Action Research
Action Research
Action Research
McClean (2016)
Introduction
“I am always ready to learn although I do not always like being taught”. These
words belong to the late British statesman, Winston Churchill (Langworth, 2008); but
these words can also be true for any student in the mind of a teacher.
In today’s classroom there are many reasons for why students are performing
unsatisfactory or why students are unmotivated. Reasons one might hear are school work
is too difficult, the subject matter is irrelevant and the teachers are boring just to name a
few. Therefore teachers have attempted to make learning more attractive, engaging and
palatable.
Much research has been carried out to answer the questions of how to get students
motivated and how to keep them motivated in the classroom (Harter, 1981; Hidi &
Harackiewicz, 2000). As a result two schools of thought have been developed: those that
hold to the constructivist theory of teaching and those who don’t (Woolfolk, 2001).
The constructivist approach to teaching stems from the idea that once students are
actively involved in the teaching-learning process they become more invested in their
learning and play an active role in their education (Shuell, 1996; Charles, 2011). Thus
resulting in them being more motivated and eager to learn and be taught. Since seeing
teachers have employed the use of incentives to encourage and keep students motivated
on tasks (Covington & Mueller, 2001; The Gale Group Inc, 2003; Ali, Tatlah, & Saeed,
2011).
This action research aims to investigate the impact of incentives and rewards on
student learning. It focuses on the impact of tangible versus intangible rewards on student
motivation in Mathematics at the 2nd form year level. The term tangible rewards refers to
encouragement.
The following questions are asked and the research is aimed at answering these
questions.
supporting student learning (Lai, 2011, p. 4). According to Anita Woolfolk (2001, p. 366)
motivation speaks to “the internal state that arouses, directs and maintains behaviour”.
This same motivation psychologists have discovered, can be broken down into two
than the enjoyment and satisfaction of engagement itself (The Gale Group Inc, 2003).
While extrinsic motivation refers to being engaged in an activity for the sake of earning
be the attainment of tangible rewards such as money, prizes or other benefits or the
avoidance of punishment. Since the latter type of motivation is widely used by teachers to
study will use this as one of the measures of student motivation in mathematics.
It has been noted by Lai (2011, p. 14) that motivation for students varies from
subject to subject and this may be dependent on the student’s interests. According to
Other researchers noted that students tend to attach more value to activities at which
they excel thus making students more motivated to learn subjects in which they
with respect to subjects such as mathematics, art and science (Epstein & McPartland,
1976; Eccles & Wigfield, 1992). Gottfried and colleagues (2001) also noted that motivation
and self-concept for students tends to increase in age, especially as students accrue more
educational experiences. Some researchers have concluded that children aged 8 – 11 tend
to have an accurate self-perception of their strengths and weaknesses across subject areas
(Guay, et al., 2010). Thus to measure the motivation in mathematics and the impact of a
rewards system on student motivation would be most appropriate amongst the 2nd form
year group.
motivation as it relates to mathematics hence the need for such a study. The focus of this
study is on the impact of intangible versus tangible rewards because most educators have
used incentives as a means of eliciting extrinsic motivation (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan,
2001). This practice of incentives has gained both supporters and non-supporters.
Some researchers believe that the use of incentives to provoke a particular type of
behaviour from students within the classroom is detrimental (Deci E. , 1971). These
researchers hold the view that academic and social skills learned in schools should be
maintained by natural consequence and not artificial rewards. They argue that the use of
incentives leads to moral problems and damages the already existing intrinsic motivation
of students (Kohn, 1994). For example they state that sharing with another child should
come naturally and should not be a forced behaviour due to a rewards system. They also
argue that rewarding one child for good work could have a negative effect on another
student whose work is not to that standard (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 2001; Horner &
Spauling, 2009). These researchers all believe that rewards systems produce children who
fail to develop intrinsic and self-managed motivation because when the rewards go so
does the motivation. Many also maintain that this type of motivation comes at the
expense of interest in and excellence at whatever tasks the students are performing
(Kohn, 1994).
On the flip side, other scholars have noted that schools have successfully
employed the use of rewards systems for decades (Slavin, 1997) and rewards are an
effective, important and fundamental part of education (Akin-Little, Eckert, Lovett, &
Little, 2004; Reiss, 2005). These researchers believe that rewards can be used as stepping
stones or the foundation in building the intrinsic motivation of students (Cameroon &
What both schools of thought for and against tangible rewards systems hold
common, is that verbal rewards or positive feedback does promote some level of
enhanced intrinsic motivation (Deci, Koestner, & Ryan, 2001; Horner & Spauling, 2009).
In addition, there are some researchers who hold the view that there is room for both
tangible and intangible rewards in the motivation of students to learn. They state that
teachers must encourage and nurture intrinsic motivation while making sure that
extrinsic motivation supports learning (Brophy, 1988; Ryan & Deci, 1996; Deci, Koestner,
intangible rewards system, plus the need to identify ways in which students are
motivated and stay motivated on subjects like mathematics. This study is aimed at adding
to or verifying the statements made by either side of the fence with respect to student
motivation in mathematics.
Methodology
This study was carried out on a second form class which contained 31 students. To
answer the questions raised for this research project; both quantitative and qualitative
data collecting methods were used. Observations during class sessions and questionnaires
were some of the instruments used to collect data. The time period over which the data
was collected spanned 6 weeks and the topic of consumer arithmetic was covered.
Below is the instructional plan that was used during the time of the study.
INSTRUCTIONAL PLAN
FORM: 2α3 AGE RANGE: 12-13 years
RATIONALE: This topic serves to educate students about the basics involved in the world of finance. This is done through deepening their
understanding of the terms and concepts surrounding buying and selling; plus implementing the calculation of wages, salaries, commission
and income tax. Students will be exposed to case studies and simulated real life scenarios as a means of assessing and assisting their decision
making processes with respect to finances.
COMMENTS: The class consists of 31 students of whom 15 are males and 16 are females. One young lady in the class must sit at the back of
the class due to eye problems and therefore any pictures or other visual aids that are used in the class are modified to accommodate her. In
addition within the class, there is an exchange male student from London, England.
At the Christ Church Foundation School the textbook used at the first year level is Mathematics for Caribbean Schools Book 2 by Althea
Foster and Terry Tomlinson. In addition the use of calculators is not permitted at this year level.
TOPICS/ GENERAL TEACHING LEARNING RESOURCES/ ASSESSMENT FOLLOW-UP
CONCEPTS OBJECTIVES STRATEGIES/ ACTIVITIES TECHNOLOGIES PROCEDURES ACTIVITIES
METHODS
Define the terms Educational Playing a few Index cards/ Strips of Oral answers Recap the work
Consumer Arithmetic
associated with game [Maths rounds of Maths paper given covered in class
consumer Taboo] Taboo in preparation
arithmetic for next class
Questioning Answering
questions related
to the consumer
arithmetic terms
Creating a
glossary of the
terms
Converting the
statements to
mathematical
equations
Website – www.mathsfoundation.weebly.com
Since the impact of tangible versus intangible rewards on student motivation in
Mathematics at this year level was the focus of this research; for the first 3 weeks of the
study only intangible rewards like words of encouragement were used to motivate the
students. For the remaining 3 weeks a tangible rewards system was implemented within
the class.
During these 3 weeks each student in the class was given a business size
mathematics badge card; a sheet explaining how each badge is earned and a bookmark
sized progress card on which stars were placed to show progress. Artefacts of these
articles can be found in the Appendix B. At the end of the tangible rewards system
period, the student who amassed the most badges was given a prize from the teacher.
Students were tested during both trial periods of intangible and tangible rewards.
The tests were given in this way to observe if the incentive of a prize affected the
academic performance of the students. Samples of the tests and students’ work can be
group collaboration and volunteering to do any tasks in class, were also carried out
before, during and after the implementation of the tangible rewards system. This was
done to obtain as accurate as possible the average number of students who actively
participated in class. This type of observation was used because participation is one of the
key indicators that teachers can use to identify students who exhibit high and low
teacher. This instrument was used to gather information concerning the students’
attitudes towards the subject and tangible rewards. The ideas for some of the items used
mathematics (Doepken, Lawsky, & Padwa, 2007) and the Motivation and Engagement
gender and age were eliminated from the second drafting of the questionnaire since this
information was irrelevant to the research. Items 1 through 10 measured the students’
attitude towards the subject inside and outside of the classroom; items 11 through to 18
measured the students’ participation in the classroom. While the last two items measured
the students’ attitude towards tangible gifts and the effort they exert with respect to the
subject.
The final pool of items on the questionnaire were used because the feedback for
these items fulfilled the aim of the study. To measure the reliability of the questionnaire,
Cronbach’s alpha was calculated for the two sections of the questionnaire. The alpha
coefficient for both sections, items 1 – 10 and items 11 – 18, was 0.7.
For the presentation and analysis of the data collected, tables were used to show
the comparison of students’ tests results as well as student responses for items on the
questionnaire. Pie charts were also be used to illustrate the comparison for some of the
responses from the questionnaire. To show the average number of students who actively
participated in class throughout the study; the mean was tabulated from an excel
Table 1 shows the responses of students to the first 10 items in the questionnaire
1 I am good at mathematics 1 8 20
From this table it is clear that the majority of the class has a positive attitude
towards the subject and they also have confidence in their ability to do mathematics.
The results in Table 2 show that most of the students believe that they participate
in class regularly.
Table 3 shows the students' responses to the last 2 items on the questionnaire
Interestingly, almost half of the participants indicated that a tangible prize would
not motivate them to work harder in mathematics. This response could be one of two
reasons, this portion of the participants either believe that they are working at their
fullest potential already or they are intrinsically motivated more so than extrinsically
motivated.
The charts below pinpoint certain items from the questionnaire and show the
8 17 7 5
6 2 2 25
3 1 28
0 5 10 15 20 25 30
Student Responses
Disagree Neutral Agree
Figure 1 shows the students’ responses for items 3, 6, 8 and 10 from the questionnaire
These questions were chosen because they give a glimpse into the students’
attitudes towards the subject inside and outside of the classroom. An alarming 97% state
that they are sure that they can learn mathematics based on the results for item 3 and
86% state that they are willing to work harder in mathematics even if their grade does
not change due to the results for item 6. However 59% state that they do not read about
mathematics outside of the classroom. While based on the responses to item 10, 76% state
that they enjoy learning more complicated and new topics in mathematics.
A Visual Comparison of Students' Responses to Items
Numbered 11 to 18
18 13 11 4 1
17 1 7 21
16 1 1 16 11
Item Number
15 4 9 8 8
14 5 8 16
13 16 7 4 2
12 10 7 11 1
11 6 6 15 2
0 5 10 15 20 25 30
Student Responses
Figure 2 shows the student responses for items 11 to 18 from the questionnaire
mathematics classes. The majority of the students stated that they ask and answer
questions in class, they gladly work in groups when asked to and come to class prepared.
This is seen in the responses for items 11 to 14. Some 72% state that they have
Although 55% state that they get bored in the sessions sometimes, an overwhelming 97%
state that they pay attention in class most of the time and actively participate in activities.
3%
97%
Figure 3 is a pie chart showing the responses of the students to item 19 on the questionnaire
Twenty-eight out of the twenty-nine students who took part in the survey
indicated that they worked hard in mathematics as shown in the pie chart above. Of all
the participants, only 55% stated that they would work harder if they were getting a
45%
55%
Figure 4 is a pie chart showing the students' responses to item 20 on the questionnaire
The table below shows the results of the students for tests carried out during the
study. Test 1 was carried out when the students were motivated by words of affirmation
and encouragement. While test 2 was carried out when the tangible rewards system was
implemented.
Table 4 shows the test results for the students before and during the tangible rewards system
100
80
Test Percentage
60
40
20
0
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31
Students
Test 1 % Test 2 %
Figure 5 gives a visual representation of the comparison between test 1 and test 2 percentages for
the students
When the percentages themselves are compared between tests for each student, 22
out of the 30 students had improved from test 1 to test 2 while one student’s percentage
remained unchanged. The chart below gives a better correlation of the distribution of the
8
Students
0
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100
Test 1 Test 2
Before the tangible rewards system was implemented eleven out of the thirty students
who took test 1 gained over 70%. For test 2, twenty out of the thirty students who took
that test gained 70% or more. This means that before the incentives were introduced 37%
of the class was gaining at least 70% and when the incentives were introduced that
observations done before the tangible rewards system compared to 21 students during the
tangible rewards system. After the tangible rewards system the average fell to 15 students
Based on the data collected from the various sources; this study proved that
researchers stated tangible rewards do elicit some measure of extrinsic motivation and
this is seen in the increase in the average number of students who actively participated in
class.
During the time span when words of affirmation were used as motivation 42% of
the students were observed to be actively participating in class. When the incentives
were introduced, that percentage rose to 68% but afterwards fell to 48%. This drop in
observed participation after the rewards system confirms two things that researchers
stated:
1. When the incentives are gone so does the extrinsic motivation (Kohn, 1994)
2. Incentives can be used to elicit extrinsic motivation which could set the
foundation for intrinsic motivation (Cameroon & Pierce, 1994; Horner & Spauling,
2009)
This second statement was seen in the fact that although the average percentage of
students participating dropped after the incentives were gone; it was still greater than the
One of the incentives used during the tangible rewards system was the Wiz Kid
badge. A student earned this badge by gaining 70% or more in one test and two
assignments. As was stated earlier in the results section, the number of students gaining at
least 70% before the incentives was 37%, after the incentives this percentage rose to 67%.
This clearly indicates that incentives can impact positively on the quality of work
produced by students; since the topic was the same and both tests were weighted
similarly. In addition although 97% stated that yes they worked hard in mathematics;
55% still indicated that they would work even harder if they were given a tangible prize.
There was not enough evidence to clearly quantify how intangible rewards
impacted student motivation for this study. This could be due to the fact that researchers
have noted that verbal rewards have a greater impact on college aged students than on
my grade does not improve” and “I enjoy the challenge of learning more complicated and
new topics in mathematics”; show that there was some level of intrinsic motivation for
this particular sample of students. Overall the responses to the first 10 items on the
questionnaire show that there was a high level of self-competence amongst the sample
(Gottfried, 1990).
Prior mathematics achievement and prior mathematics motivation was also stated
(Gottfried, 1990). This was clearly seen in the responses to items 2, 3, 4, and 10. Where
for item 2, 89% indicated that they achieved good grades in mathematics and items 3, 4
and 10 attest to the willingness of the students to learn mathematics inclusive of more
difficult topics. In addition the fact that 45% of the students stated that tangible rewards
would not motivate them to work harder in mathematics shows that almost half of the
The data gathered in this study showed that while tangible rewards do have an
can be used to positively encourage students to aim for higher marks in tests and
assignments. These rewards can also be used to elicit more involvement in classroom
activities. Tangible rewards can also be used as a stepping stone to encourage intrinsic
The research carried out also showed with this particular age group, students are
fairly evenly matched with respect to their views on tangible rewards impacting their
motivation towards mathematics. The difference between those for and against the use of
One of the limitations encountered whilst carrying out the study was determining
which type of motivation was most affected by the use of either intangible or tangible
rewards. A recommendation for further research into this area is to measure the impact of
This recommendation is made because intrinsic motivation is said to be longer lasting and
it reinforces critical thinking skills (Lai, 2011) which are necessary for the learning of
mathematics. There is a need for teachers to know what it is that makes students
in their students.
constant reinforcement of what the purpose of the rewards system was for. Some students
had misunderstandings about the way in which some of the badges were earned and tried
to undermine the process. There must be a clear understanding between the teacher and
the students as to what constitutes the earning of prizes when a rewards system is
implemented.
Overall there is place for both intangible and tangible rewards in the motivation of
trying even when a task seems difficult while tangible rewards can be used to get them
started on a task. However tangible rewards should be used sparingly but not in a
controlling manner. One of the by-products of both rewards was the camaraderie forged
amongst the students. This is one of the fundamental blocks in the learning of
mathematics.
References
Akin-Little, K., Eckert, T., Lovett, B., & Little, S. (2004). Extrinsic reinforcement in the classroom:
bribery or best practice. School Psychology Review(33), 344-362.
Ali, Z., Tatlah, I., & Saeed, M. (2011, February). Motivation and student's behaviour: a tertiary level
study. International Journal of Psychology and Counselling, 3(2), 29 - 32.
Baranek, L. (1996). The effects of rewards and motivation on student achievement. Grand Valley
State University, Graduate Research and Creative Practice, Michigan.
Brophy, J. E. (1988). On motivating students. In D. Berliner, & B. Rosenshine (Eds.), Talks to teachers
(pp. 201 - 245). New York: Random House.
Cameron, J., Banko, K., & Pierce, W. (2001). Pervasive negative effects of rewards on intrinsic
motivation: the myth continues. The Behaviour Ananlyst(24), 1- 44.
Cameroon, J., & Pierce, W. D. (1994). Reinforcement, reward and intrinsic motivation: a meta-
analysis. Review of Educational Research, 64(3), 363 - 423.
Charles, C. M. (2011). Building classroom discipline (10 ed.). Boston: Pearson Publishing.
Covington, M., & Mueller, K. (2001). Intrinsic versus extrinsic motivation: an approach/avoidance
reformulation. Educational Psychology Review, 13(2), 157 - 176.
Deci, E. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personailty
and Social Psychology(18), 105 - 115.
Deci, E., Koestner, L., & Ryan, R. (1999). A meta-analytic review of experiments examining the effects
of extrinsic rewards on intrinsic motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 627 - 668.
Deci, E., Koestner, R., & Ryan, R. (2001). Extrinsic rewards and intrinsic motivation in education:
Reconsidered once again. Review of Educational Research, 71(1), 1 - 27.
Doepken, D., Lawsky, E., & Padwa, L. (2007). Woodrod Wilson Leadership Program in Mathematics.
Retrieved from The Woodrow Wilson National Fellowship Foundation.
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (1992). The development of achievement-task values: a theoretical
analysis. Developmental Review, 12, 265 - 310.
Eccles, J. S., & Wigfield, A. (2002). Motivational beliefs, values and goals. Annual Review of
Psychology, 53, 109 - 132.
Epstein , J. L., & McPartland, J. M. (1976). The concept and measurement of the quality of school life.
American Educational Research Journal, 13, 15 - 30.
Giles-Brown, L. (2010). External rewards and intrinsic motivation. Retrieved 2016, from Learning on
the Move: www.learningonthemove.org/external-rewards--intrinsic-motivation.html
Gottfried, A. E. (1990). Academic intrinsic motivation in yound elementary school children. Journal of
Educational Psychology, 82(3), 525 - 538.
Gottfried, A. E., Fleming, J. S., & Gottfried, A. W. (2001). Continuity of academic intrinsic motivation
from childhood through adolescence: A longitudinal study. Journal of Educational
Psychology, 93(1), 3 - 13.
Guay, F., Chanal, J., Ratelle, C. F., Marsh, H. W., Larose, S., & Boivin, M. (2010). Intrinsic, identified
and controlled types of motivation for school subjects in young elementary school children.
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 711 - 735.
Harter, S. (1981). A scale of intrinsic versus extrinsic orientation in the classroom. Denver: Univerisity
of Denver (Department of Psychology).
Hidi, S., & Harackiewicz, J. (2000). Motivating the academically unmotivated: A critical issue for the
21st century. Review of Educational Research, 70(2), 151 - 179.
Horner, R., & Spauling, S. (2009). Rewards. In E. Anderman, L. Hinks Anderman, & MacMillan
Reference USA (Firm), Psychology of classroom learning: An encyclopedia. Detroit:
MacMillan Reference USA/ Gale Cengage Learning.
Kohn, A. (1994). The risks of rewards. Educational Resources Information Center (ERIC) Digest.
Langworth, R. (Ed.). (2008). Churchill by himself: The definitive collection of quotations. New York:
Public Affairs.
Martin, A. (2015). Motivation and emgagement scale - high school. Retrieved from Life Long
Achievement Group: www.lifelongachievement.com/image/data/Sample%20MES-
HS%20for%20Web.pdf
Reiss, S. (2005). Extrinsic and intrinsic motivation at 30: unresolved scientific issues. The Behaviour
Analyst, 28(1), 1 - 14.
Ryan, R., & Deci, E. (1996). When paradigms clash:comments on Cameron and Pierce's claim that
rewards do not undermine intrinsic motivation. Review of Educational Research, 66, 33 - 38.
Shuell, T. J. (1996). Teaching and learning in a classroom context. In D. C. Berliner, & R. C. Calfee
(Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychologist. New York: MacMillan.
Slavin, R. E. (1997). Educational psychology (5th ed.). Needham Heights: Allyn & Bacon.
The Gale Group Inc. (2003). Gale Cengage Learning. Retrieved from www.cengage.com.
Woolfolk, A. (2001). Educational psychology (8 ed.). Needham Heights, Massachusetts: Allyn and
Bacon.