The Impact of Smartphone Addiction On Attention Control and Sleep in Egypt-An Online Survey
The Impact of Smartphone Addiction On Attention Control and Sleep in Egypt-An Online Survey
The Impact of Smartphone Addiction On Attention Control and Sleep in Egypt-An Online Survey
Abstract
Background The widespread use of smartphones makes it imperative for researchers to study the adverse effect
of smartphone addiction. We aimed to study the risk factors of smartphone addiction, insomnia, and attention deficit
among smartphone users among a sample of Egyptian adolescents and adults.
Methods An online survey was disseminated among Egyptian social media groups. Participants were requested
to complete Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version (SAS-SV), Insomnia Severity Index (ISI) scale, and attention
control scale (ACS).
Results Two-thousand seven-hundred sixteen responded to our survey with a mean age of 31.4 ± 10.3 years. Smart‑
phone addiction was documented in 2386 (87.8%) participants, with a median daily time for smartphone use of 5 h
(IQR: 3–7). A significant association was found between smartphone addiction and younger age, higher educational
levels, and urban residency (unadjusted or adjusted). Binary logistic regression analysis showed that the only factor
affecting the probability of moderate to severe insomnia was the higher SAS-SV score, either unadjusted (OR = 1.1,
95% CI: 1.08–1.1) or adjusted (OR = 1.09, 95% CI: 1.08–1.11). Multiple linear regression analysis showed that higher
scores on SAS-SV (P < 0.001) and ISI (P < 0.001), being female (P < 0.001), and being of rural residency (P = 0.025)
were associated with lower total scores on ACS. On the other hand, older age (P < 0.001) and longer intervals
between smartphone cessation and bedtime (P = 0.004) were found to increase the attention score.
Conclusion Smartphone addiction is prevalent in Egypt, which deserves special concern as it may have negative
consequences such as insomnia and poor attention control, particularly in younger age groups.
Keywords Smartphone addiction, Insomnia, Attention control
*Correspondence:
Mennat‑Allah Tarek
[email protected]
Full list of author information is available at the end of the article
© The Author(s) 2023. Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which
permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the
original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or
other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line
to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory
regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this
licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Khalifa et al. Middle East Current Psychiatry (2023) 30:97 Page 2 of 8
Table 2 Binary logistic regression analysis (adjusted and unadjusted) for prediction of risk factors for smartphone addiction
Characteristics Nonaddict (n = 330) Addict (n = 2386) Unadjusted OR Adjusted OR
Age (mean ± SD) 33.4 ± 12.7 31.1 ± 9.9 0.98 (0.97 ± 0.99)* 0.97 (0.96–0.98)*
Sex
Male 78 (11.7%) 588 (88.3%) Reference Reference
Female 252 (12.3%) 1798 (87.7%) 0.9 (0.7–1.2) 0.87 (0.66–1.1)
Education
Until preparatory 15 (27.8%) 39 (72.2%) Reference Reference
Secondary 47 (21.1%) 176 (78.9%) 1.4 (0.7–2.8) 2 (0.99–4.1)
University 175 (11.1%) 1408 (88.9%) 3.1 (1.7–5.7)* 4.5 (12.3–8.5)*
Postgraduate 93 (10.9%) 763 (89.1%) 3.2 (1.7–5.9)* 5.3 (2.7–10.3)*
Residence
Urban 243 (11.5%) 1873 (88.5%) 1.3 (1.004–1.7)* 1.5 (1.1–1.9)*
Rural 87 (14.5%) 513 (85.5%) Reference Reference
*
P-value is significant
Table 3 Binary logistic regression analysis (adjusted and unadjusted) for prediction of risk factors for insomnia
Characteristics No and subthreshold Moderate and severe Unadjusted Adjusted
(n = 2119) (n = 597) OR OR
Age (mean ± SD) 31.7 ± 10.4 30.5 ± 9.7 0.989 (0.980–0.998)* 0.996 (0.986–1.007)
Sex
Male 541 (81.2%) 125 (18.8%) Reference Reference
Female 1578 (77.0%) 472 (23.0%) 1.3 (1.03–1.6) 1.1 (0.89–1.4)
Education
Until preparatory 44 (81.5%) 10 (18.5%) Reference Reference
Secondary 170 (76.2%) 53 (23.8%) 1.4 (0.6–2.9) 1.4 (0.6–2.9)
University 1224 (77.3%) 359 (22.7%) 1.3 (0.6–2.6) 1.02 (0.5–2.2)
Postgraduate 681 (79.6%) 175 (20.4%) 1.1 (0.6–2.3) 0.94 (0.4–2.0)
Residence
Rural 477 (79.5%) 123 (20.5%) Reference Reference 1.1 (0.9–1.4)
Urban 1642 (77.6%) 474 (22.4%) 1.1 (0.9–1.4)
SAS-SV (mean ± SD) 39.9 ± 7.5 44.9 ± 7 1.1 (1.08–1.1)* 1.09 (1.08–1.11)*
Interval between smartphone cessa‑ 15 (0–15) 0 (0–15) 0.993 (0.989–0.997)* 0.999 (0.995–1.003)
tion and bedtime
Median (IQR)
Most used applications
Social media 1409 (78.1%) 395 (21.9%) Reference Reference
Videos 77 (80.2%) 19 (19.8%) 0.9 (0.5–1.5) 1.2 (0.67, 1.9)
Games 633 (77.6%) 183 (22.4%) 1.03 (0.8–1.3) 1.1 (0.9, 1.4)
Time spent on the smartphone (hours)
Median (IQR) 5 (3.6) 6 (4.8) 1.1 (1.1–1.2)* ––––––
SAS-SV Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version
*
P-value is significant
Multiple linear regression analysis for predicting associated with lower total scores on ACS. On the other
risk factors affecting the total score of the ACS showed hand, older age and longer intervals between smartphone
that higher smartphone addiction, higher insomnia cessation and bedtime were found to increase the atten-
score, being female, and being of rural residency were tion score (Table 5).
Khalifa et al. Middle East Current Psychiatry (2023) 30:97 Page 5 of 8
Table 4 Correlation between the scores of attention domains and its total score with the other scores
Independent variables Attention shifting Attention focusing Attention total score
r p-value r p-value r p-value
SAS-SV total score − 0.105 < 0.001* − 0.379 < 0.001* − 0.305 < 0.001*
Time spent on smartphone − 0.013 0.507 − 0.158 < 0.001* − 0.111 < 0.001*
Interval between smartphone cessa‑ 0.072 < 0.001* 0.148 < 0.001* 0.136 < 0.001*
tion and bedtime
ISI total score − 0.142 < 0.001* − 0.330 < 0.001* − 0.292 < 0.001*
SAS-SV Smartphone Addiction Scale-Short Version, ISI Insomnia Severity Index scale
*
P-value is significant
Table 5 Multiple linear regression analysis for prediction of risk factors affecting the total attention score
Unstandardized coefficients t p-value 95.0% confidence interval for B
B Std. error Lower bound Upper bound
by many studies which suggested that insomnia and mul- and communication apps, which make them more dis-
tiple sleep awakening, excessive daytime sleepiness, and tracted by voice calls and apps notifications, unlike men
poor sleep quality were adverse effects of smartphone who spend most of their smartphone time in playing
addiction [30–32]. games and watching movies [45].
Although the exact cause of the connection between The current study also revealed that short intervals
smartphone addiction and insomnia is not entirely between smartphone cessation and bedtime and high
understood, several possible explanations exist. For scores on ISI are risk factors for poor attention control,
example, excessive smartphone use has a stimulating and that lack of sleep can explain an impaired person’s
effect that impairs the ability to fall asleep, the decline performance during the day, which may manifest as
in melatonin hormone secretion caused by exposure to poor attention, and also, disturbance of the circadian
smartphone screen blue light affects circadian rhythm, rhythm by the blue light emitted from screen affects
and, lastly, using social media in excess right before bed cognitive performance including attention [46, 47]. This
increases emotional and psychological stress [33–35]. finding is supported by several studies that demon-
The term “attentional control” describes the capacity strated that people with good sleep quality experience
to actively manage one’s attention, including the capac- less attentional deficits and are more able to respond to
ity to focus and shift it [36]. This study reported that auditory and visual stimuli [48, 49].
subjects with higher smartphone addiction levels have The present study had some limitations. First, a
poorer control over their attention. This finding agrees cross-sectional study cannot establish a causal associa-
with many studies that showed that participants reported tion between the study variables. Also, it reflects a spe-
severe interruption when receiving smartphone notifica- cific point in time rather than conducting a longitudinal
tions [37], and only the presence of a smartphone around observation. Second, the smartphone addiction scale,
is enough to interfere with their primary task comple- Insomnia Severity Index scale, and attention control
tion [38]. Moreover, other research findings reported scale used in this study are self-report questionnaires
that participants with smartphone addiction had shorter that may inevitably include bias.
attention spans [39], delayed reaction times, and were at
higher risk of motorcar accidents because of reading and
texting while driving [40]. All of these findings are con- Conclusion
firmed by a neuroanatomical study showing that heavy The prevalence of smartphone addiction in Egypt is
smartphone users had diminished early transcranial alarming since it may have negative consequences such
magnetic stimulation-evoked potentials in the right pre- as insomnia and poor attention control, particularly in
frontal cortex compared to nonusers, which are linked to younger age groups.
self-reported attentional difficulties [10].
Acknowledgements
Despite many studies addressing the negative impact of Not applicable.
smartphone addiction on attention, some research find-
ings demonstrated the beneficial role of excessive smart- Authors’ contributions
DK was responsible for participants recruitment and study design and helped
phone usage in video game playing that helps improve draft the manuscript. RM was responsible for participants’ recruitment and
brain synchronization in areas related to visual-spatial study design and helped draft the manuscript. MH was responsible for
processing and sensory-motor coordination [41]. In participants’ recruitment and helped in drafting the manuscript. DMK was
responsible for participants’ recruitment, performed the data analysis, and
a study conducted by Bleakley et al. [42], playing video helped draft the manuscript. AYI was responsible for participants’ recruit‑
games regularly improves cognitive functions. ment. SA was responsible for participants’ recruitment. NA was responsible for
There are two ways that smartphone addiction can participants’ recruitment. MAA was responsible for participants’ recruitment.
ME was responsible for participants’ recruitment. AME was responsible for
distract attention either by internal drive (top-down) participants’ recruitment. AH was responsible for participants’ recruitment. MA
through consciously directing the attention focus to use was responsible for participants’ recruitment. EMN was responsible for partici‑
it (intense urge to use) or by the presence of external cues pants’ recruitment. SF was responsible for participants’ recruitment. HMA was
responsible for participants’ recruitment. AGE was responsible for participants’
(bottom-up), either visual (seeing someone handhelds his recruitment. SD was responsible for participants’ recruitment. MT was respon‑
smartphone), hearing the ringtone, or feeling the vibra- sible for participants’ recruitment, worked on data interpretation, and helped
tions [43, 44]. draft the manuscript. All authors thoroughly revised the manuscript’s content
before approving the final version.
Although there was no statistically significant dif-
ference between males and females in smartphone Funding
addiction in our study, we found that the female sex is The authors did not receive any funding for this work.
associated with poor attention control, which could be Availability of data and materials
because women are more likely to use social networking Authors report that the datasets used and/or analyzed during the current
study are available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request.
Khalifa et al. Middle East Current Psychiatry (2023) 30:97 Page 7 of 8
31. Chen YL, Gau SS (2016) Sleep problems and Internet addiction among
children and adolescents: a longitudinal study. J Sleep Res 25(4):458–465.
https://doi.org/10.1111/jsr.12388
32. Liu H, Zhou Z, Huang L, Zhu E, Yu L, Zhang M (2022) Prevalence of
smartphone addiction and its effects on subhealth and insomnia: a
cross-sectional study among medical students. BMC Psychiatry 22(1):305.
https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-022-03956-6
33. Figueiro M, Overington D (2015) Self-luminous devices and melatonin
suppression in adolescents. Light Res Technol 48(8):966–975. https://doi.
org/10.1177/1477153515584979
34. Bashir H, Bhat SA (2017) Effects of social media on mental health: a review
35 Fuller C, Lehman E, Hicks S, Novick MB (2017) Bedtime use of technol‑
ogy and associated sleep problems in children. Global Pediatr Health
4:2333794x17736972. https://doi.org/10.1177/2333794x17736972
36. Derryberry D, Reed MA (2002) Anxiety-related attentional biases and
their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol 111(2):225–236.
https://doi.org/10.1037//0021-843x.111.2.225
37. Stothart C, Mitchum A, Yehnert C (2015) The attentional cost of receiv‑
ing a cell phone notification. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform
41(4):893–897
38. Sunday OJ (2021) The Effects of Smartphone Addiction on Learning: A
Meta-analysis. Comput Hum Behav Rep 4:100114
39. Egan AC (2016) The psychological impact of smartphones: the effect of
access to one’s smartphone on psychological power, risk taking, cheating,
and moral orientation
40. Caird JK, Johnston KA, Willness CR, Asbridge M, Steel P (2014) A meta-
analysis of the effects of texting on driving. Accid Anal Prev 71:311–318.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aap.2014.06.005
41. Dong G, Huang J, Du X (2012) Alterations in regional homogeneity of
resting-state brain activity in Internet gaming addicts. Behav Brain Func‑
tions 8:41. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744-9081-8-41
42. Bleakley CM, Charles D, Porter-Armstrong A, McNeill MD, McDonough
SM, McCormack B (2015) Gaming for health: a systematic review of the
physical and cognitive effects of interactive computer games in older
adults. J Appl Gerontol. 34(3):Np166-189. https://doi.org/10.1177/07334
64812470747
43. Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2002) Control of goal-directed and stimulus-
driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci 3(3):201–215. https://doi.
org/10.1038/nrn755
44. Katsuki F, Constantinidis C (2014) Bottom-up and top-down attention:
different processes and overlapping neural systems. Neuroscientist
20(5):509–521. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858413514136
45. Chen B, Liu F, Ding S, Ying X, Wang L, Wen Y (2017) Gender differences
in factors associated with smartphone addiction: a cross-sectional study
among medical college students. BMC Psychiatry 17(1):341. https://doi.
org/10.1186/s12888-017-1503-z
46. Cajochen C, Frey S, Anders D, Späti J, Bues M, Pross A, Mager R, Wirz-
Justice A, Stefani O (2011) Evening exposure to a light-emitting diodes
(LED)-backlit computer screen affects circadian physiology and cognitive
performance. J Appl Physiol (Bethesda, Md: 1985) 110(5):1432–1438.
https://doi.org/10.1152/japplphysiol.00165.2011
47. Przybylski AK, Weinstein N (2017) A large-scale test of the goldilocks
hypothesis: quantifying the relations between digital-screen use and the
mental well-being of adolescents. Psychol Sci 28(2):204–215. https://doi.
org/10.1177/0956797616678438
48. Jung CM, Ronda JM, Czeisler CA, Wright KP Jr (2011) Comparison of sus‑
tained attention assessed by auditory and visual psychomotor vigilance
tasks prior to and during sleep deprivation. J Sleep Res 20(2):348–355.
https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2869.2010.00877.x
49. Kao PC (2023) The interrelationship of loneliness, smartphone addiction,
sleep quality, and students’ attention in English as a foreign language
class. Int J Environ Res Public Health 20(4). https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerp
h20043460
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in pub‑
lished maps and institutional affiliations.