspondence
in the molecular mechanisms underlying similar embryonic processes in a wide
group of animals, which nevertheless appear quite diverse in their superficial mor-
phology (McGinnis and Krumlauf 1992; Gerhart and Kirschner 1997; Martindale
2005; Pearson et al. 2005). Furthermore, the bulk of comparative molecular and
embryological evidence strongly points toward a relatively conserved embryonic
stage (the zootype) that corresponds to the establishment of the taxonomic group’s
body plan (Slack et al. 1993). In the case of vertebrates, this corresponds to the
point in which the embryo develops the pharyngeal pouches: the pharyngula
stage. Thus, there is high diversity in early developmental processes (mainly due
to species differences in yolk content and early embryonic adaptations), followed
by a convergence in structure at mid-developmental stages, in order to diverge
again as development proceeds toward the adult state. Interestingly, the expres-
sion of specific and highly conserved regulatory genes (homeobox and related
genes) takes place in this converging embryonic stage and participates in pattern-
ing the embryo’s body plan. Homeobox (Hox) and related genes have been found
to be fundamental in anteroposterior patterning in vertebrates, in the fruit fly
Drosophila and in other animal groups, indicating that they represent quite an an-
cient developmental regulatory system (McGinnis and Krumlauf 1992; Krumlauf
1992; García-Fernández and Holland 1994; Martindale 2005; Pearson et al. 2005;
García-Fernández 2005).
In a way, this evidence has produced a turn back to the times of the pre-
evolutionary concepts of Transcendental Anatomy, in which the architectural body
plans were considered to be established by divine intervention; diversity in design
was only the result of variations within a theme, due to adaptations to contingent
Introduction 3
and were highly conserved across vertebrate species (Wilkinson and Krumlauf
1990; Nieto et al. 1991; Kimmel 1993).
In the more anterior prosencephalon, which is of more immediate interest to us,
evidence for segmentation was for some time difficult to obtain. However, analyzing
the expression domains of several transcription factors that were activated in the
developing forebrain, Puelles and Rubenstein (1993, 2003) determined that there
was a nice fit with Bergquist and Källén’s description of prosomeric segments,
which is again largely conserved across vertebrates. While in the prosomeres there
is no expression of Hox genes, several homeodomain proteins are expressed, which
perform a similar role to that of Hox gene clusters in more caudal segments. For
example, genes of the Dlx and the Emx (Emx1 and Emx2) families are respectively
expressed in the ventral telencephalon (subpallium) and the dorsal telencephalon
(pallium). Dlx genes are closely related to Hox genes (they appear to belong to the
same cluster) and are homologs to the Drosophila gene distal-less (Dll), expressed
in the embryonic imaginal disks (Panganiban 2000). The linkage between Dlx and
Hox genes also exists in invertebrates such as the nematode, suggesting that the
primordial Dlx and Hox genes were similarly linked. Importantly, in vertebrate
origins there were two duplications of the Dlx-Hox cluster (Digregorio et al. 1995;
Stock et al. 1996). On the other hand, Emx genes are orthologs of the Drosophila
gene empty spiracles (ems), responsible for the formation of head segments (Dalton
et al. 1989; Walldorf and Gehring 1992; Hirth et al. 1995).
Puelles and Rubenstein’s (1993, 2003) prosomeric model has faced some crit-
icisms such as the evidence of cell migration across prosomere borders (but not
across rhombomere borders), the fact that early patterns of gene expression may
be quite dynamic and a static picture like the one presented does not capture
this dimension, or the possibility that the authors have disregarded evidence that
does not support the model (for review see Striedter 2005). However, comparative
evidence has determined that this pattern is highly conserved across vertebrates
(it is observed not only in mammals and birds, where it was first reported, but
also in teleosts and agnathans; Wulllimann and Puelles 1999; Pombal and Puelles
1999), which strongly suggests that it reflects a phylogenetically stable framework
that can be of great utility in species comparisons.
Before starting with our analysis, it will be useful to recall some basic concepts
of neuroanatomy and embryology. In the early embryo, the central nervous system
(CNS) originates from a flat neural plate in the dorsal aspect of the animal, after the
action of inductive signals from the mesoderm. In a process called neurulation,
this plate develops into a hollow neural tube. Eventually, this tube widens in the
cephalic region, forming three main vesicles, from caudal to rostral, the rhomben-
cephalon or hindbrain, the mesencephalon or midbrain, and the prosencephalon
or forebrain (Fig. 1). The latter subdivides into a diencephalon and two telen-
cephalic vesicles (the cerebral hemispheres) that contain the future olfactory bulbs,
the cerebral cortex and the basal ganglia, among other structures. The cerebral
hemispheres have been classically subdivided into a pallium located dorsally and
a ventral subpallium (basal ganglia), both separated by the corticostriatal sulcus
Introduction 5
Fig. 1 Early development of the mammalian forebrain. Initially, the forebrain is subdivided
into three main components, the segmented rhombencephalon (R); the mesencephalon (M)
that contains the main sensory structures in nonmammals; and the prosencephalon, which
is later partitioned into a diencephalon (D) and a telencephalon (Tel). E, eye; ON, optic
nerve
develop that collectively make up the so-called cerebral amygdala and other struc-
tures, containing both pallial and subpallial elements. Recent analyses have shown
that in this region an additional embryonic pallial element is distinguishable (the
ventral pallium). In the developing brain vesicle, as in the rest of the neural tube,
cell proliferation takes place in the inner walls, lining the ventricular cavity (the
ventricular and subventricular zones). Then, immature neurons migrate radially
out to make up distinct brain nuclei. In the cerebral cortex, this migration process
makes a long journey to establish a mantle of gray matter in the periphery of the
pallium. However, there is also migration of some cells in the tangential direction,
Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon 7
i.e., in a direction parallel to the surface of the neural tube, in which neurons
originating in the subpallium move dorsally to populate the cerebral cortex.
We will start the second section with a review of the phylogenetic history of the
nervous system from the earliest multicellular animals until the appearance and
diversification of the vertebrate telencephalon. In this section, we will demonstrate
a parallelism between some early embryological processes and the phylogenetic
events that took place during the evolution of the nervous system. In the third
section, we will discuss the origin of the mammalian brain and the mammalian
neocortex. In this part, we will address two main aspects, one concerning the
structure in the reptilian brain that gave rise to the neocortex (i.e., its reptilian
homolog) and the other concerning the origin of neocortical lamination. Finally, we
will discuss all this evidence in the light of current concepts in evolutionary theory.
The different sections will discuss a wealth of material, much of which may not
be retained in the reader’s mind while he or she goes on to the following sections.
In order to make this easier for the reader, each subsection is followed by a brief
summary of the main points discussed previously. Likewise, along the article, we
will be coming back to points made earlier in order to refresh the reader’s memory.
2
Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
2.1
Animal Phylogenetic Relationships
2.1.1
Origin and Diversification of Metazoans
i.e., in a direction parallel to the surface of the neural tube, in which neurons
originating in the subpallium move dorsally to populate the cerebral cortex.
We will start the second section with a review of the phylogenetic history of the
nervous system from the earliest multicellular animals until the appearance and
diversification of the vertebrate telencephalon. In this section, we will demonstrate
a parallelism between some early embryological processes and the phylogenetic
events that took place during the evolution of the nervous system. In the third
section, we will discuss the origin of the mammalian brain and the mammalian
neocortex. In this part, we will address two main aspects, one concerning the
structure in the reptilian brain that gave rise to the neocortex (i.e., its reptilian
homolog) and the other concerning the origin of neocortical lamination. Finally, we
will discuss all this evidence in the light of current concepts in evolutionary theory.
The different sections will discuss a wealth of material, much of which may not
be retained in the reader’s mind while he or she goes on to the following sections.
In order to make this easier for the reader, each subsection is followed by a brief
summary of the main points discussed previously. Likewise, along the article, we
will be coming back to points made earlier in order to refresh the reader’s memory.
2
Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
2.1
Animal Phylogenetic Relationships
2.1.1
Origin and Diversification of Metazoans
At least in three occasions, this plexus has become condensed into a chord at the
midline to form a central nervous system (Lowe et al. 2003; Holland 2003). This
is consistent with the embryological findings indicating that the default fate of
10 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
Fig. 4 Dorsoventral organization in arthropods and vertebrates. These animals are inverted
relative to each other not only in the topographical position of their organs but also in the
patterns of gene expression. ac-sc, achaete-scute; CNS, central nervous system; EN, endo-
derm; MES, mesoderm; N, neurogenin. (With permission from Gerhart and Kirchner 1997)
strate, this proposal is supported by the fact that in some burrowing invertebrates
it is not easy to determine the dorsal or the ventral surfaces, thus making inversion
a relatively likely event (Gerhart 2000). Furthermore, the deuterostome hemi-
chordates have been claimed to be inverted, upside-down versions of chordates,
showing a dorsoventral organization similar to that of protostomes (Nübler-Jung
and Arendt 1996). Unambiguous specification of dorsal and ventral structures is
not easy in hemichordates, and by convention the mouth side had been tradition-
ally considered to be ventral. However, Nübler-Jung and Arendt (1996) noted that
the position of the heart, kidney, and gill slits were located in the so-called dorsal
aspect of the animal (opposite to the mouth), while in chordates these organs are
positioned ventrally, close to the mouth. Moreover, in hemichordates a sensory
nerve chord runs ventrally while a motor chord runs dorsally, in an anatomic or-
ganization that is inverted with respect to chordates, in which sensory neurons are
dorsal to motor neurons. An intriguing detail is that the so-called dorsal tract—at
the other side of the animal’s mouth—is internalized in the collar region of the
body by a neurulation-like process (Willmer 1990).
Recent evidence suggests that the process of nervous system condensation
occurred independently in protostomes and deuterostomes and is posterior to
the acquisition of dorsoventral organization in bilaterians. In other words, the
common bilaterian ancestor of protostomes and deuterostomes may not have had
12 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
of vertebrates are all secondary neurons that lack axons (Holland 2005). Neverthe-
less, myomeric cells of cephalochordates, which are undoubtedly homologous to
the segmented musculature of vertebrates, send a projection to the neural tube,
which synapses on motor neurons (instead of motor neurons projecting an axon
to the muscle cell, as occurs in vertebrates; Wicht and Lacalli 2005). Therefore, the
presence or absence of a projection to the CNS may not be a determining factor
for establishing homology between these structures. One possibility is that the
ancestral subepidermal nerve net is really the precursor of true, delaminated neu-
rons, while the ciliated cells of the deuterostome larvae correspond more closely
to different types of localized receptors and to the ciliated cells at the interior of
the neural tube. An interpretation related to Garstang’s is that the vertebrate brain
derives from a ciliated apical organ found in some invertebrates like cnidarians
or ctenophorans, which would be comparable to the apical organ of deuterostome
larvae (Martindale 2005). In this context, developmental evidence suggests the
possibility that the most anterior parts of the CNS rely on different inducing sig-
nals than the more caudal regions, which depend on the notochord for induction.
The notochord, secreting Noggin, Chordin, and Follistatin, has its anterior end
at the level of the future thalamus, and the most anterior aspect of the neural
14 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
2.1.3
Summary
Vertebrates derive from a basal group of deuterostome animals, whose adult stage
was possibly free-living. A condensation of the diffuse subepidermal neural plexus,
perhaps produced by a reorganization of the blastula, yielded a dorsal central ner-
vous system, which became internalized to form a neural tube. Nevertheless, the
process of delamination of neuronal cells from a proliferating epithelium seems
to be conserved in invertebrates and vertebrates, as within the vertebrate neural
tube prospective neurons delaminate in a similar manner as they do in Drosophila
epidermis. Furthermore, the proneural genetic machinery, marking the beginning
of neural differentiation, is similar in both groups. Ciliated cells, either lining the
neural tube or specialized as localized sensory receptors, may be related to cili-
ated cells observed in deuterostome larvae, which serve as sensory receptors and
also have motile functions. Given that the cephalic end of the vertebrate CNS is
induced by different factors than the caudal end, it is possible that the forebrain
and neighboring regions have a slightly different origin from the more caudal
CNS, originating from an anterior or apical plexus that condensed dorsally. In this
context, the cephalic nervous system seems to be the only one present in deuteros-
tome larvae and in sessile deuterostomes, indicating that the two components of
the CNS are separable.
The Origin of Vertebrates 15
2.2
The Origin of Vertebrates
2.2.1
Early Theories and Fossil Evidence
Early ideas about the origin of vertebrates were largely based on Garstang’s hypoth-
esis that considered vertebrates and protochordates to result from a paedomorphic
transformation of the larval tunicates (i.e., the swimming larva was transformed
into a mobile, reproducing adult; Garstang 1928). However, more recent molecular
and morphological phylogenetic analyses suggest that a free-living, swimming,
or burrowing condition is ancestral to both deuterostomes and chordates, sessile
forms being derived states (see Lacalli 2005; Vargas and Aboitiz 2005). Further-
more, the recent proposal that urochordates are closer to vertebrates than cephalo-
chordates (Delsuc et al. 2006) is consistent with the possibility that both deuteros-
tomes and olfactores (urochordates and vertebrates) were primitively free-living
animals (the most basal form of ascidians, Oikopleura, also has an adult mobile
condition, implying that ancestral urochordates were also free-living; Gee 2006).
Beside embryological evidence, there is some fossil evidence for the origin of
vertebrates. The fossil record provides evidence of the very early appearance of
chordates during the Cambrian period, a time at which most extinct and living
animal phyla also originated (Gould 1989; Conway Morris 1993; Valentine 2004).
In the sediments of the Burguess Shale, a highly rich Cambrian fossil deposit, one
highly publicized fossil, Pikaia, allegedly bearing a segmented musculature and
possibly a notochord, was first claimed to be the earliest chordate (see Gould 1989;
Shimeld and Holland 2005). However, additional studies revealed that the affinities
of Pikaia with either cephalochordates or vertebrates were unclear (Schubert et al.
2006). Subsequently, an additional early fossil, Yunnanozoon, was first described
as a cephalochordate (Chen et al. 1995), then as a vertebrate (Dzik 1995) and later
on as the earliest hemichordate (Shu et al. 1996). Further interesting fossils are
Haikouichthys and the closely related Myllokunmingia, which have been claimed
to be already vertebrates with segmented, chevron-shaped myomeres, paired eyes,
otic capsules, and paired olfactory organs (Shu et al. 1999, 2003a). A problematic
fossil is Haikouella (Mallatt and Chen 2003b; Shu 2003), as some have interpreted
it either as an early deuterostome (Shu et al. 2003b; Shu and Conway Morris 2003)
or as a vertebrate-like chordate (Mallatt and Chen 2003a,b). According to the latter
interpretation, Haikouella had a large brain, lateral eyes, a pharynx with gill slits,
and a ventral heart. Finally, there are Cambrian records of calcified denticles – con-
odonts – that were initially attributed to early vertebrates, but conclusive evidence
for this link came from a nearly complete description of a specimen with a small
eel-like body, a caudal fin and fin rays (Briggs et al. 1983). Further analysis of
these animals revealed that they had well-formed eyes with extrinsic musculature,
comparable to those of agnathans (Briggs 1992; Sansom et al. 1992; Mallatt and
Chen 2003a,b). All this evidence indicates that the origin of vertebrates was an
16 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
2.2.2
The Neural Crest and Placodes in Vertebrate Origins
The neural crest (NC) and placodes (PL) are embryonic regions located in the
region bordering the neural plate, separating it from the rest of the ectoderm. The
NC differentiates at levels behind the future prosencephalon (cerebral hemispheres
and diencephalon), while the placodal region borders the anterior neural plate,
contributing the prospective prosencephalic territory. Cells from the NC migrate
laterally above and below the mesodermic somites to invade several regions of
the embryo’s body. In the trunk and body, these cells give rise to a large part of
the peripheral nervous system and other structures such as pigment cells and
neuroendocrine cells in the adrenal glands. In the cephalic region, NC cells con-
tribute to the branchial skeleton and part of the cranial skeletal tissue. Placodes
differentiate in several types, such as the olfactory placode (giving rise to olfac-
tory epithelium, whose receptors project directly into the olfactory bulb) and the
closely related adenohypophyseal placode (contributing to the anterior pituitary
gland). These structures develop in close contiguity with the olfactory bulb and the
hypothalamus, respectively (Couly and Le Douarin 1985), and in the adult become
closely associated with the anterior end of the neural tube, located in the roof
of the mouth. Other placodal structures are the optic (producing the eye’s lens),
trigeminal (contributing to cranial sensory ganglia), otic (future internal ear), and
epibranchial (contributing to other cranial sensory ganglia). In fish and amphib-
ians, there is also a lateral line placode, which generates the mechanoreceptive
and, in many cases, electroreceptive lateral line system.
In a now classical piece of work, Northcutt and Gans (1983) and Gans and
Northcutt (1983) showed that the majority of shared-derived characters of verte-
brates arise from the differentiation of the NC and PL. For example, the vertebrate
branchial skeleton is a NC-derived character, while the branchial organs of cephalo-
cordates have only a collagenous skeleton. Furthermore, vertebrate paired sense
organs such as eyes, ears, and the olfactory epithelium relate embryologically to
the epidermal placodes. The development of a muscularized branchial skeleton
permitting a more efficient oxygenation and new feeding possibilities, together
with the development of sense organs such as vision, olfaction, and the vestibular
system was possibly related to a more active, predatory mode of life as opposed
to the ancestral filter-feeding condition. Interestingly, Lacalli (2004) has proposed
that these innovations took place at similar times in both early vertebrates and
arthropods, leading to an “arms race” between both groups in their competition
for prey and possibly also in the establishment of predator–prey relations between
them, which speeded morphological evolution.
The Origin of Vertebrates 17
Since Northcutt and Gans’s (1983) proposal, several subsequent studies de-
termined that in protochordates there are tissues comparable to the NC and PL
(Holland et al. 1996, 2000; Baker and Bronner-Fraser 1997b; Graham, 2000; Shimeld
and Holland 2000, 2005; Jeffery et al. 2004) and a peripheral, visceral nervous sys-
tem, which however, differentiates in situ and does not derive from migratory
NC-like cells as in vertebrates (Lacalli 2004). This evidence is not inconsistent with
Northcutt and Gans’s (1983) original proposal; after all, morphological structures
and their ontogenies do not arise from nothing (Braun and Northcutt 1997). It is
now widely considered that the protochordate ancestor of vertebrates included cell
populations that modified their developmental pathways to yield definitive neural
crest and placodes in vertebrate origins (Shimeld and Holland 2005). Nevertheless,
in cephalochordates or urochordates these tissues never acquire the degree of com-
plexity observed in vertebrates, and their migratory capacities, although present in
some instances, are much more limited than in vertebrates. Interestingly, in the tu-
nicate Eitenascidia a cell population emerging from near the neural tube migrates
through the body and differentiates into pigment cells (Jeffery et al. 2004), which
again supports a close phylogenetic relation between urochordates and chordates.
During amphioxus neurulation, cells of the non-neural ectoderm bordering the
NP, and expressing NC markers such as AmphiDll (ADll, homologous to verte-
brate Dlxs, which mark NC and PL), develop lobopodia and crawl over the neural
plate to meet and fuse in the dorsal midline, but they never migrate to other sites
(Holland et al. 1996, 2000; Holland and Holland 2001). In amphioxus, ADll acts
as an ectodermal marker and its epidermal expression pattern is comparable to
the amphibian XDll2 (Dirksen et al. 1994) and to the zebrafish homolog gene Dlx3
(Akimenko et al. 1994). In higher craniates, Dlx3 is an epidermal marker (Porteus
et al. 1991, 1994; Bulfone et al. 1993b). Nevertheless, in amphioxus, ADll is also
expressed in the alar region of the presumptive forebrain, including the alar region
of the ventral forebrain (Bulfone et al. 1993a,b). In the mid-neurula, cells near the
anterior end of the neural plate become ADll-positive and are incorporated into
the dorsal neural tube. These cells and others that express the gene somewhat later
demarcate a region comprising the anterior three-fourths of the amphioxus cere-
bral vesicle, which has been claimed to be homologous to the craniate forebrain
(Holland et al. 1996). This evidence underlines the close relation between Dll/Dlx
genes and both the developing forebrain and vertebrate neural crest and placodes.
While amphioxus has only one ADll gene, this gene duplicated several times in
vertebrate origins, each new gene becoming restricted to specific tissues in which
the ancestral Dll gene was expressed (for example, Dlx3 in the epidermis, Dlx5 in
the placodal region, and Dlx1/2 in the ventral telencephalon; Holland et al. 1996).
In this context, it is of interest to recall that Dlx genes are closely related to Hox
genes and that vertebrate origins are marked by two duplications of the Dlx–Hox
cluster (Digregorio et al. 1995; Stock et al. 1996). Developmentally, Dlx genes are
expressed more anteriorly than Hox genes, which in the CNS are active at the level
of the hindbrain and spinal cord (Kraus and Lufkin 1999). While the Hox cluster
may have been involved in patterning the trunk region and the posterior CNS, Dlx
18 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
genes were more related to the development of the PL-derived sensory organs and
the telencephalon and related cephalic structures.
In a recent review, Meulemans and Bronner-Fraser (2005) proposed that in ver-
tebrate origins, recruitment to the neural plate border of some mesendodermal
genes activated primitive programs in NC precursors, expanding their develop-
mental potential. According to these authors, the ancestral chordate possessed
a neural plate border including lateral neural plate (expressing the neural plate
border specifiers Zic, Pax3/7 and Msx, and the NC specifier Snail) and adjacent
epidermal ectoderm (expressing the neural plate border genes Pax3/7, Msx, and
Dlx3/5). In ascidian urochordates, Snail, Msx, and Zic homologs are expressed
within the neural plate. In cephalochordates, Pax3/7 and Msx are not expressed in
neural plate borders. In vertebrates, neural plate border cells expressing Zic, Snail,
Pax3/7, and Msx also express neural crest specifiers, consistent with co-option of
these genes in the vertebrate lineage. In our view, this evidence points to a closer
similarity of inducing mechanisms between urochordates and vertebrates than
between cephalochordates and vertebrates, thus supporting a close phylogenetic
relation between urochordates and vertebrates (Delsuc et al. 2006).
Furthermore, in both larval tunicates and cephalochordates, there are primitive
sensory organs that serve as navigational aids and may be related to vertebrate
sensory organs (despite some homologic uncertainties). Although there is species
variability, in all larval ascidians there is a balance organ (an otolith) and an ocellus
with a pigment cup, both located in the brain vesicle. In some species, there are
additional sensory structures (Lacalli 2001). Amphioxus has a frontal, medial eye,
consisting of an accumulation of photoreceptive cells in the ventral anterior part of
the forebrain, and a so-called lamellar body more posteriorly and located dorsally
(homologous to the vertebrate pineal organ), plus dispersed photoreceptive cells
along the spinal cord (the Joseph cells and the organs of Hesse; Wicht and Lacalli
2005); and a putative balance sensory organ consisting of the preinfundibular
ciliary bulb cells (Lacalli and Kelly 2003). Most of these structures are found inside
the cephalic tube, and if the homology with vertebrate sensory organs is correct, it
would imply that the differentiation of head placodes permitted the exteriorization
of some sensory organs (such as the inner ear and the olfactory epithelium), and
the generation of paired eyes, directed to the periphery.
In his more recent update of the theory, Northcutt (2005) rejected his original
proposal regarding the origin of the neural crest from a subepidermal plexus
comparable to that of hemichordates. As mentioned above, he instead suggested
that the NC and PL arose as a consequence of a redistribution of the presumptive
territories in the early blastula. This view is partly consistent with the evidence
indicating quite an early embryonic origin of the NC territory, at or even before
gastrulation (Basch et al. 2006). In our view, although the modification of the
gastrula fate maps of deuterostomes could indeed be related to the origin of
neurulation and to the presumptive territory of the NC, much embryological
evidence also points to an important interaction between the ectoderm and the
neural plate during NC and PL differentiation (Baker and Bronner-Fraser 1997a;
The Origin of Vertebrates 19
Deardorff et al. 2001; García-Castro et al. 2002; Gammill and Bronner-Fraser 2003;
Woda et al. 2003). Thus, while NC and PL precursor tissues possibly appeared
in evolution at quite an early stage, concomitant with the process of neurulation,
the expansion and differentiation of these tissues in vertebrate origins may have
largely depended on neural–epithelial (and mesodermal) interactions, not being
a direct consequence of the derived organization of the early blastula.
2.2.3
Olfactory Placode and Epithelium: Association with Adenohypophysis
Fig. 6 Scheme of a sagittal secion through the cerebral vesicle of the cephalochordate
amphioxus. Ventrally there is a cluster of photoreceptors and more dorsally there is a small
opening, Hatscheck’s pit, which has been considered by some authors a precursor of the
nasohypophyseal duct of early vertebrates. (Modified from Wicht and Lacalli 2005, with
permission)
The Origin of Vertebrates 21
2.2.4
Origin of the Telencephalon
Another claim made by Nortchcutt and Gans (1983) and by Northcutt (1996a,
2005) regarding vertebrate origins is that the rostral head of vertebrates is a neo-
morphic unit. This statement implies that, together with the NC and PL derivatives,
most anterior brain structures including pretectum, epithalamus adenohypoph-
ysis, preoptic area, telencephalic hemispheres and olfactory sensory organs arose
at relatively the same time. Intermediate forms bearing only some of these char-
acters are considered unlikely to have existed. Alternatively, Butler (2000a,b; 2006)
proposed a sequential acquisition of the different neural characters in vertebrate
origins, with an initial condition similar to that found in the extant amphioxus,
where a frontal eye projects to the tectal region. In an intermediate stage, paired
eyes developed, inducing the origin of a diencephalon. According to Butler (2006),
this stage is perhaps comparable to that of the fossil Haikouella (and maybe to con-
odonts, who also had paired eyes). The final stage is marked by the origin of paired
telencephalic vesicles in the dorsal forebrain. We somewhat agree with Butler in
that this alternative is more likely to occur than a sudden transformation yielding
all characters. However, a macrotransformation is not an impossible event, and
more evidence is needed to determine which hypothesis is correct. Further analyses
of fossil material such as Haikouella or conodonts may help resolve this issue.
Developmental evidence indicates a close relationship between telencephalic
and placodal territories, especially with the olfactory placode. The differentiation
of the forebrain and olfactory bulb depend on inductive interactions with the
olfactory placode (Gong and Shipley 1995; De Carlos et al. 1995; Reiss and Burd
1997). Moreover, at least in zebrafish the presumptive telencephalic territory limits
rostrally with that of the preplacodal region, especially with the olfactory placode,
and there is initially an important overlap between them (Whitlock 2004). Both
NC/PL and the telencephalon are partly determined by regulatory genes of the Dlx
family, such that NC and PL are specified by Dlx genes (particularly Dlx5 in the
placodal region), while at later stages, the ventral telencephalon expresses the genes
Dlx1 and Dlx2 (Holland et al. 1996; Whitlock 2004; see Sect. 2.2.2). Therefore, it is
conceivable that the differentiation of the placodal region yielding sensory systems,
and of the telencephalon, were sequential but closely related acquisitions, in which
one innovation facilitated the development of a second innovative feature.
A related issue concerns the existence of precursors for the telencephalon in non-
vertebrates. In this sense, both Northcutt (1996a,b; 2005) and Butler (2000a,b; 2006)
agree that there are no signs of a telencephalic structure in living nonvertebrate
deuterostomes (Northcutt 2005, claims that even the presence of a mesencephalon
is questionable in amphioxus). Nevertheless, recent comparative developmental
evidence has revealed that embryonic territories comparable to the forebrain can
be found in urochordates and in cephalochordates. Molecular genetic analyses of
the brains of larval urochordates, cephalochordates, and developing vertebrates in-
dicate a strong conservatism of the gene expression patterns in the rostrocaudal di-
22 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
mension of the neural tube (Fig. 7). As in vertebrates, Hox genes pattern the caudal-
most aspects of the cephalochordate and tunicate neural tube, while the anterior
neural tube (the brain vesicle) is marked by the expression of the forebrain marker
Otx2 (Williams and Holland 1996). Furthermore, in amphioxus there is a region in
the anteriormost neural tube expressing the gene Brain factor 1 (BF1), which is also
expressed in the vertebrate ventral telencephalon (Toresson et al. 1998), and we have
mentioned the expression of the Dlx-like gene ADll in the presumptive alar fore-
brain (Sect. 2.2.2). Amphioxus also expresses markers of the dorsal telencephalon
of vertebrates. This animal has one T-box gene (see Sect. 2.1.2), which is initially ex-
pressed in the archenteron and is downregulated in the neurula, but later appears in
the preoral pit (Hatscheck’s pit in the adult), which, as we have seen, is claimed to be
comparable to the olfactory/adenohypothyseal placode of vertebrates (Satoh et al.
2002). In other deuterostomes (echinoderms and hemichordates), the T-box gene
is also expressed in the archenteron (Satoh et al. 2002). In vertebrates, there was
a duplication of the T-box gene (Tbr1 and Tbr2), where Tbr2 is confined to the endo-
derm and Tbr1 expression is restricted to the olfactory/hypophyseal placode, but
later both genes appear in the dorsal telencephalon, since they are required for the
development of projection neurons, reelin expression, and proper laminar develop-
ment of the cerebral cortex. Thus, the duplication of Tbr genes in vertebrate origins
may have permitted the co-option of Tbr1 for brain and especially for telencephalic
development. Moreover, this evidence further suggests a close relation between the
olfactory placode, the adenohypophyseal placode, and early telencephalic markers.
Finally, in amphioxus, the notochord reaches levels in front of the neural tube,
supporting it in its entire length. This condition has led some to conclude that
the neural tube of amphioxus may not include CNS structures that in vertebrates
are positioned anterior to the notochord, like the forebrain. However, in larval
urochordates (which again may be closer to vertebrates than amphioxus; Delsuc
et al. 2006), the notochord ends behind the visceral ganglion, which is located
posterior to the cerebral vesicle, a condition reminiscent of that in embryonic
vertebrates, in which the notochord ends at the level of the thalamus (Sect. 2.1.2).
It is thus possible that the position of the notochord in amphioxus represents
a derived condition.
2.2.5
Summary
placodal domain and localize outside the CNS. The origin of the olfactory epithe-
lium is not yet clear, but is apparently related to the adenohypophysis, which has
been associated with the preoral or Hatscheck’s pit of amphioxus (although there
are dissenting opinions). In the amphioxus forebrain, there are some telencephalic
24 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
Fig. 8 Diagram depicting the main characters of the ancestral vertebrate brain, bearing
paired sense organs, an olfactory system, and paired cerebral hemispheres. The animal also
had a notochord, segmented musculature, and a branchial skeleton. Note that contrary to
amphioxus, in which the notochord reaches the anterior tip of the animal, the notochord
does not reach the forebrain, which is supported by a prechordal platform. (Modified with
permission from Lacalli 2005)
2.3
Evolution of the Telencephalon in Vertebrates
2.3.1
Taxonomical Relationships Among Vertebrates and Their Early Evolution
Agnathans are subdivided into the more primitive myxinoids such as the hag-
fish, and cyclostomes such as the lamprey. Among gnathostomes, chondrychthian
fishes are represented by sharks, rays, and related cartilaginous fishes, while the
osteichthies are the bony fish like trouts and most fish we know. Sarcopterygians
are a group of bony fish that are considered related to the ancestors of terrestrial
vertebrates, as they bear lobed fins that resemble primordial limbs, and some have
developed lungs to breathe air. Terrestrial vertebrates are divided into anamniotes
(amphibians), who put their eggs in water and have a larval aquatic stage; and
amniotes (reptiles, birds, and mammals), whose eggs are covered by an amniotic
fluid and can be placed outside water. The amniotic cavity locates either within the
egg shell such as in reptiles, birds, and monotreme mammals, or develops inside
the maternal uterus such as in marsupials and placental mammals (see Fig. 9).
As mentioned, some of the earliest vertebrate fossils appear to be represented
by the conodonts, soft-bodied vertebrates that had not yet developed an ossified
skeleton. Skeletal agnathan fossils date from the early Silurian, and are commonly
termed ostracoderms (shell-skinned), as they developed an extensive exoskele-
ton that could have the form of a solid carapace, large bony plates, or scales
(Carroll 1988). These animals were probably not excellent swimmers, but those
with light, flexible armors and a fusiform body may have lived in the open water.
Those with heavier armors and a dorsoventrally flattened body were probably
benthonic. Ostracoderms clearly show paired eyes, large semicircular canals, and
nasal openings, indicating a well-developed olfactory system. Two major groups of
ostracoderms have been recognized according to the structure of their nasal open-
ings: the pteraspids or diplorhins were considered to represent the more primitive
condition in which there are paired nasal sacs and external narial openings. On
the other hand, the presumably more advanced cephalaspids or monorhins had
Fig. 9 Phylogenetic relations among vertebrates. Turtles (Chelonia) are now considered to
be more related to lepidosaurians than to stem reptiles
26 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
a single, medial nasal sac and narial opening that is confluent with the hypophyseal
duct. Some of these animals may have developed electric organs.
Hagfishes, considered to represent the most basal group of living agnathans,
were initially proposed to evolve from the more primitive diplorhins (Stensio 1964).
In fact, myxinoids were considered to represent a primitive clade, the outgroup
of the clade formed by lampreys and jawed vertebrates. On the other hand, the
configuration of the endocranial cavities of monorhins suggested a brain structure
similar to that of the lamprey Petromyzon. In this animal, the anterior portion of
the brain stem is expanded laterally to locate the telencephalon just below the nasal
cavity that leads to the nasohypophyseal opening, which continues ventrally into
the area of the hypophysis (Moy-Thomas and Miles 1971). More recent reports
suggest that modern agnathans have no direct relations with fossil ostracoderms,
being more basal than these and related the fossils like Haikouichthys. The rest of
the vertebrates are an outgroup of cyclostomes, and subdivide into conodonts and
ostracoderms. Jawed vertebrates originat as a sister group of osteostracans. a clade
of monorhin ostracoderms (Gess et al. 2006).
One of the most important advances among primitive vertebrates was the origin
of jaws, allowing a truly predatory way of life. The earliest jawed fishes appear in
the lower Silurian period. These are called placoderms, but are not closely related
to modern cartilaginous or bony fish. Most were benthonic and probably poor
swimmers, despite the clear presence of pectoral fins. They had no teeth but rather
bony plates in the margin of their jaws.
Beside the fossil placoderms, two major radiations took place: cartilaginous
fishes and bony fishes. Although these two groups differ in many respects, the
structure of their jaw and the pattern of tooth replacement indicate that they
have a common ancestry that is separate from that of the placoderms (Carroll
1988). These two taxa also share the absence of an exoskeleton and the presence
of well-developed paired fins. Both groups developed as good swimmers, but
bony fish developed a swimbladder, while cartilaginous fishes attempted to solve
the problem by developing cartilage instead of bone, and a large, oil-filled liver
that greatly reduces their specific gravity. Early cartilaginous fish radiated in the
Devonian period, producing a series of different families with unclear relationships
among them. As mentioned, the extant members of this class include sharks, rays,
and the deep-sea holocephalians.
On the other hand, osteichthyes or bony fish became an extremely diverse group
that has dominated marine and freshwater environments since the paleozoic. This
class is divided into two major subclasses, the highly diversified ray-finned or
actinopterygian fishes such as the trout and the much less common lobe-finned
or sarcopterygian fishes, including the coelacanth and the lungfish. The lobe fins
of the latter represent the ancestry of the limbs of terrestrial vertebrates. Fur-
thermore, considering that the teleostean swimbladder is a derivative of the lung
sacs of lobe-finned fishes, it is possible that the common ancestor of ray-finned
and lobe-finned fishes was phenotypically close to the early members of the sar-
copterygia. In fact, the more basal extant members of the actinopterygia (polypter-
Evolution of the Telencephalon in Vertebrates 27
netic analyses mentioned above, turtles may also belong to the diapsids. There
is also the subclass parapsida, represented by fossil marine reptiles (Clack, 2002;
Carroll 1988).
The last reptilian subclass is represented by the synapsid condition (with a cra-
nial opening in the cheek region of the skull), exemplified by the mammal-like
reptiles from which the first mammals emerged. Synapsids appear at about the
same time as the stem anapsids: Protoclepsydrops is considered to be a synapsid
as old as the early anapsid Hylonomus (Carroll 1988), but the classification of the
former has not been confirmed (Reisz 1986). In any case, there are indisputable
synapsids of the pelicosaur group only slightly younger than Hylonomus, namely
Archaeothyris (Reisz 1972). The general consensus is that amniotes are a mono-
phyletic group that nevertheless diverged into the stem reptiles (protorothyridids)
and the mammal-like synapsids (pelicosaurs) at quite an early stage.
2.3.2
Evolution of the Cerebral Hemispheres: Ventral Telencephalon
of the pretectal pathway from the basal ganglia to the optic tectum (in mammals,
the optic tectum is significantly reduced in relative size) and may be concomitant
with the development of the mammalian corticospinal tract. In reptiles, projec-
tions from the basal ganglia to the dorsal thalamus appear to be less developed,
although there has been an independent development of pallidothalamic connec-
tions in birds (Brauth 1990). Another difference in output relates to the pathways
connecting the basal ganglia with the mesencephalic optic tectum. There are mul-
tiple pathways for this connection, of which the most important are a ventral route
via the substantia nigra, which is present in all tetrapods, and a dorsal route via the
pretectal nuclei. The pretectal pathway is most developed in anurans, some lizards,
turtles, crocodiles, and birds, although it is weak or absent in urodeles, some lizards,
snakes, and mammals, suggesting that it is a highly variable trait (Marín et al. 1998).
The ventral telencephalon of jawed vertebrates also contains structures like the ol-
factory tubercle in the ventral striatum, which receives olfactory information and
has relations with the olfactory cortex and the olfactory nucleus, corresponding to
lateral and medial pallial components, respectively (Butler and Hodos 1996).
2.3.3
The Brain of Jawless Fishes and the Organization of the Ancestral Dorsal Telencephalon
Myxinoids have a highly distorted brain in which there are four paired ventricles
in the embryo (two telencephalic and two diencephalic); these ventricles become
obliterated in the adult, and there is a fusion of the anterior walls of the diencephalic
vesicle with the posterior walls of the telencephalic vesicle. This region gives rise
to an evaginated component termed the central prosencephalic complex, which
includes paired cerebral ventricles containing both telencephalic and diencephalic
components, but its functions and connectivity remain obscure. This component
has been compared to the medial pallium of other vertebrates (Jansen 1930),
but this proposal has been challenged (Wicht and Northcutt 1998). Nevertheless,
recent embryological evidence indicates that the medial pallium corresponds to
the caudal forebrain, and it is separated from the more posterior thalamus by
the zona limitans intrathalamica (Kimura et al. 2005). Therefore, the topographic
position of the hagfish medial prosencephalic complex might be consistent with
the embryonic position of the medial pallium. Furthermore, the zona limitans
intrathalamica of mammals might perhaps be comparable to the region in which
the telencephalic and diencephalic walls fuse in myxinoids, as this region is a focus
of extensive neuronal proliferation and migration.
The pallium of agnathans receives an extensive olfactory projection, covering it
almost completely (Northcutt 1996a; Northcutt and Puzdrowski 1988; Wicht and
Northcutt 1992, 1993, 1998; Wicht 1996; Northcutt and Wicht 1997). The olfactory
bulbs are particularly well developed in both myxinoids and cyclostomes, but in the
latter olfactory structures resemble much more closely those of jawed vertebrates.
In hagfishes, the olfactory bulbs receive numerous olfactory nerve bundles (about
ten per side) and project to the telencephalon in three main bundles: the medial,
30 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
the lateral, and the ventral olfactory tracts, respectively innervating the septum,
the pallium, and the basal forebrain. The pallium of hagfishes is unique in that it
has a superficial cortical mantle of gray matter that is subdivided into several layers
or fields. It also has reciprocal connections with diencephalic regions including
the dorsal thalamus, with a strong tectothalamic-telencephalic component, and
has interhemispheric connections (Wicht and Northcutt 1993, 1998). Despite this
complexity, in myxinoids it is not possible yet to recognize ventral, lateral, dorsal,
and medial components with certainty (Myojin et al. 2001), which has led some
authors to propose that the entire pallium is homologous to the lateral pallium
of higher vertebrates (see Butler and Hodos 1996). (As mentioned, these animals
also have the above-mentioned central prosencephalic complex, which might be
comparable to the medial pallium.)
Although in the lamprey the olfactory bulbs are clearly evaginated, evagination
is not complete in the caudal telencephalon, forming the telencephalon impar in
which the two hemispheres are fused in the midline (Nieuwenhuys and Nicholson
1998). In the lamprey, this structure contains the septum (ventral pallium), the
primordium hippocampi (dorsal pallium), and parts of the corpus striatum (sub-
pallium). In elasmobranchs, dipnoans, the coelacanth Latimeria and amphibians,
a telencephalon impar can still be observed, but is much less extensive and most
telencephalic structures are evaginated. Lampreys already show a subdivision into
at least medial, dorsal, and lateral pallium. A ventral pallial field has not been yet
reported in agnathans, but if fate-mapping evidence is correct in positioning the
olfactory bulb in this subdivision (Cobos et al. 2001), this should also be a primitive
vertebrate characteristic. As in most jawed fishes, in the lamprey, dorsal thalamic
projections, particularly from the tectothalamic route, end in the small medial pal-
lium, which nonetheless also receives olfactory projections (Northcutt and Wicht
1997). Most olfactory projections end in the expanded lateral pallium, and there
is a subhippocampal lobe that has been homologized to the gnathostome dorsal
pallium, also receiving olfactory input. Therefore, while the agnathan pallium is
entirely olfactory, there is also a substantial diencephalic projection, which in hag-
fishes is spread over the pallium, overlapping with secondary olfactory afferents,
but in lampreys is confined to the medial pallium (where it also overlaps with
less dense secondary olfactory projections; Northcutt and Wicht 1997; Wicht and
Northcutt 1998). The projection from dorsal thalamus to the medial pallium is
shared with several gnathostomes, excluding amniotes (Northcutt 1995).
This evidence led Wicht and Northcutt (1998) to propose a tentative morpho-
type of the ancestral gnathostome brain, in which extensive olfactory projections
to the pallium and also thalamopallial projections are a primitive character for ag-
nathans. Nevertheless, ascending spinal or bulbar lemniscal systems do not reach
the dorsal thalamus, which is innervated mainly by tectothalamic or collothala-
mic projections (lemniscal systems end in the mesencephalic tegmentum, which
in turn projects to the thalamus; Ronan and Northcutt 1990; Butler and Hodos
1996). This condition is also observed in most fish (with some exceptions), with
tetrapods the first group in which the development of a strong ascending lemnotha-
Evolution of the Telencephalon in Vertebrates 31
lamic system is consistently evident (Ronan and Northcutt 1990; Muñoz et al. 1994,
1997). Nevertheless, there are direct retinothalamic visual projections that reach
the telencephalon at least in all jawed vertebrates (cartilaginous fish: Ebbesson
and Schroeder 1971; Schroeder and Ebbesson 1974; Smeets 1990; Northcutt 1990b;
bony fish: Nieuwenhuys and Meek 1990; tetrapods Butler, 1994a,b).
Finally, in all vertebrates, the pallium also receives projections from the preoptic
region, and there is a projection from the posterior tuberculum to the subpallium
and sometimes to the pallium (in agnathans, Polenova and Vesselkin 1993; Wicht
and Northcutt 1998). Furthermore, the pallium projects to the thalamus and tectum
in hagfish and lamprey, indicating that these connections are primitive.
2.3.4
The Pallium in Jawed Vertebrates
An early scenario of gnathostome brain evolution implied that the pallium was
dominated by olfactory afferents in most fishes, while in terrestrial vertebrates,
thalamic projections restricted the extension of olfactory afferents, confining them
to the lateral pallium and related structures (Ebbesson and Heimer 1970; Smeets
1983). However, it was later found that all jawed fishes display several pallial fields
(medial, dorsal, lateral and more recently, ventral), and an olfactory projection
restricted to lateral regions (for review see Northcutt 1995; Northcutt and Kaas
1995), which dramatically changed the concept of early brain evolution.
The acquisition of predatory lifestyles by the early gnathostome vertebrates,
involving the further development of other sensory modalities, implied the pro-
gressive development of ascending visual, somatosensory, and lateral line afferents
to the pallium via the dorsal thalamus. The expansion of these sensory projections
was concomitant with the enlargement of the telencephalic components receiving
the respective inputs (Northcutt 1981; Northcutt and Puzdrowski 1988; Wicht and
Northcutt 1992, 1993; Striedter 1997; Wicht 1996). With the exception of amphib-
ians, which are considered to have a secondarily simplified brain (Northcutt 1981;
Neary 1990), this phenomenon is also evident among terrestrial vertebrates. Nev-
ertheless, this expansion process appears to have been a relatively late event in the
evolution of each vertebrate class (the basal exponents usually bear quite simple
brains), and pallial morphology evolves via clearly divergent lines in each group.
In cartilaginous fishes, a division between a pallium and a ventral subpallium
is generally acknowledged, but there is no agreement on their precise boundaries.
The pallium is subdivided into medial (hippocampal), lateral (piriform), and the
dorsal or general pallium between them. The latter component occupies most of
the telencephalic roof and is further divided into an internal or central component
(which is the largest and located in the midline; recall the telencephalon impar of
cyclostomes), and a superficial or cortical region. Little is known about the afferents
to this region, but its output is directed to the hypothalamus. As mentioned, the
medial pallium receives most collothalamic sensory projections from the dorsal
thalamus, and the lateral pallium receives most olfactory projections, together with
32 Evolution of the Vertebrate Nervous System and Telencephalon
connections from some ventral telencephalic structures including the septum and
the superficial striatum (Smeets 1990).
The brains of bony fish are noticeable because their hemispheres do not show the
typical evaginated appearance, but their medial pallia separate from the midline,
opening the ventricular cavity, which remains covered by a thin layer of choroid
plexus (Butler 2000c). This process, termed eversion, results in the inversion of the
topography of cell masses, where the ventricular zone becomes the most superficial
aspect and the pial surface becomes internal to it. This is reminiscent of a condition
in which the neural tube has failed to close in its anterior region (Nieuwenhuys
and Meek 1990). Interestingly, a relatively similar condition is observed in the
holocephalans (basal cartilaginous fishes), in the lobe-finned coelacanth and in
the lungfish Neoceratodus; in these species, the hemisphere walls are separated and
are connected by an extensive ependymal membrane (Smeets 1998; Nieuwenhuys
1998d,e). This perhaps suggests that the brain of ancestral gnathostomes had its
medial pallia largely separated at the midline by an extensive mantle of choroid
plexus. This condition may have facilitated the eversion process in bony fish, which
according to Striedter and Northcutt (2006) occurs as a consequence of the very
small size of ray-finned fish embryos, which does not permit evagination of the
hemispheres, forcing these cell masses to squeeze into the space rostral to the eyes.
The pallium of lungfishes and amphibians is generally evaginated and consists
in large part of periventricular cells that show a limited degree of radial migration;
these cells do not make up a true cortical architecture. This has been interpreted to
be a manifestation of neoteny or paedomorphosis (i.e., retention of a juvenile stage;
Ten Donkelaar 1998c). The amphibian pallium has been subdivided into lateral,
dorsal, medial, and ventral components (Holmgren 1922; Neary 1990; Bruce and
Neary 1995; Puelles 1995; Brox et al. 2004; Moreno and González 2006). Based in
large part on the relative absence of direct olfactory input and on the presence
of at least thalamic visual and somatosensory projections, the medial pallium has
been considered to be comparable to both the medial/dorsomedial cortex and the
dorsal cortex of reptiles (Bruce and Neary 1995; see also Ten Donkelaar 1998b,c).
On the other hand, the dorsal pallium receives substantial input from the main
olfactory bulb and has been compared to parts of the lateral and olfactory cortices
of reptiles and mammals. The lateral pallium of amphibians is subdivided into
a dorsal component, also comparable to parts of the lateral cortex of amniotes, and
a basal part which has been compared to the basolateral amygdalar complex of
mammals and to the DVR of birds and reptiles. In amphibians, there is also a caudal
striatum (subpallial) considered to be homologous to the striatal amygdala of
reptiles and to the central amygdalar complex of mammals (Bruce and Neary 1995;
Moreno and González 2003, 2004, 2005; Moreno et al. 2004; Laberge et al. 2006).
The reptilian pallium has a three-layered cortex, consisting of a medial and
a dorsomedial moiety (both comparable to the mammalian hippocampal forma-
tion), plus a lateral (olfactory) cortex (Ulinski 1990), and finally a dorsal cortex
(equivalent to the archistriatum accessorium, or Wulst, of birds) located between
these two, part of which receives visual projections from the dorsal lateral genic-
Evolution of the Telencephalon in Vertebrates 33
ulate nucleus, as well as some somatosensory input (Medina and Reiner 2000). In
reptiles and birds, many nonolfactory sensory projections (visual, auditory, and
somatosensory) terminate in the DVR (Ten Donkelaar 1998b; Ulinski 1983), which
in birds consists of three components, originally termed the archistriatum, neos-
triatum, and hyperstriatum. The DVR was originally considered to be part of the
basal ganglia (Elliot Smith 1919; Johnston 1923), but subsequent studies (Karten
1968, 1969; Karten and Hodos 1970; Nauta and Karten 1970) established that this
structure receives a strong sensory input, which was claimed to support a pallial
origin. Furthermore, histochemical analyses probing acetylcholinesterase activ-
ity determined that the limits of the corpus striatum were located immediately
ventral to those of the DVR (Parent and Olivier 1970; Parent 1986; Ulinski 1983).
Considering these findings, a more recent nomenclature has changed these names
into arcopallium (for archistriatum), nidopallium (for neostriatum), mesopallium
(for the hyperstriatum ventrale), and hyperpallium (for the hyperstriatum ac-
cesorium, or Wulst; Reiner et al. 2004; Jarvis et al. 2005). The DVR is the most
expansive telencephalic component of reptiles and birds and is a main integra-
tory center in their brains. In reptiles, it consists of an anterior part (ADVR) and
a posterior or basal part (PDVR). The output of the ADVR is directed mainly to
the subpallial corpus striatum and to the PDVR. The latter (corresponding to the
archistriatum/arcopallium in birds) has been compared to parts of the mammalian
amygdala and projects mainly to the hypothalamus (Lanuza et al. 1998, 1999; Ten
Donkelaar 1998b; Novejarque et al. 2004).
Finally, mammals are characterized by the possession of the neocortex or iso-
cortex, which originates during development at least in large part from the dorsal
pallium (Rakic 1988; 1995; Northcutt and Kaas 1995; Voogd et al. 1998). The iso-
cortex receives ascending sensory input from the thalamus and projects to the
hippocampus and to the amygdala, as well as sending output to many lower brain
centers including the thalamus, corpus striatum, various brainstem nuclei, and the
spinal cord. In the adult brain, medial to the isocortex is the hippocampal formation
(medial pallium), and lateral to it is the olfactory cortex (lateral pallium). Finally,
there is a highly complex claustroamygdaloid complex in the ventral pallium.
2.3.5
Summary
medial pallium, while the rest of the pallium consists of a laterodorsal pallium),
in the lamprey it is already possible to separate medial, dorsal, and lateral
components of the pallium, as in gnathostomes. In the latter, olfactory projections
became largely restricted to the lateral pallium. Interestingly, the medial pallium
receives a strong input from the dorsal thalamus, largely conveying sensory
information that is relayed in the mesencephalon. This condition persists in most
fish, but changes in tetrapods, in which information from lemniscal systems
also reach the thalamus and project to the telencephalon. In early gnathostomes,
the hemispheres increased in size and may have originally tended to an everted
condition in which the medial hemispheres are separated by an extended sheet of
choroid plexus. This trend is readily apparent in bony fishes, in which hemispheres
become everted. However, cartilaginous fishes developed a prominent dorsal
pallium with an internal nucleus centered at the midline (the central nucleus), thus
closing the two hemispheres and generating an evaginated brain. Lungfishes and
amphibians are characterized by a juvenilized brain with evaginated hemispheres
as well. Amniotes are the first true land vertebrates, as they are able to reproduce
on land. Amniotes split very early in two main lineages: stem reptiles, or anapsids,
and mammal-like reptiles, or pelycosaurs. These early animals had very simple
brains, which were possibly similar to those of present-day amphibians, where
olfactory input reaches the lateral and the dorsal pallium, while dorsal thalamic
projections reach the medial pallium. Reptiles develop a small cortex, consisting
of medial, dorsomedial, dorsal, and lateral fields, plus a large periventricular
structure termed the dorsal ventricular ridge, receiving most collothalamic input.
Lemnothalamic inputs are sent mostly to the dorsal cortex. Mammals develop
a large, six-layered neocortex that is topographically equivalent to the reptilian
dorsal cortex and receives both lemnothalamic and collothalamic inputs.
3
Origin of the Mammalian Brain
This section reviews the origin of the mammalian brain, especially the neocortex,
which is the most salient and expanding neural component in this group. We will
first discuss the fossil evidence on early mammals and their brains, and will follow
with a comparison between reptilian and mammalian brains. In the reptilian brain,
the lack of any structure obviously corresponding to the mammalian neocortex
is quite evident. For this reason, there have been intense controversies in relation
to the possible reptilian homologs to the neocortex. One such approach is based
on connectional evidence indicating a similar sensory input between parts of
the neocortex and the reptilian dorsal ventricular ridge; while other evidence,
based on connectional and developmental criteria, suggests homology between
the whole mammalian neocortex and the reptilian dorsal pallium. We will discuss
these different approaches, and will propose a hypothesis for neocortical origins
based on a dorsalization mechanism by which the dorsal pallium expanded in
34 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
medial pallium, while the rest of the pallium consists of a laterodorsal pallium),
in the lamprey it is already possible to separate medial, dorsal, and lateral
components of the pallium, as in gnathostomes. In the latter, olfactory projections
became largely restricted to the lateral pallium. Interestingly, the medial pallium
receives a strong input from the dorsal thalamus, largely conveying sensory
information that is relayed in the mesencephalon. This condition persists in most
fish, but changes in tetrapods, in which information from lemniscal systems
also reach the thalamus and project to the telencephalon. In early gnathostomes,
the hemispheres increased in size and may have originally tended to an everted
condition in which the medial hemispheres are separated by an extended sheet of
choroid plexus. This trend is readily apparent in bony fishes, in which hemispheres
become everted. However, cartilaginous fishes developed a prominent dorsal
pallium with an internal nucleus centered at the midline (the central nucleus), thus
closing the two hemispheres and generating an evaginated brain. Lungfishes and
amphibians are characterized by a juvenilized brain with evaginated hemispheres
as well. Amniotes are the first true land vertebrates, as they are able to reproduce
on land. Amniotes split very early in two main lineages: stem reptiles, or anapsids,
and mammal-like reptiles, or pelycosaurs. These early animals had very simple
brains, which were possibly similar to those of present-day amphibians, where
olfactory input reaches the lateral and the dorsal pallium, while dorsal thalamic
projections reach the medial pallium. Reptiles develop a small cortex, consisting
of medial, dorsomedial, dorsal, and lateral fields, plus a large periventricular
structure termed the dorsal ventricular ridge, receiving most collothalamic input.
Lemnothalamic inputs are sent mostly to the dorsal cortex. Mammals develop
a large, six-layered neocortex that is topographically equivalent to the reptilian
dorsal cortex and receives both lemnothalamic and collothalamic inputs.
3
Origin of the Mammalian Brain
This section reviews the origin of the mammalian brain, especially the neocortex,
which is the most salient and expanding neural component in this group. We will
first discuss the fossil evidence on early mammals and their brains, and will follow
with a comparison between reptilian and mammalian brains. In the reptilian brain,
the lack of any structure obviously corresponding to the mammalian neocortex
is quite evident. For this reason, there have been intense controversies in relation
to the possible reptilian homologs to the neocortex. One such approach is based
on connectional evidence indicating a similar sensory input between parts of
the neocortex and the reptilian dorsal ventricular ridge; while other evidence,
based on connectional and developmental criteria, suggests homology between
the whole mammalian neocortex and the reptilian dorsal pallium. We will discuss
these different approaches, and will propose a hypothesis for neocortical origins
based on a dorsalization mechanism by which the dorsal pallium expanded in
The First Mammals 35
response to signals emanating from the medial hemisphere (cortical hem), the
lateral hemisphere (cortical antihem), and the frontal hemisphere. Then we will
address the origin of cortical lamination, as the mammalian neocortex consists
of six laminae, instead of the three laminae found in the reptilian brain; and the
origin of the inside-out developmental gradient that is observed in the mammalian
neocortex. In this process, the subventricular zone, a late embryonic proliferating
compartment, probably played a special role. We will also offer a scenario in which
olfaction was an especially important sense in early mammals and contributed
to early neocortical expansion. Finally, we will briefly discuss the growth and
diversification of the cerebral cortex in the mammalian radiations.
3.1
The First Mammals
3.1.1
Fossil Mammals and Their Brains
The first radiation of mammal-like reptiles (synapsids) gave rise to the pelycosaurs,
which were relatively large, lizard-like animals. In the upper permian, pelycosaurs
were gradually replaced by their descendants, the therapsids. The hands and feet
of the latter faced more directly forward instead of being oriented sideways as
in other reptiles, which gave those animals a more mammalian-like gait. Some
therapsids grew to achieve large sizes, and they are classified into carnivorous and
herbivorous therapsids. Most therapsids became extinct by the end of the Triassic,
but one group of carnivorous therapsids, the cynodonts, survived well into the
Jurassic (Kemp 2005; Carroll 1988).
Cynodonts were relatively small-bodied and had a more mammal-like jaw
musculature, but the ear ossicles were still attached to the lower jaw, as they are in
reptiles. From cynodonts arose the eucynodonts or mammaliaforms, which include
Jurassic fossils such as Sinoconodon and Morganucodon, whose gross morphology
resembled that of some present-day insectivores (Rowe 1996a,b). True mammals
descend from eucynodonts, and are defined by the presence of a single dentary bone
making up the inferior mandible and the complete detachment of the middle ear
ossicles, as in the fossils Hadrocodium (Z.X. Luo et al. 2001, 2002), Gobiconodon, and
Repenonamus (Wang et al. 2001; however, according to these authors, Hadrocodium
is a juvenile form and it is not clear whether it had a fully mammalian middle ear).
Further evolution of mammals includes the origin of monotremes, marsupials, and
placental mammals. Triconodon is another interesting fossil, originally considered
to be close to Morganucodon (Carroll 1988), but according to newer analyses it has
been classified as a true mammal, perhaps belonging to the therians (marsupials
and placental mammals; Rowe 1996a,b).
Endocasts are molds of the cranial cavity of fossil animals. Analysis of these
casts indicates that early mammal-like reptiles (therapsids) had quite narrow, tubu-
lar hemispheres with no signs of telencephalic expansion (Hopson 1979; Quiroga
36 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
1980; Kielan-Jaworowska et al. 2004; Kemp 2005; see Fig. 10). Furthermore, in
cynodonts the volume of the endocast was much larger than the volume of the
brain, because ossification of the braincase was not as complete as in mammals
and it is not possible to observe anatomical details of the brain. In true mammals,
anatomical fissures and folds are much more clearly shown in the endocast, indi-
cating a narrow fit between the braincase and the volume of the brain. Increase
in brain size from cynodonts to early mammals is clearly shown in a comparison
of their respective encephalization quotients (EQ, i.e., brain size given a specific
body size). EQs of mesozoic mammals are twofold higher than those of advanced
cynodonts (see Kielan-Jaworowska et al. 2004). This is closely correlated with an
increase in the width of the braincase relative to skull width (Z.X. Luo et al. 2001).
(Interestingly, modern primates also have a brain that is about twice the size of
the brain of an average mammal of the same size; see Aboitiz 1996.) The brain-
case in stem mammals is narrower than in modern mammals, but is significantly
wider than in nonmammalian cynodonts despite having a much smaller body
size. For example Morganucodon, a primitive mammaliaform taxonomically in-
termediate between Triconodon and smaller-brained, more primitive therapsids,
shows only partial expansion of the brain. In this species, widening of the pari-
Fig.10 Brain endocasts from fossil mammaliaforms and Didelphys (living marsupial). Brain
expansion took place as a late event, closely associated with the origin of true mammals.
(Modified from Aboitiz et al. 2003, with permission)
The First Mammals 37
3.1.2
Summary
Although early mammal-like reptiles diversified into several branches, their brains
as seen in endocasts are surprisingly simple, perhaps more similar to present-day
amphibian brains than to the differentiated brains of extant reptiles. The origin of
true mammals is a late evolutionary event in the lineage of mammal-like reptiles,
and is marked by the detachment of the middle-ear ossicles from the lower jaw,
which served to increase auditory acuity. Early mammals also had large olfactory
bulbs, indicating a well-developed sense of smell, but apparently their visual system
was reduced as a consequence of their nocturnal habits. These events (especially
the detachment of the middle-ear bones) are related to a subsequent increase
in brain size, mainly due to expansion of the neocortex and cerebellum. As in
birds, brain expansion may have been related to the acquisition of endothermy,
which permitted a more active behavior and provided energy requirements for the
development of a costly large brain.
3.2
Origin of the Mammalian Neocortex: Hypotheses on Homology
3.2.1
Mammalian Brain Expansion and the Origin of the Neocortex
The neocortex or isocortex is a six-layered organ that is located between the three-
layered lateral or olfactory cortex (lateral pallium) and the medial or hippocampal
cortex (medial pallium, also bearing three main layers), in a topographic position
corresponding to that of the dorsal pallium (Rakic 1988; Voogd et al. 1998; see
Fig. 11). This structure is unique to mammals, is a conserved feature of all mam-
malian brains and is responsible for most of the increase in forebrain size in early
and late mammalian evolution. Although in reptiles there are some laminated cor-
tical structures (dorsal, medial, and lateral cortices), these never acquire the degree
of tangential expansion and laminar differentiation that is observed in the mam-
malian neocortex. Furthermore, in reptiles, most sensory projections are delivered
to the noncortical DVR instead of to the neocortex, as is observed in mammals
(Fig. 11). In view of these differences, it has not been easy to determine the structure
homologous to the neocortex in reptiles. In other words, the question of the ances-
try of the mammalian neocortex, i.e., of which structure in the ancestral amniote
brain gave rise to it, has not been solved despite about two centuries of studies in
comparative neuroanatomy and neuroembryology. This problem is further com-
plicated by the absence of a single criterion to establish homology of neural struc-
tures. Commonly used criteria for similarity are connectivity (Bruce and Neary
1995; Butler 1994a,b; Karten 1969, 1997; Medina and Reiner 2000; Reiner 2000),
neurochemistry (Reiner 1991, 1993), and embryonic origins (Aboitiz 1992, 1995;
Källén 1951; Puelles et al. 1999, 2000; Smith Fernández et al. 1998; Striedter 1997).
Origin of the Mammalian Neocortex: Hypotheses on Homology 39
Fig. 11 The cerebral hemispheres of reptiles and mammals. The pallium of reptiles has
a cortex, subdivided into medial/dorsomedial (MC), dorsal (DC) and lateral (LC) compo-
nents; and a dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR). The medial and dorsomedial cortices of reptiles
have been compared to the hippocampal formation (HP) of mammals; and the lateral cortex
is considered homologous to the mammalian olfactory cortex (OC). The dorsal cortex of
reptiles resembles both the mammalian entorhinal cortex (adjacent to the hippocampal
formation; not shown) and the neocortex (NC). There is no agreement with respect to the
homology of the dorsal ventricular ridge. Some authors claim that this structure corre-
sponds to the lateral neocortex, and other authors argue that it relates to the mammalian
claustroamygdaloid complex (AM). PT, pallial thickening. (Modified from Northcutt and
Kaas 1995, with permission)
3.2.2
Hypotheses for Neocortical Origins
Two alternative hypotheses have been raised regarding the origins of the mam-
malian isocortex, which have been elegantly summarized by Northcutt and Kaas
(1995) as the recapitulation hypothesis and the out-group hypothesis. Proponents
of the recapitulation hypothesis suggest that a DVR-like structure existed in the
common ancestor of mammals and reptiles, which somehow became transformed
into parts of the isocortex in the origin of mammals. This perspective is mostly
based on studies on adult neuronal connectivity, and more specifically on the pat-
terns of termination of thalamic afferents to the telencephalon (Karten 1968, 1997;
Nauta and Karten 1970; Shimizu and Karten 1993). Karten’s approach was origi-
nally challenged on topographical and developmental grounds by Aboitiz (1992a,b;
1993, 1995), who proposed that the mammalian neocortex as a whole derived from
the reptilian dorsal pallium, while the reptilian DVR originated deep to the lateral
40 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
pallium. Later, Bruce and Neary (1995), Striedter (1997), Smith-Fernández et al.
(1998), and Puelles et al. (1999, 2000), based on connectional and developmen-
tal evidence, proposed homology between the DVR and parts of the mammalian
ventral pallium, including the lateral amygdala and the dorsal claustrum. This
claustroamygdalar proposal was first stated by Holmgren (1922) but remained
unacknowledged for a long time. These considerations were summarized in the
out-group hypothesis (Northcutt and Kaas 1995), according to which the common
ancestor of reptiles and mammals would have had a cerebral hemisphere similar
in its topographic organization to that of present-day amphibians. In this case,
the most likely candidate for homology with the isocortex is the reptilian dorsal
pallium. Below, we will consider some of the evidence argued in favor of each of
these hypotheses.
3.2.3
The Recapitulation Hypothesis: Connectional Evidence
Fig. 12 Karten’s hypothesis (1969, 1997) of the equivalent circuit in the mammalian neo-
cortex and the avian dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR). I, II, III, IV, V, VI represent distinct
neocortical layers; Arc, arcopallium; En, entopallium (a component of the nidopallium); H,
hyperpallium; N, nidopallium; P, pulvinar; R, nucleus rotundus
(Karten 1997; Nauta and Karten 1970; Shimizu and Karten 1993; Veenman et al.
1995; Wild 1997; see Fig. 12). Recent studies indicate that avian areas of the DVR re-
ceiving sensory projections display markers such as RORB/Nr/1/2 and the voltage-
dependent potassium channel EAG2/KCNH5, which are also found in layer IV of
primary sensory areas of the mammalian neocortex (Schaeren-Wiemers et al. 1997;
Dugas-Ford and Ragsdale 2003). (Nevertheless, these markers are also detected in
thalamorecipient areas of the avian Wulst, belonging to the dorsal pallium.) Related
hypotheses have proposed equivalence between the postero-dorsolateral nidopal-
lium (neostriatum) and the mammalian prefrontal cortex (Gagliardo and Divac
1993; Divac et al. 1994; Güntürkün 2005) and between the arcopallium (archistria-
tum) and the cortical frontal eye fields of mammals (Knudsen et al. 1995).
3.2.4
The Dorsal Cortex of Reptiles: Subicular and Neocortical Characteristics
The recapitulationist hypothesis has been largely based on the comparison be-
tween avian and mammalian brains. However, in reptiles the situation is slightly
more complicated, as it has been argued that lemnothalamic systems reaching
the dorsal cortex are not strictly comparable to those in mammals (see Martínez-
García 2003; Guirado 2003). For example, somatosensory projections to the dorsal
cortex have not been observed either in lizards (Bruce and Butler 1984a,b; Lohman
and Van Woerden-Verkley 1978; Neary and Wilczynski 1977) or turtles (Hall and
Ebner 1970). In the case of visual projections, the thalamic lateral geniculate nu-
cleus projects to a region that in turtles includes the dorsal cortex and the pallial
thickening (a thickening in the lateral aspect of the pallium that is observed in
42 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
some reptiles; see Fig. 11), and in lizards is restricted to the pallial thickening (Hall
and Ebner 1970; Kenigfest et al. 1997; Lohman and Van Woerden-Verkley 1978).
In addition, multimodal projections from the dorsal thalamus reach the basal
ganglia and the dorsal pallium in both amphibians and reptiles (Wilczynski and
Northcutt 1983; González et al. 1990; Marín et al. 1997; Ten Donkelaar 1998a,b,c;
Guirado et al. 2000; Martínez-García 2003; Guirado 2003). In amphibians, there are
multimodal projections, carrying both lemnothalamic and collothalamic informa-
tion to the mediodorsal (and lateral) pallium from the anterior thalamic nucleus
(Vesselkin et al. 1971), and in reptiles a similar projection reaches the medial and
dorsal cortices from the dorsolateral anterior thalamic nucleus (Bruce and Butler
1984a; Desan 1988; Lohman and Van Woerden-Verkley 1978; Belekhova and Ivazov
1983; Ivazov and Belekhova 1982). A more recent hodological study in the turtle de-
termined that the dorsomedial anterior nucleus of the thalamus projected mostly
to the ipsilateral medial cortex, while the dorsolateral anterior nucleus projected to
the dorsomedial cortex, and the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus projected to the
dorsal cortex, thus making a mediolateral topographic map between the distinct
thalamic nuclei and the cortical regions (Zhu et al. 2005). (This work also described
a descending pathway linking cortical regions [especially medial cortex] with the
brainstem red nucleus via the suprapeduncular nucleus of the hypothalamus, thus
providing an indirect cortical control of the red nucleus that in mammals becomes
direct.)
Considering this evidence, it has been proposed that the visual projection
from the lateral geniculate is a derived rather than an ancestral feature of rep-
tiles (Martínez-García 2003; Guirado 2003). According to these and other authors
(Powers 2003), the multimodal dorsomedial pallium of amphibians and reptiles
resembles more the mammalian hippocamposubicular cortex. This proposal is
supported by anatomical evidence indicating that, like the entorhinal cortex, the
reptilian dorsal cortex has important connections with the medial/dorsomedial or
hippocampal cortices (Hoogland and Vermeulen-Vanderzee 1989; Northcutt and
Ronan 1992; Guirado and Dávila 2002; Martínez-Marcos et al. 1999).
Furthermore, behavioral evidence in lesioned animals indicates that the reptil-
ian dorsal cortex does not participate in vision but rather in learning and memory
(Powers 1990). Lesions in the dorsal cortex produce deficits in acquisition and
reversal of pattern discriminations (Blau and Powers 1989; Cranney and Powers
1983), spatial discriminations (Grishman and Powers 1990), and in a variety of
other learning tasks (Grisham and Powers 1989; Avigan and Powers 1995; Day
et al. 2001; Peterson 1980; Petrillo et al. 1994; Moran et al. 1998), which strongly
relate this structure with the medial (hippocampal) cortex, which participates
in spatial and other forms of learning in a variety of vertebrates (Grisham and
Powers 1989; O’Keefe and Nadel 1978; Rodríguez et al. 2002a,b). Thus, the dorsal
cortex of reptiles may be compared to the entorhinal cortex of mammals, but
also containing a direct visual sensory input that, if perhaps not being the true
homolog of the mammalian visual cortex, serves to feed visual information to the
hippocampus for establishing sensory associations during learning. Furthermore,
Origin of the Mammalian Neocortex: Hypotheses on Homology 43
the dorsal cortex of reptiles differs from the amphibian dorsal pallium in that it
receives much stronger dorsal thalamic inputs, lacks direct projections from the
olfactory bulb, and projects to several extratelencephalic targets (Bruce and Butler
1984a,b), characters that are reminiscent of the mammalian neocortex.
3.2.5
Differences in Connectivity Between the DVR and the Neocortex
There is also some connectional evidence that points to differences between the
reptilian DVR and the mammalian isocortex. Firstly, the mammalian extrastriate
visual cortex receives an important input from the primary or striate visual cortex
(Montero 1993; Rosa and Krubitzer 1999). Although in reptiles, projections from
the dorsal cortex to the DVR have been described (Ten Donkelaar 1998b; Ulinski
1990), these are much less prominent than the striate-extrastriate connections
of mammals. Secondly, the mammalian isocortex projects reciprocally to the en-
torhinal cortex and from there to the hippocampus (Haberly 1990; Rosene and Van
Hoesen 1987; Van Hoesen 1982), while in reptiles few connections if any have been
reported from the DVR to the medial/dorsomedial cortex or hippocampus (Ten
Donkelaar 1998b; Ulinski 1983, 1990). Moreover, based on comparisons of connec-
tivity, Bruce and Neary (1995) have argued that the reptilian DVR is most similar
to the mammalian lateral amygdala, since both structures receive projections from
collothalamic nuclei and both project to the corpus striatum, the striatal amygdala,
and the ventromedial hypothalamus. (More precisely, the reptilian ADVR would
correspond to the mammalian basolateral amygdala, while the whole DVR might
correspond to the whole lateral amygdalar nucleus.) On the other hand, the iso-
cortex projects to many other brain regions in the brainstem and spinal cord, and
does not project to the hypothalamus. These authors claim that fewer changes in
connectivity are required by assuming homology between the lateral amygdala of
mammals and the DVR of reptiles than by considering homology between the iso-
cortex and the DVR. An additional difference between sauropsids and mammals
is that the corpus striatum receives its major projection from the ADVR and from
the isocortex, respectively. Nevertheless, the basolateral amygdala of mammals and
the dorsal cortex of reptiles also project to the corpus striatum (Bruce and Neary
1995; Ten Donkelaar 1998b), which is consistent with the similarity of connections
between the ADVR and the mammalian basolateral amygdala, and between the
dorsal cortex of reptiles and the mammalian isocortex. Note that other hodological
studies have concluded that the mammalian basolateral amygdala corresponds to
the reptilian posterior DVR (PDVR) and lateral amygdala, instead of the ADVR
(Martínez-García et al. 2002; Moreno and González, 2004, 2006; Moreno et al. 2004;
Novejarque et al. 2004; Laberge et al. 2006).
In this context, one particularly controversial issue concerns the thalamic lat-
eral posterior/pulvinar (LP/P) nucleus of mammals and its presumed homology
with the reptilian and avian rotundus nucleus. The mammalian LP/P, receiving
projections from the superior colliculus and projecting to the extrastriate visual
44 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
3.2.6
Summary
3.3
Embryological Evidence
3.3.1
Developmental Criteria for Homology
During embryogenesis, the DVR (especially its anterior part) develops from a po-
sition largely ventral and deep to the olfactory cortex. This was determined in early
embryological work on snakes (Warner 1946), lizards (von Hetzel 1974), Sphen-
odon (Hines 1923), crocodiles (Källén 1951), and turtles (Holmgren 1922; Källén
1951; Kirsche 1972), and more recently on birds (Striedter et al. 1998), establishing
that the DVR arises as a late proliferation process, after the migration of cells
belonging to lateral cortex has been completed. Later in development, the DVR
bulges into the ventricle, locating ventral to lateral cortex and immediately dorsal
to the corpus striatum (Ulinski 1983). Considering this early evidence, Northcutt
(1969) originally proposed that both the lateral cortex and the DVR of reptiles were
homologous as a whole with the mammalian lateral cortex and parts of the neocor-
tex (although he changed his view in subsequent writings, being more sympathetic
with the outgroup hypothesis; Northcutt and Kaas 1995; Northcutt 2003). Taking
a perhaps more radical position, Aboitiz (1992b, 1995) proposed that the DVR,
arising in a region that overlaps with the lateral cortex, consisted of late-produced
cells (late in relation to lateral cortex development), largely originating from a sim-
ilar embryonic region as the lateral cortex itself. Furthermore, much of the adult
avian and reptilian DVR would consist of late-produced cells that have no strict
homolog in the adult mammalian brain. This hypothesis is supported by the fact
that the amphibian lateral pallium receives not only olfactory projections but also
some inputs from the dorsal thalamus (Kicliter and Northcutt 1975), implying that
this structure is comparable to both the lateral and the ventral pallium of reptiles,
or that the amphibian lateral pallium also contains a ventral pallial component
(see also Moreno and González 2004; Moreno et al. 2004).
Considering this evidence, for the ADVR to be ancestral to the neocortex,
a massive migration of excitatory cells from the proliferative zone underneath the
lateral cortex (the region giving rise to the ADVR) toward the visual extrastriate
and auditory cortex would have to be demonstrated. However, to date evidence
suggests that in mammals these cortical areas arise from the cortical neuroep-
ithelium (Parnavelas 2000), while the neuroepithelial region corresponding to the
embryonic reptilian DVR produces ventral pallial components (Tole et al. 2005).
The migration of inhibitory interneurons from subcortical structures appears to
be a phylogenetically ancient character and may not be directly related to neo-
cortical origins, being possibly present also in agnathans (Marín and Rubenstein
2001; Meléndez-Ferro et al. 2002; Brox et al. 2003; Molnár et al. 2006).
Literally turning this issue around, Reiner (1993) and Butler (1994a) proposed
a topographic position of the DVR that is equivalent to that of lateral neocortex,
that is, between the dorsal and lateral cortices. Reiner (1993) asserted that the
48 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
dorsal pallial sector of stem amniotes possessed a lateral zone that was the fore-
runner of both DVR in sauropsids and lateral neocortex in mammals. In primitive
reptiles like Sphenodon, the DVR appears continuous with the dorsal cortex in its
anterior region, and in turtles this domain of continuity corresponds to the pal-
lial thickening (Ten Donkelaar 1998b; Ulinski 1990; Reiner and Northcutt 2000).
Nevertheless, Reiner (1993) did not mention the position of the lateral cortex,
which appears interposed between the DVR and the dorsal cortex through most
of their extent (Senn 1979; Ulinski 1990). Raising an additional hypothesis, Butler
(1994a) suggested homology between the DVR and the neocortex on the basis of
the presence of a subventricular zone (SVZ) in the two structures during develop-
ment, from which both the lateral neocortex of mammals and the DVR of reptiles
would emerge (Smart and Smart 1977; Yanes et al. 1987). We will come back to this
proposal in Sect. 3.5.1.
This problem has been recently clarified by studies of expression patterns of
regulatory homeobox-like genes in the embryonic forebrain, which have revealed
a conserved mosaic organization where the different compartments develop into
specific adult brain components (Gellon and McGinnis 1998; Moens et al. 1998;
Puelles and Rubenstein 1993; Seo et al. 1998). The embryonic lateral and medial
ganglionic eminences, located in the lateral subpallium and giving rise to the
corpus striatum and globus pallidus, express the marker genes Dlx1 and Dlx2
(Anderson et al. 1997b). The cerebral cortex arises mostly from the embryonic
pallium and is characterized by the expression of genes of the Emx and the Otx
families (Acámpora and Simeone 1999; Mallamaci et al. 1998; Pannese et al. 1998;
Puelles and Rubenstein 1993; Simeone et al. 1992). Smith Fernández et al. (1998)
identified for the first time what they termed an intermediate territory in the
equatorial region of the hemisphere, between the pallium and the subpallium
of amphibians, reptiles, birds, and mammals, which does not express either the
Emx1 or Dlx1 markers of the pallium and subpallium, respectively, but is largely
positive for the gene Pax6 (another pallial marker; Smith-Fernández et al. 1998).
More recent reports (Puelles et al. 1999, 2000) confirmed the existence of the
intermediate territory (which has been termed ventral pallium by these authors),
and extended the previous findings by showing that Pax6 and Tbr1 are expressed
in the whole pallium including the ventral pallium, but PAX6 appears mainly near
the ventricular zone and TBR1 has a more superficial activity. Thus, the medial,
the dorsal, and part of the lateral pallium express Emx1 and Tbr1 superficially
and Pax6 more internally, whereas the ventral pallium expresses Tbr1 superficially
and Pax6 deeply, but not Emx1. The presence of a ventral pallium has also been
confirmed in amphibians (Brox et al. 2002, 2004; Moreno and González 2004, 2006;
Moreno et al. 2004), indicating that it is an ancestral character and not a derived
feature of amniotes (reptiles, birds, and mammals).
In sauropsids, an important part of the DVR (more specifically, its anterior
component, including the neostriatum/nidopallium and ectostriatum/entopallial
nucleus of birds) and part of the lateral cortex develop from the Emx1-negative
ventral pallium, whereas in mammals, the basolateral amygdalar complex, part
Embryological Evidence 49
of the claustral complex, the endopiriform nucleus, and parts of the lateral or
olfactory cortex – among other structures – were claimed to derive from this re-
gion (Puelles et al. 1999, 2000; Smith-Fernández et al. 1998; see Fig. 13). More
recently, Medina et al. (2004) described the anterior and posterior amygdalar ar-
eas and the amygdalo-hippocampal area as ventropallial structures. On the other
hand, the claustrum proper (formerly the dorsolateral claustrum), the posterior
endopiriform nucleus, the dorsal part of the piriform cortex, the basolateral amyg-
dalar nucleus, and the posterolateral cortical amygdalar area appear to belong to
the lateral pallium. As mentioned, two mammalian structures have recently been
proposed to correspond to the ADVR: the basolateral amygdala (Bruce and Neary
1995; Puelles et al. 1999; Smith Fernández et al. 1998) and the endopiriform nucleus
(Striedter 1997). Developmental evidence favors both the basolateral amygdala and
the endopiriform nucleus as comparable to the DVR (Smith Fernández et al. 1998;
Puelles et al. 1999). On the other hand, while some connectional evidence indi-
cates similarity between the ADVR and the basolateral amygdala (Bruce and Neary
1995), other evidence suggests similarity between the PDVR and the basolateral
amygdala (Martínez-García et al. 2002; Moreno and González 2004; Moreno et al.
2004; Novejarque et al. 2004), and the connections of the endopiriform nucleus
parallel quite closely those of the olfactory cortex (Behan and Haberly 1999). Ho-
mology of the DVR with the mammalian claustrum was originally questioned as
a first report communicated the absence of a claustrum in monotremes (Butler
et al. 2002), while a more recent report refuted this observation (Ashwell et al.
2004). Thus, although it seems clear that the reptilian ADVR derives from a ven-
tral pallial component, there are disagreements regarding the precise mammalian
homolog of this structure.
Smith-Fernández et al. (1998) argued that in reptiles and birds the ventral pal-
lium remains as a distinct neuroepithelial zone until late development, the period
in which it gives rise to most of the ADVR, which is in agreement with previous
embryological studies (Holmgren 1922; Källén 1951; Kirsche 1972). On the con-
trary, in mammals, this territory was described as producing only early-generated
components, becoming obliterated between the Emx1-positive and the Dlx1/2-
positive zones in later development (Smith-Fernández et al. 1998). In agreement
with this interpretation, Swanson (2000) and Künzle and Radtke-Schuller (2001)
suggest that the mammalian claustral complex has developmental timing similar
to early-produced cortical elements such as the mammalian subplate (an embry-
onic layer that develops before the definitive cerebral cortex in mammals); both
structures make up about the earliest pallial neurons. Although Puelles et al. (1999)
seem to disagree with the concept of the ventral pallium disappearing from the
neuroepithelial surface in mammals, they admit that this territory is considerably
compressed between the lateral pallium and the developing striatum.
To us, this evidence suggests that in mammals, some ventral pallial components
such as the claustrum consist of early produced components and may not be
directly comparable to those late-generated components of the reptilian – and
especially avian – highly developed DVR (Källén 1951). This agrees with the concept
50 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
Fig. 13 Embryonic telencephalic territories based on regulatory gene expression data. The
medial, dorsal and lateral pallia (violet) express the markers Emx1, Tbr1 and Pax6, while
the ventral pallium (yellow) expresses Tbr1 and Pax6 but not Emx1. The subpallium (green)
expresses Dlx genes. According to this data, the anterior dorsal ventricular ridge (ADVR)
and the avian nidopallium (N) derive mainly from the ventral pallium. a, vc, termination of
auditory and visual collothalamic pathways, respectively; vl, termination of visual lemniscal
pathways. STR, corpus striatum. (Modified from Aboitiz et al. 2003, with permission. Data
from Smith Fernández et al. 1998 and Puelles et al. 1999, 2000)
Embryological Evidence 51
that in mammals there is no strict homology to the reptilian and avian ADVR, or
at least to a large part of it (Aboitiz 1992, 1995). In this way, much of the avian DVR
may consist of phylogenetically new cell groups, produced during late development,
a situation that is similar to what is observed in the mammalian neocortex, in which
superficial layers represent an evolutionary acquisition of the mammalian brain
(see Sect. 3.5.1). Nevertheless, connections from the collothalamic dorsal thalamus
to the ventral pallium are present in both reptiles and mammals, and therefore
may reflect a primitive character even if they do not reach exactly homolog regions
in each group.
Considering this evidence, Reiner (2000) modified his original proposal sug-
gesting that in the common ancestor to reptiles and mammals there was a structure
that diverged into the lateral isocortex of mammals and into the ADVR of reptiles.
This structure was either Emx1-negative and acquired Emx1 expression in mam-
mals, or, alternatively, was Emx1-positive and lost Emx1 expression in reptiles and
birds. A somewhat similar hypothesis was raised by Butler and Molnár (2002) and
Molnár and Butler (2002a,b), who proposed an alternative hypothesis by which the
ventrolateral neocortex, together with the mammalian claustrum and the pallial
amygdala, are field homologs of the sauropsidian ADVR, the former arising from
a duplication event in the ventrolateral pallium yielding a dorsal (cortical) and
a ventral (amygdaloclaustral) component. Again, difficulties with these interpre-
tations concern the ventral embryological origin of the DVR and the adult position
of the lateral cortex, which separates the DVR and the dorsal cortex through most
of their extent. The concept that the mammalian lateral cortex gained Emx1 ex-
pression (or that the DVR lost it) would need to be supported by embryological
tracing studies, indicating common embryonic origins of both structures. In this
context, Butler and Molnár (2002) reported the development of a DVR-like struc-
ture in the Pax6–/– mutant, indicating an evolutionarily atavic condition. However,
this mutant has been reported to develop a dysgenic ventral pallium (Tole et al.
2005), which is not entirely consistent with the development of a ventral pallial
DVR-like structure. In general, although these hypotheses may be consistent with
the concept of phylogenetic parcellation (Ebbesson 1980, 1984), they lack positive
evidence supporting them, such as a clear sign of ventral pallial origin of the lateral
neocortex. Furthermore, they might be impossible to refute, as any dorsal pallial
sign in these regions could be interpreted to result from the transformation of this
ventral pallial region into a dorsal pallial one.
Additional issues requiring further study concern the mammalian homologies
of the reptilian posterior DVR or avian archistriatum/arcopallium, and the hyper-
striatum ventrale/mesopallium (corresponding to the dorsolateral anterior DVR;
Guirado et al. 2000; Puelles et al. 2000) of birds. According to Smith-Fernández et al.
(1998), the reptilian posterior DVR and the avian archistriatum/arcopallium, or
at least part of these structures, express pallial markers and are comparable to the
corticomedial and central amygdala of mammals (Swanson and Petrovich 1998).
On the other hand, Puelles and colleagues (Puelles et al. 1999) argue that only
the posterior archistriatum/archipallium is pallial. The hyperstriatum ventrale or
52 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
3.3.2
Dorsoventral Gradients and Expansion of the Dorsal Pallium
Considering the evidence reviewed in the above sections, we have argued that
the mammalian neocortex arose through the tangential expansion of the reptilian
dorsal pallium (Aboitiz 1992, 1993, 1995, 1999a,b). More recently (Aboitiz et al.
2003c), we proposed that a dorsalization mechanism, producing an increase in
proliferation of dorsal pallial sectors, would have generated the required telen-
cephalic expansion leading to the origin of the neocortex. Considering that much
evidence implies a strong similarity between the reptilian dorsal cortex and the
entorhinal/subicular cortex, one interesting possibility is that the dorsal cortex
expanded medially to become the entorhinal/subicular complex and laterally to
become the sensory neocortex (Powers 2003).
The dorsal hemisphere and the mammalian cerebral cortex are patterned by
virtue of several interacting signaling cascades. Genes of the Wnt family are essen-
tial for hippocampal development and are also expressed strongly in the caudome-
dial margin of the cortical pallium (Kim et al. 2001). Wnt receptors of the Frizzled
family are most concentrated in the isocortical neuroepithelium, while being sparse
or entirely absent in the more medial hippocampal neuroepithelium (Kim et al.
2001). Similarly, two Wnt inhibitors (secreted Frizzled-related proteins 1 and 3) are
expressed in opposing anterolateral to caudomedial gradients in the telencephalic
ventricular zone. In addition, the Emx2 gene, whose expression domain is similar
to that of Emx1 but includes subpallial sectors (Gulisano et al. 1996), has been found
to be arranged in a gradient with maximal concentrations in the posteromedial
isocortex and minimal concentrations in the anterolateral isocortex (Mallamaci
et al. 2000). Furthermore, the Pax6 gene is expressed in a gradient that is comple-
mentary to that of Emx2, being maximally expressed in the anterolateral isocortex
and minimally expressed in the posteromedial isocortex (Bishop et al. 2000).
The modulation of overall dorsoventral, frontocaudal, and/or mediolateral gra-
dients of expression of the above-mentioned regulatory genes may have profound
effects in the development of specific telencephalic components (Chapouton et al.
1999; Stoykova et al. 2000; Muzio et al. 2002; Grove et al. 1998; Monuki et al. 2001;
Shinozaki et al. 2002; Meyer et al. 2002, 2004; Shimogori et al. 2004; Tole et al.
2005). It is therefore possible that the expansion of the dorsal pallium in primitive
mammals occurred partly as a consequence of the enhancement of a dorsaliz-
ing signal in telencephalic development. Since the structure that expanded most in
mammalian evolution is the dorsal pallium (neocortex) rather than the medial pal-
lium, the lateral pallium, and the ventral pallium, there may be some yet unknown
genes exclusively determining the fate of this structure, which increased their do-
Embryological Evidence 53
3.3.3
Summary
Early developmental evidence indicated that the reptilian and avian DVR origi-
nates from a late-proliferative region located deep to the lateral cortex, in a position
topographically equivalent to that of the mammalian claustroamygdalar system.
54 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
3.4
The Olfactory–Hippocampal Hypothesis
3.4.1
A Functional Interpretation of Dorsal Pallial Expansion in Mammalian Origins
If the neocortex arose from an expansion of a small field bordering the medial
pallium and the lateral pallium, the question of the adaptive benefit to develop this
structure arises. We consider that early neocortical expansion was largely due to
a strictly contingent situation that was not observed in early reptiles and relates
to the development of the sense of olfaction (see also Sagan 1977). In reptiles,
the dorsal cortex is an interface connecting the olfactory and the hippocampal
cortices and relaying dorsal thalamic input into the latter for visuospatial learning.
In early mammals, olfactory input may have been especially important for several
behaviors including learning. This condition produced an unusual development
of the olfactory–dorsal cortex–hippocampal networks, which resulted in an initial
expansion of the dorsal pallium. Subsequently, increasing lemnothalamic and col-
lothalamic projections were directed toward the dorsal pallium, thus contributing
to its further expansion.
The Olfactory–Hippocampal Hypothesis 55
As previously discussed, the reptilian medial and dorsal cortices receive multi-
modal sensory projections from the lateral anterior thalamic nucleus, and at least
visual projections from the lateral geniculate nucleus. However, the most important
sensory pathway is the collothalamic projection to the ventral pallial ADVR, carry-
ing visual, auditory and somatosensory input. In turn, the ADVR projects heavily
into the PDVR, which sends afferents to hypothalamic regions (Novejarque et al.
2004). Thus, in reptiles the ventral and dorsomedial pallia receive largely different
sensory pathways, and there is relatively little cross-talk between them. The situa-
tion is different in mammals. The lemnothalamic-receiving primary visual cortex
projects massively to the extrastriate cortical areas (which receive the collothala-
mic visual projection), and these, together with the auditory and somatosensory
cortical areas and the olfactory cortex, project (through a series of successive cor-
ticocortical projections) to the hippocampus (via the entorhinal cortex) and to
the amygdala, in order to process different types of mnemonic information (spa-
tial/episodic and emotional, respectively; Lynch 1986; Maren 1999). This implies
that in mammals there is a confluence of the lemnothalamic and collothalamic
processing pathways to a degree that is not observed in reptiles (Aboitiz 1992;
Aboitiz et al. 2003b,c; Butler 1994b). (Recall that in mammals there is also a rem-
nant of the collothalamic projection to the ventral pallium, which is important in
the generation of automatic emotional responses; Sect. 3.2.5.) Furthermore, the
direct dorsal thalamic input to the medial pallium in amphibians is weakened in
reptiles and disappears in both mammals and birds (Neary 1990; Ulinski 1990).
Thus, reptiles and amphibians have two parallel routes of dorsal thalamic input to
the medial pallium (via the dorsal pallium and directly from the thalamus), while
in mammals (and in birds) the main entrance of thalamic sensory input to the
medial pallium is via the dorsal pallium.
In addition to the thalamic sensory projections, in terrestrial vertebrates there
is an important olfactory input via the lateral pallium into the medial pallium. The
projections from both the lateral and dorsal pallium (receiving olfactory input)
into the medial pallium in amphibians may have served as precursors for the devel-
opment of olfactory–hippocampal circuits in amniotes (Ten Donkelaar 1998b,c).
In reptiles, there is a well-defined circuit connecting the medial (hippocampal), the
dorsal (receiving the lemnothalamic pathways), and the olfactory cortices (Lynch
1986; Ten Donkelaar 1998a).
There is evidence that in reptiles and probably in most other vertebrates, the
medial and the dorsal pallia participate in spatial learning (Rodríguez et al. 2002a,b;
see Sect. 3.2.4). Among reptiles, active foraging lizards tend to have larger medial
and dorsal cortices than species that hunt with a sit-and-wait strategy (Day et al.
1999). In addition, lesions in these regions impair spatial learning in these animals
(Day et al. 2001; Rodríguez et al. 2002a,b). The authors further argue that these
cortical areas may use nonspatial clues for spatial navigation, which is important
in relation to new concepts of hippocampal function described below.
In mammals, a classical understanding of hippocampal function is that this
structure creates a cartesian representation of space, in which the different places
56 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
and coordinates are mapped onto the structure itself (O’Keefe and Nadel 1978).
This concept was partly proposed on the basis of the discovery of place-cells in the
hippocampus of experimental animals, which are activated by specific positions
of the animal in a given space (O’Keefe and Dostrowsky 1971). However, the situa-
tion is slightly more complex, as evidence has shown that many hippocampal cells
fire in response to nonspatial determinants such as odors, and that the activity
of the cells depends on the behavioral state of the animal (see Eichenbaum 1999;
Eichenbaum et al. 1999). Hippocampal cells can recognize rewarded and nonre-
warded cues, spatial configurations of odors, differences between odors, or fire
at specific behavioral instances (Wiener et al. 1989; Wood et al. 1999, 2000). In
addition, it has been found that spatial and nonspatial (olfactory) information are
segregated in interleaved, oblique stripes along the hippocampus (Hampson et al.
1999). This structure has also been shown to be required for nonspatial olfactory
tasks, such as tests of transitive inference, higher-order sequential associations, or
the social transmission of food preferences (Galef 1990; Bunsey and Eichenbaum
1996; Dusek and Eichenbaum 1997; Alvarez et al. 2001; Ergorul and Eichenbaum
2006). In summary, there is substantial evidence that the mammalian hippocam-
pus participates in olfactory memory and in other memory processes in addition
to spatial mapping (Eichenbaum 1998, 2006).
Partly based on this evidence and on the finding that place cells are more
consistently controlled by local cues (see Eichenbaum et al. 1999), it has been
proposed that the representation of space in the hippocampus consists of the
specification of behaviorally relevant spots. These spots are identified by cues
such as odors and other (visual) characteristics of the environment, and include
a collection of independent representations of places, linked among them by the
behavioral context in which the animal explores its environment (Eichenbaum
1999, 2000a,b; Eichenbaum et al. 1999; Ergorul and Eichenbaum 2004). For these
authors, a fundamental function of the hippocampus is its participation in episodic
memory, that is, memory of the events that take place during a particular behavioral
action. Spatial memory emerges as consequence of the integration of successive
episodes during an exploration task and involves associations between different
sensory modalities, including vision and olfaction. This proposal could reconcile
the apparently discrepant findings that, in animals, the hippocampus participates
in spatial memory, whereas in humans, it participates in declarative memory.
Instead of spatial memory, Eichenbaum prefers to speak of a memory space,
which is an organized representation of memory episodes linked by their common
features (Eichenbaum 2000b; Eichenbaum et al. 1999; however, see alternative view
by O’Keefe 1999). We suggest that in the process of generating this memory space,
olfaction (which is an important sense used to investigate the environment by many
small mammals such as rodents and insectivores) may participate as a “glue” that
helps linking many of these spots, creating a cohesive map of the behaviorally
relevant points. In present-day mammals such as the rat, visual input is necessary
for the firing of a large number of hippocampal cells, while olfactory information
can be used to compensate for the lack of visuospatial information (Save et al.
The Olfactory–Hippocampal Hypothesis 57
2000). Therefore, visual information may have been progressively involved in the
associative hippocampal–olfactory networks during the origin of mammals, thus
triggering the expansion of the dorsal cortex. This is not to say that visual cues have
little importance in spatial or episodic learning in nonmammals, but rather that
the visual input to the mammalian hippocampus is more complex, so that more
accurate representations of both behavioral situations and their spatial context can
be made.
The olfactory–hippocampal–dorsal cortex circuit may have been put to use by
the first mammals to make relatively elaborate, largely olfactory-based representa-
tions of space, in which specific odors labeled particular places and routes (recall
that early mammals were presumably nocturnal and may not have been able to
rely on visual information as strongly as reptiles or laboratory mammals; Jerison
1973, 1990; Sagan 1977; Kemp 2005). Nevertheless, the contribution of the visual
system undoubtedly became necessary in the elaboration of more precise maps
of space, especially when mammals invaded diurnal niches after the decline of
dinosaurs. The dorsal cortex, receiving visual information from the thalamofugal
visual pathway, may have been an important element for associative learning in
the early mammalian brain, as it is now in reptiles (Aboitiz 1992a,b). Although
there is evidence for a strong conservatism in hippocampal general function and
in its participation in spatial learning (Salas et al. 2003; Colombo 2003), there
are also important differences in hippocampal connectivity between mammals
and reptiles (reviewed in Butler 1994a,b; Striedter 2005), which suggest that there
have been some changes in this overall conserved architecture, perhaps facili-
tating in mammals an expansion of associative functions and the elaboration of
more subtle forms of spatial and episodic memory. Electrophysiological evidence
indicates that the neuronal dynamics present in primitive olfactory–cortical cir-
cuits, including modulations of frequency, phase, and amplitude of oscillations
during olfactory processing are also observed in neocortical circuits, suggesting
that olfactory associative processing formed the basis for large-scale couplings
involving the expanding isocortex (Hermer-Vazquez and Hermer-Vazquez 2003).
Furthermore, these olfactory–hippocampal tangential circuits may have facilitated
the establishment of strong pallio-pallial connectivity that is related to slow-wave
sleep, a characteristic that is absent in reptiles and appears independently in mam-
mals and birds (Rattenborg 2006). Our point is that within a generally conserved
framework, early mammals made more use of olfactory–hippocampal associations
than other amniotes, and that this situation was related to the expansion of the
dorsal pallium, which received increasing dorsal thalamic input. Eventually, the
mammalian hippocampus received quite a strong thalamic sensory input via the
association areas of the neocortex and the entorhinal cortex, perhaps at the ex-
pense of the disappearance of direct thalamohippocampal projections. In a way, in
early mammaliaforms there may have been a stronger emphasis on hippocampal
function, which in part propelled dorsal pallial expansion.
In this context, a dorsalizing effect on dorsal pallial development as we have
suggested before, triggering an expansion of the dorsal cortex, may have been of
58 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
3.4.2
Summary
3.5
Origin of Neocortical Lamination
3.5.1
Laminar Organization of the Neocortex
In the adult, the neocortex consists of six layers, the most superficial of them
(layer I) characterized for containing very few neuronal elements and many tan-
gentially oriented fibers; layers II and III contain small pyramidal cells and some
interneurons and establish associative contacts with neighboring regions. Layer
IV contains spiny stellate cells that receive thalamic afferents arriving radially
from the underlying white matter and send projections to superficial layers, and
layers V and VI contain projection neurons to different subcortical targets (layer
VI cells mostly send a reciprocal projection to the thalamus). The development of
this structure is a highly ordered process (Fig. 14). During corticogenesis, there
are successive waves of neuronal migration from the cortical neuroepithelium and
other regions. The first wave constitutes an embryonic preplate (PP) that, with
the posterior arrival of cortical plate (CP) neurons, will split into a superficial
marginal zone (MZ, future layer I), and a deeper subplate (SP). Eventually, most
cells from the MZ and SP die in late development or become highly diluted due
to telencephalic expansion. CP neurons arrange between the MZ and the SP and
will make the adult layers II–VI of the neocortex. These layers are generated in
an inside-out sequence in which early born neurons remain in deeper positions,
while neurons born later migrate past the earlier ones and locate more superficially
(Angevine and Sidman 1961; Rakic 1974).
Fig. 14 Laminar organization of the neocortex. There are several waves of cell migration
making up the distinct layers of the adult neocortex. Initially, a preplate is formed which
is later split into a superficial marginal zone (MZ) and a subplate (SP) by the arrival of
cells forming the cortical plate (CP). There, cells arrange in an inside-out gradient in which
early-produced cells locate in deep layers and late-produced cells locate in superficial layers.
Arrow points to progressively later generation times in the cortical plate
60 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
In early development, the MZ contains some pioneer neurons (PNs) and reelin-
positive Cajal-Retzius cells (Meyer et al. 1998, 1999), while the SP consists of PNs
plus sparsely distributed polymorphic neurons (Kostovic and Rakic 1990). PNs are
the first cells to arrive to the cortex and their axon, directed subcortically, serves as
a guide for growing thalamocortical axons (McConnell et al. 1989, 1994). Thalamic
and corticocortical axons arriving from the underlying white matter usually reach
the SP and remain there for some time before entering the developing CP. Cajal-
Retzius cells in the MZ help in radial glia maintenance and provide clues for radial-
migrating neurons that will form the CP (Soriano et al. 1997; Supèr et al. 1998a,b,
Xie et al. 2002). Thus, the MZ and SP participate in regulating cell proliferation,
neuronal migration, axonal growth, and thalamocortical synaptogenesis in the CP
(Alcántara et al. 2006; Kanold 2004; Marín-Padilla 1998; Xie et al. 2002; Alcántara
et al. 2006).
Neurons populate the PP and the developing cortex from several sources: one is
the subcortical ganglionic eminence (principally its medial ad caudal components),
which contributes PNs and GABAergic interneurons (Anderson et al. 1997a,b;
Bystron et al. 2005; Deng and Elberger 2001, 2003; Jiménez et al. 2002a,b; Kriegstein
and Noctor 2004; Lavdas et al. 1999; Marín and Rubenstein 2001, 2003; McConnell
et al. 1989; Morante-Oria et al. 2003; Nadarajah and Parnavelas 2002). A more
important source is the cortical neuroepithelium, providing excitatory, radially
migrating cells mostly to the SP and CP (Rakic 1995, 2000; Nadarajah et al. 2003;
Kriegstein and Noctor 2004; O’Leary and Borngasser 2006). Nevertheless, other
pallial regions contribute tangentially migrating, excitatory neurons to the PP (like
the dorsomedial cortical hem, providing Cajal-Retzius cells) and perhaps also to
the CP (Del Río et al. 1995; Meyer et al. 1998, 2002, 2004; Yang et al. 2000; Abraham
et al. 2004; Bielle et al. 2005).
3.5.2
Comparison of Mammalian Neocortex and Reptilian Cortex: Layer Homologies
Compared to the reptilian cortex, which has only a few neurons in its radial depth
(Ulinski 1990; Voogd et al. 1988), the mammalian neocortex is characterized by
its radial expansion, being organized in radial columns about 100 cells in depth
(Rockel et al. 1980; Rakic 1988). In mammals, neurons at different levels of the ra-
dial column belong to each of the six tangential laminae that make up this structure,
which in reptiles are no more than three. In this context, Marín-Padilla (1971, 1972,
1978, 1992, 1998) and Marín-Padilla and Marín-Padilla (1982) originally proposed
that the amphibian pallial cells and the reptilian cortical cells actually corresponded
to the mammalian embryonic preplate (especially the SP). His view was that the de-
velopmentally earliest cortical cells might be the best candidates for homology with
the ancestral reptilian cells. Thus, the entire CP (adult cortical layers II–VI), which
develops after the PP, might be considered a newly developed structure in mam-
malian evolution. Although sometimes viewed as an outdated recapitulationist
view, this interpretation is consistent with the heterochronic concept of hypermor-
Origin of Neocortical Lamination 61
phosis, in which additional stages are added at the end of embryonic development
(see Gould 1977; McKinney and McNamara 1991). A somewhat similar hypothesis
was provided by Ebner (1969) and Reiner (1991, 1993), who proposed that instead
of corresponding to the mammalian preplate, the reptilian cortex mostly corre-
sponds to the deep layers VI and V of the mammalian CP. Cells in the superficial
layers IV–II are connectionally, morphologically, and neurochemically different
from reptilian cells, and can be considered as a derived character of the mammalian
neocortex. Moreover, early-produced, deep-layer cortical neurons migrate radially
by translocation (a mode of migration independent of radial glia, in which the api-
cal process of the cell contacts the subpial layer, and the cell body is dragged toward
the surface while the apical process shortens), while late-produced, more super-
ficial neurons migrate to the cortex by locomoting along a radial glia that serves
as a substrate, a process that requires the action of the cyclin-dependent kinase
cdk5 and its activator p35 (Nadarajah et al. 2001). These differences in migratory
processes may reflect different phylogenetic origins in time (Aboitiz 1999a, 2001a).
Furthermore, other authors consider that much of the mammalian SP is not
ancestral; instead, this structure can be viewed as an embryonic acquisition of
the mammalian brain, since it is most complex in those areas that appear later
in mammalian evolution, and in mammals with more complex brains (Kostovic
and Rakic 1990). These authors proposed that the SP evolved as an embryonic
device to support the development of corticocortical connectivity. Other evidence
suggests the existence of a preplate-like structure in embryonic reptiles (Nacher
et al. 1996), which would imply that the preplate does not correspond to an adult
but to an embryonic ancestral structure. In this context, it is important to note that
in mammals, the difference between the embryonic SP and the deepest layers of
the adult cortical plate are not absolutely clear-cut, and that these structures may
be developmentally related. For example, the gene Tbr1 marks subplate neurons
and the earliest CP neurons; and mutations in this gene cause defects in both
the preplate and the deepest layer VI of isocortex (Hevner et al. 2001; Kolk et al.
2005), suggesting similarities in genetic organization between these structures. In
a similar line, in the rat and other mammals with a limited or moderate degree of
isocortical expansion, many subplate cells survive into adulthood, forming layer
VII (Reep 2000; Woo et al. 1991; Harman et al. 1995). Furthermore, some authors
claim that in marsupials there is no subplate; rather, the equivalent to the subplate
consists of the earliest generated layer VI cells that become incorporated into the
lower CP (Harman et al. 1995; Reep 2000). This would imply that an embryonic
subplate is not ancestral to mammals. Despite these controversies (some of which
we will intend to clarify below), it seems generally acknowledged at this point that
the superficial cortical layers represent a new acquisition of the mammalian brain.
We originally proposed that the Pax6 gene was involved in the evolutionary
origin of the isocortical superficial layers (see Aboitiz et al. 2001b, 2003), and that
this involvement consisted of the recruitment of the cortical subventricular zone
(SVZ) in the neurogenetic process. The SVZ is a proliferating compartment of
the cortical neuroepithelium that is located peripheral to the ventricular zone,
62 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
an earlier proliferative layer. Mutants for Pax6 exhibit cortical migration defects,
consisting of the inability to migrate and differentiate of superficial, late-generated
isocortical neurons belonging to layers IV to II, which are generated in the SVZ
(Caric et al. 1997; Fukuda et al. 2000). Furthermore, Pax6 promotes conversion
of ventricular zone to SVZ progenitors and stimulates neurogenesis (Heins et al.
2002; Scardigli et al. 2003). The genes Ngn1 and Ngn2 also regulate the transit of
cortical progenitors from the ventricular to the subventricular zones (Britz et al.
2006), but possibly in a Pax6-dependent manner (Scardigli et al. 2003). Further
evidence indicates that, while activation of the genes Ngn1 and Ngn2 is required to
specify the laminar characteristics of early-born cortical neurons, upper layer cor-
tical neurons are specified in an Ngn-independent manner, requiring instead the
synergistic activities of Pax6 and Tlx (Schuurmans et al. 2004). Recently, Molnár
et al. (2006) and Kriegstein et al. (2006) presented evidence indicating a progressive
development of the subventricular zone in large-brained mammals, its near ab-
sence in the reptilian embryonic dorsal cortex, and its parallel development in the
avian nidopallium, which supports the interpretation above. It will be of interest
to determine if there is a contribution of the subventricular zone and of the Pax6
gene and related ones to dorsal cortical development in reptiles.
In cortical development, the subventricular zone appears first in the lateral
aspect of the hemisphere, near the pallial-subpallial boundary, and in later
stages expands into more dorsal regions (Nieto et al. 2004; Zimmer et al. 2004).
Perhaps one possibility to reconcile the two hypotheses of lateral neocortical
origins (ventral pallial/DVR vs dorsal pallial; see Sects. 3.2 and 3.3) is that the
mammalian cortical subventricular zone originated in evolution as a consequence
of the propagation of a late-proliferating activity originally restricted to the
lateral aspect of the hemisphere. This event might be related to the enhanced
morphogenetic activity of the cortical antihem or a related source in the lateral
and ventral pallium, and to the consequent diffusion of some morphogenetic
signal(s) into more dorsal pallial regions. This wave inducing late proliferative
activity recruited neural precursors from the dorsal neuroepithelium and gave
rise to the neocortical subventricular zone. In this way, although originating in
the dorsal pallial region, cells from the subventricular zone could have arisen
as a consequence of the influence of a late-proliferating ventral or lateral pallial
proliferative activity. This possibility would be partly consistent with Butler’s
(1994a) proposal of the involvement of a subventricular zone in the origin of both
the reptilian DVR and the mammalian neocortex and could also help explain
the complex relation between cortical layering and the claustrum in the insula of
some basal insectivores (Künzle and Raddtke-Schuller 2001).
3.5.3
Is There a Preplate in Reptiles?
the MZ of the developing cortex of turtles, lizards, and crocodiles; at lower levels,
reelin expression has also been detected below the developing cortex of turtles and
lizards and in the ventricular zone of crocodiles (Bar et al. 2000; Bernier et al. 2000;
Goffinet et al. 1999; Pérez-García et al. 2001; Tissir et al. 2003). In crocodiles, these
neurons were found to co-express p73 (Tissir et al. 2003), which is a marker of
reelin-positive Cajal-Retzius cells that derive from the cortical hem in the medial
hemisphere (Meyer et al. 2002). Considering that in the mammalian dentate gyrus,
one of the roles of reelin is to guide growing entorhinal axons (Förster et al. 2006b),
it is possible that the ancestral function of this molecule in early amniotes was to
attract dorsal cortex axons into the medial cortex. This would explain the origin
of most Cajal-Retzius cells from the cortical hem in the dorsomedial aspect of
the telencephalon. Interestingly, a recent report showed that HAR1F, an RNA gene
co-expressed with reelin in Cajal-Retzius cells, has been the subject of intense
selection during human evolution (Pollard et al. 2006).
In mammals, both the level of reelin expression per cell and the number of
reelin-positive neurons in the marginal zone are much higher than in reptiles,
indicating that with the appearance of the neocortex there has been a unique
amplification of reelin expression in Cajal-Retzius cells (Bar and Goffinet 2000;
Bar et al. 2000). Furthermore, the levels of reelin expression seem to correlate
with the degree of cortical columnarity in different reptilian species (Bar and
Goffinet 2000; Bar et al. 2000; Meyer et al. 2002). It is possible that in mammals,
the increase of reelin expression occurred in the context of controlling the final
stages of cortical migration and layer positioning (Aboitiz 1999; Aboitiz et al. 2001,
2003c). Nevertheless, the fact that in reptiles reelin is expressed in the marginal
zone and in other regions below the cortex suggests that this protein may have
other functions besides controlling the end stages of neuronal migration.
In the developing reptilian cortex, some authors argue for the presence of an
incipient PP that is split by the arrival of the CP (Bernier et al. 1999; Goffinet et al.
1999; Supèr et al. 1998b; Tissir et al. 2003). In our view, this preplate-like structure
might consist in large part of Cajal-Retzius-like cells and subpallial, tangentially
migrating cells, but it is not clear that it contains radial-migrating excitatory ele-
ments. Early migration of subpallial, inhibitory neurons into the pallium has been
suggested in amphibians (Brox et al. 2003), birds (Cobos et al. 2001a), and rep-
tiles. In the developing lizard brain, Nacher et al. (1996) described the presence of
somatostatin-positive cells appearing first in the inner plexiform layer and later in
the outer plexiform layer of the medial and dorsal cortices. Cordery and Molnár
(1999) observed cells positive for neuropeptide Y, scattered in all regions of the
ventral pallium of the turtle. Finally, some early tangentially migrating neurons
observed in the mammalian intermediate zone may perhaps correspond to tan-
gential neurons traveling in the reptilian intermediate zone (Cordery and Molnár
1999; Bar et al. 2000). Therefore, although in reptiles there may be early tangen-
tially migrating neurons, some of which are perhaps comparable to interneurons
of the mammalian preplate and SP, it is possible that the latter are more complex
in terms of cell numbers and types.
64 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
3.5.4
Origin of the Inside-out Developmental Gradient
Fig.15 This diagram depicts a possible sequence in the acquisition of laminar characteristics
of the neocortex. In reptiles, with an outside-in neurogenetic gradient (arrow upwards)
afferents follow the pioneer neurons (red diamonds) in the superficial marginal zone, above
the cortex (ovals). In the hippocampus, there is already an inside-out gradient (arrow
downwards) but afferents and pioneer neurons are still located superficially. In Erinaceus,
axons enter the cortical plate from below but may run obliquely and end in the marginal
zone. Finally, in the primary sensory cortex of advanced mammals, most pioneer neurons
locate in the subplate, and axons enter the cortex radially, synapsing with neurons in layer IV
splitting into a MZ above and a SP (containing PNs that guide thalamic axons) below
the developing CP, with the origin of the inside-out neurogenetic gradient (this
link is supported by recent developmental evidence; Rakic et al. 2006). However,
it leaves wanting an adaptive explanation of these changes (Aboitiz et al. 2003b).
In addition, comparative evidence suggests that inside-out lamination and the
change from superficial to deep position of afferents are dissociable to some
extent. The mammalian hippocampus has an inside-out laminar pattern while
its afferents are still organized superficially in layer I, suggesting that the inside-
out lamination pattern originated before the rerouting of afferents (Aboitiz et al.
2003c). Furthermore, in the neocortex of small-brained insectivores, axons initially
enter the cortical plate in a radial direction, but then follow an oblique course, reach
the superficial layer I and run tangentially in it for a long distance (Valverde et al.
1986; Supèr et al. 1998a,b), indicating that the superficial, tangential arrangement
of thalamic afferents remained an important feature after the origin of inside-out
cortical lamination.
Considering that early-produced cortical plate cells may represent an ancestral
phenotype, while late-produced cells are considered to be an evolutionary acqui-
sition, we have suggested that the cdk5/p35 pathway, which assists late-produced
neurons while migrating along the radial glia (Nadarajah et al. 2001; see Sect. 3.5.2),
is somehow related to the migration of the phylogenetically new cell types of the
mammalian isocortex and to the origin of the inside-out neurogenetic gradient
of the isocortex (Aboitiz 1999a, 2001; Aboitiz et al. 2001a,b). More specifically,
although cdk5 is required for locomotion along the radial glia, its activator p35 is
66 Origin of the Mammalian Brain
important for migrating neurons to cross the previously produced layers of cells
inside the CP. Cells in isocortical layer V might represent an intermediate condition
between the ancestral phenotypes and the new ones, as they originate in the deep
ventricular zone instead of the subventricular zone, and are morphologically and
neurochemically reptilian-like; but like younger cells, they seem to be more depen-
dent on cdk5 than cells of layer VI to cross the subplate and other cortical layers.
Again, the study of cdk5/p35 expression in the developing cortex of reptiles may
provide substantial information on isocortical evolution. “Additionally, reelin may
have contributed to the inverted layering by arresting migration and detaching
neurons from the radial glia, thus permitting late-born neurons to use the latter
to migrate past early-born neurons. If reelin fails as in the reeler mutant, neurons
stack in a roughly outside-in gradient, maintaining abnormal attachments to the
radial glia (Caviness 1976; Pinto-Lord et al. 1982).”
3.5.5
Pioneer Neurons and the Transition from a Tangential to a Radial Synaptic Organization
in the Neocortex
cially malpositioned (Caviness 1976; Caviness et al. 1988; Molnár 1998; Molnár
and Cordery 1999; Higashi et al. 2005). Moreover, like the normal SP, the reeler
superplate expresses neurocan, which serves as a guide for thalamocortical axonal
growth (Li et al. 2005b). In addition, in prenatally irradiated mice, in which the
subplate is partially destroyed, thalamocortical axons traverse the CP obliquely to
run up to the marginal zone, somewhat resembling more primitive arrangements
(Li et al. 2005a). Likewise, cdk5 and p35 mutants apparently show defects in pre-
plate splitting and an alteration of the trajectory of thalamic axons, which may be
a consequence of a misplacement of PNs (Rakic et al. 2006). Overall, this evidence
may indicate that the reelin and cdk5/p35 signaling systems were also involved in
the repositioning of PNs during neocortical evolution.
3.5.6
Summary
3.6
Expansion of the Neocortex
3.6.1
Multiplication of Cortical Areas
After the neocortex acquired its basic laminar structure and its main visual, au-
ditory, and somatosensory inputs, it began a progressive tangential expansion in
several mammalian lineages, leading to the multiplication of sensory and motor
areas receiving distinct thalamic inputs. Thus, while some small-brained mam-
mals may have less than 20 distinct cortical areas, in macaques there may be more
than 50 (Northcutt and Kaas 1995; Catania et al. 1999; Van Essen et al. 1992). Al-
though Brodmann (1909) counted roughly the same number of areas in the human
brain, it is quite likely that this number is an underestimation (Wallace et al. 2002;
Striedter 2005). Possibly, the last common ancestor of extant mammals already had
a primary and a secondary visual area, an auditory area, a primary and a supple-
mentary somatosensory area, and a motor area that probably overlapped to some
extent with the somatosensory area (Northcutt and Kaas 1995). These areas have
been found in all mammals studied, and therefore are likely to have been present
in their common ancestor. Across species, the increase in the number of so-called
higher-order cortical areas that receive input from these primary areas (although
corticocortical connections are largely reciprocal) is related to the tangential ex-
pansion of the neocortex. Although increase in total cortical surface also produces
some increase in the size of each area, this does not account for the full expansion
of the cortical mantle (Changizi 2001; Changizi and Shimojo 2005). Tangential
cortical expansion has occurred several times in mammals (for example, primates
and carnivores, the most well-studied orders), producing a proliferation of distinct
cortical areas, many of them being largely unimodal but several being polymodal,
integrating different types of inputs. Some similarities have been observed in the
organization of these higher-level sensory areas between primates and carnivores
(Payne 1993), but it is not clear if these represent true homology or parallelism.
The addition of new cortical areas in phylogeny has been proposed to result
from the parcellation of initially overlapping inputs into distinct cortical fields
(Ebbesson 1980, 1984). Similarly, Krubitzer (1995, 2000) and Krubitzer and Kahn
(2003) proposed that new cortical areas emerged from the selective aggregation of
modular heterogeneities within phylogenetically more ancient areas. One such ex-
ample could be the overlap between the somatosensory and motor areas observed
in some marsupials, which become segregated in placental mammals (Lende 1963).
Another, not alternative possibility is that the addition of new cortical areas results
from the expansion of the morphogenetic fields generated by distinct signaling
sources during cortical development. Disbalances in FGF, EMX, or PAX6 signaling
during corticogenesis may have profound effects in the relative extension of occip-
ital vs frontal areas (Bishop et al. 2000, 2002; Muzio and Mallamaci 2005; Zhou et al.
2001; Mallamaci et al. 2000; Grove and Fukuchi-Shigomori 2003). This situation
can be likened to the expansion of distinct cortical fields in different species: for
Expansion of the Neocortex 69
indicating that dexterity is not the only factor involved in its growth (Nudo and
Masterton 1990a,b; Striedter 2005). Nevertheless, considering a fixed brain size,
animals with higher dexterity tend to have an increased length of the corticospinal
tract. Furthermore, the large-brained ungulates and cetaceans have been omitted
from these analyses, which could become important exceptions to the dependency
of this tract on brain size.
Interhemispheric connections are also a feature of mammalian brains. We
(Aboitiz and Montiel 2003; Aboitiz et al. 2003a) have proposed that these fibers
originated in early mammals as a consequence of the development of precise topo-
graphic maps of the sensory surfaces (visual, somatosensory, and also the tonotopic
representations of the auditory cortices). In these conditions, each hemisphere
contained a topographic map of the contralateral sensory field, there being no
continuity between them at the midline. Thus, interhemispheric fibers may have
served as a bridge between the two hemi-representations at the midline, restoring
the continuity of the sensory field (Lepore et al. 1985, 1986; Ptito 2003). In the
case of audition, the connection between the hemispheres may have contributed
to the spatial location of sounds at an additional, cortical level (sound localiza-
tion mechanisms are already present at brainstem levels). In reptiles, topographic
sensory representations are largely localized in the optic tectum, which has a well-
developed tectal commissure to serve the midline fusion process, but mammals
had to develop a similar system when the neocortex acquired topographic maps
of the sensory surface. Speed of communication between the hemispheres may
have been crucial at these early processing levels, which is consistent with the fact
that primary and secondary sensory areas have the largest and faster-conducting
callosal fibers (with the largest fibers communicating auditory areas; Aboitiz et al.
1992). In many marsupials, interhemispheric connections make a long turn via the
anterior commissure, but in some larger-brained marsupials, interhemispheric
axons take a shortcut via the fasciculus aberrans running via the internal capsule
to the anterior commissure. In placental mammals, interhemispheric fibers run
via the massive corpus callosum, which develops dorsal to the hippocampal com-
missure, thus minimizing axonal distance and signal transmission time between
the hemispheres (Shang et al. 1997). During development, callosal axons cross the
midline via the formation of a glial wedge that establishes a sort of bridge between
the hemispheres (Shu and Richards 2001). Furthermore, the molecular control
of this process is based on an ancient mechanism consisting on the orchestrated
action of genes such as Robo and Slit, which are related to midline axonal crossing
in both vertebrates and invertebrates (T Shu et al. 2003). Thus, while the develop-
ment of a glial structure allowing callosal axon passage across the midline may be
a novelty of early placental mammals, the molecular mechanisms involved were
probably co-opted from pre-existing devices.
Discussion 71
3.6.2
Summary
4
Discussion
4.1
An Overview
4.1.1
Early Neuronal Differentiation
Embryonic neural specification, in which epidermal cells are prevented from ac-
quiring neural fates by autosecretion of BMP proteins, is paralleled with the evi-
dence on the earliest nervous systems as basiepidermal neural nets derived from
the proliferating ectoderm. Furthermore, the delamination from a proliferating
epithelium and the participation of bHLH factors in early neuronal differentia-
tion seem to be common mechanisms in a wide variety of animals (Ross et al.
2003). Likewise, the aggregation of ciliated cells (receptors) may have also been
Discussion 71
3.6.2
Summary
4
Discussion
4.1
An Overview
4.1.1
Early Neuronal Differentiation
Embryonic neural specification, in which epidermal cells are prevented from ac-
quiring neural fates by autosecretion of BMP proteins, is paralleled with the evi-
dence on the earliest nervous systems as basiepidermal neural nets derived from
the proliferating ectoderm. Furthermore, the delamination from a proliferating
epithelium and the participation of bHLH factors in early neuronal differentia-
tion seem to be common mechanisms in a wide variety of animals (Ross et al.
2003). Likewise, the aggregation of ciliated cells (receptors) may have also been
72 Discussion
4.1.2
Patterning
the anterior neural ridge, secreting FGF8, which is later expressed in the dorsal
aspect of the frontal hemisphere; caudally, there is the telencephalic roof plate,
secreting several factors (BMPs, Wnts) that induce other factors (Emxs, Pax6),
which in some instances cooperate but in others (as in the cerebral cortex) they
antagonize each other. These factors trigger development of the medial pallium
and the dorsal pallium at the border between the FGF and the BMP/Wnt signaling
fields. An additional morphogenetic center is the antihem, located in the lateral
pallium. The dorsal pallium is quite a narrow strip of tissue in most nonmammals,
but expands significantly with the origin of the mammalian neocortex.
4.1.3
Diversification of the Hemispheres and Neocortical Origins
4.1.4
Olfaction, the Hippocampus and the Amygdala
The expansion of the dorsal pallium in early mammals may have related to the
development of extensive networks connecting the olfactory cortex and hippocam-
pus. These circuits are present in amphibians and reptiles, but in early mammals
the sense of olfaction acquired great relevance owing to their presumed nocturnal
habits (Jerison, 1973; Sagan, 1977; Lynch, 1986; Kemp 2005). Likewise, there was
a noticeable development of the auditory apparatus, making it possible to localize
sound sources in the dark. Although primitive cynodonts and mammal-like rep-
tiles had quite a narrow braincase with no signs of telencephalic expansion, the
acquisition of the mammalian grade (marked by the detachment of the inner ear
ossicles from the inner jaw) roughly coincides with a dramatic increase in brain size
(Kielan-Jaworowska et al. 2004). This may have been related to the growth of the
dorsal pallium, which began to receive an increasing number of sensory afferents,
both lemnothalamic and collothalamic. These inputs were then relayed to both the
hippocampus and the amygdala (Aboitiz et al. 2003, 2003c). This situation con-
trasts with that observed in reptiles, in which the bulk of collothalamic projections
is sent directly to the ventral pallial DVR, with little interaction with the medial
pallium. Nevertheless, in mammals a direct connection from the collothalamus
to the amygdala persists and operates as a fast-response system in situations of
alarming stimuli. This system relates to emotional responses to threatening stimuli
and to the response to stressful events.
4.1.5
Cortical Lamination
such as the apparent absence of a subplate proper in some marsupials (the subplate
appears to be included in the deepest cortical plate layers; Harman et al. 1995; Reep
2000). Perhaps more parsimonious, but in the same line, is the interpretation that
the deepest cortical plate cells correspond to the cells of the reptilian dorsal cortex.
More superficial, later-appearing and phylogenetically newer cortical neurons are
produced in the subventricular zone instead of the ventricular zone and depend on
Pax6 for their differentiation (Schuurmans et al. 2004). Thus, we proposed that this
gene along with others were fundamental in the acquisition of the superficial layers
of the neocortex, which derived mostly from the proliferative subventricular zone
(Aboitiz et al. 2003c). Thus, a fundamental event in neocortical origins consisted
of the differentiation of a subventricular zone in the cortical neuroepithelium,
which was probably associated with both the tangential and radial expansion of
the dorsal pallium. This proliferating zone originates in the lateral aspect of the
hemisphere, and in reptiles and birds it may have been related to the development
of the DVR, since it was restricted to ventral pallial sectors.
Another curious neocortical feature is its inside-out neurogenetic gradient. We
suggested that this was a strategy for these late-produced neurons to establish
synaptic contacts with the thalamic and cortical afferents that were located super-
ficially in layer I. The hippocampus resembles this primitive condition, in which the
developmental gradient is inside-out, and the afferents are located superficially in
layer I. In the development of the inside-out gradient, the signaling pathway of the
cyclin-dependent kinase 5 (cdk5) was possibly of special importance; in particular,
its activator p35 may have been crucial for neurons to be able to cross the layers
of previously migrated cells (Chae et al. 1997). Furthermore, the protein reelin,
secreted by Cajal-Retzius cells in the marginal zone, may have contributed to this
organization by permitting the generation of a cell-free layer I, which these neu-
rons, with increased migratory capacity, tended to penetrate. In addition, reelin is
hypothesized to block neural-glia, and perhaps neuron-to-neuron, attachments,
mechanism that may have provided interneuronal space for cortical plate neurons
to cross the subplate and layers of older neurons (this does not exclude other de-
velopmental functions for reelin). As the neocortex expanded tangentially, some
axons begun to enter the cortex via the underlying white matter, instead of run-
ning in the superficial marginal zone as they do in the reptilian cortex. This new
position served as a shortcut to access the dorsal pallium. Intermediate forms are
seen in primitive mammals, where axons penetrate the cortex from the underlying
white matter but later cross to the marginal zone to run tangentially along it for
some distance. In this process, the position of pioneer neurons, migrating from the
subpallium and directing the growth of thalamocortical axons, gradually shifted
from an ancestral position in the superficial marginal zone to a deeper position
below the cortical plate, in the mammalian subplate. Genes and signaling pathways
involved in the generation of the inside-out cortical lamination such as reelin and
cdk5/p35 may have been co-opted for the redistribution of pioneer neurons.
Issues with Evolutionary Theory 77
4.1.6
Tangential Expansion and the Origin of New Areas
Finally, the cortical sheet expands dramatically in some mammals with the conse-
quent increase in number of cortical areas. Most mammals share at least two
visual areas (striate, lemnothalamic-receiving, and extrastriate, collothalamic-
receiving), an auditory (collothalamic) area, and a somatosensory/motor (lem-
nothalamic) area. However, in large-brained mammals the number of areas may
exceed 50. The origin of new areas has been explained on the basis of segregation of
heterogeneous inputs to one region and the consequent homogeneization of these
fields; and on the appearance of new signaling centers that trigger the differenti-
ation of new cortical regions (Krubitzer 1995, 2000; Allman 1999; Krubitzer and
Kahn 2003; Striedter 2005). Both mechanisms may be valid; for example in mar-
supials there is significant overlap between the somatosensory and motor areas,
while in placental mammals these regions are well separated; in addition, it has
been postulated that the primate visual area MT is a signaling center that patterns
the organization of other visual areas (Rosa 2002). The isocortex is also notorious
for its connectivity: it develops a long corticospinal tract, related to increasing
voluntary control of the spinal cord, and interhemispheric connections, which we
proposed to be originally related to the fusion of the two hemirepresentations
of the sensory surfaces in each hemisphere at the midline (Aboitiz and Montiel
2003). Auditory interhemispheric connections are among the fastest-conducting
of all and may have participated in the localization of sound sources.
Provided this synopsis of the subjects dealt with in this article, we will now
turn into some more general issues that bear relation with theoretical concepts of
evolutionary biology and comparative morphology.
4.2
Issues with Evolutionary Theory
4.2.1
Genetic Conservatism Versus Morphological Diversity
One of our goals was to establish a continuity from the origin of the vertebrate
central nervous system to the appearance and diversification of the mammalian
cerebral cortex. In doing this, it became apparent that there have been key inno-
vations such as the grouping of ciliated sensory receptors in both the imaginal
disks of Drosophila and in the neural tube of vertebrates, and the delamination
process by which neurons segregate from the proliferating epithelium, which are
shared by many metazoans and may reflect common developmental mechanisms.
These correspondences may reflect a common origin or a different origin starting
from a shared ancestral developmental schedule. Considering the morphological
dissimilarities in the different taxa (for example, that neurulation appears to be
a late event in the history of protostomes; Lowe et al. 2003, 2006), we tend to prefer
78 Discussion
the hypothesis that there was a common developmental schedule prior to the diver-
sification of protostomes and deuterostomes, which became involved in different
morphogenetic processes in the different lineages. One particularly good example
of this situation is the role of the Pax6 gene in eye development; mutations in this
gene produce defects in eye development in Drosophila, mouse, and humans (Quir-
ing et al. 1994). Furthermore, if the mouse Pax6 gene is expressed ectopically in
Drosophila, it triggers the development of eye structures in regions such as wings
or legs (Halder et al. 1995). The Pax6 gene has been involved in photoreceptor
development in a variety of animals and is likely to have had a role in the original
specification of photoreceptors (Gehring 1996; Glardon et al. 1997; Pineda et al.
2000). However, the morphogenetic processes giving rise to the different types
of eye were acquired independently in the history of each lineage (for example,
arthropods and insects, mollusks and vertebrates), with Pax6 being a crucial el-
ement in all of them, perhaps owing to its early participation in photoreceptor
specification.
This situation is somehow reminiscent of the old discussion about whether all
animals could be described according to a common body plan or instead each phy-
lum was characterized by a unique body structure. This controversy dates from
at least two centuries ago, clearly exemplified by the Cuvier-Geoffroy debate of
pre-Darwinian times, in which Etienne Geoffroy Saint-Hilarie defended the unity
of composition doctrine, while Georges Cuvier posited four basic plans of animal
organization: vertebrates, articulates, mollusks, and radiates (Appel 1987). Since
1830, it became widely accepted that separate phyla are indeed characterized by
their unique body plans, thus supporting Cuvier’s position, which interpreted mor-
phology from a functional viewpoint (although being antievolutionist in the sense
that it rejected Lamarck’s transformism). In light of recent evidence, showing the
striking conservation of regulatory genes such as the Dlx/Hox cluster and other
genes that are essential for the formation of the body plan in most known phyla,
these ideas may be reconciled by considering an ancestral genetic patterning device
that generated different morphogenetic mechanisms in each lineage (Slack et al.
1993). Thus, although vertebrate brains share molecular patterning mechanisms
with Drosophila and other animals, the morphogenetic processes in which they
participate have diverged in each phylum, making a specific morphological orga-
nization for vertebrates, another for arthropods, another for mollusks and so on.
4.2.2
Development as a Clue to Evolution
Another issue is that developmental processes may yield clues about how evolution-
ary changes occurred. Although Haeckel’s concept that ontogeny recapitulates phy-
logeny is usually seen as an outdated preformist conception, evidence has shown
an important concordance between developmental events and evolutionary tran-
sitions. In this sense, we should consider the developmental process not as strictly
recapitulating earlier adult phylogenetic stages as considered by Haeckel, but rather
Issues with Evolutionary Theory 79
4.2.3
Developmental Processes and Homology Criteria
Perhaps the most basic issue in comparative biology relates to the concept of
homology. In order to determine the ancestry of a given structure, a criterion
for homology must be established. The modern concept of homology specifies
identity between two structures, each present in one of two different species,
which is established in the context of the hypothesis of a common ancestor that
80 Discussion
while there is also good evidence for dramatic divergence in brain structure during
late development (Northcutt 1981, 1984, 1990b, 2002; Striedter 2005).
One particularly controversial case of homology is the origin of the neocortex.
Hypotheses based on adult connectivity have pointed to homology between the
lateral neocortex and the reptilian DVR, while other hypotheses (based on to-
pography, developmental patterns and on adult connectivity, Aboitiz 1992, 1995;
Bruce and Neary 1995) have proposed homology with the reptilian dorsal pallium.
We conclude that the reptilian/avian DVR and the mammalian neocortex have
different developmental origins and are not likely to be homologs. Nevertheless,
according to Northcutt (2003) even if these structures had a similar developmental
origin, a differentiated DVR should prove to be ancestral to the neocortex to be con-
sidered its homolog. Fossil evidence indicates that early mammal-like reptiles had
quite a simple telencephalon with no signs of expansion, indicating that reptilian
and mammalian brains underwent independent histories of brain development.
4.2.4
Development, Adaptation, and Behavior
not give rise to big branches because they look so different. The possibility of major
changes occurring through the accumulation of small changes is logically possible,
and although for practical reasons has not been demonstrated, there is no evidence
against it. This does not mean that in some instances there may be some discon-
tinuous changes, such as polyploidy, as it occurs in several plant species or some
homeotic transformations of body parts. For example, most mammals (including
the giraffe) have a fixed number of cervical vertebrae (seven). However, in birds the
number of cervical vertebrae is highly variable (Carroll 1988), and the addition or
deletion of vertebrae must have occurred by discontinuous events (one individual
may be born with one extra vertebra but not half of it). Something similar may hap-
pen with frameshift events in which the identity of some body parts or segments
have been displaced caudally or rostrally (Wagner and Chiu 2001). But these are rel-
atively simple developmental changes, involving the number or relative position of
already formed organs. In our opinion, the evolution of biologically complex struc-
tures such as the eye, in which the development of different tissues must be tightly
coordinated in order to produce an elaborate optic device, is unlikely to have arisen
in a single or a few steps. Rather, phylogenetic evidence strongly indicates a series
of intermediate steps prior to the acquisition of complex eyes such as those of
insects, cephalopods, or vertebrates (Dawkins 1997). For example, in the evolution
of the mollusk eye, small improvements in image formation took place (gradual
increase in curvature, forming either a concave or a convex light-receiving surface,
and eventually, the gradual narrowing of the pupil and the formation of a lens in
concave eyes). Likewise, the origin of the mammalian cerebral cortex, although it is
a structure at first sight radically different from comparable structures in reptiles,
can be explained by a series of changes in region-specifying genes and neuronal
migration-controlling genes. Theories invoking macromutations producing the
whole diverse array of structural characteristics of the neocortex, i.e., its inverted
lamination, radial organization, tangential expansion and proliferation of sensory
areas, are still in lack of serious supporting evidence. Another issue refers to the
origin of the vertebrate brain. While Northcutt (2005) argues for a practically si-
multaneous origin of the different vertebrate characters, Butler (2006) claims for
a serial transformation in which telencephalic origin was the latest event. In our
view, this may have been a continuous but rapidly changing scenario, in which
acquisition of one character facilitated the development of new characters and so
on (Sect. 3.2.4). In this context, a major discontinuity in the origin of vertebrates
is evidenced by the double duplication of the Dlx-Hox cluster (Digregorio et al.
1995; Stock et al. 1996). Although the genetic duplication events were most likely
discrete (but possibly appearing in more than one individual), this does not imply
that morphological change occurred equally suddenly. Rather, it is quite likely that
in a first instance, this mutation had no major phenotypic consequences. In fact, re-
cent evidence indicates that mammalian Hox genes, despite being present in larger
numbers than other vertebrates, are in many cases redundant in function and the
loss of redundancy does not imply major phenotypic changes (García-Fernández
2005; Tvrdik and Capecchi 2006). After genetic duplication, genes may have started
Final Comments 83
4.3
Final Comments
There has been a renewed interest in the study of the relation between development
and evolution, which promises to yield a wealth of interesting evidence that may
dramatically change many of our current conceptions. Although this interest has
been focused on many systems, we consider that the case of the central nervous
system may be a particularly enlightening example of this relation. This is not
only for the selfish reason that in our brains resides the capacity to be thinking
beings, but also because this is probably one of the systems in which the relations
84 Discussion
between form and function acquire their most elaborate expression. Thus, interdis-
ciplinary approaches including physiology, development, behavior, and genetics
may become especially fruitful to unveil these complex interactions. More than
trying to confirm and make known our own views, our intention here has been to
provide a series of working hypotheses that may motivate other workers to pursue
experimental and theoretical work on these lines.
References
Aboitiz F, Montiel J, García RR (2005) Ancestry of the mammalian preplate and its deriva-
tives: relicts or embryonic adaptations? Rev Neurosci 16:359–376
Abraham H, Pérez-García CG, Meyer G (2004) P73 and reelin in Cajal-Retzius cells of the
developing human hippocampal formation. Cereb Cortex 14:484–495
Acámpora D, Simeone A (1999) Understanding the roles of Otx1 and Otx2 in the control of
brain morphogenesis. Trends Neurosci 22:116–122
Akimenko MA, Ekker M, Wegner J, Lin W, Westerfield M (1994) Combinatorial expression
of three zebrafish genes related to distal-less: part of a homeobox gene code for the head.
J Neurosci 14:3475–3486
Alcántara S, Pozas E, Ibáñez CF, Soriano E (2006) BDNF-modulated spatial organization of
Cajal-Retzius and GABAergic neurons in the marginal zone plays a role in the develop-
ment of cortical organization. Cereb Cortex 16:487–499
Allman JM (1999) Evolving brains. Scientific American, New York
Allman JM, Kaas JH, Lane RH, Miezin FM (1972) A representation of the visual field in
the inferior nucleus of the pulvinar in the owl monkey (Aotus trivirgatus). Brain Res
40:291–302
Alonso MC, Cabrera CV (1988) The achaete-scute gene complex of Drosophila melanogaster
comprises four homologous genes. EMBO J 7:2585–2591
Altman J, Bayer SA (1981) Time of origin of neurons of the rat superior colliculus in relation
to other components of the visual and visuomotor pathways. Exp Brain Res 42:424–434
Álvarez P, Lipton PA, Melrose R, Eichenbaum H (2001) Differential effects of damage within
the hippocampal region on memory for a natural, nonspatial odor-odor association.
Learn Mem 8:79–86
Anderson SA, Eisenstat DD, Shi L, Rubenstein JLR (1997a) Interneuron migration from
basal forebrain to neocortex: dependence on Dlx genes. Science 278:474–76
Anderson SA, Qiu M, Bulfone A, Eisenstat DD, Meneses J, Pedersen R, Rubenstein JLR
(1997b) Mutations of the homeobox genes Dlx-1 and Dlx-2 disrupt the striatal subven-
tricular zone and differentiation of late born striatal neurons. Neuron 19:27–37
Angevine JB, Sidman RL (1961) Autoradiographic study of cell migration during histogen-
esis of cerebral cortex in the mouse. Nature 192:766–768
Appel TA (1987) The Cuvier-Geoffroy debate. French biology in the decades before Darwin.
Oxford University Press, Oxford
Arthur W (2004) Biased embryos and evolution. Cambridge Univeristy Press, Cambridge
Ashwell KW, Hardman C, Paxinos G (2004) The claustrum is not missing from all monotreme
brains. Brain Behav Evol 64:223–241
Assimacopoulos S, Grove EA, Ragsdale CW (2003) Identification of a Pax6-dependent
epidermal growth factor family signaling source at the lateral edge of the embryonic
cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 23:6399–6403
Avigan MR, Powers AS (1995) The effects of MK-801 injections and dorsal cortex lesions on
maze learning in turtles (Chrysemis picta). Psychobiology 23:63–68
Ayala R, Shu T, Tsai L (2007) Trekking across the brain: the journey of neuronal migration.
Cell 128:29–43
Baker CV, Bronner-Fraser M (1997a) The origins of the neural crest. Part I: embryonic
induction. Mech Dev 69:3–11
Baker CV, Bronner-Fraser M (1997b) The origins of the neural crest. Part II: an evolutionary
perspective. Mech Dev 69:13–29
Bar I, Goffinet AM (2000) Evolution of cortical lamination: the reelin/Dab1 pathway. Novartis
Found Symp 228:114–125
Bar I, Lambert de Rouvroit C, Goffinet A (2000) The evolution of cortical development. An
hypothesis based on the reelin signaling pathway. Trends Neurosci 23:633–638
References 87
Barlow LA, Northcutt RG (1997) Taste buds develop autonomously from endoderm without
induction by cephalic neural crest or paraxial mesoderm. Development 124:949–957
Basch ML, Bronner-Fraser M, Garcia-Castro MI (2006) Specification of the neural crest
occurs during gastrulation and requires Pax7. Nature 441:218–222
Behan M, Haberly LB (1999) Intrinsic and efferent connections of the endopiriform nucleus
of the rat. J Comp Neurol 408:532–548
Belekhova MG, Ivazov NI (1983) Analysis of the conduction of visual, somatic and au-
diovibrational sensory information of the hippocampal cortex in the lizard (Russian)
Neirofiziologiia 15:153–160
Bergquist H, Källén B (1953a) Studies on the topography of the migration areas in the
vertebrate brain. Acta Anat 17:353–359
Bergquist H, Källén B (1953b) On the development of neuromeres to migration areas in the
vertebrate cerebral tube. Acta Anat 18:65–73
Bergquist H, Källén B (1954) Notes on the early histogenesis and morphogenesis of the
central nervous system in vertebrates. J Comp Neurol 100:627–659
Bernier B, Bar I, Pieau C, Lambert De Rouvroit C, Goffinet AM (1999) mRNA expression
during embryonic brain development in the turtle Emys orbicularis. J Comp Neurol
413:463–479
Bernier B, Bar I, D’Arcangelo G, Curran T, Goffinet AM (2000) Reelin mRNA expression
during embryonic brain development in the chick. J Comp Neurol 422:448–463
Bhattacharyya S, Bailey AP, Bronner-Fraser M, Streit A (2004) Segregation of lens and
olfactory precursors from a common territory: cell sorting and reciprocity of Dlx5 and
Pax6 expression. Dev Biol 271:403–414
Bielle F, Griveau A, Narboux-Neme N, Vigneau S, Sigrist M, Arber S, Wassef M, Pierani A
(2005) Multiple origins of Cajal-Retzius cells at the borders of the developing pallium.
Nat Neurosci 8:1002–1012
Bishop K, Goudreaua MG, O’Leary DD (2000) Regulation of area identity in the mammalian
neocortex by Emx2 and Pax6. Science 288:344–349
Bishop KM, Rubenstein JL, O’Leary DD (2002) Distinct actions of Emx1, Emx2, and Pax6 in
regulating the specification of areas in the developing neocortex. J Neurosci 22:7627–738
Blau A, Powers AS (1989) Discrimination learning in turtles after lesions of the dorsal cortex
or basal forebrain. Psychobiology 17:445–449
Bond J, Roberts E, Mochida GH, Hampshire DJ, Scott S, Askham JM, Springell K, Mahade-
van M, Crow YJ, Markham AF, Walsh CA, Woods CG (2002) ASPM is a major determinant
of cerebral cortical size. Nat Genet 32:316–320
Bota M (2003) From axis to triangle: the role of orbital cortex. Behav Brain Sci 26:552–553
Bourlat SJ, Nielsen C, Lockyer AE, Littlewood DT, Telford MJ (2003) Xenoturbella is
a deuterostome that eats molluscs. Nature 424:925–928
Bourlat SJ, Juliusdottir T, Lowe CJ, Freeman R, Aronowicz J, Kirschner M, Lander ES,
Thorndyke M, Nakano H, Kohn AB, Heyland A, Moroz LL, Copley RR, Telford MJ
(2006) Deuterostome phylegeny reveals monophyletic chordates and the new phylum
Xenoturbella. Nature 444:85–88
Bourne JA, Rosa MG (2006) Hierarchical development of the primate visual cortex, as
revealed by neurofilament immunoreactivity: early maturation of the middle temporal
area (MT). Cereb Cortex 16:405–414
Braun CB, Northcutt RG (1997) The lateral line system of hagfishes Craniata: Myxinoidea.
Acta Zool (Stockh) 78:247–268
Brauth SE (1990) Histochemical strategies in the study of neural evolution. Brain Behav Evol
36:100–115
88 References
Briggs DE (1992) Conodonts: a major extinct group added to the vertebrates. Science
256:1285–1286
Briggs DE, Clarkson ENK, Aldridge RJ (1983) The conodont animal. Lethaia 16:1–14
Britz O, Mattar P, Nguyen L, Langevin LM, Zimmer C, Alam S, Guillemot F, Schuurmans C
(2006) A role for proneural genes in the maturation of cortical progenitor cells. Cereb
Cortex 16 [Suppl 1]:i138-i151
Brodmann K (1909) Vergleichende localisationslehre der Grosshirnrinde in ihren Prinzipien
dargestellt auf grund des Zellenbaues. Barth, Leipzig
Brox A, Puelles L, Ferreiro B, Medina L (2003) Expression of the genes GAD67 and Distal-
less-4 in the forebrain of Xenopus laevis confirms a common pattern in tetrapods. J Comp
Neurol 461:370–393
Brox A, Puelles L, Ferreiro B, Medina L (2004) Expression of the genes Emx1, Tbr1, and
Eomes (Tbr2) in the telencephalon of Xenopus laevis confirms the existence of a ventral
pallial division in all tetrapods. J Comp Neurol 474:562–577
Bruce LL, Butler AB (1984a) Telencephalic connections in lizards: I. Projections to cortex.
J Comp Neurol 229:585–601
Bruce LL, Butler AB (1984b) Telencephalic connections in lizards. II. Projections to anterior
dorsal ventricular ridge. J Comp Neurol 229:602–615
Bruce LL, Neary TJ (1995) The limbic system of tetrapods: a comparative analysis of cortical
and amygdalar populations. Brain Behav Evol 46:224–234
Bulfone A, Kim HJ, Puelles L, Porteus MH, Grippo JF, Rubenstein JL (1993a) The mouse
Dlx-2 (Tes-1) gene is expressed in spatially restricted domains of the forebrain, face and
limbs in midgestation mouse embryos. Mech Dev 40:129–140
Bulfone A, Puelles L, Porteus MH, Frohman MA, Martín GR, Rubenstein JL (1993b) Spatially
restricted expression of Dlx-1, Dlx-2 (Tes-1), Gbx-2, and Wnt-3 in the embryonic day 12.5
mouse forebrain defines potential transverse and longitudinal segmental boundaries.
J Neurosci 13:3155–3172
Bunsey M, Eichenbaum H (1996) Conservation of hippocampal memory function in rats
and humans. Nature 379:255–257
Butler AB (1994a) The evolution of the dorsal thalamus of jawed vertebrates, including
mammals: cladistic analysis and a new hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 19:29–65
Butler AB (1994b) The evolution of the dorsal pallium in the telencephalon of amniotes:
cladistic analysis and a new hypothesis. Brain Res Rev 19:66–101
Butler AB (2000a) Chordate evolution and the origin of craniates: an old brain in a new
head. Anat Rec 261:111–125
Butler AB (2000b) Sensory system evolution and the origin of craniates. Philos Trans R Soc
Lond B 355:1309–1313
Butler AB (2000c) Topography and topology of the teleost telencephalon: a paradox resolved.
Neurosci Lett 293:95–98
Butler AB (2006) The serial transformation hypothesis of vertebrate origins: comment on
“The new head hypothesis revisited”. J Exp Zoolog B (Mol Dev Evol) 306:419–424
Butler AB, Hodos W (1996) Comparative vertebrate neuroanatomy. Evolution and adapta-
tion. Wiley-Liss, New York
Butler AB, Molnár Z (2002) Development and evolution of the collopallium in amniotes:
a new hypothesis of field homology. Brain Res Bull 57:475–479
Butler AB, Molnár Z, Manger PR (2002) Apparent absence of claustrum in monotremes:
implications for forebrain evolution in amniotes. Brain Behav Evol 60:230–240
Bystron I, Molnár Z, Otellin V, Blakemore C (2005) Tangential networks of precocious
neurons and early axonal outgrowth in the embryonic human forebrain. J Neurosci
25:2781–2792
References 89
Caric D, Gooday D, Hill RE, McConnell SK, Price DJ (1997) Determination of the migratory
capacity of embryonic cortical cells lacking the transcription factor Pax-6. Development
124:5087–5096
Carroll RL (1988) Vertebrate paleontology and evolution. Freeman Press, New York
Catania KC (1999) A nose that looks like a hand and acts like an eye: the unusual mechanosen-
sory system of the star-nosed mole. J Comp Physiol [A] 185:367–372
Catania KC, Lyon DC, Mock OB, Kaas JH (1999) Cortical organization in shrews: evidence
from five species. J Comp Neurol 410:55–72
Caviness VS Jr (1976) Patterns of cell and fiber distribution in the neocortex of the reeler
mutant mouse. J Comp Neurol 170:435–447
Caviness VS, Crandall JE, Edwards MA (1988) The reeler malformation. Implications for
neocortical histogenesis. In: Peters A, Jones EG (eds) Cerebral cortex, Vol. 7. Development
and maturation of the cerebral cortex. Plenum Press, New York, pp 59–89
Chae T, Kwon YT, Bronson R, Dikkes P, Li E, Tsai LH (1997) Mice lacking p35, a neuronal spe-
cific activator of Cdk5, display cortical lamination defects, seizures, and adult lethality.
Neuron 18:29–42
Changizi MA (2001) Principles underlying mammalian neocortical scaling. Biol Cybern
84:207–215
Changizi MA, Shimojo S (2005) Parcellation and area-area connectivity as a function of
neocortex size. Brain Behav Evol 66:88–98
Chapouton P, Gärtner A, Götz M (1999) The role of Pax6 in restricting cell migration
between developing cortex and basal ganglia. Development 126:5569–5579
Chen JY, Dzik J, Edgecombe GD, Ramsköld L, Zhou GQ (1995) A possible early cambrian
chordate. Nature 377:720–722
Chenn A, Walsh CA (2002) Regulation of cerebral cortical size by control of cell cycle exit
in neural precursors. Science 297:365–369
Ciani L, Salinas PC (2005) WNTs in the vertebrate nervous system: from patterning to
neuronal connectivity. Nat Rev Neurosci 6:351–362
Clack JA (2002) Gaining ground, The origin and evolution of tetrapods. Indiana University
Press, Bloomington, Indiana
Clack JA, Ahlberg PE, Finney SM, Alonso PD, Robinson J, Ketcham RA (2003) A uniquely
specialized ear in a very early tetrapod. Nature 425:65–69
Coates MI, Clack JA (1990) Polydactyly in the earliest known tetrapod limbs. Nature 347:66–
69
Coates MI, Clack JA (1991) Fish-like gills and breathing in the earliest known tetrapod.
Nature 352:234–236
Cobos I, Puelles L, Martínez S (2001a) The avian telencephalic subpallium originates in-
hibitory neurons that invade tangentially the pallium (dorsal ventricular ridge and
cortical areas). Dev Biol 239:30–45
Cobos I, Shimamura K, Rubenstein JL, Martínez S, Puelles L (2001b) Fate map of the avian
anterior forebrain at the four-somite stage, based on the analysis of quail-chick chimeras.
Dev Biol 239:46–67
Colombo M (2003) Avian and mammalian hippocampus: no degrees of freedom in evolution
of function. Behav Brain Sci 26:554–555
Conway Morris S (1993) The fossil record and the early evolution of the Metazoa. Nature
361:219–225
Cordery P, Molnár Z (1999) Embryonic development of connections in turtle pallium.
J Comp Neurol 413:26–54
90 References
Couly GF, Le Douarin NM (1985) Mapping of the early neural primordium in quail-chick
chimeras. I. Developmental relationships between placodes, facial ectoderm, and pros-
encephalon. Dev Biol 110:422–439
Cranney J, Powers AS (1983) The effects of core nucleus and cortical lesions in turtles on
reversal and dimensional shifting. Physiol Psychol 11:103–111
Dalton D, Chadwick R, McGinnis W (1989) Expression and embryonic function of empty
spiracles: a Drosophila homeo box gene with two patterning functions on the anterior-
posterior axis of the embryo. Genes Dev 3:1940–1956
Darwin C (1859) On the origin of species by means of natural selection. Murray, London
Darwin C (1871) The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. Murray, London
Dávila JC, Guirado S, Puelles L (2000) Expression of calcium-binding proteins in the dien-
cephalon of the lizard Psammodromus algirus. J Comp Neurol 427:67–92
Dávila JC, Andreu MJ, Real MA, Puelles L, Guirado S (2002) Mesencephalic and diencephalic
afferent conections to the thalamic nucleus rotundus in the lizard, Psammodromus
algirus. Eur J Neurosci 16:267–282
Dawkins R (1997) Climbing mount improbable. Norton & Company, London
Dawkins R (2003) A devil’s chaplain. Houghton Mifflin, Boston
Day LB, Crews D, Wilczynski W (1999) Relative medial and dorsal cortex volume in relation
to foraging ecology in congeneric lizards. Brain Behav Evol 54:314–322
Day LB, Crews D, Wilczynski W (2001) Effects of medial and dorsal cortex lesions on spatial
memory in lizards. Behav Brain Res 118:27–42
Deardorff MA, Tan C, Saint-Jeannet JP, Klein PS (2001) A role for frizzled 3 in neural crest
development. Development 128:3655–3663
De Beer GR (1924) The evolution of the pituitary. Br J Exp Biol 1:271–291
De Beer GR (1971) Homology, an unsolved problem. Oxford University Press, Oxford
De Carlos JA, López-Mascaraque L, Valverde F (1995) The telencephalic vesicles are inner-
vated by olfactory placode-derived cells: a possible mechanism to induce neocortical
development. Neuroscience 68:1167–1178
Del Río JA, Martínez A, Fonseca M, Auladell C, Soriano E (1995) Glutamate-like immunore-
activity and fate of Cajal-Retzius cells in the murine cortex as identified with calretinin
antibody. Cereb Cortex 5:13–21
Delsuc F, Brinkmann H, Chourrout D, Philippe H (2006) Tunicates and not cephalochordates
are the closest living relatives of vertebrates. Nature 439:965–968
Deng J, Elberger AJ (2001) The role of pioneer neurons in the development of mouse visual
cortex and corpus callosum. Anat Embryol 204:437–453
Deng J, Elberger AJ (2003) Corticothalamic and thalamocortical pathfinding in the mouse:
dependence on intermediate targets and guidance axis. Anat Embryol 207:177–192
Denton M (2002) Nature’s destiny: how the laws of biology reveal purpose in the universe.
Free Press, Sydney
De Robertis EM, Sasai Y (1996) A common plan for dorsoventral patterning in Bilateria.
Nature 380:37–40
Desan PH (1998) Organization of the cerebral cortex in trutle. In: The forebrain of reptiles.
Current concepts of structure and function. WK Schwerdtfeger, WJAJ Smeets (eds)
Karger, New York, pp 35–56
Desmond A (1982) Archetypes and ancestors. Paleontology in victorian London. University
of Chicago Press, Chicago
Deutsch J, Le Guyader H (1998) The neuronal zootype. An hypothesis. C R Acad Sci III
321:713–719
References 91
Di Fiore MM, Rastogi RK, Ceciliani F, Messi E, Botte V, Botte L, Pinelli C, D’Aniello B,
D’Aniello A (2000) Mammalian and chicken I forms of gonadotropin-releasing hormone
in the gonads of a protochordate, Ciona intestinalis. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:2343–
2348
Di Gregorio A, Spagnuolo A, Ristoratore F, Pischetola M, Aniello F, Branno M, Cariello L, Di
Lauro R (1995) Cloning of ascidian homeobox genes provides evidence for a primordial
chordate cluster. Gene 156:253–257
Dirksen ML, Morasso MI, Sargent TD, Jamrich M (1994) Differential expression of a Distal-
less homeobox gene Xdll-2 in ectodermal cell lineages. Mech Dev 46:63–70
Divac I, Thibault J, Skageberg G, Palacios JM, Dietl MM (1994) Dopaminergic innervation of
the brain in pigeons. The presumed ‘prefrontal cortex’. Acta Neurobiol Exp 54:227–234
Doron NN, Ledoux JE (1999) Organization of projections to the lateral amygdala from
auditory and visual areas of the thalamus in the rat. J Comp Neurol 412:383–409
Doron NN, Ledoux JE (2000) Cells in the posterior thalamus project to both amygdala
and temporal cortex: a quantitative retrograde double-labeling study in the rat. J Comp
Neurol 425:257–274
Døving KB (1998) The olfactory system of hagfishes. In: Jørgensen JM, Lomholt JP, Weber RE,
Malte H (eds) The biology of hagfishes. Chapman and Hall, London, pp 35–66
Dugas-Ford J, Ragsdale CW (2003) Some nuclei in chick dorsal telencephalon have the
molecular signature of layer 4 of the mammalian cerebral cortex. Brain Behav Evol
62:170
Dzik J (1995) Yunnanozoon and the ancestry of chordates. Acta Paleontol Polonica 40:341–
360
Dusek JA, Eichenbaum H (1997) The hippocampus and memory for orderly stimulus rela-
tions. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:7109–7114
Ebbesson SOE (1980) The parcellation theory and its relation to interspecific variability in
brain organization, evolutionary and ontogenetic development, and neuronal plasticity.
Cell Tissue Res 213:179–212
Ebbesson SOE (1984) Evolution and ontogeny of neural circuits. Behav Brain Sci 7:321–366
Ebbesson SOE, Heimer L (1970) Projections of the olfactory tract fibers in the nurse shark
(Ginglymostoma cirratum). Brain Res 17:47–55
Ebbesson SOE, Schroeder D (1971) Connections of the nurse shark’s telencephalon. Science
173:254–256
Ebner FF (1969) A comparison of primitive forebrain organization in metatherian and
eutherian mammals. Ann N Y Acad Sci 167:241–257
Edinger L (1906) Einiges von “Gehirn” des Amphioxus. Anat Anz 28:417–428
Eichenbaum H (1998) Using olfaction to study memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci 855:657–669
Eichenbaum H (1999) The hippocampus and mechanisms of declarative memory. Behav
Brain Res 103:123–133
Eichenbaum H (2000a) A cortical-hippocampal system for declarative memory. Nat Rev
Neurosci 1:41–50
Eichenbaum H (2000b) Hippocampus: mapping or memory? Curr Biol 10: R785–R787
Eichenbaum H, Dudchenko P, Wood ER, Shapiro M, Tanila H (1999) The hippocampus,
place cells, and memory: is it spatial memory or a memory space? Neuron 23:209–226
Elliot Smith G (1919) A preliminary note on the morphology of the corpus striatum and the
origin of the neopallium. J Anat 53:271–291
Ergorul C, Eichenbaum H (2004) The hippocampus and memory for “what,” “where,” and
“when”. Learn Mem 11:397–405
92 References
Gerhart J (2000) Inversion of the chordate body axis: are there alternatives? Proc Natl Acad
Sci U S A 97:4445–4448
Gerhart J, Kirschner M (1997) Cells, embryos and evolution. Toward a cellular and develop-
mental understanding of phenotypic variation and evolutionary adaptability. Blackwell,
Cambridge, MA
Gess RW, Coates MI, Rubidge BS (2006) A lamprey from the Denovian period of South
Africa. Nature 443:981”984
Gilissen E, Smith T (2003) Mesozoic mammals and early mammalian brain diversity. Behav
Brain Sci 26:556–557
Glardon S, Callaerts P, Halder G, Gehring WJ (1997) Conservation of Pax-6 in a lower
chordate, the ascidian Phallusia mammillata. Development 124:817–825
Glardon S, Holland LZ, Gehring WJ, Holland ND (1998) Isolation and developmental expres-
sion of the amphioxus Pax-6 gene (AmphiPax-6): insights into eye and photoreceptor
evolution. Development 125:2701–2710
Glinka A, Wu W, Delius H, Monaghan AP, Blumenstock C, Niehrs C (1998) Dickkopf-1 is
a member of a new family of secreted proteins and functions in head induction. Nature
391:357–362
Goffinet AM, Daumerie C, Langerwerf B, Pieau C (1986) Neurogenesis in reptilian cortical
structures: 3 H-thymidine autoradiographic analysis. J Comp Neurol 243:106–116
Goffinet AM, Bar I, Bernier B, Trujillo C, Raynaud A, Meyer G (1999) Reelin expression
during embryonic brain development in lacertilian lizards. J Comp Neurol 414:533–550
Gong Q, Shipley MT (1995) Evidence that pioneer olfactory axons regulate telencephalon
cell cycle kinetics to induce the formation of the olfactory bulb. Neuron 14:91–101
González A, Russchen FT, Lohman AHM (1990) Afferent connections of the striatum and
the nucleus accumbens in the lizard Gekko Gecko. Brain Behav Evol 36:39–58
Gorbman A (1983) Early development of hagfish pituitary gland: evidence for the endoder-
mal origin of adenohypophysis. Amer Zool 23:639–654
Gorbman A (1995) Olfactory origins and the evolution of the brain-pituitary endocrine
system: facts and speculation. Gen Comp Endocrinol 97:171–178
Gould SJ (1977) Ontogeny and phylogeny. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Gould SJ (1989) Wonderful life: the Burguess Shale and the nature of history. Norton, New
York
Graham A (2000) The evolution of the vertebrates–genes and development. Curr Opin Genet
Dev10:624–628
Grisham W, Powers AS (1989) Function of the dorsal and medial cortex of turtles in learning.
Behav Neurosci 103:991–997
Grove EA, Fukuchi-Shimogori T (2003) Generating the cerebral cortical area map. Annu
Rev Neurosci 26:355–380
Grove EA, Tole S, Limon J, Yip L, Ragsdale CW (1998) The hem of the embryonic cerebral
cortex is defined by the expression of multiple Wnt genes and is compromised in Gli3-
deficient mice. Development 125:2315–2325
Guirado S (2003) The dorsal thalamic connection in the origin of the isocortex. Behav Brain
Sci 26:557–558
Guirado S, Dávila JC (2002) Thalamo-telencephalic connections: new insights on the cortical
organization in reptiles. Brain Res Bull 57:451–454
Guirado S, Dávila JC, Real MA, Medina L (2000) Light and electron microscopic evidence for
projections from the thalamic nucleus rotundus to targets in the basal ganglia, the dorsal
ventricular ridge, and the amygdaloid complex in a lizard. J Comp Neurol 424:216–232
Guirado S, Real MA, Dávila JC (2005) The ascending tectofugal visual system in amniotes:
new insights. Brain Res Bull 66:290–296
94 References
Gulisano M, Broccoli V, Pardini C, Boncinelli E (1996) Emx1 and Emx2 show different
patterns of expression during proliferation and differentiation of the developing cerebral
cortex. Eur J Neurosci 8:1037–1050
Gunhaga L, Marklund M, Sjodal M, Hsieh JC, Jessell TM, Edlund T (2003) Specification
of dorsal telencephalic character by sequential Wnt and FGF signaling. Nat Neurosci
6:701–707
Güntürkün O (2003) Occam’s razor and the collothalamic projection. Behav Brain Sci 26:558–
559
Güntürkün O (2005) The avian ‘prefrontal cortex’ and cognition. Curr Biol 15:686–693
Haberly LB (1990) Comparative aspects of olfactory cortex. In: Jones EG, Peters A (eds)
Cerebral cortex, vol. 8B. Comparative structure and evolution of cerebral cortex, part II.
Plenum Press, New York, pp 137–164
Haeckel E (1892) The history of creation. Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co., London
Halanych KM, Passamaneck Y (2001) A brief review of metazoan phylogeny and future
prospects in Hox-research. Am Zool 41:629–639
Halder G, Callaerts P, Gehring WJ (1995) Induction of ectopic eyes by targeted expression
of the eyeless gene in Drosophila. Science 267:1788–1792
Hall WC, Ebner FF (1970) Thalamotelencephalic projections in the turtle (Pseudemys
scripta). J Comp Neurol 140:101–122
Hampson RE, Simeral JD, Deadwyler SA (1999) Distribution of spatial and nonspatial
information in dorsal hippocampus. Nature 402:610–614
Hanganu IL, Kilb W, Luhmann HJ (2001) Spontaneous synaptic activity of subplate neurons
in neonatal rat somatosensory cortex. Cereb Cortex 11:400–410
Hanganu IL, Kilb W, Luhmann HJ (2002) Functional synaptic projections onto subplate
neurons in neonatal rat somatosensory cortex. J Neurosci 22:7165–7176
Harman AM, Eastough NJ, Beazley LD (1995) Development of the visual cortex in a wallaby–
phylogenetic implications. Brain Behav Evol 45:138–152
Hashimoto H, Itoh M, Yamanaka Y, Yamashita S, Shimizu T, Solnica-Krezel L, Hibi M,
Hirano T (2000) Zebrafish Dkk1 functions in forebrain specification and axial mesendo-
derm formation. Dev Biol 217:138–152
Heffner R, Masterton B (1975) Variation in form of the pyramidal tract and its relationship
to digital dexterity. Brain Behav Evol 12:161–200
Heins N, Malatesta P, Cecconi F, Nakafuku M, Tucker KL, Hack MA, Chapouton P, Barde YA,
Götz M (2002) Glial cells generate neurons: the role of the transcription factor Pax6. Nat
Neurosci 5:308–315
Hermer-Vazquez R, Hermer-Vazquez L (2003) The evolution of neural dynamics permitting
isocortical-limbic-motor communication. Behav Brain Sci 26:559–560
Hevner RF, Shi L, Justice N, Hsueh YP, Sheng M, Smiga S, Bulfone A, Goffinet AM, Cam-
pagnoni AT, Rubenstein JLR (2001) Tbr1 regulates differentiation of the preplate and
layer 6. Neuron 29:353–366
Higashi S, Hioki K, Kurotani T, Kasim N, Molnár Z (2005) Functional thalamocortical
synapse reorganization from subplate to layer IV during postnatal development in the
reeler-like mutant rat (shaking rat Kawasaki). J Neurosci 25:1395–1406
Hillenius WJ (1994) Turbinates in therapsids: evidence for late permian origins of mam-
malian endothermy. Evolution 48:207–229
Hines M (1923) The development of the telencephalon in Sphenodon punctatum. J Comp
Neurol 35:483–537
Hirth F, Therianos S, Loop T, Gehring WJ, Reichert H, Furukubo-Tokunaga K (1995) De-
velopmental defects in brain segmentation caused by mutations of the homeobox genes
orthodenticle and empty spiracles in Drosophila. Neuron 15:769–778
References 95
Johnston JB (1923) Further contributions of the study of the evolution of the forebrain.
J Comp Neurol 36:143–192
Källén B (1951) On the ontogeny of the reptilian forebrain. Nuclear structures and ventric-
ular sulci. J Comp Neurol 95:307–347
Kanold PO (2004) Transient microcircuits formed by subplate neurons and their role in
functional development of thalamocortical connections. NeuroReport 15:2149–2153
Karten HJ (1968) The ascending auditory pathway in the pigeon (Columba livia). II. Telen-
cephalic projections of the nucleus ovoidalis thalami. Brain Res 11:134–153
Karten HJ (1969) The organization of the avian telencephalon and some speculations on
the phylogeny of the amniote telencephalon. Ann N Y Acad Sci 167:164–179
Karten HJ (1997) Evolutionary developmental biology meets the brain: the origins of mam-
malian neocortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 94:2800–2804
Karten HJ, Hodos W (1970) Telencephalic projections of the nucleus rotundus in the pigeon
(Columba livia). J Comp Neurol 140:35–51
Katoh YY, Benedek G (1995) Organization of the colliculo-suprageniculate pathway in the
cat: a wheat germ agglutinin-horseradish peroxidase study. J Comp Neurol 352:381–397
Kazanskaya O, Glinka A, Niehrs C (2000) The role of Xenopus dickkopf1 in prechordal plate
specification and neural patterning. Development 127:4981–4992
Kemp TS (2005) The origin and evolution of mammals. Oxford University Press, Oxford
Kenigfest N, Martínez-Marcos A, Belekhova M, Font C, Lanuza E, Desfilis E, Martínez-
García F (1997) A lacertilian dorsal retinorecipient thalamus: a re-investigation in the
old-world lizard Podarcis hispanica. Brain Behav Evol 50:313–334
Kicliter E, Northcutt RG (1975) Ascending afferents to the telencephalon of ranid frogs: an
anterograde degeneration study. J Comp Neurol 161:239–253
Kielan-Jaworowska Z, Cifelli RL, Luo Z (2004) Mammals from the Age of Dinosaurs.
Columbia University Press, New York
Kim AS, Lowenstein DH, Pleasure SJ (2001) Wnt receptors and Wnt inhibitors are expressed
in gradients in the developing telencephalon. Mech Dev 103:167–172
Kimmel CB (1993) Patterning the brain of the zebrafish embryo. Annu Rev Neurosci 16:707–
732
Kimura J, Suda Y, Kurokawa D, Hossain ZM, Nakamura M, Takahashi M, Hara A, Aizawa S
(2005) Emx2 and Pax6 function in cooperation with Otx2 and Otx1 to develop caudal
forebrain primordium that includes future archipallium. J Neurosci 25:5097–5108
Kirsche W (1972) Die Entwicklung des Telencephalons der Reptilia und deren Beziehung
zur Hirn-Bauplanlehre. Nova Acta Leopoldina 37:1–78
Knudsen EI, Cohen YE, Masino T (1995) Characterization of a forebrain gaze field in the
archistriatum of the barn owl: microstimulation and anatomical connections. J Neurosci
15:5139–5151
Kolk SM, Whitman MC, Yun ME, Shete P, Donoghue MJ (2005) A unique subpopulation of
Tbr1-expressing deep layer neurons in the developing cerebral cortex. Mol Cell Neurosci
30:538–551
Kölliker A (1843) Über das Geruchsorgan von Amphioxus (Müllers). Arch Anat Physiol
(Berlin) 1843:33–55
Kostovic I, Rakic P (1990) Developmental history of the transitional subplate zone in vi-
sual and somatosensory cortex of macaque monkey and human brain. J Comp Neurol
297:441–470
Kraus P, Lufkin T (1999) Mammalian Dlx homeobox gene control of craniofacial and inner
ear morphogenesis. J Cell Biochem Suppl 32–33:133–140
Kriegstein AR, Noctor SC (2004) Patterns of neuronal migration in the embryonic cortex.
Trends Neurosci 27:392–399
References 97
Kriegstein AR, Noctor SC, Martínez-Cerdeño V (2006) Patterns of neural stem and pro-
genitorcell devisionmay underlie evolutionary cortical expansion. Nat Rev Neurosci
7:883–890
Krubitzer LA (1995) The organization of neocortex in mammals: are species differences
really so different? Trends Neurosci 18:408–417
Krubitzer LA (2000) How does evolution build a complex brain? Novartis Foundation
Symposium 228:206–220
Krubitzer L, Kahn DM (2003) Nature versus nurture revisited: an old idea with a new twist.
Prog Neurobiol 70:33–52
Krumlauf R (1992) Evolution of the vertebrate Hox homeobox genes. Bioessays 14:245–252
Künzle, H, Raddtke-Schuller (2001) Cortical connections of the claustrum and subjacent
cell groups in the hedgehog tenrec. Anat Embryol 203:403–415
Laberge F, Muhlenbrock-Lenter S, Grunwald W, Roth G (2006) Evolution of the Amygdala:
New Insights from Studies in Amphibians. Brain Behav Evol 67:177–187
Lacalli TC (1999) Tunicate tails, stolons, and the origin of the vertebrate trunk. Biol Rev
Camb Philos Soc 74:177–198
Lacalli TC (2001) New perspectives on the evolution of protochordate sensory and locomo-
tory systems, and the origin of brains and heads. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci
356:1565–1572
Lacalli TC (2003) Evolutionary biology: Body plans and simple brains. Nature 424:263–264
Lacalli TC (2004) Sensory systems in amphioxus: a window on the ancestral chordate
condition. Brain Behav Evol 64:148–162
Lacalli TC (2005) Protochordate body plan and the evolutionary role of larvae: old contro-
versies resolved? Can J Zool 83:216–224
Lacalli TC, Kelly SJ (2003) Ventral neurons in the anterior nerve cord of amphioxus larvae.
I. An inventory of cell types and synaptic patterns. J Morphol 257:190–211
Lacalli TC, Holland ND, West JE (1994) landmarks in the anterior central nervous system
of amphioxus larvae. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B 344:165–185
Lanuza E, Belekhova M, Martínez-Marcos A, Font C, Martínez-García F (1998) Identification
of the reptilian basolateral amygdala:an anatomical investigation of the afferents to
the posterior dorsal ventricular ridge of the lizard Podarcis hispanica. Eur J Neurosci
10:3517–3534
Lanuza E, Martínez-Marcos A, Martínez-García F (1999) What is the amygdala? A compar-
ative approach. Trends Neurosci 22:207
Lavdas AA, Grigoriou M, Pachnis V, Parnavelas JG (1999) The medial ganglionic eminence
gives rise to a population of early neurons in the developing cerebral cortex. J Neurosci
19:7881–7888
Leach WJ (1951) The hypophysis of lampreys in relation to nasal apparatus. J Morphol
89:217–246
LeDoux JE, Farb C, Ruggiero DA (1990) Topographic organization of neurons in the acoustic
thalamus that project to the amygdala. J Neurosci 10:1043–1054
LeDoux JE, Farb CR, Romanski LM (1991) Overlapping projections to the amygdala and
striatum from auditory processing areas of the thalamus and cortex. Neurosci Lett
134:139–144
Lende RA (1963) Cerebral cortex: a sensorimotor amalgam in the Marsupialia. Science
141:730–732
Lepore F, Ptito M, Guillemot JP, Samson A, Richer L, Prevost L, Provencal C (1985) Role
of the corpus callosum and other neocortical commissures in the transfer of sensory
information. Union Med Can 114:1012–1018
98 References
Mazet F, Shimeld S (2005) Molecular evidence from ascidians for the evolutionary origin of
vertebrate cranial sensory placodes. J Exp Zool (Mol Dev Evol) 304B:340–346
McConnell SK, Ghosh A, Shatz CJ (1989) Subplate neurons pioneer the first axon pathway
from the cerebral cortex. Science 245:978–982
McConnell SK, Ghosh A, Shatz CJ (1994) Subplate pioneers and the formation of descending
connections from cerebral cortex. J Neurosci 14:1892–1907
McDonald AJ (1991) Organization of amygdaloid projections to the prefrontal cortex and
associated striatum in the rat. Neuroscience 44:1–44
McGinnis W, Krumlauf R (1992) Homeobox genes and axial patterning. Cell 68:283–302
McKinney ML, McNamara KJ (1991) Heterochrony. The Evolution of Ontogeny. Plenum
Press, New York
Medina L (2003) Histogenetic divisions, developmental mechanisms, and cortical evolution.
Behav Brain Sci 26:563–564
Medina L, Reiner A (1995) Neurotransmitter organization and connectivity of the basal
ganglia in vertebrates: implications for the evolution of basal ganglia. Brain Behav Evol
46:235–258
Medina L, Reiner A (2000) Do birds possess homologues of mammalian primary visual,
somatosensory and motor cortices? Trends Neurosci 23:1–12
Meléndez-Ferro M, Pérez-Costas E, Villar-Cheda B, Abalo XM, Rodríguez-Muñoz R, Rod-
icio MC, Anadón R (2002) Ontogeny of gamma-aminobutyric acid-immunoreactive
neuronal populations in the forebrain and midbrain of the sea lamprey. J Comp Neurol
446:360–376
Meulemans D, Bronner-Fraser M. (2005) Central role of gene cooption in neural crest
evolution. J Exp Zoolog B Mol Dev Evol 304:298–303
Meyer G, Soria JM, Martínez-Galán JR, Martín-Clemente B, Fairén A (1998) Different origins
and developmental histories of transient neurons in the marginal zone of the fetal and
neonatal rat cortex. J Comp Neurol 397:493–518
Meyer G, Goffinet AM, Fairén A (1999) What is a Cajal-Retzius cell? A reassessment of
a classical cell type based on recent observations in the developing neocortex. Cereb
Cortex 9:765–775
Meyer G, Schaaps JP, Moreau L, Goffinet AM (2000) Embryonic and early fetal development
of the human neocortex. J Neurosci 20:1858–1868
Meyer G, Perez-García CG, Abraham H, Caput D (2002) Expression of p73 and reelin in the
developing human cortex. J Neurosci 22:4973–4986
Meyer G, Cabrera Socorro A, Pérez García CG, Martínez Millan L, Walker N, Caput D (2004)
Developmental roles of p73 in Cajal-Retzius cells and cortical patterning. J Neurosci
24:9878–9887
Moens CB, Cordes SP, Giorgianni MW, Barsh GS, Kimmel CB (1998) Equivalence in the
genetic control of hindbrain segmentation in fish and mouse. Development 125:381–391
Molnár Z (1998) Development of thalamocortical connections. Springer, Berlin
Molnár, Z, Butler AB (2002) The corticostriatal junction: a crucial region for forebrain
development and evolution. BioEssays 24:530–541
Molnár Z, Cordery P (1999) Connections between cells of the internal capsule, thalamus,
and cerebral cortex in embryonic rat. J Comp Neurol 413:1–25
Molnár Z, Métin C, Stoykova A, Tarabykin V, Price DJ, Francis F, Meyer G, Dehay C,
Kennedy H (2006) Comparative aspects of cerebral cortical development. Eur J Neurosci
23:921–934
Montero V (1993) Retinotopy of cortical connections between the striate cortex and extras-
triate visual areas in the rat. Exp Brain Res 94:1–15
References 101
Nadarajah B, Alifragis P, Wong RO, Parnavelas JG (2003) Neuronal migration in the develop-
ing cerebral cortex: observations based on real-time imaging. Cereb Cortex 13:607–611
Nauta WHJ, Karten HJ (1970) A general profile of the vertebrate brain, with sidelights on
the ancestry of the cerebral cortex. In: Schmitt FO (ed) The neurosciences, second study
program. Rockefeller University Press, New York, pp 7–26
Neary TJ (1990) The pallium of anuran amphibians. In: Jones EG, Peters A (eds) Cerebral
cortex, vol. 8A. Comparative structure and evolution of cerebral cortex, Part I. Plenum
Press, New York, pp 107–138
Neary TJ, Wilczynski W (1977) Ascending thalamic projections from the obex region in
ranid frogs. Brain Res 138:529–533
Nielsen C (2001) Animal evolution. Interrelationships of the living phyla. Oxford University
Press, Oxford
Nieto MA, Bradley LC, Wilkinson DG (1991) Conserved segmental expression of Krox-20 in
the vertebrate hindbrain and its relationship to lineage restriction. Development [Suppl
2]:59–62
Nieto M, Monuki ES, Tang H, Imitola J, Haubst N, Khoury SJ, Cunningham J, Götz M,
Walsh CA (2004) Expression of Cux-1 and Cux-2 in the subventricular zone and upper
layers II-IV of the cerebral cortex. J Comp Neurol 479:168–180
Nieuwenhuys R (1977) The brain of the lamprey in a comparative perspective. Ann N Y
Acad Sci 299:97–145
Nieuwenhuys R (1998a) Amphioxus. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C
(eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 1. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, pp 365–
396
Nieuwenhuys R (1998b) Brachiopterygian fishes. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ,
Nicholson C (eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 1. Springer-Verlag,
Berlin New York Heidelberg, pp 655–699
Nieuwenhuys R (1998c) Chondrostean fishes. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nichol-
son C (eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 1. Springer-Verlag, Berlin
New York Heidelberg, pp 701–757
Nieuwenhuys R (1998d) Lungfishes. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C
(eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 1. Springer-Verlag, Berlin New
York Heidelberg, pp 939–1006
Nieuwenhuys R (1998e) The coelacanth Latimeria Chalumnae. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten
Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C (eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 2.
Springer-Verlag, Berlin New York Heidelberg, pp 1007–1043
Nieuwenhuys R, Meek J (1990) The telencephalon of actinopterygian fishes. In: Peters A,
Jones EG (eds) Cerebral cortex Vol 8A: Comparative structure and evolution of cerebral
cortex. Plenum Press, New York, pp 31–73
Nieuwenhuys R, Nicholson C (1998) Lampreys, Petromyzontidea. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten
Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C (eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 1.
Springer-Verlag, Berlin New York Heidelberg, pp 397–495
Northcutt RG (1969) Discussion of the preceding paper. An N Y Acad Sci 167:180–185
Northcutt RG (1981) Evolution of the telencephalon in nonmammals. Ann Rev Neurosci
4:301–50
Northcutt RG (1984) Evolution of the vertebrate central nervous system: patterns and
processes. Amer Zool 24:701–716
Northcutt RG (1990a) Ontogeny and phylogeny: a re-evaluation of conceptual relationships
and some applications. Brain Behav Evol 36:116–140
Northcutt RG (1990b) Visual pathways in elasmobranchs: organization and phylogenetic
implications. J Exp Zool [Suppl 5]:97–107
References 103
Panganiban G (2000) Distal-less function during Drosophila appendage and sense organ
development. Dev Dyn 218:554–5562
Pannese M, Lupo G, Kablar B, Boncinelli E, Barsacchi G, Vignali R (1998) The Xenopus Emx
genes identify presumptive dorsal telencephalon and are induced by head organizer
signals. Mech Dev 73:73–83
Parent A (1986) Comparative neurobiology of the basal ganglia. John Wiley, New York
Parent A, Hazrati LN (1995a) Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. I. The cortico-basal
ganglia-thalamo-cortical loop. Brain Res Rev 20:91–127
Parent A, Hazrati LN (1995b) Functional anatomy of the basal ganglia. II. The place of
subthalamic nucleus and external pallidum in basal ganglia circuitry. Brain Res Rev
20:128–154
Parent A, Olivier A (1970) Comparative histochemical study of the corpus striatum. J Hirn-
forsch 12:73–81
Parnavelas JG (2000) The origin and migration of cortical neurones: new vistas. Trends
Neurosci 23:126–131
Payne BR (1993) Evidence for visual cortical area homologs in cat and macaque monkey.
Cereb Cortex 3:1–25
Pearson JC, Lemons D, McGinnis W (2005) Modulating Hox gene functions during animal
body patterning. Nat Rev Genet 6:893–904
Pérez-García CG, González-Delgado FJ, Suárez-Sola ML, Castro-Fuentes R, Martín-
Trujillo JM, Ferres-Torres R, Meyer G (2001) Reelin-immunoreactive neurons in the
adult vertebrate pallium. J Chem Neuroanat 21:41–51
Peterson E (1980) Behavioral studies of telencephalic functions in reptiles. In: Ebbesson SOE
(ed) Comparative neurology of the telencephalon. Plenum press, New York, pp 343–388
Petrillo M, Ritter CA, Powers AS (1994) A role for acetylcholine in spatial memory in turtles.
Physiol Behav 56:135–141
Petrone A, Battaglia F, Wang C, Dusa A, Su J, Zagzag D, Bianchi R, Casaccia-Bonnefil P,
Arancio O, Sap J (2003) Receptor protein tyrosine phosphatase alpha is essential for
hippocampal neuronal migration and long-term potentiation. EMBO J 22:4121–4131
Pineda D, González J, Callaerts P, Ikeo K, Gehring WJ, Salo E (2000) Searching for the
prototypic eye genetic network: Sine oculis is essential for eye regeneration in planarians.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:4525–4529
Pinto-Lord MC, Evrard P, Caviness VS, (1982) Obstructed neuronal migration along radial
glial fibers and young neurons migrating to the neocortex in the reeler mouse. A Golgi-
EM analysis. Brain Res 256:379–393
Polenova OA, Vesselkin NP (1993) Olfactory and nonolfactory projections in the river
lamprey (Lampetra fluviatilis). J Hirnforsch 34:261–279
Pollard KS, Salama SR, Lambert N, Lambot MA, Coppens S, Pedersen JS, Katzman S, King B,
Onodera C, Siepel A, Kern AD, Dehay C, Igel H, Ares M Jr, Vanderhaeghen P, Haussler D
(2006) An RNA gene expressed during cortical development evolved rapidly in humans.
Nature 443:167–172
Pombal MA, Puelles L (1999) Prosomeric map of the lamprey forebrain based on calretinin
immunocytochemistry, Nissl stain, and ancillary markers. J Comp Neurol 414:391–422
Pombal MA, El Manira A, Grillner S (1997a) Afferents of the lamprey striatum with special
reference to the dopaminergic system: a combined tracing and immunohistochemical
study. J Comp Neurol 386:71–91
Pombal MA, El Manira A, Grillner S (1997b) Organization of the lamprey striatum—
transmitters and projections. Brain Res 766:249–254
References 105
Porteus MH, Bulfone A, Ciaranello RD, Rubenstein JL (1991) Isolation and characterization
of a novel cDNA clone encoding a homeodomain that is developmentally regulated in
the ventral forebrain. Neuron 7:221–229
Porteus MH, Bulfone A, Liu JK, Puelles L, Lo LC, Rubenstein JL (1994) DLX-2, MASH-1, and
MAP-2 expression and bromodeoxyuridine incorporation define molecularly distinct
cell populations in the embryonic mouse forebrain. J Neurosci 14:6370–6383
Powers AS (1990) Brain mechanisms of learning in reptiles. In: Kesner RP, Olton DS (eds)
Neurobiology of comparative cognition. Erlbaum Press, New York, pp 75–87
Powers AS (2003) Relevance of medial and dorsal cortex function to the dorsalization
hypothesis. Behav Brain Sci 26:566–567
Ptito A (2003) Functions of the corpus callosum as derived from split-chiasm studies of
cats. In: Zaidel E, Iacoboni M (eds) The parallel brain. The cognitive neuroscience of the
corpus callosum. MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 139–153
Puelles L (1995) A segmental morphological paradigm for understanding vertebrate fore-
brains. Brain Behav Evol 46:319–337
Puelles L, Medina L (2002) Field homology as a way to reconcile genetic and developmental
variability with adult homology. Brain Res Bull 57:243–255
Puelles L, Rubenstein JL (1993) Expression patterns of homeobox and other putative regula-
tory genes in the embryonic mouse forebrain suggest a neuromeric organization. Trends
Neurosci 16:472–479
Puelles L, Rubenstein JL (2003) Forebrain gene expression domains and the evolving pro-
someric model. Trends Neurosci 26:469–476
Puelles L, Kuwana E, Puelles E, Rubenstein JLR (1999) Comparison of the mammalian
and avian telencephalon from the perspective of gene expression data. Eur J Morphol
37:139–150
Puelles L, Kuwana E, Puelles E, Bulfone A, Shimamura K, Keleher J, Smiga S, Rubenstein JLR
(2000) Pallial and subpallial derivatives in the embryonic chick and mouse telencephalon,
traced by the expression of the genes Dlx-2, Emx-1, Nkx-2.1, Pax-6 and Tbr-1. J Comp
Neurol 424:409–438
Quiring R, Walldorf U, Kloter U, Gehring WJ (1994) Homology of the eyeless gene of
Drosophila to the Small eye gene in mice and Aniridia in humans. Science 265:785–789
Quiroga J (1980) The brain of the mammal-like reptile Probainognathus jenseni (Therapsida,
Cynodontia), A correlative paleo-neurological approach to the neocortex at the reptile-
mammal transition. J Hirnforsch 21:299–336
Rakic P (1974) Neurons in rhesus monkey: systematic relation between time of origin and
eventual disposition. Science 183:425–427
Rakic P (1988) Specification of cerebral cortical areas. Science 241:170–176
Rakic P (1995) A small step for the cell, a giant leap for mankind: a hypothesis of neocortical
expansion during evolution. Trends Neurosci 18:383–388
Rakic P (2000) Radial unit hypothesis of neocortical expansion. Novartis Found Symp
228:30–42
Rakic S, Davis C, Molnár Z, Nikolic M, Parnavelas JG (2006) Role of p35/Cdk5 in preplate
splitting in the developing cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 16 [Suppl 1]:i35–i45
Rattenborg NC (2006) Evolution of slow-wave sleep and palliopallial connectivity in mam-
mals and birds: A hypothesis. Brain Res Bull 69:20–29
Redies C, Ast M, Nakagawa S, Takeichi M, Martínez-de-la-Torre M, Puelles L (2000) Mor-
phologic fate of diencephalic prosomeres and their subdivisions revealed by mapping
cadherin expression. J Comp Neurol 421:481–514
Reep RL (2000) Cortical layer VII and persistent subplate cells in mammalian brains. Brain
Behav Evol 56:212–234
106 References
Shimeld SM, Holland PW (2000) Vertebrate innovations. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 97:4449–
4452
Shimeld SM, Holland ND (2005) Amphioxus molecular biology: insights into vertebrate
evolution and developmental mechanisms. Can J Zool 83:90–100
Shimizu T, Karten HJ (1993) Multiple origins of neocortex:contributions of the dorsal
ventricular ridge. In: Ziegler HH, Bischof JH (eds) Vision, brain and behavior in birds.
MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 103–114
Shimogori T, Banuchi V, Ng HY, Strauss JB, Grove EA (2004) Embryonic signaling cen-
ters expressing BMP, WNT and FGF proteins interact to pattern the cerebral cortex.
Development 131:5639–5647
Shinozaki K, Miyagi T, Yoshida M, Miyata T, Ogawa M, Aizawa S, Suda Y (2002) Absence of
Cajal-Retzius cells and subplate neurons associated with defects of tangential cell migra-
tion from ganglionic eminence in Emx1/2 double mutant cerebral cortex. Development
129:3479–3492
Shu DG (2003) A paleontological perspective of vertebrate origin. Chin Sci Bull 48:725–735
Shu D, Conway Morris S (2003) Response to comment on “A new species of Yunnanozoan
with implications for deuterostome evolution”. Science 300:1372d
Shu DG, Zhang X, Chen L (1996) Reinterpretation of Yunnanozoon as the earliest known
hemichordate. Nature 380:428–430
Shu DG, Luo HL, Conway Morris S, Zhang XL, Hu SX, Chen L, Han J, Zhu M, Chen LZ (1999)
Lower cambrian vertebrates from south china. Nature 402:42–46
Shu DG, Conway Morris S, Han J, Zhang ZF, Yasui K, Janvier P, Chen L, Zhang XL, Liu JN, Li Y,
Liu HQ (2003a) Head and backbone of the Early Cambrian vertebrate Haikouichthys.
Nature 421:526–529
Shu D, Conway Morris S, Zhang ZF, Liu JN, Han J, Chen L, Zhang XL, Yasui K, Li Y (2003b)
A new species of Yunnanozoan with implications for deuterostome evolution. Science
299:1380–1384
Shu T, Richards LJ (2001) Cortical axon guidance by the glial wedge during the development
of the corpus callosum. J Neurosci 21:2749–2758
Shu T, Sundaresan V, McCarthy MM, Richards LJ (2003) Slit2 guides both precrossing and
postcrossing callosal axons at the midline in vivo. J Neurosci 23:8176–8184
Simeone A, Gulisano M, Acámpora D, Stornaiuolo A, Rambaldi M, Boncinelli E (1992) Two
vertebrate homeobox genes related to the Drosophila empty spiracles gene are expressed
in the embryonic cerebral cortex. EMBO J 11:2541–2550
Skeath JB, Carroll SB (1992) Regulation of proneural gene expression and cell fate during
neuroblast segregation in the Drosophila embryo. Development 114:939–946
Slack JM, Holland PW, Graham CF (1993) The zootype and the phylotypic stage. Nature
361:490–492
Smart IHM, Smart M (1977) The location of nuclei of different labelling intensities in
autoradiography of the anterior forebrain of postnatal mice injected with [3 H] thymidine
on the eleventh and twelfth days postconception. J Anat 123:515–525
Smeets WJAJ (1983) The secondary olfactory connections in two chondrichthians, the shark
Scyliorhinus canicula and the ray Raja clavata. J Comp Neurol 10:334–344
Smeets WJAJ (1990) The telencephalon of cartilaginous fishes. In: Peters A, Jones EG (eds)
Cerebral cortex, Vol 8A: Comparative structure and evolution of cerebral cortex. Plenum
Press, New York, pp 3–30
Smeets WJAJ (1998) Cartilaginous fishes. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C
(eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 1. Springer-Verlag, Berlin New York
Heidelberg, pp 551-654
References 109
Smeets WJAJ, Marín O, González A (2000) Evolution of the basal ganglia: new perspectives
through a comparative approach. J Anat 196:501–517
Smith Fernández A, Pieau C, Repérant J, Boncinelli E, Wassef M (1998) Expression of the
Emx-1 and Dlx-1 homeobox genes define three molecularly distinct domains in the
telencephalon of mouse, chick, turtle and frog embryos:implications for the evolution
of telencephalic subdivisions in amniotes. Development 125:2099–2111
Soriano E, Alvarado-Mallart RM, Dumesnil N, Del Río JA, Sotelo C (1997) Cajal-Retzius
cells regulate the radial glia phenotype in the adult and developing cerebellum and alter
granule cell migration. Neuron 18:563–577
Stensiö E (1964) Les cyclostomes : fossiles ou ostracoderms. In: Piveteau J (ed) Traité de
paléontologie, Tome IV, Vol 1, Masson, Paris, pp 96–382
Stephan H (1983) Evolutionary trends in limbic structures. Neurosci Behav Physiol 7:367–
374
Stock DW, Ellies DL, Zhao Z, Ekker M, Ruddle FH, Weiss KM (1996) The evolution of the
vertebrate Dlx gene family. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93:10858–10863
Storm EE, Rubenstein JL, Martin GR (2003) Dosage of Fgf8 determines whether cell survival
is positively or negatively regulated in the developing forebrain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A
100:1757–1762
Stoykova A, Treichel D, Hallonet M, Gruss P (2000) Pax 6 modulates the dorsoventral
patterning of the mammalian telencephalon. J Neurosci 20:8042–8050
Striedter GF (1997) The telencephalon of tetrapods in evolution. Brain Behav Evol 49:179–
213
Striedter GF (2005) Principles of brain evolution. Sinauer Associates, Sunderland
Striedter GF, Northcutt RG (1991) Biological hierarchies and the concept of homology. Brain
Behav Evol 38:177–189
Striedter GF, Northcutt RG (2006) Head size constrains forebrain development and evolution
in ray-finned fishes. Evol Dev 8:215–222
Striedter GF, Marchant A, Beydler S (1998) The “neostriatum” develops as part of the lateral
pallium in birds. J Neurosci 18:5839–5849
Supèr H, Uylings HB (2001) The early differentiation of the neocortex: a hypothesis on
neocortical evolution. Cereb Cortex 11:1101–1109
Supèr H, Martínez A, Del Río JA, Soriano E (1998a) Involvement of distinct pioneer neurons
in the formation of layer-specific connections in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 18:4616–
4626
Supèr H, Soriano E, Uylings HBM (1998b) The functions of the preplate in development
and evolution of the neocortex and hippocampus. Brain Res Rev 27:40–64
Sur M, Rubenstein JL (2005) Patterning and plasticity of the cerebral cortex. Science 310:805–
810
Swanson LW (2000) Cerebral hemisphere regulation for motivated behavior. Brain Res
886:113–164
Swanson LW, Petrovich GD (1998) What is the amygdala? Trends Neurosci 21:323–331Tautz D
(2003) Chordate evolution in a new light. Cell 113:812–813
Taylor JS, Raes J (2004) Duplication and divergence: the evolution of new genes and old
ideas. Annu Rev Genet 38:615–643
Ten Donkelaar HJ (1998a) Anurans. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C
(eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 2. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, New
York Heidelberg, pp 1151–1314
Ten Donkelaar HJ (1998b) Reptiles. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C
(eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 2. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, New
York Heidelberg, pp 1315–1524
110 References
Ten Donkelaar HJ (1998c) Urodeles. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C
(eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates, Vol. 2. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, New
York Heidelberg, pp 1045–1150
Tissir F, Lambert De Rouvroit C, Sire JY, Meyer G, Goffinet AM (2003) Reelin expression
during embryonic brain development in Crocodylus niloticus. J Comp Neurol 457:250–
262
Tjoa LT, Welsch U (1974) Electron microscopical observations on Kölliker’s and Hatschek’s
pit and on the wheel organ in the head region of Amphioxus (Branchiostoma lanceola-
tum). Cell Tissue Res 153:175–187
Tole S, Remedios R, Saha B, Stoykova A (2005) Selective requirement of Pax6, but not Emx2,
in the specification and development of several nuclei of the amygdaloid complex.
J Neurosci 25:2753–2760
Toresson H, Martínez-Barbera JP, Bardsley A, Caubit X, Krauss S (1998) Conservation of
BF-1 expression in amphioxus and zebrafish suggests evolutionary ancestry of anterior
cell types that contribute to the vertebrate telencephalon. Dev Genes Evol 208:431–439
Turner BH, Herkenham M (1991) Thalamoamygdaloid projections in the rat: a test of the
amygdala’s role in sensory processing. J Comp Neurol 313:295–325
Tvrdik P, Capecchi MR (2006) Reversal of Hox1 gene subfunctionalization in the mouse.
Dev Cell 11:239–250
Ulinski PS (1983) Dorsal ventricular ridge: a treatise on brain organization in reptiles and
birds. John Wiley, New York
Ulinski PS (1990) The cerebral cortex of reptiles. In: Jones EG, Peters A (eds) Cerebral cortex.
Vol. 8A. Comparative structure and evolution of cerebral cortex, part I. Plenum Press,
New York, pp 139–215
Valentine JW (2004) On the origin of phyla. Chicago University Press, Chicago
Valverde F, De Carlos JA, López-Mascaraque L, DoZate-Oliver F (1986) Neocortical layers
I and II of the hedgehog (Erinaceus europaeus). II. Thalamo-cortical connections. Anat
Embryol 173:413–430
Van Essen DC, Anderson CH, Felleman DJ (1992) Information processing in the primate
visual system: an integrated systems perspective. Science 255:419–423
Van Hoesen GW (1982) The parahippocampal gyrus. New observations regarding its cortical
connections in the monkey. Trends Neurosci 10:345–350
Vargas AO, Aboitiz F (2005) How ancient is the adult swimming capacity in the lineage
leading to Euchordates? Evol Dev 7:171–174
Veenman CL, Wild JM, Reiner A (1995) Organization of the avian “corticostriatal” projection
system: a retrograde and anterograde pathway tracing study in pigeons. J Comp Neurol
354:87–126
Vesselkin NP, Agayan AL, Nomokonova LM (1971) A study of thalamo-telencephalic afferent
systems in frogs. Brain Behav Evol 4:295–306
Von Baer KE (1828) Über Entwicklungsgechichte der Thiere. Beobachtung und Reflexion.
Bornträger, Königsberg
Von Hetzel W (1974) Die Ontogenese des Telencephalons bei Lacerta sicula (Rafinesque),
mit besonderer Berücksichtigung der pallialen Entwicklung. Zool Beit NS 20:361–458
Voogd J, Nieuwenhuys R, Van Dongen PAM (1998) Mammals. In: Nieuwenhuys R, Ten
Donkelaar HJ, Nicholson C (eds) The central nervous system of vertebrates. Vol. 3.
Springer-Verlag, Berlin, New York Heidelberg, pp 1637–2098
Wagner GP, Chiu CH (2001) The tetrapod limb: a hypothesis on its origin. J Exp Zool
291:226–240
Wallace MN, Johnston PW, Palmer AR (2002) Histochemical identification of cortical areas
in the auditory region of the human brain. Exp Brain Res 143:499–508
References 111
Walldorf U, Gehring WJ (1992) Empty spiracles, a gap gene containing a homeobox involved
in Drosophila head development. EMBO J 11:2247–2259
Wang Y, Hu Y, Meng J, Chuankui L (2001) An ossified Meckel’s cartilage in two cretaceous
mammals and origin of the mammalian middle ear. Science 294:357–361
Warner FJ (1946) The development of the forebrain of the american watersnake (Natrix
sipedon). J Comp Neurol 84:385–418
Weiner J (1995) The beak of the finch. A story of evolution in our time. Vintage, New York
Whitlock KE (2004) A new model for olfactory placode development. Brain Behav Evol
64:126–140
Whitlock KE, Wolf CD, Boyce ML (2003) Gonadotropin-releasing hormone (GnRH) cells
arise from cranial neural crest and adenohypophyseal regions of the neural plate in the
zebrafish, Danio rerio. Dev Biol 257:140–152
Wicht H (1996) The brains of lampreys and hagfishes: characteristics, characters and com-
parisons, Brain Behav Evol 48:248–261
Wicht H, Lacalli TC (2005) The nervous system of amphioxus: structure, development, and
evolutionary significance. Can J Zool 83:122–150
Wicht H, Northcutt RG (1992) The forebrain of the pacific hagfish: a cladistic reconstruction
of the ancestral craniate forebrain. Brain Behav Evol 40:25–64
Wicht H, Northcutt RG (1993) Secondary olfactory projections and pallial topography in
the Pacific hagfish, Eptatretus stouti. J Comp Neurol 337:529–542
Wicht H, Northcutt RG (1994) An immunohistochemical study of the telencephalon and
the diencephalon in a Myxinoid jawless fish, the Pacific hagfish, Eptatretus stouti. Brain
Behav Evol 43:140–161
Wicht H, Northcutt RG (1998) Telencephalic connections in the Pacific hagfish (Eptatretus
stouti), with special reference to the thalamopallial system. J Comp Neurol 395:245–260
Wiener SI, Paul CA, Eichenbaum H (1989) Spatial and Behavioral correlates of hippocampal
neuronal activity. J Neurosci 9:2737–2763
Wilczynski W, Northcutt RG (1983) Connections of the bullfrog striatum: afferent organi-
zation. J Comp Neurol 214:321–332
Wild JM (1997) The avian somatosensory system: the pathway from wing to Wulst in
a passerine (Chloris chloris). Brain Res 759:122–134
Wilkinson DG, Krumlauf R (1990) Molecular approaches to the segmentation of the hind-
brain. Trends Neurosci 13:335–339
Wilkinson DG, Bhatt S, Cook M, Boncinelli E, Krumlauf R (1989) Segmental expression
of Hox-2 homoeobox-containing genes in the developing mouse hindbrain. Nature
341:405–409
Williams NA, Holland PW (1998) Molecular evolution of the brain of chordates. Brain Behav
Evol 52:177–185
Willmer P (1990) Invertebrate relationships. Patterns in vertebrate evolution. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge
Wilson SW, Rubenstein JL (2000) Induction and dorsoventral patterning of the telen-
cephalon. Neuron 28:641–651
Wilson SI, Rydstrom A, Trimborn T, Willert K, Nusse R, Jessell TM, Edlund T (2001) The
status of Wnt signalling regulates neural and epidermal fates in the chick embryo. Nature
411:325–330
Witham LA (2002) Where Darwin meets the bible. Creationists and evolutionists in America.
Oxford University, Oxford
Woda JM, Pastagia J, Mercola M, Artinger KB (2003) Dlx proteins position the neural plate
border and determine adjacent cell fates. Development 130:331–342
112 References
Woo TU, Beale JM, Finlay BL (1991) Dual fate of subplate neurons in a rodent. Cereb Cortex
1:433–443
Wood ER, Dudchenko PA, Eichenbaum H (1999) The global record of memory in hippocam-
pal neuronal activity. Nature 397:613–616
Wood ER, Dudchenko PA, Robitsek JR, Eichenbaum H (2000) Hippocampal neurons encode
information about different types of memory episodes occurring in the same location.
Neuron 27:623–633
Wu CC, Russell RM, Karten HJ (2000) Ontogeny of the tectorotundal pathway in chicks
(Gallus gallus): birthdating and pathway tracing study. J Comp Neurol 417:115–132
Wullimann MF, Puelles L (1999) Postembryonic neural proliferation in the zebrafish fore-
brain and its relationship to prosomeric domains. Anat Embryol (Berl) 199:329–348
Xie Y, Skinner E, Landry C, Handley V, Schonmann V, Jacobs E, Fisher R, Campagnoni A
(2002) Influence of the embryonic preplate on the organization of the cerebral cortex:
a targeted ablation model. J Neurosci 22:8981–8991
Yang A, Walker N, Bronson R, Kaghad M, Oosterwegel M, Bonnin J, Vagner C, Bonnet H,
Dikkes P, Sharpe A, McKeon F, Caput D (2000) p73-deficient mice have neurological,
pheromonal and inflammatory defects but lack spontaneous tumours. Nature 404:99–103
Yoon MS, Puelles L, Redies C (2000) Formation of cadherin-expressing brain nuclei in
diencephalic and alar plate divisions. J Comp Neurol 421:461–480
Young JZ (1962) The life of vertebrates. Clarendon Press, Oxford. Reprinted in Spanish,
Ediciones Omega, Barcelona, 1962
Zardoya R, Meyer A (2001) The evolutionary position of turtles revised. Naturwis-
senschaften 88:193–200
Zhou C, Tsai SY, Tsai MJ (2001) COUP-TFI: an intrinsic factor for early regionalization of
the neocortex. Genes Dev 15:2054–2059
Zhu D, Lustig KH, Bifulco K, Keifer J (2005) Thalamocortical connections in the pond turtle
Pseudemys scripta elegans. Brain Behav Evol 65:278–292
Zimmer C, Tiveron MC, Bodmer R, Cremer H (2004) Dynamics of Cux2 expression suggests
that an early pool of SVZ precursors is fated to become upper cortical layer neurons.
Cereb Cortex 14:1408–1420
Subject Index
Acanthostega 27 cartilaginous 26
adenohypophysis (adenohypohysea) cartilaginous fishes 25, 31
16 Casineria 27
ADll (AmphiDll) 17, 22 catenin 53
agnathans 24, 28, 73 cdk5 (cyclin-dependant kinase 5) 61,
amniotes 27 65
amphibians 31, 32 central prosencephalic complex 29
amphioxus 8, 9, 12 central prosencephalic nucleus 79
amygdala 6, 33, 51 cephalochordates 8, 9, 13, 15, 17, 21
– basolateral 43, 48, 49, 58 Cerberus 14, 72
– lateral 43, 45, 46 cerebellum 37
– striatal 43 chordates 8, 11, 15
anapsid 27 Chordin 10, 12, 13, 72
anterior commissure 70 cilia (ciliated) 12–14, 20, 71
anterior neural ridge 19, 73 claustroamygdalar complex 6
anterior thalamic nucleus 42, 55 claustroamygdaloid complex 33, 39
anterior visceral endoderm 14 claustrum 45, 49, 51, 62
antihem 74 cnidarians 7, 13
apical organ 13, 72 CNS condensation 9–11
apical pole 12 collothalamic 31, 40, 44, 55, 58
Archaeothyris 28 conodonts 15, 25
arcopallium 33, 40, 51 corpus callosum 37, 70
area dorsomedialis 79 cortex
area MT 69 – dorsal 32, 38, 39, 41, 43, 57
auditory area 68 – dorsomedial 32, 39, 43
– lateral 32, 38, 39, 47
bHLH (basic helix-loop-helix) 71 – medial 32, 38, 42, 43
bilaterian 7 cortical plate (CP) 59, 61, 63, 64, 76
biogenetic law 1, 79 corticospinal tract 29, 69, 77
blastopore 8 corticostriatal sulcus 4
blastula 18 ctenophorans 13
BMP (BMP4) 10, 12, 14, 53, 74 ctenophores 7
bony fish 25, 26, 32 cyclostomes 25, 31
Brain factor (BF1) 22 cynodonts 35, 36
branchial skeleton 16, 24
Branchiostoma see amphioxus deuterostome 11
Burguess Shale 15 deuterostomes 8, 15
developmental evolutionary genetics
Cajal-Retzius cells 60, 63, 64 see evo-devo
114 Subject Index
diapsid 27 Haikouichthys 15
Dickkopf 14, 72 HAR1F 63
dipleurula 14 Hatscheck’s pit 20
Dlx (Dlx1, Dlx2) 4, 17, 21, 48–50, 72 hem
dorsal ventricular ridge see DVR – cortical 6, 63
dpp (decapentaplegic) 10 hemichordate 14, 72
Drosophila 2, 4, 10, 12, 72, 77, 78 hemichordates 8, 11, 12, 18
DVR 28, 33, 39, 47, 48, 74, 79, 81 hippocampal 6
– anterior (ADVR) 33, 44, 49–51, 55 hippocampus 33, 39, 43, 55, 64
– posterior (PDVR) 33 homeobox see Hox
homeothermy 37
ear ossicles 35 homology 3, 38, 44, 51
echinoderms 8, 14 Hox 2, 4, 22, 48, 72
Eitenascidia 17 Hylonomus 27, 28
ems (empty spiracles) 4 hypermorphosis 79
EMX 68 hyperpallium 33
Emx (Emx1, Emx2) 4, 48–50, 52 hypophyseal duct 26
encephalization 36 hypophyseal placode 14
endocasts 35 hypothalamus 16, 19, 31
endopiriform nucleus 49
entopallial nucleus 40 Ichthyostega 27
entorhinal cortex 39, 42, 43, 57 imaginal disks 72
episodic memory 56, 58 inside-out 76
evo-devo 1–3 insula 62
eye interhemispheric connections 70, 77
– evolution 82 interneurons 6, 59, 60
– medial 18 inversion
– paired 21, 25 – dorsoventral 10
eye development 78
Joseph cells 18
fasciculus aberrans 70
FGF (FGF8) 19, 68, 69, 74 lamprey 25
Follistatin 10, 13, 72 lateral posterior and pulvinar complex
Frizzled 14, 52 43–45
lemnothalamic 31, 40, 55, 58
ganglionic eminences 6, 48, 60 locomotion 61
gastrula 2, 7 LP/P nucleus 45
gastrulation 8 lungfishes 32
geniculate nucleus
– lateral 41, 42, 55 macro evolution 81
– medial 46 marginal zone (MZ) 59, 63–66
Gli3 53 mesencephalon 5
glial wedge 70 mesomere 3
gnathostome 31 mesopallium 33, 51
gnathostomes 24, 73 migration
Gobiconodon 35, 37 – radial 61, 63
– tangential 63
Hadrocodium 35, 37 monorhins 25
hagfish 25, 30 Morganucodon 35
Haikouella 15, 21 motor area 68
Subject Index 115